PQA-R - HighScope

101
Preschool Program Quality Assessment — Revised Manual PQA-R

Transcript of PQA-R - HighScope

Page 1: PQA-R - HighScope

Preschool Program Quality Assessment — Revised Manual

PQA-R

Page 2: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

1

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments 2

Introduction to PQA-R Manual 4

Assessment Tool Example 9

Classroom Materials Checklist Example 19

Scoring Guide Introduction and Example 22

Research Base 51

Technical Report 97

Page 3: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 2

Acknowledgments

Many people have worked to bring the Preschool Program Quality Assessment — Revised

(PQA-R) together. We would like to thank the initial revision team: Tomoko Wakabayashi, Jill

Claxton, Eleanor Martin, and Beth Scholz. They took the ideas from the field about what

updates would make the 2003 Preschool PQA a better and more viable tool and turned those

ideas into a working draft assessment. A special thank you to Jeff Beal for his leadership and his

passion for better assessment tools in early childhood classrooms. Beth Hardin, Mary Delcamp,

and Anthony Nielsen provided excellent data collection and coordination throughout the

various trails and implementation year. Several content experts in the world of early childhood

reviewed numerous drafts. We would especially like to thank Sue Bredekamp, Nell Duke, Susan

Fitzpatrick, Susan Hyatt, Kay Neff, Amy Parks, and Tanya Wright for their critical comments that

made the tool better and more relevant. Staff from the Early Childhood Applied Practices team

and HighScope field consultants offered insightful feedback. The data analysis was provided by

Everett Smith and Fernando Andrade. They worked with all staff to ensure accuracy and to

explore ideas. We would also like to extend a heartfelt thank you to members of the Michigan

Department of Education and Early Childhood Investment Corporation for their invaluable

input. Joanne Tangorra, Marcella Weiner, Samantha Smith, and Henry Brown provided editorial

assistance. Over one hundred early childhood specialists and independent assessors

participated in the iterate trial process and implementation year. To the teachers and

administrators, we could not have completed this project without allowing observations to take

place in your buildings and classrooms. Funding for this project was provided by the HighScope

Page 4: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

3

Educational Research Foundation, and we would like to thank Cheryl Polk, President, and the

HighScope Board of Directors for the support necessary to complete this important assessment

revision. To everyone who had a part in bringing the Preschool Program Quality Assessment —

Revised (PQA-R) to fruition: Thank You!

Page 5: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 4

HighScope’s Preschool Program Quality Assessment – Revised (PQA-R)

Manual

Introduction

The Preschool PQA-R is an observation instrument that measures the quality of preschool

classrooms. It comprehensively measures the effectiveness of the learning environment;

teaching and learning routines and adult-child interactions; and curriculum, planning,

assessment and parent engagement in promoting the development of the whole child within a

supportive learning environment. The instrument can be used in all center-based settings. The

PQA-R intentionally reflects research-based and field-tested “best practices” in preschool

settings. The measure identifies the structural characteristics and dynamic relationships that

effectively promote the development of preschool-aged children.

HighScope developed the PQA-R to evaluate the quality of early childhood classrooms for four-

to five-year olds as well as identify the training needs of the classroom staff. The assessment is

intended for classroom evaluation in which a trained independent evaluator rates the

classroom characteristics. Alternatively, the tool can be used as a self-assessment tool by

administrators or instructional staff for classroom planning and monitoring. The PQA-R can be

used for research, evaluation, and to provide information to policymakers, program

administrators, parents, and researchers.

Each section is composed of items that describe a broad array of classroom characteristics and

rows that describe the quality of the classroom characteristics. Assessors assign a level, from 1

Page 6: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

5

(no or poor quality) to 4 (highest quality) for each row. The rows are summed to produce an

aggregate score for each section. Each section is then assigned a performance level of low,

medium, or high. The PQA-R defines quality along a continuum. Teachers and program

administrators are able to see where each classroom lies along the continuum of quality and

use the results to systematically plan school improvement goals and objectives. Based on

classroom observations and structured interviews with teaching staff, PQA-R assessors rate

classrooms on the quality of the learning environment, teaching and learning routines, adult-

child interactions, curriculum, planning and assessment, and family engagement.

Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation

The PQA-R is an observational assessment and thus the reliability of the results and the

effectiveness of its use relies on the ability of the observer to both observe accurately and score

reliably. The administration of the PQA-R, for accountability purposes, requires a trained and

reliable observer to spend at least three hours and up to an entire day in the preschool

classroom to complete the form, as evidence for each row must be entered before scores are

assigned. The trained observer completes the Materials Checklist and keeps a running log of all

the activities and adult-child interactions observed during the observation period. Sections I

and II contain rows that can be scored through observation only and Section III contains rows

that can only be scored by gathering additional information from classroom staff, using the

guiding questions provided. Each row is scored on a 4-point scale. Indicators for all scores 1–4

are described to assist assessor scoring, and the scoring guide provides examples of evidence

for each level to be used in determining the quality level for that row on a scale from 1 to 4.

Page 7: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 6

The row scores are then totaled to get a Section Score. The Section Score is then interpreted

into a Performance Level (i.e., low, medium, and high). The overall results are reported as three

Section Scores and three Performance Levels.

Structure of the Assessment

The assessment is made up of one form across three Sections: I. Learning Environment; II.

Teaching and Learning Routines and Adult-Child Interactions; and III. Curriculum, Planning,

Assessment, and Parent Engagement. The form is comprised of 20 items and 61 rows across the

three Sections. The PQA-R Sections and Items are as follows:

I. LEARNING ENVIRONMENT

I‐A: The indoor space has interest areas that have names and are intentionally organized.

I‐B: Classroom materials are plentiful.

I‐C: There is a safe outdoor play area with ample space, structures, and materials to support

many types of movement and play.

I‐D: Children’s work and environmental print are on display.

II. TEACHING AND LEARNING ROUTINES and ADULT‐CHILD INTERACTIONS

II‐A: The classroom follows a consistent sequence of events (daily routine) during the day.

II‐B: There is time each day for child‐initiated activities in the classroom and during outdoor

play.

Page 8: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

7

II‐C: Adults support children’s ideas, actions, and developmental levels during child‐initiated

activities.

II‐D: There is time each day for adult‐initiated large‐group activities that support each child’s

developmental level.

II-E: There is time each day for adult-initiated small-group activities that support each child’s

developmental level.

II‐F: Adults create a sensitive and responsive learning environment for all children.

II‐G: Adults support children to make plans for and reflect upon their work.

II‐H: Adults support children’s language and literacy development throughout the day.

