Summers and Humphrey 2006 SIOP.pdf

18
POSTER TITLE DEVIATIONS FROM RATIONAL DECISION- MAKING: AN INTERACTIVE APPROAC H ABSTRACT Whereas the main effects of individual, structural, and environmental factors have been thoroughly examined in the decision-making literature, the paucity of res earch e xamining interactions across levels is stunning. In this manuscript, we present an introductory interaction model that lays the foundation for more complex decision-making models. PRESS PARAGRAPH The present paper posits interactive effects of factors th at influence the decision -making process. Whereas the main effects of individual, structural, and environmental factors have been thoroughly examined in the literatur e, the paucity of research examining interactions across levels is stunning, especially as most decisi ons are made embedded in complex contexts. In this manuscript, we present an introductory interaction model that lays the foundation for more complex d ecision - making models.

Transcript of Summers and Humphrey 2006 SIOP.pdf

  • POSTER TITLE DEVIATIONS FROM RATIONAL DECISION-MAKING: AN INTERACTIVE APPROACH ABSTRACT Whereas the main effects of individual, structural, and environmental factors have been thoroughly examined in the decision-making literature, the paucity of research examining interactions across levels is stunning. In this manuscript, we present an introductory interaction model that lays the foundation for more complex decision-making models.

    PRESS PARAGRAPH The present paper posits interactive effects of factors that influence the decision-making process. Whereas the main effects of individual, structural, and environmental factors have been thoroughly examined in the literature, the paucity of research examining interactions across levels is stunning, especially as most decisions are made embedded in complex contexts. In this manuscript, we present an introductory interaction model that lays the foundation for more complex decision-making models.

  • In recent years, the role of risk in organizations has gained increased recognition as the

    consequences of irrational decisions have become more visible. Most scholars who have studied

    decision-making behavior regarding uncertain organizational situations have focused on single

    determinants of this behavior (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992). Main effects, such as environmental (e.g.,

    Astely & Vande Ven, 1983; Griffin, Tesluk, & Jacobs, 1995; Romanelli & Tushman, 1988;

    Staw, Sandelands, & Dutton 1981; Venkatraman & Prescott, 1990; Wiersema & Bantel, 1993)

    individual (e.g., Driver, Brousseau, & Hunsaker, 1990; Rowe & Mason, 1987; Whyte, Saks, &

    Hook, 1997), and structural factors (e.g., Fredrickson, 1986; March & Simon, 1958; Simon,

    1976) on these outcomes have been examined and accepted in research. However, this approach

    does not reflect the complexity of real life, as decisions are not made in a vacuum where only the

    environment or structure or personality of the decision maker(s) influences the decision. This has

    led to potentially inaccurate conclusions about the causes of irrational behavior (Sitkin & Pablo,

    1992).

    The focus of this paper is on the factors that increase the likelihood of deviating from the

    rational decision model. More specifically, we suggest that individual, structural, and

    environmental factors are all intertwined and thus interact to produce irrational behavior. The

    importance of interactions among constructs is threefold. First, instead of believing that one

    factor is more important than other factors, we suggest that a new dynamic surfaces as a result of

    the interaction. Second, the effects of interactions are not all additive or linear (i.e., some

    interactions may result in multiplicative or exponential effects). Finally, to truly understand what

    factors lead to irrational decisions and for organizations to be able to more accurately influence

    decisions, it is essential to conceptualize an interactive framework.

    It is stressed that the importance of this model is the integration across levels. Prior

  • researchers have suffered from a fragmented, issue-oriented focus that has resulted in overly

    simplified models in which a variety of individual and organizational characteristics are posited

    to separately influence individual decision making behaviors (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992). We

    endeavor to integrate different factors across various levels logically and systematically to

    increase knowledge of the interaction of decision-making factors.

    LITERATURE REVIEW AND THEORETICAL BACKGROUND As shown in Table 1, there are many factors that influence decision making. It is beyond

    the scope of this manuscript to describe and predict how each of these factors interact to impact

    decision-making. Instead, in an effort to both highlight the importance of interactions across

    levels and provide initial theoretical guidance on how outcomes will be impacted by these

    interactions, we present a simplified model that considers only one dimension from each

    category (i.e., core self-evaluations, individuals vs. teams, and threat).

