Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

download Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

of 29

Transcript of Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    1/29

    Constructing the Citizen

    Helen Haste

     Department of Psychology, University of Bath, and Harvard Graduate School

    of Education

     Discussions of citizenship and citizenship education have been conducted largely within

    the worldview of stable, Western societies and have been based on psychological models

    that emphasize individual cognition. The concepts of citizenship that evolved in this context 

    have become taken for granted. But during the past decade, different concepts of citizen-

    ship have arisen from emergent democracies, from societies in transition, from the disso-

    lution of the left-right spectrum in Western society, and from a changing perspective in

     psychological theory that attends to language and to social and cultural context. These

    developments have implications for defining the goals of citizenship education and for for-

    mulating educational programs, particularly in relation to identity, positioning, narratives,

    and efficacy.

    KEY WORDS: citizenship education, political development, culture, identity, narrative, positioning,

    efficacy

    At one of the first international conferences at which there was substantial

    representation from the former “Eastern bloc,” more than a decade ago, a young

    North American researcher compared “democratic values” among young people

    in a North American sample and a sample from an Eastern bloc country. Theresearcher concluded that the North American sample had a more “sophisticated”

    understanding of “democracy.” However, many Western members of the audience

    found this study embarrassing, and many Eastern bloc members were angered by

    it. The measure was blatantly parochial, embedded in North American practices.

    There was no attempt to find parallel examples from the Eastern bloc country; no

    account was taken of how its young people might be formulating their own dem-

    ocratic understanding. Hopefully, such hegemonic mistakes would no longer be

    made. The young researcher was in good company. Francis Fukuyama (1992)

    declared the “end of history” in the wake of triumphal liberal democracy. In fact,

    Political Psychology, Vol. 25, No. 3, 2004

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    2/29

    in the past decade we have seen the proliferation of versions of democracy (and

    Fukuyama has retracted his position).

    The past decade has brought “surprises” that confront taken-for-granted

    assumptions about political processes and their psychological parameters, chal-

    lenge both theory and method in the field of citizenship, and challenge how we

    think about democracy, its functions and antecedents. I consider three such sur-

    prises below. I also consider how recent developments in psychological episte-

    mology provide a different, critical understanding of the implications of these

    surprises, and how this affects the agendas of citizenship “construction.” How do

    we construct individuals as citizens, and how do we construct the concept of

    citizenship itself?

    The first surprise concerns the distinction between “stable” and “changing”

    or “transitional” societies. It is hardly news that a society in flux has different

    characteristics from one in a static state, but the nature of those differences has

    become apparent, in particular through the observation of post–Soviet bloc coun-

    tries. It has also become clear that much of the tradition of research on citizen-

    ship has been implicitly located within the assumptions of stable societies.

    The second surprise concerns nationalism. Much research on ideology treats

    nationalism as an extreme right-wing position and in some sense pathological.

    However, as recent events show, political upheaval creates the need for new nar-

    ratives to appeal to different historical precedents that justify new systems; nations

    recreate themselves through reconstruction of their history. Newly emergent

    democracies sought to define their systems and find a new identity, not by copying

    current Western systems (whether U.S. or northern European) but by seeking a

    period of their own history that reflected a “golden age” of democracy and 

    national pride (Janowski, 1999; Valkova & Kalous, 1999; Wertsch, 1998). For

    some nations, this golden period was one in which the nation fought off an oppres-

    sor; for others, it was a period of enlightenment and cultural expansion. The

    message is twofold: Nationalism is not necessarily pathological, and “democracy”

    is not a universal or a unitary concept but is transmuted by each state through its

    own cultural narratives.

    A third surprise comes from the dissolution of the left-right spectrum in

    Western democracies and the fragmentation of old ideological boundaries. The split

    in the Right between “free market” libertarian thought and “traditional” religious,

    socially conservative thought has been appraised by political philosophers for three

    decades, and is reflected in both extreme and moderate right-wing parties (Eatwell,

    2003; Eatwell & O’Sullivan, 1990). The emergence of “Third Way” socialism and

    liberalism reflects the fragmentation of traditional socialism based on class strug-

    gle and the welfare state. A key development is the emergence of “emancipatory

    politics,” political movements that reflect the moralization of politics, driven by a

    rhetoric based on justice or on responsibility (Giddens, 1994, 1998). These move-

    414 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    3/29

    reconsider pervasive assumptions about the nature of belief systems and ideology,

    and their philosophical and psychological underpinnings.

    What do these surprises tell us? First, they undermine some of the frame-

    works within which we have been studying citizenship. They challenge many

    assumptions about the nature of beliefs and their consistency, such as the idea that

    beliefs are static, enduring attributes of the individual. They show that ideologi-

    cal structures in stable societies (where most of the research has been done until

    recently) may be very different from those in changing societies. This leads us to

    look more carefully at just how these structures function. If nationalism is a key

    part of the construction of new identity for a nation in transition, we should also

    look again at national identity and collective memory in stable societies.

    Second, we should look more carefully at political motivation and engage-

    ment. Party commitment is of low salience to most citizens, as research has con-

    tinually shown. So when we see how effectively a morally engaging issue (such

    as the environment) can motivate even pre-adolescent children, we should attend

    to the implications of this for citizenship education.

    Finally, we should ask questions about what ideology does—what is its func-

    tion? How does it work? Weltman and Billig (2001) argued that “ideology is not

    so much a preset pattern of thinking, but provides the elements of dilemmas for

    the ideological subjects to think and argue about . . . . The psychological study of 

    ideology, therefore, needs to pay close attention to examining how the themes of 

    ideology are used and managed in ordinary talk” (p. 369). In all the surprises, we

    see ideology in process, we see beliefs serving a function in context —providing

    narratives to make sense of history and the future, to justify political or social

    practices, to sustain shared identity.

    The surprises, I argue, lead us to view the individual as an active being con-

    structing—and co-constructing with others—explanations and stories that enable

    him or her to make sense of experience, and to develop an identity in a particu-

    lar social context. This shifts the psychological emphasis away from looking

    solely at internal cognition, toward investigating how active construction and dia-

    logue take place within a framework of multiple and parallel interactions situated

    in a social, cultural, and historical context. This is consistent with theoretical

    developments in social and developmental psychology that take on board culture

    and historical context and have challenged the search for “universals.” Taking a

    cultural perspective requires attention not only to what is believed and valued, but

    how and why. This is reflected, for example, in Bar-On’s (2001) critique of Israeli

    psychologists for implicitly accepting a U.S. worldview and ignoring not only

    how the specific Israeli experience might challenge U.S.-based normative assump-

    tions, but also ignoring the very rich social-psychological material that the Israeli

    experience could uniquely offer.

    Attending to culture and context also entails a less “top down” view of polit-

    Constructing the Citizen 415

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    4/29

    at what kinds of experience engage them. We also need to address the diverse

    definitions of “participation” and the implications of these for the construction of 

    the citizen. As the British political philosopher Bernard Crick, who chaired a

    recent government committee on citizenship education, wrote: “Too much of 

    political socialisation research turns out simply to be over-structured investiga-

    tion of the attitudes of schoolchildren to adult political concepts. There is too little

    on the political language and lore of schoolchildren, there is no political Piaget”

    (Crick, 1999, p. 342).

    In this paper I argue that focusing more on cultural, social, and linguistic

    processes will enable us to make sense of the “surprises” of the last decade and

    to provide a richer approach to, and agenda for, citizenship education. I draw on

    recent research that offers a somewhat more optimistic, and hopefully less theo-

    retically sterile, picture of political development than Crick indicates.

    Stable and Changing Societies

    In a stable society, the structures of governance remain largely intact even in

    times of temporary change (such as economic crisis). The individual citizen may

    participate through voting, campaigning, or becoming involved in a pressure

    group. If he or she wishes to challenge the status quo, there are channels for

    confrontation and persuasion, whether conventional or unconventional, legal or

    illegal. The skills and motivation required are personal efficacy and the ability to

    organize within the institutional and human resources of the political milieu.

    This is different from a changing society where, although the individual

    citizen does not have any more real power than in a stable state, he or she is likely

    to feel more personally affected by the changes and thus potentially more engaged.

