The pipeline not only leaks - UZHb13591f5-cc5f-4d55-8388-84b6a957… · scientists from 32...

2
Is gender bias real? Headwinds and tailwinds There is a growing body of evidence showing that women face unconscious and pervasive discrimination at many points along their scientific education and career. Women in science, for example, are graded more critically, receive fewer awards and grants, and are subconsciously viewed as natively less scientifically able than men (see columns below). Each incident of discrimination could be subtle or small in magnitude, but their accumulated influence over time acts as a headwind making women’s advancement in science more difficult. In stark contrast, men and women alike tend to assess the scientific competency of males more favorably than that of females across all aspects of academic and scientific practice. Male students, for example, get more encourage- ment in their studies, are picked more often for academic awards, are recommended more convincingly for jobs, and are ultimately judged more hirable. Each of these examples Classroom, lab, and field Women students in science face challenges in how they are perceived, graded, and treated. Professors might favor male students. Researchers sent identical letters, purportedly from students, to more than 6,500 professors at 259 top American universities asking to discuss research opportunities. Professors were more likely to respond to email from “Brad Anderson” than from other fictitious aspirants with names like “Claire Smith” or “Juan Gonzalez.” (Milkman et al, 2015). Undergraduate students often prefer men both when rating their peers (Grunspan et al, 2016) and their professors (Mac- Nell et al, 2015). Nearly two-thirds of field scientists reported in a survey that they had been sexually harassed in the field. 666 field scientists from 32 disciplines including biology and geology were surveyed. 3/4th were female. More than 20% reported sexual assault. (Clancy et al, 2014). Females physics students can be graded more harshly than their male peers. Experimental results show a significant gap between boys’ and girls’ scores for the exact same ans- wer when graded by teachers with up to 10 years of experi- ence (Hofer, 2015). Awards and grants Women often have to prove more evidence of competence than men in order to seem as equally competent. Women needed 2.5 times the publications of their male counterparts to mitigate the bias favoring males in the ap- plication process. This disadvantage was not found among those female candidates who knew someone on the panel. These were the conclusions of a groundbreaking study of postdoc fellowships awarded by Sweden’s Medical Research (Wenneras & Wold, 1997). A strong CV can compensate for a weaker grant proposal, but only for men. An analysis of application and review ma- terials (n= 2823) for a prestigious personal research grant in the Netherlands found evidence of gender bias favoring males. Men received significantly more competitive “quality of researcher” evaluations and had significantly higher ap- plication success rates despite receiving “quality of propo- sal” evaluations on par with women applicants (van der Lee & Ellemers, 2015). Females are underrepresented as recipients of scholarly and research awards and tend to receive awards at higher rates for teaching and service (AWIS, 2015). This is also evi- dent here at the MNF where the odds of male PhD students getting a distinction are 1.76 times more than for females. PhD‘s distinctions awarded by the Faculty of Science 2008-16 Publications and citations Studies analyzing decades of scientific publishing and hundreds of thousands of papers show significant differences in the pat- terns of women and men authors. New papers also show the value of gender diversity in the team of authors. Analysis of over 8 million papers from natural sciences, social sciences, and humanities reveals persistent, sub- tle gender inequality. For example, men predominate the prestigious first and last author positions, and women are significantly underrepresented as authors of single-author papers (West et al,2013). Women scientists who collaborate and publish together with their husbands/partners are often described as taking a “partnership” advantage, while this is hardly the case vice versa (Ahlqvist et al, 2014). Male and female experiment participants rated publications purportedly from male authors as higher in scientific quality, especially if the topic was male-typed. Collaboration inte- rest was highest for male authors working on male-typed topics. These are the results of an experiment in which 243 young communication scholars rated conference abstracts ostensibly authored by men or women, but in fact the author gender was randomly assigned Knobloch-Westerwick et al, 2013). Gender-heterogeneous working groups generally produce papers with higher perceived quality than other groups comprised of highly-performing members of the same gen- der (Campbell et al, 2013). Networking and letters of recommendation Women have less access to powerful networks, fewer powerful mentors, and less convincing letters of recommendation. Networking is complex and subconscious. The facts that men tend to form social bonds more easily with other men and that the majority of academics in senior positions are men means there are strong and informal networks in which men recommend and support each other, cite each other’s works, and keep each other informed of job opportunities (van den Brink & Benschop, 2011). Women are under-represented in the world’s science aca- demies and research councils – worldwide, most are more than 80% male. Fewer of half of academies and councils have strategies or policies in place to address the issue (Gibney, 2016). High-achieving and elite male researchers in the life scien- ces train 10 - 40% fewer women than do their peers. The more decorated the male professor, the greater the skew. Women professors do not show this bias. A