    ------------------------------

    Insert Table 1 about here

    ------------------------------

    For the purposes of this manuscript, irrational decisions are those that deviate from the

    rational decision making model. It is important to note that within this context, irrational

    decisions do not comprise those behaviors that are considered counterproductive work behaviors

    such as workplace violence and aggression, antisocial behaviors, and paranoia. They do,

    however, include those decisions that do not follow basic economic tenants, including risk-

    seeking behaviors and non-risk seeking behaviors. As such, each of these behaviors does not

    follow basic economic theory and, thus, are considered irrational. Risk-seeking and non risk-

    seeking behaviors are further elaborated in the following paragraphs.

  • Risk-seeking behaviors

    According to Sitkin and Pablo (1992, p. 10), risk is a characteristic of decisions that is

    uncertainty about whether potentiality significant and/or disappointing outcomes of decisions

    will be realized. This definition encapsulates three fundamental elements that are necessary for

    its comprehension: outcome uncertainty, outcome expectations, and outcome potential. Risk

    behavior can be characterized by the degree of risk associated with the decisions being made. A

    decision is riskier to the extent that: (1) their expected outcomes are more uncertain; (2) decision

    goals are more difficult to achieve; or, (3) the potential outcome set includes some extreme

    consequences (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992).

    Non risk-seeking behaviors

    Even though the general consensus is that decision makers are non risk-seeking, taken to

    the extreme these behaviors can have many damaging effects. It has been well established in

    financial theory that the more risky assets must compensate risk-averse investors with higher

    expected returns (Huang & Litzenberger, 1988). The more non risk-seeking investors are, the

    lower their expected returns on investments will be (Jianakoplos & Bernasek, 1998). Many

    times, this applies to agents because they are thought to be more risk-averse while their principal

    is more risk-neutral to risk-seeking (Eisenhardt, 1989). Here, non risk-seeking, by itself, is not

    enough to be considered irrational; the behavior demonstrated by the decision maker as to be in

    excess. Put another way, decision makers avoid potential decisions that have even the most

    minute amounts of risk, i.e., they will always go for the sure bet.

    Individual differences

    While decision making has been the subject of long-standing conceptual concern, despite

    some conceptual work (Driver, Brousseau, & Hunsaker, 1990; Rowe & Mason, 1987), there has

  • been little consideration of the impact of individual differences between decision makers

    approaches to or styles of decision making (Spicer & Sadler-Smith, 2005). Thus, different

    interpretations of the same problem can be attributed to individual differences in processing

    capacity combined with factors such as personality and perception.

    Among the most pivotal, fundamental factors of individuals is the rudimentary conviction

    they have of themselves. Researchers have long been interested in how a decision makers self-

    concept affects his or her behaviors, but have lacked a theoretically grounded, validated

    construct for conducting systematic inquiry (Hiller & Hambrick, 2005). However, recent

    research has suggested tha t the concept of core self-evaluation (CSE) may concisely summarize

    this general self-concept (Judge, Erez, Locke, & Thoresen, 2002; Judge, Erez, Bono, &

    Thoresen, 2002; Judge, Erez, & Bono, 1998). Core self-evaluations are the fundamental,

    subconscious conclusions individuals reach about themselves, other people, and the world

    (Judge, Locke, & Durham, 1997).

    Core-self evaluations (CSE) impact an individuals perception of the self. At a basic

    level, the high-CSE individual is characterized by self-confidence, self-worth, self-potency, and

    freedom from anxiety (Hiller & Hambrick, 2005). Decision makers who are high-CSE

    individuals would exhibit more confidence in their decisions due to their high self-confidence

    and low anxiety levels. This suggests that these individuals are more likely to display

    overconfidence in estimating their certainty when being correct or producing a certain outcome

    (Hiller & Hambrick, 2005).

    The importance of core-self evaluations, as a personal disposition in decision making, is

    how it effects an individuals perception of the self. At a basic level, the high-CSE individual is

    characterized by self-confidence, self-worth, self-potency, and freedom from anxiety (Hiller &

  • Hambrick, 2005). It then makes sense that decision makers who are high-CSE individuals would

    exhibit more confidence in their decisions due to their high self-confidence and low anxiety

    levels. Taking this another step, these individuals are more likely to display overconfidence in

    estimating their certainty when being correct or producing a certain outcome (Hiller &

    Hambrick, 2005).