    Van Hoorn, Komlosi, Suchar, and Samuelson (2000) studied young people in

    Hungary and Poland during the period 1990–1995. There was an initial period of 

    optimism and desire to formulate new ways of thinking about—and enacting—

    “democracy,” which engaged these young people to a far greater extent than

    would be found in a parallel cohort in a stable society. Even the disillusionment

    that followed the failure of early efforts in many cases sprang from engagement

    that did not succeed, rather than merely loss of trust in government. Other studies

    of former Eastern bloc countries reported similar findings (Flanagan et al., 1999).

    In a study of urban black South African adolescents during the immediate post-

    apartheid era, Abrahams (1995) found extraordinary optimism about both their

    own new opportunities and the potential for a new society. Most of them had been

    involved in protest throughout their lives; they were engaged and involved, and

    were constructing their personal identities around these expectations.

    National Identity and Nationalism

    416 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    5/29

    race, religion, or language. Nationalism in this form is not treated in the research

    literature as a morally legitimate basis for group identity—in contrast to “patri-

    otism,” a desirable commitment to one’s nation and its values. It is described as

    “tribalism,” “ethnocentrism,” bigotry, or racism (Hall, 1993). Psychologists have

    also explained such manifestations, at least in part, through individual pathology.

    In his insightful book  Banal Nationalism (1995), Billig pointed out that

    pathologizing nationalism ignores the universality of identity attached to nation-

    hood. He analyzed how our membership of our nation is affirmed daily, through

    symbols and rhetoric so routine that we do not consciously notice them. It is

    because this is so taken for granted that we are “surprised” when a particular

    group, perceiving itself threatened or in need of bolstering its identity, makes the

    underlying nationalist criteria explicit. Nationalism, Billig argued, is all around

    us all the time, in “banal” forms. The kinds of nationalism that have emerged in

    the past decade cannot be explained only by pathologizing models.

    As Hall and Billig reminded us, we can learn much from the great period of 

    “nation-building” in the 19th century. At that time there were several different

    kinds of nation: those that were politically well established (like the United

    Kingdom), and nations that were newly created through reorganization into

    republics (such as France) or through unification (like Italy and Germany). All

    were engaged in generating a “mythic” history, with hero figures who were “form-

    ative” in the “nation’s” past, and symbols of nationality that would coalesce the

    people into a new national identity. As Massimo d’Azeglio said after the

    Risorgimento, “We have created Italy, now we must create Italians” (Billig, 1995,

    p. 25).

    This process frequently involves the suppression of “minority” symbols, such

    as languages (as in the suppression of Welsh in British schools, or Cajun in U.S.

    schools). Unsurprisingly, resistant minorities retained these very symbols—

    overtly or covertly—as a mark of their own “national identity,” and in response

    to such resistance “nationalism” becomes defined by the majority culture as a

    pathological extremism, for it threatens the “official” national identity. Many

    examples of the power of resistant identity could be found in former Soviet states.

    Wertsch (1998) noted that in Estonia, throughout the Soviet era two parallel ver-

    sions of history flourished: the “official” Soviet version, as taught in schools and

    acknowledged in public discourse, and the “unofficial” version—which has now

    become the “official” version.

    We speak of the “new” post-Soviet nations, but most of them are in fact very

    old. These nations are redefining their history, their culture, and their salient

    symbols. They are also defining the outgroups that are perceived to threaten this

    new nationhood. These developments challenge us to find explanations in terms

    of a psychology of nation-building rather than mass deviance. They show how,

    through collective memory, present identity is formed: Salient past events—and

    Constructing the Citizen 417

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    6/29

    national identity and moral links to past suffering—as, for example, in projects

    to ensure remembering of the Holocaust. Past “golden ages” provide guidelines

    for values and practices in creating an effective nation; past tragedies confirm

    shared identities.

    How the nation’s history is drawn upon to formulate new versions of democ-

    racy is evident in the interviews with young people in Hungary and Poland con-

    ducted by van Hoorn et al. (2000). The young people’s discourse reflected the

    public debates, tensions, and contradictions as different versions of what it means

    to be “Hungarian” or “Polish” emerged during the period. Bar-Tal (2000) showed

    how the “story” of Israeli nationhood is told within a recurrent narrative of siege

    and response to siege, and how the issue of “security” plays a vital and central

    role in concepts of patriotism and nationhood. Like other Israeli social psychol-

    ogists, Bar-Tal is sensitive to the contradictions of applying stable, Western con-

    cepts of nationalism and citizenship to the Israeli situation.

    These developments direct us to take national identity seriously in the devel-

    opment of citizenship. They also draw our attention to the role of narrative and

    collective memory in identity. This also shifts explanations of nationalism away

    from primarily individual cognitive or personality characteristics, to include the

    cultural and historical context within which meaning is negotiated.

    The Dissolution of the Left-Right Spectrum

    My third “surprise” is a challenge not only to how we think about the polit-

    ical values map, but to the psychological basis of ideology itself. In psychology

    there has been a rich tradition of explaining belief systems on the basis of per-

    sonality function, or cognitive style. Such explanations make certain assumptions

    about the nature of belief systems—most notably, that beliefs are enduring and

    are conjoined by an underlying principle (such as attitudes to authority). The argu-

    ment for a functional basis for beliefs—serving cognitive or personality “needs”—

    is much stronger if beliefs are seen as consistent and as bounded. These

    psychological models have mapped easily onto the left-right political dimension,

    which has existed in some form for more than 200 years. However, the con-

    stituents of this dimension have changed substantially over that time. Within both

    “left” and “right” there have always been strange bedfellows, held together by

    political expediency and also by self-identification with party membership, such

    as the liberal and populist-conservative wings of the U.S. Democrats and the

    mixture of free-market libertarians and social traditionalists of the British

    Conservative party (Eatwell & O’Sullivan, 1990).

    The dissolution of the left-right spectrum as a credible and useful value

    dimension has come about in part through major changes in the map of ideology.

    The split between free-market libertarian thought and “traditional” religious and

    418 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    7/29

    forefronted some fundamentally different underlying assumptions. The emergence

    of “Third Way” versions of socialism and liberalism, expressly proposed by the

    British Labour party and inherent in the British Liberal Democrats, reflected also

    a fragmentation of traditional socialism based on neo-Marxist perspectives and

    class struggle (Blair, 1998; Giddens, 1994, 1998; Weltman, 2003). These devel-

    opments are manifested not only in political writings but in lay thinking: Indi-

    viduals may still identify themselves as “left” or “right” but the definitions have

    become problematic; even the party faithful struggle to find discourses that avoid

    polarization (Eatwell, 2003; Weltman, 2003; Weltman & Billig, 2001). As Eatwell

    (2003) noted, one of the paradoxes of emergent extreme right-wing parties in

    Europe is that they have benefited from the growing “centrism” of the mainstream,

    for it provides a niche for explicit non-mainstream positions; yet at the same time,

    many such parties declare that they are “neither right nor left.”

    A major aspect of the fragmentation is the emergence of what are variously

    termed “emancipatory” or “liberationist” politics, driven by justice for minority

    groups or by moral concerns. Such movements cut across the traditional left-right

    spectrum, even though they may take different forms when manifested by liber-

    als and conservatives. The late 20th-century feminist movements derived initially

    from the Left, but challenging gender discrimination is no longer confined to the

    Left. “Right-wing” (or “liberal”) feminists, who vote for conservative political

    parties, also emphasize free-market values, liberal principles of equal opportunity,

    and individual achievement and progress. “Left-wing” feminists are more likely

    to locate gender politics within the rhetoric and structures of class and inequality,

    and to seek more social structural changes to overcome these (Bull, Diamond, &

    Marsh, 2001; Haste, 1992, 1994, 2001a; Klatch, 1987). But whole areas of fem-

    inism fall outside these parameters, focusing on language and culture, which may

    draw from rhetorics of the Left but equally draw from psychoanalysis or literary

    theory (Haste, 2001a). Even if one can trace the roots of these perspectives to left-

    aligned intellectual movements, it is not useful to categorize them primarily within

    this framework.