II‐I: Adults support children’s mathematics development throughout the day.

II‐J: Adults support children’s reasoning and problem‐solving throughout the day.

II‐K: Adults encourage thoughtful social interaction among all children throughout the day.

II‐L: Adults diffuse conflicts and support all children in resolving conflicts.

III. CURRICULUM, PLANNING, ASSESSMENT, and PARENT ENGAGEMENT

III‐A: Adults use a comprehensive, evidence‐based educational model(s)/approach(es) to guide

teaching practices.

III‐B: Adults document the developmental progress of each child using measures validated for

preschool‐aged children.

III‐C: Adults record and use anecdotal notes to plan.

Page 9: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 8

III‐D: Adults share responsibilities for planning activities connected to the comprehensive

educational model(s)/approach(es) that are focused on play.

III‐E: Adults provide many parent engagement options, encourage two‐way sharing of child

information, and support families with resources about child development and program

transitioning.

Page 10: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

9

Hig

hSc

op

e’s

Pre

sch

oo

l Pro

gram

Qu

alit

y A

sses

smen

t —

Rev

ised

I.LE

AR

NIN

G E

NV

IRO

NM

ENT

I‐A

: Th

e in

do

or

spac

e h

as a

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

hav

e n

ames

an

d a

re in

ten

tio

nal

ly o

rgan

ized

. Pag

e 4

I‐B

: C

lass

roo

m m

ater

ials

are

ple

nti

ful.

Pag

e 5

I‐C

: Th

ere

is a

saf

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

area

wit

h a

mp

le s

pac

e, s

tru

ctu

res,

an

d m

ater

ials

to

su

pp

ort

man

y ty

pes

of

mo

vem

ent.

Pag

e 6

I‐D

: C

hild

ren

’s w

ork

an

d e

nvi

ron

men

tal p

rin

t ar

e o

n d

isp

lay.

Pag

e 7

II.

TEA

CH

ING

AN

D L

EAR

NIN

G R

OU

TIN

ES a

nd

AD

ULT

‐CH

ILD

INT

ERA

CTI

ON

SII‐

A:

The

clas

sro

om

follo

ws

a co

nsi

sten

t se

qu

ence

of e

ven

ts d

uri

ng

the

sch

oo

l day

. Pag

e 8

II‐B

: Th

ere

is t

ime

eac

h d

ay fo

r ch

ild‐i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m a

nd

du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me

. Pag

es 9

-10

II-C

: Ad

ult

s su

pp

ort

ch

ildre

n’s

idea

s, a

ctio

ns,

an

d d

evel

op

men

tal l

evel

s d

uri

ng

child

-in

itia

ted

act

ivit

ies.

Pag

e 1

1

II‐D

: Th

ere

is t

ime

eac

h d

ay fo

r ad

ult

‐in

itia

ted

, lar

ge‐g

rou

p a

ctiv

itie

s th

at s

up

po

rt e

ach

ch

ild’s

dev

elo

pm

enta

l lev

el.

Pag

es 1

2-1

3

II-E

: Th

ere

is t

ime

each

day

fo

r ad

ult

-in

itia

ted

, sm

all-

gro

up

act

ivit

ies

that

su

pp

ort

eac

h c

hild

’s d

evel

op

men

tal l

evel

. Pag

es 1

4-1

6

II‐F:

Ad

ult

s cr

eate

a s

ensi

tive

an

d r

esp

on

sive

lear

nin

g en

viro

nm

ent

for

all c

hild

ren

. Pag

es 1

7-1

8

II‐G

: Ad

ult

s en

cour

age

and

sup

po

rt c

hild

ren

to m

ake

pla

ns

for

and

ref

lect

up

on

th

eir

wo

rk. P

ages

19

-20

II‐H

: Ad

ult

s su

pp

ort

ch

ildre

n’s

lan

guag

e a

nd

lite

racy

dev

elo

pm

ent

thro

ugh

ou

t th

e d

ay. P

ages

21

-24

II‐I:

Ad

ult

s su

pp

ort

ch

ildre

n’s

mat

hem

atic

s d

eve

lop

men

t th

rou

gho

ut

the

day

. Pag

es 2

5-2

8

II-J

: A

du

lts

sup

po

rt c

hild

ren

’s r

easo

nin

g an

d p

rob

lem

so

lvin

g th

rou

gho

ut

the

day

. Pag

es 2

9-3

0

II‐K

: A

du

lts

enco

ura

ge t

ho

ugh

tfu

l so

cial

inte

ract

ion

am

ong

all c

hild

ren

th

rou

gho

ut

the

day

. Pag

e 3

1

II‐L:

Ad

ult

s d

iffu

se c

on

flic

ts a

nd

su

pp

ort

all

child

ren

in r

eso

lvin

g co

nfl

icts

. Pag

e 3

2

III.

CU

RR

ICU

LUM

, PLA

NN

ING

, ASS

ESSM

ENT

, an

d F

AM

ILY

EN

GA

GEM

ENT

III‐A

: A

du

lts

use

a c

om

pre

hen

sive

, evi

den

ce‐b

ased

ed

uca

tio

nal

mo

del

(s)/

app

roac

h(e

s) t

o g

uid

e t

each

ing

pra

ctic

es. P

age

33

III‐B

: A

du

lts

do

cum

ent

the

dev

elo

pm

enta

l pro

gres

s o

f eac

h c

hild

usi

ng

mea

sure

s va

lidat

ed fo

r p

resc

ho

ol‐

aged

ch

ildre

n. P

age

34

III‐C

: A

du

lts

reco

rd a

nd

use

an

ecd

ota

l no

tes

to c

reat

e le

sson

pla

ns

that

are

co

nn

ecte

d t

o le

arn

ing

goal

s an

d f

ocu

sed

on

lear

nin

g th

rou

gh d

evel

op

men

tally

ap

pro

pri

ate

pra

ctic

es (

pla

y). P

ages

35

-36

III‐D

: A

du

lts

pro

vid

e m

any

fam

ily e

nga

gem

ent

op

tio

ns,

en

cou

rage

tw

o‐w

ay s

har

ing

of c

hild

info

rmat

ion

, an

d s

up

po

rt fa

mili

es w

ith

pro

gram

tra

nsi

tio

nin

g.