    In addition, because CSE has a historical component, i.e., it is reinforced (or diminished)

    by long-term feedback processes, and finally, subject to further adjustments in the face of recent

    life events (Weinberger, 1994). Therefore, an individual who has had success in the past is more

    likely to exhibit a higher CSE than those individuals who have not; thus,

    Proposition 1: Decision makers who are high-CSE will be more likely to demonstrate

    risk-seeking characteristics than low-CSE decision makers.

    Structure

    The relationship between organizational structure and decision making behavior has been

    a frequent topic for speculation among organizational scientists (Blankenship & Miles, 1968;

    Wally & Baum, 1994). However, to understand why it is logical for the decision process to be

    affected by structure, one must understand the relationship between decision making and

    structure (Fredrickson, 1986). March and Simon (1958) argued that an organizations structure

    imposes boundaries of rationality that accommodate members cognitive limitations. By

    delimiting responsibilities and communication channels, structure allows organizations to

    achieve organizationally rational outcomes in spite of their members cognitive limitations

    (Simon, 1976). They suggest that structure helps management to control the decision-making

    environment and facilitate the processing of information.

    Within the context of this paper, we focus on the differential impact of making decisions

  • in groups versus individuals. A number of researchers have noted the importance of groups in

    decision-making processes (Cohen & Bailey, 1997; Parks & Cowlin, 1995), although most of the

    research on decision-making has occurred at the individual level (Moon et al., 2003). It is

    therefore important to investigate whether groups and individuals differ on irrational behavior.

    Research on group decision making has demonstrated that compared to individuals,

    groups sometimes make better decisions (Libby, Trotman, & Zimmer, 1987; Sniezek & Henry,

    1989) and sometimes make inferior decisions (Gigone & Hastie, 1997). Thus, neither individuals

    nor groups are impervious to irrational decisions.

    The concepts of groupthink (Janis, 1982) and group polarization (Whyte, 1989) suggest

    reasons why groups may make excessively rash decisions. Groupthink refers to a mode of

    thinking that people engage in when they are deeply involved in a cohesive in-group, when the

    members striving for unanimity overrides their motivation to realistically appraise alternative

    course of action (Janis, 1982, p. 9), whereas group polarization is the tendency for group

    discussion to enhance the point of view initially dominant within the group (Myers & Lamm,

    1976; Whyte, 1989).

    Core self-evaluations and groups

    Within groups, CSEs should have a dramatic impact on the decision making process.

    Individuals who have high CSEs would have the propensity to engage in both risk-seeking and

    escalation of commitment behaviors. Compared to high CSE individuals, high CSE groups are

    expected to make even riskier decisions than individuals and commit more resources to failing

    endeavors than will individuals. The notion of groupthink suggests that groups have a tendency

    to make decisions that are in the extremes, both positively and negatively. Consequently, when

    groups with a high overall CSE engage in the decision-making process, initial preferences for

  • risk will be amplified by groupthink. Because one of the symptoms of groupthink is an illusion

    of invulnerability, the overconfidence that is already present by high levels of CSEs will be

    intensified by the illusion of invulnerability created by groupthink.

    Conversely, groups with low CSEs will be more risk averse than individuals with low

    CSEs. When a group has low CSEs, they will lack confidence in their decisions and have high

    levels of anxiety. Because the groups initial response to a decision-making situation will be one

    that is risk averse, group polarization will exaggerate the risk aversion of the group, producing

    decisions that are more risk averse than if an individual alone with the same level of CSE would

    make.

    Proposition 2: Core self-evaluations and decision structure will interact, such that high-

    CSE groups will make the most risky decisions, high-CSE and low-CSE individual will

    make moderate risk decisions, and low CSE groups will make low risk decisions

    Environment

    A popular belief in the business literature is that the competitive environment is growing

    increasingly complex, uncertain, and adverse (Harrington, Lemak, & Kendall, 2002). Hitt,

    Ireland, and Hoskisson (1999) suggested that organizations in the new competitive landscape

    need to have the ability to adapt to environmental change with innovation and speed.

    Contingency theorists have asserted that organizations in uncertain environments should create

    flexible processes to react effectively to adversity or unanticipated change (Burns & Stalker,

    1961; Galbraith, 1977; Lawrence & Lorsch, 1967).