disproportionate number of assistant professors are trained in and recruited from such elite “gateway” labs, thus affecting the number of highly competitive women in faculty job searches (Sheltzer & Smith, 2014). Female postdocs are less likely than men to get a glowing reference. Men more often received superlative adjectives like “remarkable” or “outstanding,” while women were often described as “hard-working.” These were findings of an ana- lysis of tone and length of over 1000 letters of recommen- dation (Skibba, 2016). A study of over 300 letters of recommendation for medical school showed letters for women were shorter, showed less conviction, and more often mentioned the women’s perso- nal lives. Women were more often described as students and teachers, while men were described as researchers and pro- fessionals (Trix & Psenka, 2003) Hiring Women walk a fine line when presenting themselves in hiring situations. Those who present themselves as highly confident and competent can be perceived as bossy, too ambitious, too aggressive. Those who adhere more closely to the social norms of their gender — warm, gentle and modest - are rated as less competent. In a randomized, double-blind study involving 127 nearly identical applications for a lab tech position, which only dif- fered by a randomly assigned male or female name, male and female science faculty members alike rated male ap- plicants as more scientifically competent and more hirab- le. The study participants also offered the males a higher starting salary and more career mentoring. (Moss-Racusin et al, 2012) When symphony orchestras conduct “blind” auditions by using screens to conceal candidates’ identities, the hiring of women increased. The inability to identify the gender of the musician enabled more impartiality and diminished the influence of common assumptions that female musicians produce “poorer sound” with “smaller techniques.” Similar results occurred in blind auditions for programmers and en- gineers (Goldin & Rouse, 2000). Men were two times more likely than women to be hired for a job requiring math according to an experimental hi- ring situation in which candidates were asked to perform an arithmetic task that men and women can perform equally on average. When candidates were allowed to self-report their success on the task, women still are discriminated against, because employers do not fully account for men’s tendency to boast about performance (Reuben et al, 2014) Leadership and society Gender biases stem “from repeated exposure to pervasive cultural stereotypes that portray women as less competent but simulta- neously emphasize their warmth and likeability compared with men” (Moss-Racusin et al, 2012). Men are stereotypically judged to be stronger leaders than women. However, a randomized field experiment (of 927 group members and 70 group leaders at a large universi- ty in Western Europe) showed that when the team being led was comprised of approximately 40% women or more, evaluations of female leaders rose to levels on par with the evaluations of male leaders (Gloor et al, 2016). Pervasive cultural stereotypes portray women as wives and mothers. Woman scientists who have a partner who also works in science often have to adapt their career to the mo- ves to the often somewhat older/scientifically more advan- ced partner. The “baby penalty” affects women with children, making them far less likely to receive tenure than childless women or men with or without children (Mason et al, 2013). Simi- larly, a “maybe baby” bias in hiring disadvantages women without children (Stöcklin, 2016). When women aren’t present for senior-level decision ma- king, organizations can fail to see the value of programs and initiatives that benefit the female half of the population. Even if an organizational culture explicitly promotes me- ritocracy, it nevertheless shows bias in favor of men over equally performing women when it comes to performance assessment, promotion, and monetary rewards. This “para- dox of meritocracy” was shown in experiments involving 445 experienced managers performing staff evaluations in which the gender of the employee was manipulated (Castil- la & Benard, 2010). The pipeline not only leaks... and other similar patterns act as tailwinds, subtly suppor- ting and promoting men in their scientific careers. It is im- portant to think of the accumulated influence of bias when evaluating the metrics that finally make it to a candidate’s CV. Am I gender biased? Our subconscious influences how we make sense of the world around us, and this can be extremely useful. However, sometimes the way our subconscious naturally processes information can result in faulty interpretations and bia- sed decisions, which we are often not fully aware of and that may directly conflict with our consciously held views. A good exercise for understanding your own unconscious thought processes is the Implicit Association Test availa- ble at https://implicit.harvard.edu. The IAT measures how strongly you associate certain concepts (e.g., women, im- migrants) and evaluations such as good or bad. Your results might surprise you, and this could very well be the best first step towards counteracting bias. Examples of biases in academic settings: Start (1994, 1995,1996) Bach. & Mast. Students 1998 Graduation (2000, 2001, 2002) PhD (2003, 2004, 2005) Post Doc (2016) Assistant Professor 2016 Professor 2016 41% 38% 11% 0 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% 59% 41% 59% 41% 58% 42% 62% 62% 11% 16% 38% 38% 84% 89% Unconscious bias acts as headwinds and tailwinds treating men and women differently as each progresses through his or her scientific training & career Designed by Freepik Umbrella: Designed by Balasoiu / Freepik Percentages of men and women at the Faculty of Science OR 95% CI 1.76 1.18 to 2.63 Graduations Distinctions m f m f 951 799 77 38

Transcript of The pipeline not only leaks - UZHb13591f5-cc5f-4d55-8388-84b6a957… · scientists from 32...