    There are many parallels with environmentalist movements. Both the roots

    of these movements and their current manifestations draw from diverse sources

    and discourses (Harré, Brockmeier, & Mühlhausler, 1999). The “left” versions

    may emphasize globalization and anti-capitalism, but there are strong environ-

    mentalist elements on the extreme right who focus on “blood and soil” and their

    heritage (Eatwell, 2003). More “mainstream” environmentalists eschew the polit-

    ical labels but have been criticized for an inherently conservative, romantic

    “wilderness” myth of a North America empty of indigenous peoples (Cronon,

    1995). However, environmentalism is interesting not only for its political diver-

    sity but for its moral power. It is astonishing that within little more than 20 years,

    environmentalism has moved from being a radical fringe movement to being a

    Constructing the Citizen 419

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    8/29

    There are important messages for citizenship education from these develop-

    ments, particularly about what motivates engagement . Issues that have a moral

    connotation engage the individual through compassion, anger, or moral outrage.

    This may be associated with personal identity—either through identification with

    a specific social category (such as gender or ethnicity) or through taking personal

    responsibility for pursuing one’s moral insight. Ecological issues carry this moral

    force. As many studies have found, the environment tops the list of young people’s

    concerns, and voting comes lowest. Few young people become enthralled by the

    issues that preoccupy political parties. Most strikingly, young children are moti-

    vated by ecological moral concerns; a large number of 6-year-olds want to save

    the rainforest (Flanagan et al., 1999; Greenall Gough, 1993; Kahn, 1999).

    Critical and Cultural Psychology; Challenging Epistemology

    The fragmentation of the left-right dimension undermines the assumption that

    a particular belief will necessarily be associated with a particular ideology. To

    deal with this, as Billig noted, explanation has to move away from models based

    on consistency and enduring belief patterns, and needs to consider how particu-

    lar beliefs and values function in context—in identity, in discursive and social

    practices, in rhetoric, in narratives. Our “surprises” all demonstrate that people

    negotiate meaning, rhetoric, narrative, and explanation in highly contextualized

    ways. To understand this, we need to look at language and dialogue. We need to

    look at rhetorical and discursive processes, not only at cognitive processes

    (Billig, 1996; Edwards, 1997; Harré & Gillett, 1994). We need to look particu-

    larly at narrative: shared narrative, competing narrative, narratives that are taken

    for granted, narratives that locate, explain, and justify the citizen and the nation.

    The message of the “surprises” is that a model primarily focused on individual

    cognition is inadequate. Constructing the citizen does not go on just inside

    individual heads.

    We must pay attention to the individual actively in dialogue, rather than the

    individual at the end of a conduit of “influence.” Rather than being regarded as

    passively “socialized,” the individual actively constructs—and co-constructs with

    others—explanations and stories that make sense of experience, to develop an

    identity that locates her or him in a social, cultural, and historical context. Self 

    and group identity, negotiated through narrative and dialogue as well as through

    trying to make sense of social structures and representations, are crucial to under-

    standing the construction of the citizen. Identity comprises group membership and

    self-definition in terms of social categories, including nationhood, community,

    sense of place, and ethnic and religious identity, where these are salient. It defines

    who shall be deemed ingroup and outgroup, and therefore, what shall be the basis

    for sharing symbols and metaphors with others. It also includes self-identity, in

    420 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    9/29

    point for dialogue and argumentation. The construction of the citizen is in part

    the construction of an identity (Antaki & Widdicombe, 1998).

    This is consistent with theoretical developments in social and developmen-

    tal psychology. Bruner (1990) distinguished “paradigmatic” and “narrative” forms

    of knowledge. The tension between the paradigmatic and the narrative is long-

    standing—contrasting a form of knowledge that is representational, and legit-

    imized by factual evidence, and ways of knowing based on narrative storytelling,

    drawing upon cultural allusion and shared references, with an emphasis on the

    negotiation of meaning. Bruner’s work is part of the rise of discursive social psy-

    chology, in which psychologists are drawing on anthropology, linguistics, and

    semiotics to analyze narrative, discourse, and rhetoric, and to see how dialogue

    actually works (Antaki & Widdicombe, 1998; Billig, 1996; Edwards, 1997; Harré

    & Gillett, 1994).

    Bruner originally conceptualized the narrative approach as a rich alternative

    to the narrowly paradigmatic, but he later argued that this is an incomplete picture

    (Bruner, 1995). The problem with the paradigmatic model was its exclusion of 

    the social; the problem with the narrative model is that it focused only on the

    social, leaving out the active individual agent. In The Culture of Education (1995),

    Bruner integrated neo-Vygotskian psychology as a means of bringing the indi-

    vidual agent into the arena, as an active participant in the process of construction

    and negotiation of meaning.

    Vygotsky conceptualized the human being as interdependent with others, in

    scaffolding development of understanding, in active use of language and dialogue

    to construct meaning—within a cultural context (Rogoff, 1990; Vygotsky, 1978;

    Wertsch, 1985, 1998). Incorporating culture has had a somewhat problematic

    history in psychology (Shore, 1996). For example, Wundt (1921) distinguished

    two psychologies: one focused on processes which might have some physiologi-

    cal basis; the other on social and cultural processes, involving language and higher

    mental processes. For Wundt, a full picture of the human being was impossible

    without both. However, under the pressures for developing psychology as a pos-

    itivistic science, only the first was historically credited as the “origin” of psy-

    chology, the second being lost and indeed marginalized as “folk psychology”—a

    mistranslation that damned it thoroughly for a century. It has taken a substantial

    challenge to that very tradition to restore a cultural and language-based perspec-

    tive to psychology. For some psychologists, personal experience provided the

    impetus for taking culture seriously. Cole (1996) reported starting his research

    career in West Africa administering U.S.-created tests to children, and wondering

    why they did not work. His intellectual journey took him far beyond merely

    finding better tests, to recognition that we cannot understand psychological

    processes without an appreciation of the cultural infrastructure and how it facili-

    tates development.

    Constructing the Citizen 421

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    10/29

    1998). The tool-user idea is consistent with philosophical positions such as that

    of Rorty (1979) and Bergson’s (1911/1983) argument that we are  Homo faber 

    rather than Homo sapiens. It is through our artifacts (which include the tools of 

    language and linguistic devices such as metaphor) that we experience and inter-

    pret our world. A striking example is Gigerenzer’s (2000) work on how early cog-

    nitive scientists’ metaphors of mind derived from—and differed according

    to—their own interaction with the hardware and software of computers.

    Cultural Narratives and Discourses

    A paradigmatic approach to values and beliefs tends to map individual cog-

    nition. An approach that recognizes the narrative and cultural, by contrast, focuses

    on values as  processes—on how values are used, invoked, negotiated, and

    managed in dialogue between persons and in the individual’s interaction with a

    situation. How are the narratives and stories that underpin values used ? Is it to

    provide explanation, moral justification, prediction, or expectations based on pre-

    vious experience? Why is this story chosen rather than others? When is entitle-

    ment, for example, based on a narrative of fair division, and when on restitution

    for past inequity? How does the narrative position the speaker—as authority, as

    victim, as expert? How are narratives and stories negotiated with the audience?

    In what ways is common ground—shared understanding—established? What

    allusions and referents are used? What rhetorical devices? What is taken for

    granted, implying shared meaning, explanations, and values, and what is pre-

    sented as problematic, to be justified, elaborated, explained? As Billig (1996)

    argued, dialogue involves argumentation, the presentation of a point of view with

    the intent to counter another point of view: We only understand what is being

    intended when we understand what is being countered.

    Values and beliefs held within a particular cultural context are shared, and

    thus taken for granted. It is not necessary to explain or elaborate that which can

    be assumed to be common ground. Consider one example: The metaphor of

    “frontier” in North America evokes a threshold between “civilization” and “the

    wilderness”; in Europe the frontier is an administrative boundary between two

    equally “civilized” and populated states (Cronon, 1995). The frontier metaphor in

    North America conjures up personal challenge, confrontation with the elements,

    and a testing ground—or re-invigoration—for the spirit. This elides easily into

    Star Trek , space exploration in general—and the Marlboro Man. It does not have

    the same connotation in Europe.

    Cultural narratives provide explanations and convey normative values. Values

    and beliefs are made salient through how they are represented and argued, in-

    cluding whether they are taken for granted or presented as problematic. At the

    most basic level, is the material accessible? American children, for example,

    422 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    11/29

    accessible material, the differently nuanced American and British versions of 

    1776. Formal access to cultural narratives comes through school curricula, and it

    is the overt policy of all governments to ensure that children receive a version of 

    history that develops national identity and the desired cultural values. The mes-

    sages of globally transmitted television programs and films that convey a (pri-

    marily) Western worldview are informal but pervasive, as innumerable analyses

    have shown.