    This literature suggests that in order for decision makers to be successful, they need to be

    vigilant in their assessment of environmental conditions. However, conditions of threat (i.e., an

    impending event that can create negative consequences) impact decision makers ability to reach

  • rational and effective responses (Staw et al., 1981). According to the threat-rigidity thesis (Staw

    et al., 1981), the dominant responses will be maladaptive if the task or environment has radically

    changed and adaptive if the causal relationship between process and performance is stable.

    Broadly, they suggested that an event that has potentially negative or harmful consequences for

    the vital interests of an individual or group leads to a set of responses that tend to be less varied

    or more rigid (Griffin et al., 1995).

    According to Staw et al. (1981) threats have cognitive and motivational effects on

    decision makers. Restriction in information processing may result when stress and anxiety

    induced by threats cause decision makers to confine their range of attention and decrease their

    awareness of peripheral cues. This makes processing of new or complex information difficult

    and restricts alternatives considered to those that are consistent with conservative and well-

    learned interpretive frames. Consequently, when under highly threatening situations, individuals

    will reduce their effort to search for information (Staw et al., 1981), leading to a sub-optimal

    decision.

    Core self-evaluations, individuals vs. groups, and threat

    We suggest that high CSE groups who face environments that are exceedingly

    threatening will make the most risky decisions. Because high CSE groups already have a

    predisposition towards risky and escalating behaviors, amalgamating this with a threatening

    environment will only exacerbate risk-seeking and escalating behaviors. Due to a predilection for

    risk-seeking and escalating behaviors, group polarization will be exacerbated by the threat that

    the group experiences. However, threat will only exacerbate the level of risk-seeking and

    escalating behaviors if the group makes internal attributions for the threat (Harrington et al.,

    2002).

  • The least risky behaviors would be expected from low-CSE groups experiencing high

    levels of threat. Just as low CSE groups will be risk-averse, when high levels of threat interact

    with low CSE groups the level of risk-aversion increases. The high levels of threat will decrease

    information search while simultaneously increasing the levels of anxiety within the group. This

    will lead to the well- learned risk-averse responses that the group has regressed to in the past.

    First, the group polarization that increases the level of risk-aversion initiated by the low CSE

    group will only be exacerbated by the high levels of threat experienced by the group. Moreover,

    when high levels of threat interact with a predisposed position, threat amplifies the tendency of

    the group beyond that of group polarization. However, threat will only exacerbate risk-aversion

    if the group makes internal attributions for the threat (Harrington et al., 2002).

    Proposition 3: CSE, structure, and threat will interact, such that the most risky decisions

    will be made by high-CSE groups with internal attributions for threat, whereas the least

    risky decisions will be made low-CSE groups with internal attributions for threat

    DISCUSSION

    Conceivably the most significant contribution of this paper is that, through a concurrent

    investigation of formerly unconnected streams of research, we have suggested gaps in the

    decision making literature that need to be addressed. Even though the preexisting fragmented

    state of research on risk-seeking and non risk-seeking behaviors were not conducive to be readily

    identified, the proposed model has perhaps provided a reformulated theory that is more complete

    and parsimonious than previous theories. The analysis presented in this paper thus advances the

    understanding of deviations from rational decision making by suggesting that interactions among

    various decision making factors can increase the likelihood of behaviors that do follow the basic

    tenets of utility theory.

  • The theory of risk behavior propositioned here builds directly on current theories of risk-

    seeking and non risk-seeking behaviors in illuminating individual risk behavior. Academics have

    previously illustrated the roles of individual differences (Driver, Brousseau, & Hunsaker, 1990;

    Rowe & Mason, 1987; Whyte, Saks, & Hook, 1997), structural factors (Fredrickson, 1986;

    Gigone & Hastie, 1997; Libby, Trotman, & Zimmer, 1987; Sniezek & Henry, 1989), and

    environmental factors (Harrington et al., 2002; Staw et al., 1981). Therefore, one contribution of

    this paper is that it draws together these previously unrelated streams of research and

    demonstrates how they offer the groundwork for a theoretical model of risk-seeking and non

    risk-seeking behavior that is more comprehensive and accurate than prior theories.

    One of the reasons for formalizing the proposed theory in the form of an interactive

    model is to facilitate cumulative empirical research in this area. In part, this is in response to the

    observation that a great deal of has been learned within relatively narrow specialized subareas of

    risk behavior and non risk behavior, but that there has been little integration across the

    historically discrete theoretical perspectives. The ideas proposed in this paper are meant to

    provide preliminary evidence of the value of such cross-specialty research, and it is hoped that

    this framework can stimulate additions, revisions, and challenges to this analysis. By attempting

    to delineate more explicitly the different constructs and theories about their interactive

    relationships, the goals here are to provide a broader and more coherent framework for designing

    empirical research on risk-seeking and non risk-seeking that can clearly and effectively

    distinguish these important variables.