Page 1: The pipeline not only leaks - UZHb13591f5-cc5f-4d55-8388-84b6a957… · scientists from 32 disciplines including biology and geology were surveyed. 3/4th were female. More than 20%

Is gender bias real? Headwinds and tailwindsThere is a growing body of evidence showing that women face unconscious and pervasive discrimination at many points along their scientific education and career. Women in science, for example, are graded more critically, receive fewer awards and grants, and are subconsciously viewed as natively less scientifically able than men (see columns below). Each incident of discrimination could be subtle or small in magnitude, but their accumulated influence over time acts as a headwind making women’s advancement in science more difficult.

In stark contrast, men and women alike tend to assess the scientific competency of males more favorably than that of females across all aspects of academic and scientific practice. Male students, for example, get more encourage-ment in their studies, are picked more often for academic awards, are recommended more convincingly for jobs, and are ultimately judged more hirable. Each of these examples

Classroom, lab, and field

Women students in science face challenges in how they are

perceived, graded, and treated.

• Professors might favor male students. Researchers sent

identical letters, purportedly from students, to more than

6,500 professors at 259 top American universities asking to

discuss research opportunities. Professors were more likely

to respond to email from “Brad Anderson” than from other

fictitious aspirants with names like “Claire Smith” or “Juan

Gonzalez.” (Milkman et al, 2015).

• Undergraduate students often prefer men both when rating

their peers (Grunspan et al, 2016) and their professors (Mac-

Nell et al, 2015).

• Nearly two-thirds of field scientists reported in a survey

that they had been sexually harassed in the field. 666 field

scientists from 32 disciplines including biology and geology

were surveyed. 3/4th were female. More than 20% reported

sexual assault. (Clancy et al, 2014).

• Females physics students can be graded more harshly than

their male peers. Experimental results show a significant

gap between boys’ and girls’ scores for the exact same ans-

wer when graded by teachers with up to 10 years of experi-

ence (Hofer, 2015).

Awards and grants

Women often have to prove more evidence of competence than men in order to seem as equally competent. • Women needed 2.5 times the publications of their male

counterparts to mitigate the bias favoring males in the ap-plication process. This disadvantage was not found among those female candidates who knew someone on the panel. These were the conclusions of a groundbreaking study of postdoc fellowships awarded by Sweden’s Medical Research (Wenneras & Wold, 1997).

• A strong CV can compensate for a weaker grant proposal, but only for men. An analysis of application and review ma-terials (n= 2823) for a prestigious personal research grant in the Netherlands found evidence of gender bias favoring males. Men received significantly more competitive “quality of researcher” evaluations and had significantly higher ap-plication success rates despite receiving “quality of propo-sal” evaluations on par with women applicants (van der Lee & Ellemers, 2015).

• Females are underrepresented as recipients of scholarly and research awards and tend to receive awards at higher rates for teaching and service (AWIS, 2015). This is also evi-dent here at the MNF where the odds of male PhD students getting a distinction are 1.76 times more than for females. PhD‘s distinctions awarded by the Faculty of Science 2008-16

Publications and citations

Studies analyzing decades of scientific publishing and hundreds

of thousands of papers show significant differences in the pat-

terns of women and men authors. New papers also show the

value of gender diversity in the team of authors.

• Analysis of over 8 million papers from natural sciences,

social sciences, and humanities reveals persistent, sub-

tle gender inequality. For example, men predominate the

prestigious first and last author positions, and women are

significantly underrepresented as authors of single-author

papers (West et al,2013).

• Women scientists who collaborate and publish together

with their husbands/partners are often described as taking

a “partnership” advantage, while this is hardly the case vice

versa (Ahlqvist et al, 2014).

• Male and female experiment participants rated publications

purportedly from male authors as higher in scientific quality,

especially if the topic was male-typed. Collaboration inte-

rest was highest for male authors working on male-typed

topics. These are the results of an experiment in which 243

young communication scholars rated conference abstracts

ostensibly authored by men or women, but in fact the author

gender was randomly assigned Knobloch-Westerwick et al,

2013).