    The evidence of parallel or “alternative” historical narratives in societies

    under oppressive rule indicates the importance of not only the overt narrative but

    the covert, and also how the narrative is interpreted. A “knowledge” or paradig-

    matic model tempts us to examine how well the historical facts conveyed by the

    narrative have been internalized, but in fact, we need to understand how the nar-

    rative is used —in particular, how it is managed in contradiction to, or competi-

    tion with, other available narratives. An example is what is  forefronted . For

    instance, one Northern U.S. narrative was that the Civil War was fought against

    slavery. However, the South’s narrative was not that they fought in order to

    support slavery—they fought to retain the autonomy of states to make their own

    laws (which happened to include legal slave ownership).

    Narrative is central to collective memory; governments and cultural forces

    deliberately sustain appropriate collective memory that carries ethical force and

     justifies current practices in education, policy, and the treatment of outgroups. In

    the Greek-speaking part of Cyprus, to take just one example, until the recent

    rapprochement, schoolchildren were exposed daily to the motto “I struggle and I

    do not forget” and reminded constantly of the events of 30 years ago when the

    island was divided after the Turkish invasion (Christou, 2002). The use of “I” in

    that context was intended to engage the personal and national identity of these

    young people, even though the events in question occurred when their own parents

    were children.

    Positioning

    A central feature of dialogue and argumentation is the process of  position-

    ing—of oneself and of others. In managing dialogue between two persons, we

    position ourselves in relation to the other (Harré & Van Langenhove, 1991;

    Hollway, 1989). This is a skilled and often subtle process; I may “position” you

    as my friend by the act of self-disclosure, or, by interrupting you, as someone not

    to be taken seriously (Tannen, 1994). Davies and Harré (1990) spelled out a

    provocative example of disputed positioning: He, seeing himself as a caring

    person, tries to look after her when she feels ill; she objects that such caretaking

    is positioning her as weak, and is therefore sexist; he feels hurt that she is label-

    ing him as sexist.

    Constructing the Citizen 423

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    12/29

    symbols and referents indicative of that identity. We define as “other” those

    persons whose national characteristics, by being different from ours, affirm our

    own nationhood. Recent work suggests that, in contrast to earlier explanations

    that treated stereotyping as meeting a need to reduce information overload (the

    “cognitive miser” model), we in fact elaborate stereotypes. We generate narra-

    tives that make sense of the “otherness” of the Xanodian, by choosing to define

    the Xanodian negatively on those attributes that are salient to our positive defini-

    tion of our own ingroup (Gibson & Gouws, 2003; Reicher & Hopkins, 2001).

    One of the earliest social skills is turn-taking. From this early base the child

    learns how to manage the social and linguistic cues that position him or her

    vis-à-vis interlocutors, and to explicit or implicit social categories or groups. The

    child learns when and how to express “we-ness” and “theyness,” and how to use

    “otherness” as a way of confirming “we-ness” and selfhood (Rogoff, 1990). “We”

    share; “other” (nasty) children don’t share. “We” share with certain people, those

    defined as our ingroup, but not with outgroup members. The reasons for sharing,

    and the boundaries within which it is required, position the child in relation to

    other children and to behaviors that are, or are not, part of “our” identity. On such

    early foundations are the basis of both nationality and ethics laid. It is noteworthy

    that politicians invoke such childhood rhetoric, knowing its power; Bush senior

    and junior both used the playground metaphor of “the bully and the hero who

    stops him” with regard to Saddam Hussein (Rohrer, 1995).

    The Process of Construction and the Agenda for Citizenship Education

    What does this theoretical framework tell us about how we “construct the

    citizen”? At the very least, the implications of this approach must take us beyond

    citizenship education purely as the pursuit of paradigmatic knowledge. How do

    the three issues—identity, positioning, and narrative—translate into educational

    practice? How can curricula and school experience work with the processes of 

    identity formation? How can an understanding of positioning enable us to

    strengthen community commitment while providing defenses against outgroup

    discrimination? How can we use narratives to transmit political knowledge as well

    as motivate nationhood?

    There is a fourth element also, which is central to effective citizenship: effi-

    cacy and agency, in particular the capacity to take responsibility and to par-

    ticipate in the community. The foregoing “active” model of the individual

    presupposes agency, purposive seeking of knowledge, narrative, and interpreta-

    tion, and proactive engagement in argumentation. The individual does this through

    and with others, drawing upon cultural resources. How does actively making sense

    of experience translate into educational practice, to foster active and efficacious

    citizens?

    424 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    13/29

    of citizenship inform discussions about citizenship education? What are the

    implicit and explicit models of human development, and what are the implica-

    tions of both for what is assumed to be effective in education for citizenship?

    Where does the construction process take place? Such models and assumptions—

    both what is agreed, and what is contested and problematic—can be elicited from

    writings explicitly on citizenship education, as well as from research agendas.

    First, there is a longstanding tension in education theory between “knowl-

    edge” and “praxis” as the route to understanding. This has resonance with the dis-

    tinction between paradigmatic and narrative ways of knowing, and with the

    tool-user model of engagement with our world through artifacts, practices, and

    language. The “knowledge” model presumes that information of itself will lead

    to understanding and to appropriate motivation. By providing children with infor-

    mation (including the skills to treat that information critically), both enlighten-

    ment and involvement will follow; “appropriate” civic knowledge will motivate

    civic participation. In contrast, the “praxis” model assumes that practical and the-

    oretical knowledge, and particularly the motivation to use them, are acquired

    through actively engaging with relevant tasks. The assumption is that knowledge

    comes from making sense of experience rather than vice versa, and that knowl-

    edge has limited usefulness unless it translates into the individual’s own encoun-

    ters with salient materials. Praxis-based programs of citizenship education provide

    opportunities for community engagement and active participation in the political

    process appropriate to the child’s age and interests. This is in the direct tradition

    of John Dewey as well as Vygotsky, and informs the tool-user model.

    Underlying this distinction is another question: Should the goal be an

    informed citizenry or an efficacious citizenry? Is the aim to produce knowledge-

    able people (with the assumption that they will then be efficacious), or is it to

    ensure that people are efficacious (which might require practical experience in

    addition to knowledge)?

    Second, discussions of what constitutes the “good citizen” also draw on the

    rhetoric of the “responsible” citizen. This term has at least three contested mean-

    ings (Haste, 1993, 2001, in press). Responsibility may mean duty and obliga-

    tion—conformity to social expectations and rules. A second meaning of 

    responsibility concerns connection—ties of affect and interdependence, relation-

    ship in the family or the wider community. Responsibility here derives from

    mutual care and support, based on affective ties. A third form of responsibility

    lies in the imperative deriving from a judgment of principle: If I perceive an injus-

    tice or a harm, I must act upon my moral judgment. These three forms of “respon-

    sibility” are implicitly in conflict. The responsibility to act in the pursuit of 

    principle may—and often does—lead the individual to confront the status quo and

    act unconventionally, possibly illegally. The responsibility to personal ties may

    conflict with demands of the wider community (including legal demands) or with

    Constructing the Citizen 425

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    14/29

    Third, over the past 40 years there have been significant changes in how “par-

    ticipation” has been researched. Early work looked at “conventional” participa-

    tion “inside” the system—voting, canvassing, fund-raising, by implication the

    primary attributes of a “good citizen.” With the social and political upheavals of 

    the 1960s and 1970s, especially peace movements and the emergence of minor-

    ity politics, research attention came to include such activities—which were now

    also seen as a manifestation of “good citizenship.” From asking “What is strange

    about these people?”, research shifted to asking what facilitated such high-risk 

    behavior. Some research contrasted the moral health of protesters with “apathetic”

    non-protesters (Haan, Smith, & Block, 1968). “Unconventional” political activ-

    ity, efficacy, and social responsibility, and the willingness to challenge unjust laws,

    became valued (Jennings & Van Deth, 1990; Sigel & Hoskins, 1981).