Pag

es 3

7-3

8

Page 11: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

10

Ass

essm

ent

Pro

toco

ls

Fou

r st

eps

for

com

ple

tin

g th

e P

QA

-R:

1.O

bse

rve

in a

pre

sch

oo

l cla

ssro

om

wh

ile r

eco

rdin

g o

bje

ctiv

e ev

iden

ce a

nd

co

mp

leti

ng

the

Cla

ssro

om

Mat

eria

ls C

he

cklis

t (C

MC

). I

t is

rec

om

men

ded

to s

ched

ule

tim

e p

rio

r to

th

e st

art

of

the

sch

oo

l day

fo

r th

e co

mp

leti

on

of

the

CM

C a

nd

Sec

tio

n I:

Lea

rnin

g En

viro

nm

ent.

2.A

sk a

ll o

f th

e gu

idin

g q

ues

tio

ns

in S

ecti

on

III.

3.R

eco

rd o

bje

ctiv

e ev

iden

ce f

or

each

ro

w (

wit

hin

on

line

PQ

A-R

).

4.R

ead

th

e d

escr

ipto

r(s)

, sco

rin

g n

ote

(s),

an

d o

bje

ctiv

e ev

iden

ce a

nd

ass

ign

th

e ap

pro

pri

ate

row

sco

re le

vel (

1, 2

, 3, o

r 4

).

Des

crip

tors

: So

me

des

crip

tors

are

wri

tten

on

a c

on

tin

uu

m o

f n

o o

r p

oo

r q

ual

ity

to h

igh

qu

alit

y. T

his

is r

epre

sen

ted

in s

ever

al w

ays,

incl

ud

ing

rare

ly o

r n

ever

, so

met

imes

, usu

ally

, an

d a

lwa

ys.

•R

are

ly o

r n

ever

sh

ou

ld b

e se

lect

ed w

hen

th

ere

is n

o e

vid

ence

to

su

pp

ort

a h

igh

er le

vel.

•So

met

imes

sh

ou

ld b

e th

ou

ght

of

as m

ore

th

an o

nce

bu

t n

ot

use

d c

on

sist

entl

y th

rou

gho

ut

the

ob

serv

atio

n.

•U

sua

lly s

ho

uld

be

tho

ugh

t o

f as

mo

re t

han

a f

ew a

nd

fre

qu

entl

y u

sed

th

rou

gho

ut

the

ob

serv

atio

n.

•A

lwa

ys s

ho

uld

be

sele

cted

if t

he

re is

no

evi

den

ce t

hro

ugh

ou

t th

e o

bse

rvat

ion

co

ntr

ary

to t

he

hig

hes

t le

vel.

Alw

ays

on

ly r

efer

s to

wh

en y

ou

are

ob

serv

ing.

Som

e d

escr

ipto

rs a

re r

epre

sen

ted

by

no

ne,

few

, so

me,

an

d m

an

y. P

rofe

ssio

nal

exp

erie

nce

, kn

ow

led

ge, a

nd

jud

gmen

t sh

ou

ld g

uid

e th

e sc

ori

ng

for

thes

e d

escr

ipto

rs.

Clo

sed

bu

llete

d p

oin

t lis

ts: W

hen

clo

sed

bu

llete

d p

oin

ts a

re u

sed

in a

list

, eve

ry b

ulle

t p

oin

t m

ust

be

met

to

sco

re a

leve

l 4.

Op

en b

ulle

ted

po

int

lists

: Op

en b

ulle

ted

po

ints

are

exa

mp

les;

no

t ev

ery

exa

mp

le m

ust

be

met

. Th

e o

pe

n b

ulle

ted

list

s ar

e n

ot

exh

aust

ive

lists

— t

hey

are

si

mp

ly e

xam

ple

s.

Page 12: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

11

Pre

sch

oo

l Pro

gram

Qu

alit

y A

sses

smen

t —

Rev

ised

(P

QA

‐R)

Prog

ram

/Cla

ssro

om/A

sses

sor

Info

rmat

ion

Prog

ram

info

rmat

ion

H

ou

rs p

rogr

am/c

lass

roo

m in

ses

sio

n

Nam

e o

f age

ncy:

D

ays

of

wee

k (c

ircl

e a

ll th

at a

pp

ly)

Nam

e o

f cen

ter:

M

ON

TU

E W

ED

THU

FR

I SA

T

Nam

e of

cla

ssro

om b

eing

obs

erve

d:

Tim

e se

ssio

n b

egin

s eac

h d

ay:

Prog

ram

dir

ecto

r/ad

min

istr

ator

or

cont

act

pers

on

Tim

e se

ssio

n e

nd

s eac

h d

ay:

Nam

e:

Posi

tio

n/t

itle

: A

sses

sor

info

rmat

ion

Nam

e:

Cont

act

info

rmat

ion

of

prog

ram

/cla

ssro

om b

eing

obs

erve

d

Ph

on

e:

Ext:

City

/sta

te/z

ip:

Fax:

Phon

e:

Ext:

Em

ail:

Fax:

Emai

l: PQ

A‐R

adm

inis

trat

ion

info

rmat

ion

Dat

e of

obs

erva

tion

:

Clas

sroo

m s

taff

B

egin

ning

tim

e:

Nam

e he

ad/l

ead

tea

cher

: En

ding

tim

e:

Nam

e as

soci

ate

teac

her:

Oth

er s

taff

: Co

mm

ents

or n

ote

s ab

out

adm

inis

teri

ng th

e P

QA

‐R in

this

clas

sro

om

:

Nam

e:

Posi

tion

/tit

le:

Nam

e:

Posi

tion

/tit

le:

Nam

e:

Posi

tion

/tit

le:

Child

ren

Num

ber

in th

e cl

assr

oo

m:

Rat

er’s

sig

natu

re:

Age

rang

e o

f chi

ldre

n:

Dat

e:

Page 13: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

12

I.LE

AR

NIN

G E

NV

IRO

NM

ENT

Item

I‐A

Th

e in

do

or

spac

e h

as a

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

hav

e n

ames

an

d a

re in

ten

tio

nal

ly o

rgan

ized

.

SCO

RE

Leve

ls

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

A

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

p

rovi

de

div

ers

e ac

tivi

ties

an

d c

an

be

tailo

red

to

in

div

idu

al

child

ren

’s in

tere

sts

(e.g

., c

reat

ive,

co

nst

ruct

ion,

d

ram

atic

pla

y,

inq

uir

y b

ased

, lit

erac

y) a

re

evid

ent

and

hav

e n

ames

.

1‐N

on

e 2

‐Few

3

‐So

me

4‐M

any

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: To

sco

re a

4, i

nter

est

area

s ha

ve n

ames

and

are

evi

dent

, an

d it

is c

lear

th

at t

he

ad

ult

s an

d c

hild

ren

kn

ow

th

e n

ames

of

the

inte

rest

are

as.