• Gender-heterogeneous working groups generally produce

papers with higher perceived quality than other groups

comprised of highly-performing members of the same gen-

der (Campbell et al, 2013).

Networking and letters of recommendation

Women have less access to powerful networks, fewer powerful mentors, and less convincing letters of recommendation. • Networking is complex and subconscious. The facts that

men tend to form social bonds more easily with other men and that the majority of academics in senior positions are men means there are strong and informal networks in which men recommend and support each other, cite each other’s works, and keep each other informed of job opportunities (van den Brink & Benschop, 2011).

• Women are under-represented in the world’s science aca-demies and research councils – worldwide, most are more than 80% male. Fewer of half of academies and councils have strategies or policies in place to address the issue (Gibney, 2016).

• High-achieving and elite male researchers in the life scien-ces train 10 - 40% fewer women than do their peers. The more decorated the male professor, the greater the skew. Women professors do not show this bias. A disproportionate number of assistant professors are trained in and recruited from such elite “gateway” labs, thus affecting the number of highly competitive women in faculty job searches (Sheltzer & Smith, 2014).

• Female postdocs are less likely than men to get a glowing reference. Men more often received superlative adjectives like “remarkable” or “outstanding,” while women were often described as “hard-working.” These were findings of an ana-lysis of tone and length of over 1000 letters of recommen-dation (Skibba, 2016).

• A study of over 300 letters of recommendation for medical school showed letters for women were shorter, showed less conviction, and more often mentioned the women’s perso-nal lives. Women were more often described as students and teachers, while men were described as researchers and pro-fessionals (Trix & Psenka, 2003)

Hiring

Women walk a fine line when presenting themselves in hiring situations. Those who present themselves as highly confident and competent can be perceived as bossy, too ambitious, too aggressive. Those who adhere more closely to the social norms of their gender — warm, gentle and modest - are rated as less competent.• In a randomized, double-blind study involving 127 nearly

identical applications for a lab tech position, which only dif-fered by a randomly assigned male or female name, male and female science faculty members alike rated male ap-plicants as more scientifically competent and more hirab-le. The study participants also offered the males a higher starting salary and more career mentoring. (Moss-Racusin et al, 2012)

• When symphony orchestras conduct “blind” auditions by using screens to conceal candidates’ identities, the hiring of women increased. The inability to identify the gender of the musician enabled more impartiality and diminished the influence of common assumptions that female musicians produce “poorer sound” with “smaller techniques.” Similar results occurred in blind auditions for programmers and en-gineers (Goldin & Rouse, 2000).

• Men were two times more likely than women to be hired for a job requiring math according to an experimental hi-ring situation in which candidates were asked to perform an arithmetic task that men and women can perform equally on average. When candidates were allowed to self-report their success on the task, women still are discriminated against, because employers do not fully account for men’s tendency to boast about performance (Reuben et al, 2014)

Leadership and society

Gender biases stem “from repeated exposure to pervasive cultural stereotypes that portray women as less competent but simulta-neously emphasize their warmth and likeability compared with men” (Moss-Racusin et al, 2012).• Men are stereotypically judged to be stronger leaders than

women. However, a randomized field experiment (of 927 group members and 70 group leaders at a large universi-ty in Western Europe) showed that when the team being led was comprised of approximately 40% women or more, evaluations of female leaders rose to levels on par with the evaluations of male leaders (Gloor et al, 2016).

• Pervasive cultural stereotypes portray women as wives and mothers. Woman scientists who have a partner who also works in science often have to adapt their career to the mo-ves to the often somewhat older/scientifically more advan-ced partner.

• The “baby penalty” affects women with children, making them far less likely to receive tenure than childless women or men with or without children (Mason et al, 2013). Simi-larly, a “maybe baby” bias in hiring disadvantages women without children (Stöcklin, 2016).

• When women aren’t present for senior-level decision ma-king, organizations can fail to see the value of programs and initiatives that benefit the female half of the population.

• Even if an organizational culture explicitly promotes me-ritocracy, it nevertheless shows bias in favor of men over equally performing women when it comes to performance assessment, promotion, and monetary rewards. This “para-dox of meritocracy” was shown in experiments involving 445 experienced managers performing staff evaluations in which the gender of the employee was manipulated (Castil-la & Benard, 2010).

The pipeline not only leaks...and other similar patterns act as tailwinds, subtly suppor-ting and promoting men in their scientific careers. It is im-portant to think of the accumulated influence of bias when evaluating the metrics that finally make it to a candidate’s CV.