    As activist movements in “stable” Western societies have waned, “compe-

    tence,” broadly defined, has become an emergent research field. Competence is

    contrasted, optimistically, with the apparent rise in political apathy among young

    people reflected in low turnout at elections. For “stable” societies, the definition

    of democracy is entwined with the notion of participation. A healthy democracy

    depends on an active citizenry; apathy is cause for concern (Ichilov, 1998). The

    research suggests that the “competent individual” is self-sufficient, able to focus

    attention and plan, has a future orientation, is adaptable to change, and has a sense

    of responsibility and commitment, including believing that one can oneself have

    an effect. Agency and efficacy are therefore core elements of competence. These

    attributes seem to be fostered by families and communities that provide compe-

    tent role models, set goals and assign responsibilities, and actually create practi-

    cal opportunities for involvement (Hamilton & Fenzel, 1988; Hart & Fegley,

    1995; Lenhart & Rabiner, 1995). A recent project by OECD has begun to explore

    the concept of competence extensively (Rychen & Salganik, 2001).

    Defining the attributes of the “competent citizen” more broadly has also

    unpacked the concept of “participation.” There is first a distinction between “polit-

    ical” and “civic” participation. Then, it is useful to differentiate forms of partici-

    pation that involve service to the community from those that are an extension of 

    private transactions, and those that involve more effort or cost in terms of com-

    mitment. So, giving (in the form of charity, tithing, or alms) is low-cost in terms

    of time, can be anonymous, and can be construed as a private act rather than public

    service. Helping is also a “private,” interpersonal transaction, a response to per-

    ceived need. Campaigning, whether in the form of canvassing, petition-signing,

    or organizing support, is a “public” activity, an overt statement of commitment.

    More “public” still, protesting and other forms of unconventional participation

    can have high costs.

    To explore assumptions and goals behind potential agendas of citizenship

    education, I draw on two examples. The purpose is not to present a comprehen-

    426 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    15/29

    British initiative to develop citizenship education, under the direction of Bernard

    Crick. Britain is the only “advanced” country that has not had (until recently) a

    school citizenship curriculum. The program has needed to make explicit and

     justify its assumptions. The second resource is the material from the IEA (Inter-

    national Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement) study of 28

    nations, which gathered data from 90,000 14- and 16-year-old students and from

    teachers on a range of citizenship issues. This explored values, expectations, and

    practice. The data therefore elucidate the context of citizenship education, and

    also point up striking cultural differences that by no means map onto any simple

    East versus West, or First versus Third World, boundary (Torney-Purta, Lehmann,

    Oswald, & Schulz, 2001; Torney-Purta & Amadeo, 2003; Torney-Purta &

    Richardson, in press).

    Crick chaired a government committee to explore the criteria and goals for

    citizenship education (Crick, 1998). The report uncompromisingly states its aims

    and assumptions:

    We aim at no less than a change in the political culture of this country

    both nationally and locally; for people to think of themselves as active

    citizens, willing, able and equipped to have an influence in public life

    and with the critical capacities to weigh evidence before speaking and

    acting, to build on and to extend radically to young people the best in

    existing traditions of community involvement and public service, and tomake them individually confident in finding new forms of involvement 

    and action among themselves. (Crick, 1998, p. 10; emphasis added)

    The report favorably cites another British educationalist, David Hargreaves:

    Active citizens are as political as they are moral; moral sensibility derives

    in part from political understanding: political apathy spawns moral

    apathy. (Crick, 1998, p. 10)

    This explicitly defines participation as proactive and critical, not just reactively

    supportive of the status quo. Crick identified three distinct goals: social and moral

    responsibility, community involvement , and  political literacy. These goals are

    desired outcomes—the product of education—but they are also  practices in

    the classroom that will develop the relevant skills. The goals incorporate both

    knowledge and action.

    For social and moral responsibility, Crick identified “children learning from

    the very beginning, self-confidence and socially and morally responsible behav-

    ior both in and beyond the classroom.” For community involvement, he identi-

    fied “learning about and becoming helpfully involved in the life and concerns of 

    their communities, including community involvement and service to the commu-

    nity.” For political literacy, he identified “realistic knowledge of and preparation

    Constructing the Citizen 427

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    16/29

    These goals are notable for their breadth and for incorporating practical as

    well as conceptual knowledge. The implicit definition of a “citizen” is not con-

    fined to participating in narrowly “political” activity such as voting, or party

    support. The “citizen” here configured will take an active role in the community

    and will engage in informed judgment about social and political issues. He or she

    will also have conflict resolution skills, which apply in many areas of life. The

    moral dimension—particularly moral responsibility—is a salient feature. The

    “Crick Report” was a quite radical document. However, its translation into policy

    has been less assertive; the actual new school curriculum for England and Wales

    (instituted in 2002) emphasizes qualities of “helpfulness” and “consideration for

    others” rather more than the kind of critical agency that might lead to proactive

    whistle-blowing (Quality and Curriculum Authority, 1999). How the new

    curriculum will work in practice is still under review.

    The major IEA international study directed by Torney-Purta and her col-

    leagues provides rich and current data on what is happening in schools, and what

    explicit and implicit expectations teachers and students have of “civic education.”

    Although there are interesting national differences on some issues, an overview

    of the general findings provides a useful picture of what might be termed a “global

    agenda” of citizenship education: what teachers believe students should be learn-

    ing, and what students perceive that they have learned (Torney-Purta et al., 2001).

    The study addressed a range of political activity and goals of citizenship, not

    confined to “conventional” participation, and so it is possible to compare how stu-

    dents and teachers value different forms of engagement. Teachers’ perspectives

    reflected a general tendency toward liberal values and support for broad-based

    activism. In all the nations surveyed except Cyprus and Romania, fewer than 20%

    of teachers thought that students should be taught that joining a political party

    was important. More than half of the teachers thought that pupils should be aware

    of the importance of ignoring a law that violates human rights; this rose to more

    than 85% in eight countries. Nearly two-thirds thought that pupils should feel con-

    fident to participate in peaceful protest; in 11 countries, this rose to more than

    80%. Apart from the importance of knowing national history and obeying the law,

    most support came for protecting the environment and promoting human rights,

    endorsed in 25 of the countries by more than 90% of teachers.

    The messages that the students absorbed also reflect this pattern. More than

    72% of students believed that they had learned to cooperate with others and to

    understand people with different ideas, to care what happens in other countries,

    and to protect the environment. Fewer than 60% claimed they had learned the

    importance of voting.

    It is useful to compare what students perceive that they have learned with

    their expectations of future civic and political action. In 17 countries, more than

    80% of students expected to vote (Torney-Purta & Amadeo, 2003). Collecting

    428 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    17/29

    for a petition and taking part in a demonstration, varied considerably by country,

    and there are clearly cultural differences in norms for grassroots activism. In 14

    countries, more than 45% would collect signatures, and in nine countries, more

    than 45% would take part in a demonstration, but the figures were much lower in

    other nations. Certain nations stood out as having a culture of active participa-

    tion, including protest, and a high level of engagement in general. These included

    Greece, Italy, Cyprus, Chile, and Colombia. Other nations—including some

    highly industrialized Western nations where students had a high level of political

    knowledge—were notable for the low level of engagement and participation; these

    included the Czech Republic, Finland, and Sweden.

    A recent British study is instructive. This research was conducted by MORI

    Polls for the Nestlé Family Monitor (Nestlé UK, 2003), with a sample of 914

    students aged 11 to 18. The results make an interesting comparison with the

    IEA data. The majority were “uncertain” about intentions to vote; only 12% were

    “absolutely certain” to vote. But when asked about the importance of voting, 44%

    agreed that voting is important and 63% agreed that voting had an effect on how

    the country was run. These figures do not suggest that voting is highly significant

    for these young people, but the data about other forms of participation are more

    encouraging: More than 40% had participated in fundraising or a sponsored activ-

    ity in the past year, 25% had signed a petition, and about 12% had taken part in

    a protest demonstration or collected petition signatures. Only 2% had helped with

    a political party. More expected to take part in protest in the future (23%) and to

    help with a political party (10%).

    As the authors noted, the relatively high participation by these young people

    might be due in part to the data being collected during the Iraq war. However, the

    figures overall compare somewhat unfavorably with the IEA data for England,

    where 14-year-olds’ future expectations were that 80% would vote, 57% would

    collect money for a social cause, 45% would collect petition signatures, and 28%

    would take part in a protest. England’s figures for expected “conventional” polit-

    ical activity were similar to the international IEA average, but for “unconven-

    tional” activity, including both legal and illegal protest, they were well below; for

    example, the international average for expected participation in non-violent

    protest was 44%, compared with England’s 28%. (The U.S. data are close to the

    international average on all measures of conventional and unconventional—

    including illegal—political activity.)