Ro

w 2

Le

vel 1

M

ater

ials

are

m

inim

ally

org

aniz

ed

or

are

no

t o

rgan

ized

in

to in

tere

st a

reas

.

Leve

l 2

At

leas

t o

ne

of

the

b

ulle

ts in

le

vel 4

is

in p

lace

.

Leve

l 3

At

leas

t tw

o o

f th

e b

ulle

ts in

le

vel 4

are

in

pla

ce.

Leve

l 4

The

mat

eria

ls in

th

e

inte

rest

are

as a

re

•In

tent

iona

llyo

rgan

ized

.

•G

rou

ped

by

fun

ctio

n.

•A

cces

sib

le t

och

ildre

nth

rou

gho

ut t

he

day

.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: To

sco

re a

4, t

he

mat

eria

ls m

ust

b

e ac

cess

ible

to

all

child

ren

th

rou

gho

ut t

he d

ay.

Pu

ttin

g th

e sa

me

item

s in

mu

ltip

le

inte

rest

are

as,

wh

ere

they

may

co

ntr

ibut

e to

ch

ildre

n’s

pla

y, is

ac

cep

tab

le (

e.g.

, wri

tin

g u

ten

sils

in

the

ho

me

are

a as

wel

l as

the

art

area

).

Exam

ple

s o

f gr

ou

pin

g b

y fu

nct

ion

o

r ty

pes

: o

Thin

gs t

hat

fast

en (

e.g.

,ta

pe,

sta

ple

r, p

aper

clip

s)o

Thin

gs t

o b

uild

wit

h (

e.g.

,u

nit

blo

cks,

Bri

stle

Blo

cks,

card

bo

ard

blo

cks)

Page 14: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

13

Item

I‐B

Cla

ssro

om

mat

eria

ls a

re p

len

tifu

l. (U

se C

lass

room

Mat

eria

ls C

hec

klis

t fo

r sc

ori

ng.

)

SCO

RE

Leve

l 1

Leve

l 2

Leve

l 3

Leve

l 4

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 8

or

less

on

th

e lit

erac

y co

nte

nt

area

.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 9

or

10 o

n

the

liter

acy

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 11

on

th

e lit

erac

y co

nten

t ar

ea.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 1

2 o

n t

he

liter

acy

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: O

nly

en

ter

the

TOTA

L o

bse

rve

d s

core

fo

r ev

iden

ce.

Ro

w 2

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 6

or

less

on

th

e m

ath

emat

ics

cont

ent

area

.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 7

or

8 o

n

the

mat

hem

atic

s co

nte

nt

area

.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 9 o

n t

he

mat

hem

atic

s co

nte

nt

area

.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 1

0 o

n t

he

mat

hem

atic

s co

nte

nt

area

.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: On

ly

ente

r th

e TO

TAL

ob

serv

ed

sco

re f

or

evid

ence

.

Ro

w 3

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 7

or

less

on

th

e p

erce

ptu

al, m

oto

r, a

nd

p

hys

ical

dev

elo

pm

ent

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 8

or

9 o

n t

he

per

cep

tual

, mo

tor,

an

d p

hys

ical

d

evel

op

men

t co

nten

t ar

ea.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 10

on

th

e p

erce

ptu

al, m

oto

r,

and

ph

ysic

al

dev

elo

pm

ent

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 1

1 o

n t

he

per

cep

tual

, m

oto

r, a

nd

ph

ysic

al

dev

elo

pm

ent c

ont

ent

area

.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: O

nly

en

ter

the

TOTA

L o

bse

rve

d s

core

fo

r ev

iden

ce.

Ro

w 4

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 2

or

less

on

th

e so

cial

stu

die

s/so

cial

an

d e

mo

tio

nal

co

nte

nt

area

.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 3

or

4 o

n

the

soci

al

stu

die

s/so

cial

an

d

emo

tio

nal

co

nte

nt

area

.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 5

on

th

e so

cial

st

ud

ies/

soci

al a

nd

em

oti

on

al c

on

ten

t ar

ea.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 6

on

th

e so

cial

st

ud

ies/

soci

al a

nd

em

oti

on

al c

on

ten

t ar

ea.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: O

nly

en

ter

the

TOTA

L o

bse

rve

d s

core

fo

r ev

iden

ce.

Ro

w 5

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 1

or

no

ne

on

th

e sc

ien

ce c

on

ten

t ar

ea.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 2

on

th

e

scie

nce

co

nte

nt

area

.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 3

on

th

e sc

ien

ce c

on

ten

t ar

ea.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of 4

on

th

e sc

ien

ce

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: O

nly

en

ter

the

TOTA

L o

bse

rve

d s

core

fo

r ev

iden

ce.

Ro

w 6

Li

mit

ed Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of

2 o

r le

ss o

n t

he

div

ersi

ty o

f h

um

an

exp

erie

nce

s co

nte

nt

area

.

Mo

der

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 3

on

th

e d

iver

sity

of

hu

man

ex

per

ien

ces

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Ad

equ

ate

Qu

anti

ty:

Sco

re o

f 4

on

th

e d

iver

sity

of

hu

man

ex

per

ien

ces

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Ple

nti

ful Q

uan

tity

: Sco

re

of

5 o

n t

he

div

ersi

ty o

f h

um

an e

xper

ien

ces

con

ten

t ar

ea.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: O

nly

en

ter

the

TOTA

L o

bse

rve

d s

core

fo

r ev

iden

ce.

Page 15: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

14

Item

I‐C

Th

ere

is a

saf

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

area

wit

h a

mp

le s

pac

e, s

tru

ctu

res,

an

d m

ater

ials

to

su

pp

ort

man

y ty

pes

of m

ove

men

t.

SCO

RE

Leve

l 1

Leve

l 2

Leve

l 3

Leve

l 4

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

Th

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

area

is

un

safe

or

ther

e is

no

o

utd

oo

r p

lay

area

.

The

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea is

saf

e, a

nd

th

ere

is s

pac

e an

d

pla

y st

ruct

ure

s th

at a

llow

fo

r a

few

typ

es o

f m

ove

men

t.

The

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea

is s

afe

, an

d t

her

e is

sp

ace

and

pla

y st

ruct

ure

s th

at a

llow

fo

r so

me

typ

es o

f m

ove

men

t.