Am I gender biased?Our subconscious influences how we make sense of the world around us, and this can be extremely useful. However, sometimes the way our subconscious naturally processes information can result in faulty interpretations and bia-sed decisions, which we are often not fully aware of and that may directly conflict with our consciously held views. A good exercise for understanding your own unconscious thought processes is the Implicit Association Test availa-ble at https://implicit.harvard.edu. The IAT measures how strongly you associate certain concepts (e.g., women, im-migrants) and evaluations such as good or bad. Your results might surprise you, and this could very well be the best first step towards counteracting bias.

Examples of biases in academic settings:

Start (1994, 1995,1996)

Bach. & Mast. Students 1998

Graduation (2000, 2001, 2002)

PhD (2003, 2004, 2005)

Post Doc (2016) Assistant Professor 2016 Professor 2016

41%38%

11%

0

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%59%

41%

59%

41%

58%

42%

62% 62%

11%

16%

38% 38%

84%89%Unconscious bias acts as headwinds and tailwinds treating men and women differently as each progresses through his or her scientific training & career

Designed by Freepik Umbrella: Designed by Balasoiu / Freepik

Perc

enta

ges

of m

en a

nd w

omen

at t

he F

acul

ty o

f Sci

ence

OR 95% CI

1.76 1.18 to 2.63

Graduations Distinctionsm f m f

951 799 77 38

Page 2: The pipeline not only leaks - UZHb13591f5-cc5f-4d55-8388-84b6a957… · scientists from 32 disciplines including biology and geology were surveyed. 3/4th were female. More than 20%

Faculty of Science

Dear Colleagues,

Hiring, retaining, and promoting exceptional academic col-leagues is one of the most important activities we do as faculty, and I thank you for your continued energy and commitment to finding and selecting the best candidates. However, our efforts to recruit, retain, and promote women have produced uneven results. In the ten years from 2007 to 2016, the percentage of women professors in our science faculty has remained stag-nant at around 15%, even though we added 19 new professors-hips in this time period. Recognizing this, we implemented in 2014 new measures in the professorial hiring process designed to increase the number of highly competitive women involved. We are confident these measures will lead to a very different result in the coming decade.

The focus upon women is part of a larger commitment to fair and self-critical recruitment and hiring processes. We have a collective responsibility to question our evaluation criteria to define and assess “quality,” “excellence,” and “expertise” in science. Is there a gap between measurable-impact and percei-ved-impact when using popular metrics such as the H index to evaluate candidates? We signed the San Francisco Declaration on Research Assessment (DORA) confirming that scientific out-put is measured accurately and evaluated wisely. And if “excel-lence in science” is more than a singular quantitative indicator of publications, what does this mean for the other key factors such as impact in one’s field?

We need to be introspective about our current practices at both personal and institutional levels. Men and women alike unwittingly allow unconscious thoughts and experiences to

unintentionally influence what they believe to be objective decisions. Many studies show that merely changing the gender identity of an applicant radically and consistently alters the way others judge the quality of that person’s work. We need to have the courage to enact the measures necessary to pre-vent unintended discrimination. With this brochure, we want to make you, serving as committee members in professorial appointments and mentoring our excellent scientists, aware of the growing body of knowledge showing the pervasiveness of gender bias in academia.

The selection of new professors has far-reaching consequen-ces, some of which can reduce gender bias. All students need formal and informal role models. The relative lack of female professors in our faculty creates a conscious and subconscious sense in our female students of not belonging. Furthermore, the gender diversity of the MNF professoriate has important implications upon the equitable representation of women in decision-making, and we have strong evidence that a gen-der-balanced faculty increases the creativity, innovation, and productivity of an institution. All of which would increase our competitive advantage on the short, medium, and long term.

I thank you all for contributing to our faculty being a mirror of society, both in terms of topics covered and in gender diversity.

Prof. Dr. Michael SchaepmanDean, Faculty of Science (MNF), UZH

Be aware of cognitive biases• People generally have a bias in favor of the status quo. Change creates

discomfort.• Confirmation bias influences us to selectively search for and interpret

evidence that verifies our preconceptions or expectations. For examp-le: grant reviewers favor proposals for projects that confirm their own views (Ernst et al, 1992)

• Attribution error is our tendency to make assumptions, for example, that high performing males achieved success by their own merit, while high performing females had a good team of support (Pern Kandola, 2014)

Gender balanced structure and search committees □ Include at least two female professors from MNF in committees prepa-

ring the Statements on Professorial Positions and in hiring committees. □ Determine the selection criteria and their relative weight prior to adver-

tisement of the position and apply them consistently to all applicants.Note:• The lower the percentage of women on selection committees and the

less transparent the criteria for selection, the less likely women are to be appointed (EC, 2009; Zinovyeva & Bagues, 2010).