    The study by Flanagan et al. (1999) adds further information about national

    variations and alerts us to the cultural meaning of certain forms of action. This

    study involved 5,000 young people aged 12 to 19 in seven countries. The range

    of “volunteering in the community” was from 19.9% in Sweden to 60.4% in

    Hungary. In the United States the figure was 51.5%. The authors argued that vol-

    unteering plays a very different role in Western nations with and without welfare

    Constructing the Citizen 429

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    18/29

    bloc countries, volunteering was a significant aspect of youth organizations (as it

    is in the United States) and remains a normative activity.

    These figures of political intent, and awareness of modes of political and

    social activism, would seem to undermine the rather widespread anxiety that “the

    young are apathetic.” If voting intention is taken as the criterion, then there are

    grounds for such concern, but as noted earlier, voting behavior is a relatively infre-

    quent activity, with low cost (in most nations) and, from the point of view of the

    voter, fairly low effectiveness. The voter is less likely to feel the power of his or

    her individual vote; much more subjective efficacy comes from campaigning and

    persuading other people to vote (or demonstrate, or sign petitions). Macro analy-

    sis of political change must pay attention to the pattern of voting behavior, but

    this is not necessarily a good measure of individual motivation, engagement, or

    efficacy (Eatwell, 2003). To comprehend participation, we must look at the sub-

     jective perceptions and motivations of the citizen.

    The Antecedents of Participation

    To understand efficacy and agency, and how they relate to identity, we need

    to know about the preconditions for engagement and the antecedents of partici-

    pation. What happens to make someone get involved? What kind of experiences

    facilitate commitment? Effective participation requires identification with the

    project; it must be an element of selfhood. It requires a sense of one’s agency and

    efficacy—that action is possible and potentially effective and that the individual

    can personally take such action, alone or with others.

    The evidence suggests that level of civic knowledge is only a partial predic-

    tor of participation, and that cultural factors are significant. For example, the IEA

    data show an overall relationship between political knowledge and conventional

    political activity. Nonetheless there are large variations between nations, with

    some having a low level of civic knowledge but a high level of participation—

    Chile, Colombia, Portugal, and Romania. Other countries had a high level of civic

    knowledge and low level of participation, such as Australia, the Czech Republic,

    and Finland. High levels of both participation and knowledge were found in

    Cyprus and Greece; low levels of both, in Belgium and Estonia.

    In a more detailed analysis of four nations (Australia, England, Norway, and

    the United States), Torney-Purta and Richardson (in press) found that different

    types of predicted participation had different correlates. Voting was associated

    with school factors, such as civic knowledge, curricular emphasis on the impor-

    tance of elections, students’ confidence in their ability to participate, and an open

    classroom climate. Additionally, family discussion and trust in government insti-

    tutions predicted voting intent. Future  party membership was predicted only by

    current political interest (and in any case only 20% of students expected to join

    430 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    19/29

    tions and sense of efficacy. Political interest had no relationship to volunteering.

    The likelihood of taking part in a non-violent protest was associated only with

    discussion with parents, not with school factors. These data underline the impor-

    tance of distinguishing different kinds of participation, and also of recognizing

    that the “effective” citizen is not necessarily only the “voting” citizen.

    Culture also influences the meaning of an activity. I noted earlier that a low

    level of volunteering, or organizing for charity, in some nations may reflect not

    apathy but an effective welfare state (Flanagan et al., 1999). There are also very

    different cultural traditions regarding protest demonstrations and how different

    forms of participation are perceived. Do the cultural narratives of protest, for

    example, cast the protesters as heroes, subversives, or nuisances? How is current

    protest located in the historical story? A culture of protest is reflected in frequent

    marches and demonstrations (as perhaps in Greece, Cyprus, and Italy); without

    such a culture, these will be resorted to only in extremis. In the United Kingdom,

    marches have been associated primarily with the Left, or with a few very small

    extreme right-wing groups. Yet one of the largest street demonstrations ever in

    the United Kingdom was the Countryside March in September 2002, an event

    organized and supported by primarily “conservative” opponents of legislation to

    ban fox-hunting and other traditional country pursuits. Such people would never,

    in the past, have taken to marching. This may suggest a cultural shift; recent anti-

    war demonstrations in Britain, drawing from a wide political spectrum, have also

    been exceptionally large.

    There are two main strands of work on the antecedents and processes of 

    engagement. The first comes from studies of the peace movement in the period

    1964–1988, the second from more recent work on volunteering and community

    activity. The first was more overtly “political.” The second area of research has

    surfaced particularly in reaction to the pessimistic picture of young people’s

    apathy; these researchers find extensive community activity among young people.

    Both the peace movement and the community action research demonstrate that

    the experience of being an effective agent both influences the individual’s per-

    spective on an issue and fosters personal engagement and a sense of efficacy.

    Active participation in youth groups—such as membership in 4H and, to a lesser

    extent, youth activities such as Scouts—also predicts participation and civic lead-

    ership in later life. Many studies on former peace and campus activists from the

    1960s and 1970s show that although their political values may moderate, they

    retain a lifelong propensity toward engagement, activism, volunteering, and

    choosing careers in education or service (McAdam, 1988; Stewart, Settles, &

    Winter, 1998; Verba, Schlozman, & Brady, 1995; Youniss, Mclellan, & Yates,

    1997).

    The peace movement research showed that those who became actively

    engaged had a high level of personal efficacy, combined with low trust in gov-

    Constructing the Citizen 431

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    20/29

    suggested that frequently, activist individuals had been sensitized by events that

    led to focused attention to salient information. This work was particularly instruc-

    tive about management of affective responses to perceived threat, and about the

    roles of trust and personal efficacy in mediating such affect. It also underlined the

    importance of experience. Although preexisting beliefs were important, particu-

    larly in relation to level of trust, it was clear that attitudes alone did not account

    for commitment; previous personal experience of agency was a vital element in

    generating motivation, efficacy, and action (Andrews, 1991; Colby & Damon,

    1992; Fiske, 1987; Fogelman, 1994; Hamilton, Chavez, & Keilin, 1986; Haste,

    1989, 1990; Locatelli & Holt, 1986).

    In several studies, we found that anger about the nuclear threat, anger in

    response to the 1986 Chernobyl disaster, and anger in response to witnessing

    cruelty or injustice were associated with a sense of efficacy, and tended to pre-

    cipitate engagement and action, including heightened sensitization to relevant

    information (Haste, 1989, 1990; Haste, Sharpley, & Wallace, 1987; Thearle &

    Haste, 1986). We referred to this pattern as “Affective Actor.” Fear, in contrast,

    when associated with low trust and low sense of personal efficacy, produced what

    we termed the “Powerless Pessimist.” Fear combined with high trust and low per-

    sonal efficacy was associated with faith that the government would keep the

    nuclear threat under control, or deal effectively with the fallout from Chernobyl.

    We termed this the “Deferring Defender.” We also identified a fourth group, which

    other researchers had noted—people with high trust in government who expressed

    little affective response; they believed that the nuclear weapons policy of power

    balance was successful. We termed them “Resistant Rationalizers.”

    The work on voluntary activity clearly shows that young people’s hands-on

    experience of sociopolitical issues, and the processes by which they become

    engaged, are central to political development and considerably more relevant to

    it than party affiliation. This work meets Crick’s argument that research should

    address young people’s own worlds and lore. Yates and Youniss (1999) brought

    together an international group of researchers who were exploring young people’s

    involvement in community work and informal political activities. This research

    identifies three different kinds of community work, which should be looked at

    somewhat differently: helping, fund-raising, and campaigning. It is important to

    note the subjective meaning of such participation. For example, helping a dis-

    advantaged person, as well as involving personal connection, both requires and

    fosters sensitivity to others’ needs. In contrast, protecting an environmental

    resource can be seen as saving “our” world (Flanagan et al., 1999). As Flanagan

    et al. noted, environmental concerns had a particular political dimension in former

    Eastern bloc countries; because pollution could be seen as the responsibility of 

    the old regime, campaigning against it was a way of indirectly pushing for polit-

    ical change within a fairly safe discourse.

    432 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    21/29

    and black—and therefore made them aware that they had been stereotyping.

    Second, the ethnic mix of the homeless confronted their own black identity and

    its assumptions about white social power. Third, they began to think about per-

    sonal and institutional moral responsibility and where the problems of homeless-

    ness lay. Finally, their activity gave them access to the experience of civic agency.