    The combination of various theoretical perspectives (e.g.) implies that future empirical

    research should not merely examine isolated cells in a two-by-two matrix (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992),

    but should modify completely individual, structural, and environmental dimensions to provide a

  • test of ideas proposed here as well as the theories and empirical findings on which they are

    based. Consequently, this paper responds to the call for the development of theories that can

    concurrently investigate the effects of disposition (individual differences) and situations

    (structure and environment) (Davis-Blake & Pfeffer, 1989). Additionally, it is essential for future

    empirical studies to reflect the operational intricacy of the constructs presented in this paper,

    taking into account the importance of the numerous variables of individual, structural, and

    environmental in a systematic and valid approach.

    Future research on interaction terms

    There are many additional plausible interaction effects suggested by the proposed model.

    This limitation, however, suggests another way by which researchers could extend the ideas

    proposed herethrough the theoretical evaluation and empirical testing of possible interaction

    effects among the independent variables in Table 1. Researchers interested in specialized

    subareas of risk-seeking, escalation of commitment, and risk-aversion may choose to focus on

    smaller sets of variables so as to address the possible interaction effects among them.

    CONCLUSION A sizeable body of prior theoretical and empirical research appeared to support the

    primary influence of objective or perceived interactional effects of individual differences,

    structural, and environmental factors on risk-seeking and non risk-seeking behaviors. Yet the

    reconceptualization presented in this paper suggests that this earlier work and the seemingly

    unconnected results it generated can be explained by a theory in which the determinants of

    deviations from rational decision making interact to form new decision making dynamics. By

    providing a single, integrated and interactive model of the determinants of deviations from

    rational decision making, this paper not only draws together conclusions of a number of theories

  • that have not previously been compared, but also highlights opportunities of for future theoretical

    and empirical work.

    As should be noted in Table 1, the potential opportunities for research on this topic are

    virtually limitless. Furthermore, the empirical testing among the various factors would be

    extremely beneficial for the field, as not much has been done to date. By testing the interaction

    of assorted variables from the individual differences, structural, and environmental columns,

    research on decision making could make quantum leaps in our field.

  • REFERENCES

    Astley, W.G., & Van de Ven, A.H. (1983). Central perspectives and debates in organizational

    theory. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28, 245-273.

    Blankenship L.V., & Miles, R.E. (1968). Organizational Structure and Managerial Decision

    Making. Administrative Science Quarterly, 13, 106-120.

    Burns, R.J., & Stalker, G.M. (1961). The management of innovation. London: Tavistock

    Productions Limited.

    Cohen, S.G., & Bailey, D.E. (1997). What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research from

    the shop floor to the executive suite. Journal of Management, 23, 239-290.

    Davis-Blake, A., & Pfeffer, J. (1989). Just a mirage: The search for dispositional effects in

    organizational research. Academy of Management Review, 14, 385-400.

    Driver, M.J., Brousseau, K.E., & Hunsaker, P.L. (1990). The dynamic decision-maker. New

    York: Harper.

    Eisenhardt, K.M. (1989). Agency theory: An assessment and review. Academy of Management

    Review, 14, 57-74.

    Fredrickson, J.W. (1986). The strategic decision making process and organizational structure.

    The Academy of Management Review, 11, 280-297.

    Galbraith, J.R. (1977). Organizational design. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing

    Company.

    Gigone, D., & Hastie, R. (1997). Proper analysis of the accuracy of group judgments.

    Psychological Bulletin, 121, 149-167.

    Griffin, M. A., Tesluk, P. E., & Jacobs, R. R. (1995). Bargaining cycles and work-related

    attitudes: Evidence for threat-rigidity. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 1709-1723.

  • Harrington, R., Lemak, D., & Kendall, K.W. (2002). The threat-rigidity thesis in new formed

    teams: An empirical test. Journal of Business and Management, 8, 127-145.

    Hiller, N.J., & Hambrick, D.C. (2005). Conceptualizing executive hubris: The role of (hyper-)

    core self-evaluations in strategic decision-making. Strategic Management Journal, 26,

    297-319.