The

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea is

saf

e,

and

th

ere

is s

pac

e an

d p

lay

stru

ctu

res

that

allo

w f

or

man

y

typ

es o

f m

ove

men

t.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: If

ch

ildre

n d

o n

ot

go o

uts

ide

du

rin

g th

e o

bse

rvat

ion

, sco

re

the

ou

tsid

e ar

ea a

nd

no

te in

th

e ev

iden

ce c

olu

mn

wh

y th

e o

uts

ide

area

was

no

t u

sed

th

at d

ay (

e.g.

, lac

k o

f ti

me,

d

ange

rou

s w

eath

er [

i.e.,

lig

htn

ing,

hea

vy r

ain

, hai

l, st

ron

g w

ind

s, b

lizza

rds,

ver

y h

igh

or

low

tem

per

atu

res]

).

Exam

ple

s o

f ty

pes

of

mo

vem

ent:

o

Jum

pin

go

Clim

bin

go

Swin

gin

go

Slid

ing

oR

un

nin

go

Dig

gin

g

Ro

w 2

Th

ere

are

no

p

ort

able

m

ater

ials

fo

r th

e ou

tdoo

r p

lay

area

.

The

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea in

clu

des

p

ort

able

mat

eria

ls

for

a fe

w t

ypes

of

acti

ve p

lay.

The

ou

tdo

or

play

are

a in

clu

des

po

rtab

le

mat

eria

ls fo

r so

me

typ

es

of a

ctiv

e p

lay.

The

ou

tdo

or

area

incl

ud

es

po

rtab

le m

ater

ials

fo

r m

any

typ

es o

f act

ive

pla

y.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: If

nee

ded

, ask

wh

ere

po

rtab

le

mat

eria

ls a

re s

tore

d.

Exam

ple

s o

f po

rtab

le

mat

eria

ls:

oTr

icyc

les

oSl

eds

oB

alls

oSt

on

eso

Bo

xes

oB

uck

ets

oC

hal

ko

Scar

ves

oP

ain

tbru

shes

Page 16: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

15

Item

I‐D

Ch

ildre

n’s

wo

rk a

nd

en

viro

nm

enta

l pri

nt

are

on

dis

pla

y.

SCO

RE

Lev

el 1

L

evel

2

Lev

el 3

L

evel

4

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

O

ne

or

no

ne

of

the

bu

llete

d p

oin

ts in

le

vel 4

is/

are

in

pla

ce.

At

leas

t tw

o o

f th

e b

ulle

ted

p

oin

ts in

leve

l 4

are

in p

lace

.

At

leas

t th

ree

of

the

bul

lete

d

po

ints

in le

vel

4 ar

e in

pla

ce.

Ad

ult

s d

isp

lay

child

ren

’s w

ork

•A

t ch

ild le

vel.

•Th

rou

gho

ut

the

lear

nin

gen

viro

nm

ent.

The

dis

pla

ys c

on

sist

of

•M

ost

ly c

hild

‐in

itia

ted

wo

rk (

no

tad

ult

-in

itia

ted

art

pro

ject

s).

•A

var

iety

of c

hild

ren

’s w

ork

,su

ch a

s ar

two

rk, p

ho

tos

of b

lock

stru

ctu

res,

sam

ple

s o

f em

erge

nt

wri

tin

g, a

nd

res

ult

s o

f sci

enti

fic

exp

erim

ents

.

Sco

rin

g n

ote

: A

du

lts

may

dis

pla

y ch

ildre

n’s

wo

rk in

or

ou

tsid

e t

he

clas

sro

om

(s

uch

as

hal

lway

s), b

ut

they

sh

ou

ld d

isp

lay

the

wo

rk

wh

ere

it is

vis

ible

to

ch

ildre

n a

nd

wh

ere

ch

ildre

n a

nd

th

eir

fam

ilies

sp

end

a s

ub

stan

tial

am

ou

nt

of

tim

e w

hile

in s

cho

ol.

Ro

w 2

M

any

exam

ple

s o

f en

viro

nm

enta

l Le

vels

1‐

No

ne

Sc

ori

ng

no

te:

To s

core

a 4

, th

ere

mu

st b

e sp

ecif

ic e

vid

ence

of p

rin

t p

rin

t th

at

2‐Fe

w

that

en

cou

rage

s ch

ildre

n t

o w

rite

lett

ers,

nu

mb

ers,

en

cou

rage

3‐

Som

e

nam

es, a

nd

wo

rds.

ch

ildre

n t

o w

rite

4‐

Man

y le

tter

s, n

um

ber

s,

Exam

ple

s o

f en

viro

nm

enta

l pri

nt

that

en

cou

rage

n

ames

, an

d

child

ren

to

wri

te le

tter

s, n

um

ber

s, n

ames

, an

d

wo

rds

are

w

ord

s:

inte

nti

on

ally

o

Sign

‐in

sh

eets

th

at in

clu

de

ch

ildre

n’s

age

pla

ced

o

Inte

rest

are

a si

gns

thro

ugh

ou

t th

e o

Shel

f/to

y la

bel

s w

ith

nu

mb

ers

of o

bje

cts

clas

sro

om

. o

Alp

hab

et a

nd

nu

mer

ical

str

ips

oA

tten

dan

ce c

har

ts w

ith

dat

eo

Job

ch

arts

wit

h n

ames

oC

hild

ren

’s n

ames

on

cu

bb

ies

oH

and

‐was

hin

g re

min

der

s w

ith

ste

p 1

, ste

p 2

oLa

bel

ed o

bjec

tso

Clo

ck w

ith

nu

me

rals

oC

alen

dar

s w

ith

all

dat

es a

nd

day

s o

f th

e w

eek

Page 17: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

16

II.T

EAC

HIN

G A

ND

LEA

RN

ING

RO

UTI

NES

an

d A

DU

LT‐C

HIL

D IN

TER

AC

TIO

NS

Item

II‐A

Th

e c

lass

roo

m fo

llow

s a

con

sist

ent

seq

uen

ce o

f ev

ents

du

rin

g th

e sc

ho

ol d

ay.

SCO

RE

Leve

ls

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

A

du

lts

sup

po

rt c

hild

ren

’s u

nd

erst

and

ing

of t

he

con

sist

ent

rout

ine

and

seq

uen

ce

of e

ven

ts d

uri

ng t

he

sch

oo

l day

.