    A similar pattern of themes emerged in a study of British volunteers (Roker, Player,

    & Coleman, 1999). So participation leads to an awareness of social problems, the

    opportunity to try to alleviate them, and the growth, through experience, of a real-

    istic assessment of what an individual can do. Also, through participation, actors

    form a connection to community organizations and to committed individuals.

    Conclusion

    My intent here has not been to propose specific curriculum developments,

    but rather to take a critical theoretical overview and to reflect on the principles

    behind citizenship education. I have selected from existing research to elaborate

    concepts and arguments; a more comprehensive literature review, not within the

    scope of this paper, might begin to tell us about the effectiveness of educational

    practices and of individual experience. I have explored four concepts that emerge

    from the critical psychology approach that I have adopted, in relation to citizen-

    ship: identity, positioning, narrative, and efficacy. Let us look at these again in

    the light of the data to which I have referred, and consider some educational

    implications.

     Identity emerges as central to engagement. To become involved requires that

    one have a sense of ownership of the issue, that one define oneself as a member

    of a group or as a holder of particular beliefs. The data show that civic knowl-

    edge is not enough; such knowledge has to become salient to the individual

    through the experience of participation in relevant action, through the negotiation

    of identity with others, and through incorporating narratives about values, self-

    hood, and national identity into one’s self-definition.

    There is a large literature on social identity in relation to social categoriza-

    tion and social groups (such as ethnic groups) that demonstrates how ingroups are

    defined by the “other” who characterizes the outgroup, and how people become

    members of ingroups. Although this paper has focused on the more desirable and

    community-oriented aspects of youth movements, there is a body of data on youth

    resistance, ritual, and involvement with football support, squatting, counterculture

    leisure, etc. (Feixa & Costa, 2003; Hall & Jefferson, 1983; Skelton & Valentine,

    1998; Stott, 2001; Widdicombe & Wooffitt, 1995) that illuminates identity for-

    mation. This work is outside the scope of this paper, but may provide insights into

    the active construction of identity.

    A particularly useful feature of the concept of  positioning is the slant it

    Constructing the Citizen 433

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    22/29

    psychologists note. This counters the more simplistic view that those who are

    othered—by stereotyping or by language style—are passive victims. The insights

    of positioning, while not denying the oppressive effects of powerful and consis-

    tent othering, nonetheless lead us to look at how this is a continual iterative

    process within which there are many forms of resistance.

    The processes of engagement can usefully be interpreted as positioning and

    repositioning. Consider Yates’ (1999) study of young black students working in

    a soup kitchen for the homeless. Taking the liberty of reflecting on these data

    within the theoretical framework I am discussing, one can suggest that, before

    their experience, they had positioned themselves (and had been positioned) as part

    of a black community within the framework of white power. They themselves

    were relatively well off, and “the poor”—whom they assumed to be universally

    black—were seen as (positioned as) feckless, drug-ridden, or otherwise morally

    suspect. First, their involvement in the soup kitchen required that they position

    themselves—collaboratively and with teachers’ assistance—as able to be helpful,

    as wishing to identify with helpfulness, and also, perhaps, as being a little brave

    in so doing. Yates noted that once they were involved in the activity, there was a

    large shift in “positioning” (although she did not use this language to describe it).

    Students discovered that the homeless included white as well as black people,

    which disrupted their monolithic view of white power. They discovered that the

    homeless were unfortunate, or in other ways did not merit positioning as morally

    deficient—and that they also had self-respect, and resisted being positioned as to

    be pitied. Finally, their experience made them aware of other “positionings” in

    the situation: the role of local agencies and local government in providing, or not,

    facilities for the homeless, and in exacerbating their plight in various ways, and

    the possible mechanisms by which citizens like themselves could become agents

    of social and political pressure.

    I have discussed narrative in two ways: in Bruner’s sense as a form of 

    knowing that counters the paradigmatic, linear, and factual, and takes account of 

    the central role of stories that include cause and consequence, and which allude

    to shared cultural resources, in the acquisition of knowledge, and in the applica-

    tion of this to how narratives are pivotal to personal, social, and national identity.

    As I noted, education programs explicitly provide narratives of national history

    and identity, as well as the moral and other stories that underpin social values and

    expectations. New narratives emerge as needed—in changing societies, as we

    have seen, reconstructed versions of history and of relations with other states

    provide an identity that forges new independence and unity. We also see new

    transnational narratives: Globalization, in both its negative and positive connota-

    tions, requires us to find the stories that will enable our young people to frame

    their identity beyond the local boundaries—and will also provide the story that

    motivates them to question multinational capitalism (Winter, 2003). And it is

    434 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    23/29

    The importance of narrative has been explicit in debates about history cur-

    ricula for some time. Ethnic identity in particular has been fostered in schools by

    forefronting minority groups’ history within a multicultural curriculum approach.

    In some countries, this has come into conflict with pressure to tell a “national”

    story of the heritage of current identity and institutions. What is interesting is that

    all parties concerned recognize the power of narrative—not only the power of 

    factual information. This is a major insight of the theoretical approach I am dis-

    cussing—that values and factual data are not, of themselves, enough to engage

    and motivate, to become part of the young person’s identity. It is through the nar-

    ratives that underpin and justify values that “ownership” of those values appears

    to happen.

    Finally, how do we foster efficacy and agency through education? Torney-

    Purta and Richardson (in press) reported that pupils’ expected future voting cor-

    related with an open classroom climate and students having a role in school

    decision-making. One interpretation of this is that the values of democratic dis-

    cussion and involvement are presented through institutional practice; we might

    add that, through active practical involvement, children learn both the values and

    the sense of agency. We might also add that by according children the right, and

    the expectation, to make their voice heard, we are positioning them as efficacious

    and enabling them to position themselves as such. We have seen that voluntary

    community activities, and campaigning and protest activities, are highly effective

    means of equipping young people with the identity, values, skills, and efficacy

    that make them effective participants. These examples are mainly educational

    interventions, working within a stable school and societal environment. The data

    from “real-life” situations—societies in transition—also show the power of active

    engagement in social change, as we saw in the studies by van Hoorn et al. (2000)

    in Hungary and Poland and by Abrahams (1995) in South Africa.

    The brief overall conclusion must be that a “knowledge” model of citizen-

    ship education is not enough. It is through praxis, whether in the school or in the

    community, that the young person gains an identity as an active citizen, and the

    skills and efficacy to become one. A paradigmatic knowledge model, focusing on

    factual material about institutions, is also unlikely to fire the growing person. We

    may be suspicious that narratives can justify the “wrong” kinds of positioning,

    but understanding discursive processes requires us to recognize the inevitability

    of narrative, as well as its appeal—and to make use of it effectively in education.

    Much research on discursive processes needs to be done; knowing that certain

    experiences and practices facilitate different kinds of participation is the first step.

    Now we need to find out how.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    Constructing the Citizen 435

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    24/29

    spondence concerning this article should be sent to Helen Haste, Department of 

    Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

    REFERENCES

    Abrahams, S. (1995).  Moral reasoning in context: The construction of the adolescent moral world under apartheid . Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard Graduate School of Education.

    Andrews, M. (1991). Lifetimes of commitment . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Antaki, C., & Widdicombe, S. (1998). Identities in talk . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Bar-On, D. (2001). The silence of psychologists. Political Psychology, 22, 331–345.

    Bar-Tal, D. (2000). Shared beliefs in a society. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Bergson, H. (1911/1983). Creative evolution. New York, NY: Holt.

    Billig, M. (1995). Banal nationalism. London: Sage.

    Billig, M. (1996). Arguing and thinking (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Blair, T. (1998). The Third Way; new politics for a new century. London: Fabian Society.

    Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Bruner, J. (1995). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Bull, A., Diamond, H., & Marsh, R. (Eds.) (2001). Feminisms and women’s movements in contempo-rary Europe. New York: St. Martin’s.

    Christou, M. (2002). Fragments of memory, visions of struggle; political imagination in a Greek Cypriot high school. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Graduate School of Education.

    Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of moral commitment . New York:Free Press.

    Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.

    Crick, B. (1998).  Education for citizenship and the teaching of democracy in schools. London:Qualifications and Curriculum Authority.

    Crick, B. (1999). The presuppositions of citizenship education.  Journal of Philosophy of Education,33, 337–352.