    Hitt, M.A., Ireland, R.D., & Hoskisson, R.E. (1999). Strategic management competitiveness and

    globalization. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western College Publishing.

    Huang, C., & Litzenberger, R.H. (1988). Foundation for financial economics. New York: North

    Holland.

    Janis, I.L. (1982). Groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Jianakoplos, N.A., & Bernasek, A. (1998). Are women more risk averse? Economic Inquiry, 36,

    620-630.

    Judge, T.A., Erez, A., & Bono, J.E. (1998). The power of being positive: The relation between

    positive self-concept and job performance. Human Performance, 11, 167-187.

    Judge, T.A., Erez, A., Bono, J.E., & Thoresen, C.J. (2002). Are measures of self-esteem,

    neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core

    construct? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 693-710.

    Judge, T.A., Locke, E.A., & Durham, C.C. (1997). The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A

    core evaluations approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19, 151-188.

    Lawrence, P.R., & Lorsch, J.W. (1967). Organization and environment managing differentiation

    and integration. Boston, MA: Harvard University.

    Libby, R., Trotman, K.T., & Zimmer, I. (1987). Member variation, recognition of expertise, and

    group performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 81-87.

  • March, J.G., & Simon, H.A. (1958). Organizations. New York: Riley.

    Moon, H., Conlon, D.E., Humphrey, S.E., Quigley, N, Devers, C.E., & Nowakowski, J.M.

    (2003). Group decision process and incrementalism in organizational decision making.

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 92, 67-79.

    Myers, D.G., & Lamm, H. (1976). The group polarization phenomenon. Psychology Bulletin, 83,

    602-627.

    Parks, D.C., & Cowlin, R. (1995). Group discussion as affects by number of alternatives and by

    a time limit. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 267-275.

    Romanelli, E., & Tushman, M.L. (1988). Executive leadership and organizational outcomes: An

    evolutionary perspective, in D. Hambrick (Ed.), (pp. 129-140). The Executive Effect:

    Concepts and Methods for Studying Top Managers, Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Rowe, A.J., & Mason, R.O. (1987). Managing with style: A guide to understanding, assessing,

    and improving decision making. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Simon, H.A. (1976). Administrative Behavior (3rd ed.). New York: Free Press.

    Sitkin, S.B., & Pablo, A.L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior.

    Academy of Management Review, 17, 9-38.

    Sniezek, J.A., & Henry, R.A. (1989). Accuracy and confidence in group judgment.

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 1-28.

    Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. (1981). Threat-rigidity effects in organizational

    behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26, 501-524.

    Venkatraman, N., & Prescott, J.E. (1990). The environment-strategy coalignment: An empirical

    test of its performance implications. Strategic Management Journal, 11, 1-23.

    Wally, S., & Baum, J.R. (1994). Personal and structural determinants of the pace of strategic

  • decision making. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 932-956.

    Whyte, G. (1989). Groupthink reconsidered. Academy of Management Review, 14, 40-56.

    Whyte, G., Saks, A.M., & Hook, S. (1997). When success breeds failure: The role of self-

    efficacy in escalating commitment to a losing course of action. Journal of Organizational

    Behavior, 18, 415-432.

    Wiersema, M.F., & Bantel, K.A. (1993). Top management team turnover as an adaptation

    mechanism: The role of the environment. Strategic Management Journal, 14, 485-504.

  • Table 1 Determinants of Decision Making

    Individual Differences Structure Environment Core Self-Evaluations Individual vs. Team Threat

    Core External Evaluations Organizational Structure: Vertical vs. Flat

    Past Performance

    Five Factor Model of Personality

    Organizational Structure: Mechanistic vs. Organic Politics

    Individualism/Collectivism Organizational Structure: Divisional vs. Functional Technology

    Attribution Style/Biases Rewards (agency issues) Dynamic vs. Static (Time) Myers-Brigg Type Indicator

    (MBTI) Hierarchical Teams vs. Self-

    Managing Work Teams Innovative vs. Mature

    Affective Disposition Level of Empowerment Cultural Risk Values

    Social Skill Organizational Control Systems

    Leader Risk Orientation

    Political Skill Team Composition Problem Familiarity Risk Preference, Risk

    Perceptions, Risk Propensity Size Social Influences

    General Decision Making Style (GDMS) Satisficing vs. Maximizing Level of Uncertainty

    National/Cultural