• The less transparent the definition of “hirable” is in searches for acade-mic leaders, the more likely men are chosen over women (LERU, 2012).

• With unclear criteria committee members may tend to judge using cri-teria that favor candidates from well-represented demographic groups (Biernat & Fuegen, 2001; Uhlmann & Cohen, 2005)

Advertising the job □ Define the position in the widest possible terms consistent with the

needs of the university/department. Make sure the position description does not unintentionally exclude female applicants by focusing too nar-rowly on subfields in which few specialize.

□ Include only those qualifications that are vital. Research shows that wo-men apply for jobs if they meet 7 of 10 listed qualifications; men apply if they meet 4 of 10 (Pearn Kandola, 2014).

□ Use gender neutral vocabulary. Be aware that men are typi-cally described with individual and authoritative words whe-reas women are often described with communal words.

Individual, authoritative words Communal words

... determined and independent student ... ... committed and responsible student ...

... analytical work style … … conscientious work style …

Determined, autonomous, competencies,

champion, promising success, leadership

position, aggressive, analytical, courage, and

outspoken

Committed, responsible, talents, helpful,

support, responsibility for employees,

conscientious, trustworthy, collaborative,

and sociable

□ Include proactive language which indicates a commitment to diversity: ° “The University of Zurich is an equal opportunities employer.” ° “The University is especially interested in qualified candidates

who can contribute, through their research, teaching, and/or ser-vice to the diversity and excellence of the academic community.” (from the University of Michigan)

Selecting the short list □ Evaluate both the full CV and the 1-page CV. The 1-page CV is a form

submitted by the applicant which provides information on periods of

time in which the applicant was not engaged in an academic activity on a full-time basis (possibly due to family commitments, time spent working in industry, long periods of illness, etc.) and can be used to estimate the “academic age” in a fair way.

□ Select at least one female referee for writing a review letter for each candidate of the final selection. If comparative review letters are requi-red, at least on must be from a female referee.

□ Statements on Professorial Positions are to contain gender-balanced lists (50/50) of academics that the structure committee considers to be possible candidates for the position. The Dean rejects structure reports until they comply. The search committee contacts all listed individuals.

Note:• The University of Michigan introduced a set of measures addressing the

way applications are judged (criterion-specific instead of global judg-ments) and has increased the proportion of appointments of female professors from 9% in 2001-2 to 31% in 2002-08.

Organize talks, interviews, and site visit □ Write a set of core questions before the interviews to be used with

every candidate. The more structured the interview, the more compa-rable the result.

□ Do not ask about family issues related to a potential move to Zurich during the interviews.

□ Avoid solo status, if at all possible. Research shows that if there is only one candidate who differs from the others in some aspect such as gen-der, ethnicity, or age, chances to be hired decrease for this candidate.

Note:• The odds of hiring a woman were 80 times greater if there were at least

two women in the finalist pool (regardless of the size of the finalist pool.) And there was statistically NO chance of hiring a woman when she was the sole female in the finalist pool. These were the conclusions from an empirical study that looked at a university’s hiring decisions involving 598 job finalists for 174 positions over 3 years (Johnson et al, 2016).

M

F 67%

50%

0%

F

F

F

Composition of finalist pool

Likelihood of hiring a womanF

F

F F

F

M

M

M

M M

• Gender stereotypes are likely to negatively influence evaluation of women when they represent a small proportion (less than 25%) of the pool of candidates. In other words, seeming different from the group makes a candidate’s gender difference the most salient point in her evaluation. Focusing on a candidate as female instead of the candida-te as a scientist leads to inferences of incompetence. (Heilman, 1980).

Selection of new professors• The selection of new professors is an imperfect process. Import-

ant reasons for this are that academic excellence is not inherently a gender-neutral standard of merit and the criteria developed in a hiring process are different from those actually applied, often due to constraints on time. Focus is instead upon the “suitable” candidate.

How to counteract unconscious gender biases in professorial appointments

The MNF appoints its professors in accordance with federal and cantonal law, as well as university and faculty regulations. Moreo-ver, the Swiss federal Constitution contains an anti-discrimination clause, which protects the characteristics of origin, race, gender, age, language, social position, way of life, religious, ideological or political convictions, and physical, mental or psychological di-sability (Article 8). The Constitution also specifically states that women and men have equal rights both in law and in practice.