    Cronon, W. (1995). The trouble with wilderness: or, getting back to the wrong nature. In W. Cronon(Ed.), Uncommon ground: Towards reinventing nature (pp. 69–90). New York: Norton.

    Davies, B., & Harré, R. (1990). Positioning: The discursive production of selves.  Journal for the

    Theory of Social Behavior, 20, 43–63.

    Eatwell, R. (2003). Ten theories of the extreme right. In P. Merkl & L. Weinberg (Eds.), Right-wingextremism in the twenty-first century (pp. 47–73). London: Frank Cass.

    Eatwell, R., & O’Sullivan, N. (1990). The nature of the Right . Boston: Twayne.

    Edwards, D. (1997). Discourse and cognition. London: Sage.

    Feixa, C., & Costa, C. (2003, October). From “okupas” to “makineros”: Citizenship and youthcultures in Spain. Paper presented to the European Science Foundation workshop, Strasbourg.

    Fiske, S. (1987). People’s reactions to nuclear war: Implications for psychologists.  AmericanPsychologist, 42, 207–217.

    Flanagan, C., Jonsson, B., Botcheva, L., Csapo, B., Bowes, J., Macek, P., Averina, I., & Sheblanova,

    E. (1999). Adolescents and the “social contrast”: Developmental roots of citizenship in sevencountries. In M. Yates & J. Youniss (Eds.),  Roots of civic identity (pp. 135–155). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press

    436 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    25/29

    Gibson, J. L., & Gouws, A. (2003). Overcoming intolerance in South Africa: Experiments indemocratic persuasion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Giddens, A. (1994). Beyond left and right: The future of radical politics. Cambridge: Polity.

    Giddens, A. (1998). The Third Way: The renewal of social democracy. Cambridge: Polity.Gigerenzer, G. (2000).  Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world . Oxford: Oxford University

    Press.

    Greenall Gough, A. (1993). Founders in environmental education. Geelong, Victoria, Australia:Deakin University Press.

    Haan, N., Smith, M. B., & Block, J. H. (1968). Motivational reasoning of young adults . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 183–201.

    Hall, J. A. (1993). Nationalisms; classified and explained.  Daedalus, 122(3), 1–28.

    Hall, S., & Jefferson, T. (1983).  Resistance through ritual: Youth subcultures in post-war Britain.London: Hutchinson.

    Hamilton, S. B., Chavez, E. L., & Keilin, W. G. (1986). Thoughts of Armageddon: The relationshipbetween attitudes towards the nuclear threat and cognitive/emotional responses.  International

     Journal of Mental Health, 15, 189–207.

    Hamilton, S. F., & Fenzel, L. M. (1988). The impact of volunteer experience on adolescent socialdevelopment. Journal of Adolescent Research, 3, 65–80.

    Harré, R., Brockmeier, J., & Mühlhäusler, P. (1999). Greenspeak: A study of environmental discourse.Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Harré, R., & Gillett, G. (1994). The discursive mind. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Harré, R., & Van Langenhove, L. (1991). Varieties of positioning.  Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 21, 393–407.

    Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Prosocial behavior and caring. Child Development, 66 , 1346–1359.Haste, H. (1989). Everybody’s scared—but life goes on: Coping, defense and action in the face of 

    nuclear threat. Journal of Adolescence, 12, 11–26.

    Haste, H. (1990). Moral responsibility and moral commitment: The integration of affect and cogni-tion. In T. Wren (Ed.), The moral domain: Essays in the ongoing discussion between philoso-

     phy and the social sciences (pp. 315–359). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Haste, H. (1992). The dissolution of the Right in the wake of theory. In G. Breakwell (Ed.), Social psychology of political and economic cognition (pp. 33–76). London: Academic Press.

    Haste, H. (1993). Moral creativity and education for citizenship. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1–2),153–164.

    Haste, H. (1994). The sexual metaphor . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Haste, H. (2000). Are women human? In N. Roughley (Ed.),  Being human (pp. 175–196). Berlin: deGruyter.

    Haste, H. (2001a). Sexual metaphors and current feminisms. In A. Bull, H. Diamond, & R. Marsh(Eds.), Feminisms and women’s movements in contemporary Europe (pp. 21–34). New York: St.Martin’s.

    Haste, H. (2001b). Ambiguity, autonomy and agency: Psychological challenges to new competence.In D. Rychen & L. Salganik (Eds.),  Defining and selecting key competencies (pp. 92–120).Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Haste, H. (in press). Moral responsibility and citizenship education. In D. Wallace (Ed.),  Art, scienceand morality: Creative journeys. New York: Plenum.

    Haste, H., Sharpley, F., & Wallace, D. (1987, July). Coping, defence and action after Chernobyl. Paperpresented at the annual meeting of the International Society of Political Psychology, SanFrancisco

    Constructing the Citizen 437

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    26/29

    Janowski, A. (1999). The specific nature and objectives of civic education in Poland: Some reflec-tions. In J. Torney-Purta, J. Schwille, & J. Amadeo (Eds.), Civic education across countries (pp.463–482). Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

    Jennings, K., & Van Deth, J. W. (1990). Continuities in political action. Berlin: de Gruyter.Kaase, M. (1999). Interpersonal trust, political trust and non-institutionalised political participation in

    Western Europe. West European Politics, 22(3), 1–21.

    Kaase, M., & Newton, K. (1995).  Beliefs in government . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Kahn, P. (1999). The human relationship with nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Klatch, R. (1987). Women of the new right . Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

    Lenhart, L. A., & Rabiner, D. L. (1995). Social competence in adolescence.  Developmental Psy-chopathology, 2, 543–561.

    Locatelli, M. G., & Holt, R. R. (1986). Antinuclear activism, psychic numbing and mental health. International Journal of Mental Health, 15, 143–161.

    Margalit, A. (2002). The ethics of memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Marsh, A. (1977). Protest and political consciousness. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    McAdam, D. (1988). Freedom summer . New York: Oxford University Press.

    Milbrath, L. W., & Goel, M. L. (1977). Political participation. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Nestlé Family Monitor 16. (2003). Young people’s attitudes towards politics. Croydon, UK: NestléUK Limited.

    Quality and Curriculum Authority (1999). The National Curriculum for England: Citizenship. London:Quality and Curriculum Authority & Department for Education and Employment.

    Reicher, S., & Hopkins, N. (2001). Psychology and the end of history: A critique and a proposal forthe psychology of social categorization. Political Psychology, 22, 383–407.

    Rogoff, B. (1990).  Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context . New York:Oxford University Press.

    Rohrer, T. (1995). The metaphysical logic of (political) rape: The new wor(l)d order.  Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 115–138.

    Roker, D., Player, K., & Coleman, J. (1999). Exploring adolescent altruism: British young people’sinvolvement in voluntary work and campaigning. In M. Yates & J. Youniss (Eds.), Roots of civicidentity (pp. 56–72). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton: Princeton University.

    Rychen, D., & Salganik, L. (2001).  Defining and selecting key competencies. Seattle: Hogrefe &Huber.

    Scott, W. A. H., & Gough, S. (2003). Key issues in sustainable development and learning: A criticalreview. London: Routledge Falmer.

    Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture and the problem of meaning. New York: OxfordUniversity Press.

    Sigel, R., & Hoskins, M. (1981). The political involvement of adolescents. Brunswick, NJ: RutgersUniversity Press.

    Skelton, T., & Valentine, A. (1998). Cool places: Geographies of youth cultures. London: Routledge.

    Stewart, A. J., Settles, I. H., & Winter, N. J. G. (1998). Women and the social movements of the 1960s:Activists, engaged observers, and nonparticipants. Political Psychology, 19, 63–94.

    Stott, C. (2001). “Hooligans” abroad? Intergroup dynamics, social identity and participation in

    collective “disorder” at the 1998 World Cup finals.  British Journal of Social Psychology,40, 359–384.

    Tannen D (1994) Gender and discourse New York: Oxford University Press

    438 Haste

  • 8/19/2019 Haste (2004) Constructing the Citizen

    27/29

    Torney-Purta, J., Lehmann, R., Oswald, H., & Schulz, W. (2001). Citizenship and education in twenty-eight countries: Civic knowledge and engagement at age fourteen. Amsterdam: InternationalAssociation for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

    Torney-Purta, J., & Amadeo, J. (2003). A c