1‐R

arel

y o

r n

eve

r 2‐

Som

etim

es

3‐U

sual

ly

4‐A

lway

s

Exam

ple

s o

f su

pp

ort

ing

child

ren

’s

un

der

stan

din

g o

f th

e s

equ

ence

of e

ven

ts:

oA

du

lts

po

st t

he

dai

ly r

ou

tin

e a

t ch

ildle

vel i

n a

fo

rmat

th

at is

eas

y fo

r al

lch

ildre

n t

o u

nd

erst

and

, su

ch a

s te

xtal

on

g w

ith

pic

ture

s o

r sy

mb

ols

.o

Ad

ult

s re

view

th

e d

aily

ro

utin

eth

roug

hout

th

e d

ay.

oA

du

lts

let

child

ren

kn

ow

ab

ou

tch

ange

s in

th

e ro

uti

ne

ahea

d o

f ti

me.

Ro

w 2

A

du

lts

are

th

ou

ghtf

ul a

bo

ut le

ttin

g ch

ildre

n k

no

w w

hen

tra

nsi

tio

ns

to a

d

iffe

ren

t ar

ea (

wit

hin

an

d o

uts

ide

of t

he

clas

sro

om

), g

rou

p, o

r ac

tivi

ty w

ill o

ccu

r.

1‐R

arel

y o

r n

eve

r 2‐

Som

etim

es

3‐U

sual

ly

4‐A

lway

s

Exam

ple

s th

at a

re t

ho

ugh

tfu

l: o

An

no

un

cin

g th

at s

nac

k w

ill c

om

e a

fter

larg

e‐gr

ou

p t

ime,

bef

ore

sm

all‐

gro

up

tim

e b

egin

s.o

Qu

ietl

y te

llin

g ch

ildre

n w

ho

hav

e a

lot

top

ut

away

th

at p

lay

tim

e is

en

din

g so

on

.

Exam

ple

s th

at a

re n

ot

tho

ugh

tfu

l:

oLo

ud

ly a

nn

ou

nci

ng

to e

very

on

e, "

Five

mo

re m

inu

tes

un

til c

lean

up

tim

e.”

oSa

yin

g “O

ne,

tw

o, t

hre

e e

yes

on

me,

”an

d m

akin

g ch

ildre

n s

top

pla

yin

g o

rin

terr

up

tin

g ch

ildre

n a

s th

ey e

at a

nd

con

vers

e.

Page 18: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

17

Item

II‐B

Th

ere

is t

ime

eac

h d

ay fo

r ch

ild‐i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s in

th

e c

lass

roo

m a

nd

du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me.

SCO

RE

Lev

els

No

tes:

Ev

iden

ce:

Ro

w 1

A

du

lts

allo

w

child

ren

to

car

ry

ou

t th

eir

inte

nti

on

s u

sin

g al

l acc

essi

ble

m

ater

ials

du

rin

g cl

assr

oo

m c

hild

‐ in

itia

ted

act

ivit

y fo

r an

ext

end

ed

per

iod

of

tim

e.

1‐R

arel

y o

r n

eve

r 2‐

Som

etim

es

3‐U

sual

ly

4‐A

lway

s

Sco

rin

g n

ote

s:

To s

core

a 4

, th

ere

mu

st b

e sp

ecif

ic e

vide

nce

th

at

sho

ws

that

ALL

ch

ildre

n a

re a

ble

to

car

ry o

ut

thei

r in

ten

tio

ns.

Th

ere

can

no

t b

e an

y ev

iden

ce t

hat

co

ntr

adic

ts t

he

exam

ple

s p

rovi

ded

bel

ow

.

Re

sear

ch s

up

po

rts

that

ch

ild-i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s sh

ou

ld b

e fo

r at

leas

t 30

min

ute

s; h

ow

ever

, lo

nge

r p

erio

ds

of

tim

e m

ay b

e b

enef

icia

l as

evid

ence

d in

th

e b

est

pra

ctic

es o

f m

any

hig

h-

qu

alit

y p

rogr

ams

that

allo

w 6

0 m

inu

tes

for

child

-in

itia

ted

wo

rk/p

lay

tim

e.

Exam

ple

s o

f ad

ult

s al

low

ing

child

ren

to

car

ry o

ut

the

ir in

ten

tio

ns:

o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

ch

oo

se a

reas

, peo

ple

,an

d m

ater

ials

.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re fr

ee t

o p

rete

nd

, pla

y al

on

e, o

rp

lay

in g

rou

ps.

oC

hild

ren

are

fre

e t

o in

ven

t ac

tivi

ties

.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

mo

ve m

ater

ials

thro

ugh

ou

t th

e ar

ea.

oC

hild

ren

are

fre

e t

o u

se m

ater

ials

cre

ativ

ely.

oC

hild

ren

are

fre

e t

o c

han

ge a

ctiv

itie

s.

Page 19: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2

019

Hig

hSc

op

e Ed

uca

tio

nal

Res

earc

h F

ou

nd

atio

n

18

Item

II‐B

(Co

ntin

ued)

Th

ere

is t

ime

each

day

fo

r ch

ild‐i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s in

th

e c

lass

roo

m a

nd

du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me.

SC

OR

E L

evel

s N

ote

s:

Evid

ence

:

Ro

w 2

A

du

lts

allo

w

child

ren

to

car

ry

ou

t th

eir

inte

nti

on

s u

sin

g al

l acc

essi

ble

eq

uip

men

t an

d

mat

eria

ls d

uri

ng

the

out

do

or

child

‐in

itia

ted

ac

tivi

ty f

or

an

exte

nd

ed

per

iod

of

tim

e.

1‐R

arel

y o

r n

eve

r 2‐

Som

etim

es

3‐U

sual

ly

4‐A

lway

s

Sco

rin

g n

ote

s:

To s

core

a 4

, th

ere

mu

st b

e sp

ecif

ic e

vide

nce

th

at

sho

ws

that

ALL

ch

ildre

n a

re a

ble

to

car

ry o

ut

thei

r in

ten

tio

ns.

Th

ere

can

no

t b

e an

y ev

iden

ce t

hat

co

ntr

adic

ts t

he

exam

ple

s p

rovi

ded

bel

ow

.

Sco

re, e

ven

if c

hild

ren

do

no

t go

ou

tsid

e d

uri

ng

the

sch

edu

led

tim

e, b

ased

on

ab

ility

of

child

ren

to

car

ry o

ut

thei

r in

ten

tio

ns

(e.g

., if

ad

ult

s re

ad

alo

ud

du

rin

g th

is t

ime,

th

en a

ssig

n a

sco

re o

f 1

).

The

reco

mm

end

ed a

mo

un

t o

f ti

me

for

ou

tdo

or

pla

y is

at

leas

t 30

min

ute

s; h

ow

ever

, res

earc

h

sup

po

rts

up

to

60

min

ute

s.