For the MNF this means that the way we recruit must afford all ap-plicants the same opportunity to convince us of their excellence. The following numbers raise some questions about how well the MNF is currently doing:

• The MNF appointed 67 professors in the decade ending 2016, 10 (15%) of these were women.

• In the same time period, the MNF professoriate grew from 93 (2007) to 110 (2016), an increase of 18%. However, the num-ber of women MNF professors grew only from 14 in 2007 to 16 in 2016, which implies a stagnation in the percentage of women professors at 15%.

The Faculty of Science has taken steps to unpack the professorial recruitment process. Data for 2007 – 2016 show that: 1 • Women submitted 14% of all applications (444 of 3136 appli-

cants were women)• Women constituted 21% of job talk invitees (48 of 227 invi-

tees were women)• Women made up 17% of primo loco-placed candidates after

the job talks (6 of 35 primo-loco ranked applicants were wo-men)

• Women were 17% of new professorial appointments (6 of 35 new appointments were women)

Women constitute a fifth or less of the potential appointees th-roughout the process. This is not enough to increase the number of MNF women professors substantially. Why do not more women scientists apply at MNF? A larger pool of applications by women would enable the Faculty to also increase the percentages of wo-men in the subsequent states of the recruitment process. On a positive note, all primo-loco placed women candidates did even-tually become professors at the MNF.

1 The data on applications refer to 33 recruitment processes; the data on job talks refer to 35 recruit-ment processes; the data on rankings refer to: 35 recruitment processes; the data on appointments refer to 35 recruitment processes. This is due to missing data.

Recruiting for Excellence

What are we doing now?

SOURCE STEFANIE K. JOHNSON ET AL

Man and women professors at the MNF 2007 to 2016

Number of man and woman new appointments at the MNF 2007 to 2016

AuthorsSara Petchey, UZH Faculty of ScienceDr. Karin Gilland-Lutz, UZH Office for Gender Equality Petra Seibert, UZH Faculty of ScienceDr. Ines Kohler, UZH Faculty of Science

2007 2008 2009 2010 20152011 2012 2013 2014 20160

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%79 76 9493888579807978

14 15 1615161615131313

Assistant Professors

University goals

Professors15 93

Male Professors

Number of Professors at the MNF

Female Professors

25%

2007 2008 2009 2010 20152011 2012 2013 2014 20160

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

79 76 9493888579807978

14 15 1615161615131313

University goalsUZH target percentage for woman professors by 2016

15 93 numberman Professors

Professors at the MNF

numberwoman Professors

25%

percentage of female professors at the MNF

UZH target percentage for woman professors in the longer term

50%

2007 2008 2009 2010 20152011 2012 2013 2014 20160

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%7 1 198104653

3 111121

15 93

Male ProfessorsFemale Professors

2007 2008 2009 2010 20152011 2012 2013 2014 20160

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

7 1

1

98104653

3 111121

15 93

Male ProfessorsFemale Professorsaverage percentage of female new appointments at the MNF

0

2

4

6

8

10

2007 2008 2009 2010 20152011 2012 2013 2014 2016man Professorswomen Professors

Ahlqvist, V et al (2014) A Gender Neutral Process? – A Qualitative Study of the Evaluation of Research Grant Applications [Online]. Available at https://publikationer.vr.se/produkt/a-gender-neutral-process/ [Accessed 24 April 2017].

AWIS (Association for Women in Science) (2015) Fact Sheet: Improving Recognition & Advancement Through Awards Equity [Online]. Available at https://c.ymcdn.com/sites/awis.site-ym.com/resource/resmgr/Fact_Sheets/AWIS_Fact_Sheet_AWARDS_2015.pdf [Accessed 12 January 2017].

Biernat, M & K Fuegen (2001) Shifting Standards and the Evaluation of Competence: Complexity in Gender-Based Judgment and Decision Making. Journal of Social Issues, 57: 707-724. DOI: 10.1111/0022-4537.00237

Campbell, LG et al (2013) Gender-Heterogeneous Working Groups Produce Higher Quality Science. PLoS ONE, 8(10): e79147. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079147

Castilla, EJ & S Benard (2010) The Paradox of Meritocracy in Organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 55 (2010): 543-576. doi:10.2189/asqu.2010.55.4.543

Clancy, KB et al (2014) Survey of Academic Field Experiences (SAFE): Trainees Report Harassment and Assault. PLoS ONE, 9(7): e102172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102172

Ernst, E et al (1992) Reviewer Bias. Annals of Internal Medicine, 116(11): 958.