Exam

ple

s o

f ad

ult

s al

low

ing

child

ren

to

car

ry o

ut

the

ir in

ten

tio

ns:

o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

ch

oo

se p

eop

le,

equi

pmen

t, an

d m

ater

ials

to

pla

y w

ith

.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re fr

ee t

o p

rete

nd

, pla

y al

on

e, o

rp

lay

in g

rou

ps.

oC

hild

ren

are

fre

e t

o in

ven

t ac

tivi

ties

.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

mo

ve m

ater

ials

thro

ugh

ou

t th

e ou

tdoo

r are

a.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

use

mat

eria

ls c

reat

ivel

y.o

Ch

ildre

n a

re f

ree

to

ch

ange

act

ivit

ies.

Page 20: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 19

Preschool Program Quality Assessment — Revised (PQA-R)

Classroom Materials Checklist (CMC)

The Classroom Materials Checklist is a convenient way for assessors to collect information

about the classroom materials accessible to children. The Classroom Materials Checklist

provides a systematic method to collect information about the types of materials and

equipment in a given classroom. Assessors can record the presence or absence of materials as

they move around a classroom, by simply checking Yes or No for each item, thus easing the

burden of creating individual lists of materials. The Classroom Materials Checklist is divided into

6 content areas: Literacy; Mathematics; Perceptual, Motor, and Physical Development; Social

Studies/Social and Emotional Development; and Science and Diversity of Human Experiences.

The materials assessed reflect different types of play and learning materials that children and

teachers may use in a preschool classroom for a variety of learning experiences.

Instructions for completing the Classroom Materials Checklist:

1. Before observing, become familiar with each item descriptor and the examples ofmaterials.

2. You may take photos of classroom materials to help complete this checklist. Do notinclude children or adults in any photos.

3. Walk around the classroom and look at the materials in each area that are easilyaccessible to children. (Do not include those in storage, too high to reach, and notvisible.) If you see materials that fit within an item, check Yes; if not, check No. Theexamples provided are not exhaustive lists; include other materials, as appropriate, for agiven item.

4. Some classroom materials may count for two items (e.g., a basket of 40 small rockscould count toward Item13. Counting, sorting and classifying, and Item 38. Items fromnature).

5. Each item must be scored on the Classroom Materials Checklist.6. After completing the Classroom Materials Checklist, follow the directions on the final

scoring page to record the number of items checked Yes for each content area in theTotal Observed column. NOTE: The Social Studies/Social and Emotional, and Sciencecontent areas require you to include items from other content areas. The Diversity ofHuman Experiences content area is comprised of items from other content areas.

7. Transfer the Total Observed numbers (page 8) to the corresponding content area rowsin Item I-B on the PQA-R. The Classroom Materials Checklist is the evidence for rows 1–6of Item I-B. NOTE: You do not have to list materials on the PQA-R. Just record the totalobserved number of yes responses for each content area in the evidence column.

Page 21: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 20

Literacy

1. The classroom has at least 3 books per child representing a variety of genres

(e.g., informational books, poetry books, songbooks, storybooks).

Note: Count the number of books found anywhere in the classroom. Books can

be in different formats (e.g., big books, student- and class-created books, soft

cover, hardcover, and board books).

Yes

No

2. Of the books in the classroom, there are 5 or more nonfiction books related

to science (e.g., nature, plants, insects, animals, electricity, space, and

weather).

Yes

No

*Score also for

Science

3. Of the books in the classroom, there are 4 or more nonfiction books

related to communities/social sciences (e.g., geography, community

events, holidays, differing family structures, and professions/occupations).

Yes

No

*Score also for

Social

Studies/Social

and Emotional

4. Of the books in the classroom, there are 4 or more nonfiction books related

to diversity of human experiences (e.g., gender with nonstereotyped role

models, different times and places, cooking recipes from different

countries, art from different countries, world cultures, pictures of people

from around the world, and special needs such as depicting people with

disabilities).

Yes

No

*Score also for

Diversity of

Human

Experiences

5. Of the books in the classroom, there are 5 or more books with rhyming

and/or rhythmic features, such as song books, nursery rhymes, and poetry

books.

Yes

No

Page 22: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

21

6. Of the books in the classroom, there are at least 2 alphabet books. Yes

No

7. Of the books in the classroom, there are 5 or more books with math

content (e.g., numbers, counting, shapes, patterns, comparing attributes,

and measurement).

Yes

No

8. Of the books in the classroom, there are 5 or more books with social-

emotional content (e.g., conflict resolution, sharing, friendship, accepting

differences, family relationships, becoming a big sister/brother, and

prosocial behavior).

Yes

No

*Score also for

Social

Studies/Social

and Emotional

9. The classroom has at least 2 complete sets of uppercase and 2 complete

sets of lowercase 3-D letters of the alphabet.

Yes

No

10. Somewhere within the classroom, all children’s names are visible at the

child level (e.g., name cards, sign-in sheets for children, names on

clothespins or Popsicle sticks, and labels on cubbies).

Yes

No

11. The classroom has enough crayons, pencils, pens, and markers in sufficient

quantity for small groups of children to use at the same time.

Yes

No

Page 23: PQA-R - HighScope

22

Pre

sch

oo

l Pro

gra

m Q

ual

ity

Ass

essm

ent

(Rev

ised

) —

PQ

A-R

Sco

rin

g G

uid

elin

es

Dev

elo

ped

by

the

Cen

ter

for

Earl

y Ed

uca

tio

n R

esea

rch

an

d E

valu

atio

n (

CEE

RE)

at

Hig

hSc

op

e

The

Pre

sch

oo

l PQ

A-R

is a

n a

ll-in

clu

sive

ob

serv

atio

n in

stru

men

t th

at m

easu

res

the

qu

alit

y o

f p

resc

ho

ol c

lass

roo

ms.

It

com

pre

hen

sive

ly m

easu

res

the

effe

ctiv

enes

s o

f th

e le

arn

ing

envi

ron

men

t; t

each

ing

and

lear

nin

g ro

uti

nes

an

d a

du

lt-c

hild

inte

ract

ion

s; a

nd

cu

rric

ulu

m, p

lan

nin

g, a

sses

smen

t an

d p

aren

t en

gage

men

t w

hile

pro

mo

tin

g t

he

dev

elo

pm

ent

of

the

wh

ole

child

wit

hin

a s

up

po

rtiv

e cl

assr

oo

m le

arn

ing

envi

ron

men

t an

d t

hro

ugh

tea

cher

s’ c

on

nec

tio

ns

to c

hild

ren

’s h

om

e

envi

ron

men

ts.