European Commission (2009) The Gender Challenge in Research Funding. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/research/swafs/pdf/pub_gender_equality/gender-challenge-in-research-funding_en.pdf [Accessed 24 April 2017].

Gibney, E (2016) Women under-represented in world’s science academies [Online]. Nature, doi:10.1038/nature.2016.19465 [Accessed 24 April 2017].

Gloor, JL, MC Morf, & U Backes-Gellner (2016) Fix the Game – Not the Dame: A Team Intervention for Gender Equality in Leadership [Online.] Available from: http://gap.hks.harvard.edu/fix-game-%E2%80%93-not-dame-team-intervention-gender-equality-leadership#findings [Accessed 10 January 2017].

Goldin C & C Rouse (2000) Orchestrating Impartiality: The Impact of “Blind” Auditions on Female Musicians. American Economic Review, 90(4): 715-741.

Grunspan, DZ et al (2016) Males under estimate academic performance of their female peers in the undergraduate biology classrooms [Online]. PLoS One, 11(2): e0148405. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0148405 [Accessed 24 April 2017].

Heilman, ME (1980) The impact of situational factors on personnel decisions concerning women: varying the sex composition of the applicant pool. Orga-nizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26:386-395.

Hofer, SI (2015) Studying Gender Bias in Physics Grading: The role of teaching experience and country. International Journal of Science Education, 37(17): 2879-2905, DOI: 10.1080/09500693.2015.1114190

Johnson, SK et al (2016) If There’s Only One Woman in Your Candidate Pool, There’s Statistically No Chance She’ll Be Hired [Online]. Harvard Business Review. Available at: https://hbr.org/2016/04/if-theres-only-one-woman-in-your-candidate-pool-theres-statistically-no-chance-shell-be-hired [Accessed

Sheltzer, JM & JC Smith (2014) Elite male faculty in the life sciences employ fewer women. PNAS 111(28):10107-10112. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1403334111

Skibba, R (2016) Women postdocs less likely than men to get a glowing reference [Online]. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature.2016.20715

Stöcklin, S (2016) Subtile Diskriminierungen verhindern. UZH Journal (6):5.

Trix, F & C Psenka (2003) Exploring the color of glass: letters of recommendation for female and male medical faculty. Discourse & Society, 14(2):191-220.

Uhlmann, EL & GL Cohen (2005) Constructed Criteria: Redefining Merit to Justify Discrimination. Psychological Science, 16(6):474-480.

van den Brink, M & Y Benschop (2011) Gender practices in the construction of academic excellence: Sheep with five legs. Organization, 19(4):507-524.

van der Lee, R & N Ellemers (2015) Gender contributes to personal research funding success in The Netherlands. PNAS, 112(40):12349–12353.

Wenneras, C & A Wold (1997) Nepotism and sexism in peer-review. Nature, 387:341-343.

West, JD et al (2013) The Role of Gender in Scholarly Authorship. PLoS ONE, 8(7): e66212. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066212

Zinovyeva, N & M Bagues (2010) Does gender matter for academic promotion? Evidence from a randomized natural experiment. IZA Discussion Paper No. 5537. Available at https://ssrn.com/abstract=1771259 [Accessed 24 April 2017].

24 April 2017].

Knobloch-Westerwick, S et al (2013) The Matilda effect in science communication: And experiment on gender bias in publication quality perceptions and collaboration interest. Science Communication, 35(5):603–625. doi: 10.1177/1075547012472684.

League of European Research Universities (2012) Women, research and universities: excellence without gender bias. Available at: http://www.leru.org/files/publications/LERU_Paper_Women_universities_and_research.pdf [Accessed 24 April 2017].

MacNell, L, A Driscoll, & AN Hunt (2015) What’s in a Name: Exposing Gender Bias in Student Ratings of Teaching. Innovative Higher Education, 40: 291–303. doi: 10.1007/s10755-014-9313-4

Mason, MA, NH Wolfinger, & M Goulden (2013) Do Babies Matter?: Gender and Family in the Ivory Tower. Rutgers University Press.

Milkman, KL, M Akinola, & D Chugh (2014) What Happens Before? A Field Experiment Exploring How Pay and Representation Differentially Shape Bias on the Pathway into Organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(6): 1678-1712.

Moss-Racusin, CA et al (2012) Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. PNAS 109(41): 16, 474-9.

Pern Kandola (2014) Workshop on gender bias at MNF. 8 September 2014.

Reuben, E, P Sapienza, & L Zingales (2014) How stereotypes impair women’s careers in science. PNAS, 111(12):4403-4408. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314788111.

RESOURCES CITED