The

follo

win

g sc

ori

ng

guid

elin

es

pre

sen

t ex

amp

les

of

anec

dota

l no

tes

that

mig

ht

be

take

n d

uri

ng

a cl

ass

roo

m o

bse

rvat

ion

an

d

teac

her

inte

rvie

w. A

nec

do

tal n

ote

s ta

ken

du

rin

g an

ob

serv

atio

n a

re u

sed

to

sco

re S

ecti

on

I an

d II

of

the

PQ

A-R

. An

ecd

ota

l

no

tes

take

n d

uri

ng

a te

ach

er in

terv

iew

are

use

d t

o s

core

Sec

tio

n II

I. T

he

Sco

rin

g G

uid

elin

es in

clu

de

exam

ple

s fo

r ea

ch it

em o

f

the

PQ

A-R

; eac

h r

ow

has

th

ree

exa

mp

les

at e

ach

sco

rin

g le

vel (

I, 2,

3, o

r 4)

. Th

ese

are

just

exa

mp

les

of

anec

do

tal n

ote

s an

d

are

no

t ex

hau

stiv

e. T

o u

se t

he

Sco

rin

g G

uid

elin

es, c

om

par

e th

e an

ecd

ota

l no

tes

you

hav

e ta

ken

to

th

e ev

iden

ce p

rese

nte

d f

or

a p

arti

cula

r ro

w. T

o d

eter

min

e a

sco

re, r

evie

w t

he

exam

ple

s p

rovi

ded

an

d p

ick

the

sco

rin

g le

vel t

hat

mo

st c

lose

ly m

atch

es

you

r ev

iden

ce. M

ove

to

th

e n

ext

row

an

d r

epea

t th

e p

roce

ss t

o s

core

th

e ev

iden

ce y

ou

co

llect

ed.

All

row

s o

f th

e P

QA

-R s

ho

uld

be

sco

red

.

Ther

e ar

e se

vera

l ab

bre

viat

ion

s u

sed

th

rou

gho

ut

the

Sco

rin

g G

uid

elin

es: T

= T

each

er; T

A =

Tea

chin

g A

ssis

tan

t; L

T =

Lead

Teac

her

; A =

Ad

ult

; Ch

= C

hild

. Fo

r ex

amp

le, “

Ch

1, C

h2

, Ch

3 ta

lkin

g” in

yo

ur

anec

do

te w

ou

ld b

e sh

ort

han

d f

or

“Ch

ild 1

, Ch

ild 2

and

Ch

ild 3

in a

co

nve

rsat

ion

.” T

hes

e ty

pes

of

abb

revi

atio

ns

are

hel

pfu

l sh

ort

cuts

to

use

as

you

are

co

llect

ing

anec

do

tal n

ote

s.

Som

etim

es a

no

te ju

st in

clu

des

a c

hild

’s in

itia

l or

just

th

e ch

ild’s

fir

st n

ame

, fo

r b

revi

ty a

nd

co

nfi

den

tial

ity.

Page 24: PQA-R - HighScope

23

Item

I‐A

Th

e in

do

or

spac

e h

as a

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

hav

e n

ames

an

d a

re in

ten

tio

nal

ly o

rgan

ized

. R

ow

1: A

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

pro

vid

e d

iver

se a

ctiv

itie

s an

d c

an b

e ta

ilore

d t

o in

div

idu

al c

hild

ren

’s in

tere

sts

are

evid

ent

and

nam

ed.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

The

clas

sro

om

has

2 a

reas

, a

toy

area

an

d a

kit

chen

are

a.

The

kitc

hen

are

a w

as t

urn

ed

tow

ard

th

e w

all,

blo

cked

off

fr

om

use

fo

r th

e d

ay.

Ther

e ar

e n

o a

pp

aren

t ar

eas.

B

ins

are

sto

red

alo

ng

the

wal

l h

aph

azar

dly

, wit

h n

o

org

aniz

atio

n.

The

roo

m h

ad t

wo

are

as:

on

e ar

ea

con

tain

ed b

ins

mix

ed w

ith

blo

cks,

h

ou

se it

ems,

an

d m

usi

cal

inst

rum

ents

. Th

e o

ther

are

a h

ad a

ta

ble

wit

h b

oo

ks, c

rayo

ns,

an

d

pap

er in

a b

aske

t. T

he

area

s w

ere

no

t la

bel

ed.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

The

clas

sro

om

had

a H

ou

se

Are

a an

d B

lock

Are

a; b

oth

ar

eas

wer

e la

bel

ed. T

her

e w

as a

bas

ket

of

bo

oks

on

th

e fl

oo

r in

th

e co

rner

.

Ther

e ar

e 2

inte

rest

are

as,

ho

use

an

d b

oo

ks. N

eith

er a

rea

is la

bel

ed. T

he

ho

use

are

a an

d

bo

ok

area

s ar

e ac

ross

fro

m

each

oth

er a

lon

g th

e w

alls

.

The

clas

sro

om

has

3 a

reas

: a

ho

use

ar

ea, a

blo

ck a

rea,

an

d a

rt a

rea.

Th

e ar

t ar

ea h

as n

o m

ater

ials

; th

e h

ou

se a

rea

was

sp

read

ou

t, w

ith

th

e ki

tch

en o

n o

ne

sid

e o

f th

e ro

om

an

d d

ress

up

all

the

way

acr

oss

th

e ro

om

.

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Are

as o

f th

e ro

om

are

m

arke

d a

nd

div

ided

in t

he

spac

e. A

du

lts

use

th

e ar

ea

nam

es. L

T: “

Let’

s m

eet

at t

he

San

d T

able

.”, “

I saw

yo

u w

ere

exp

erim

enti

ng

in D

isco

very

.”

Dis

cove

ry C

en

ter

Art

s &

Cra

fts

San

d a

nd

Wat

er t

able

s B

uild

ing

Spac

e

Ho

use

kee

pin

g an

d P

rete

nd

P

lay

Ther

e ar

e in

tere

st a

reas

set

up

fo

r ch

ildre

n. A

reas

incl

ud

e a

ho

use

are

a, b

oo

k ar

ea, b

lock

ar

ea, a

nd

art

are

a. T

he

ho

use

, b

oo

k, a

nd

blo

ck a

reas

had

la

bel

s. D

uri

ng

pla

nn

ing,

th

e te

ach

er a

sked

a c

hild

, “W

her

e ar

e yo

u g

oin

g to

pla

y?”

The

child

po

inte

d a

cro

ss t

he

roo

m.

T re

plie

d: “

The

ho

use

are

a?”

The

follo

win

g in

tere

st a

reas

are

n

amed

an

d c

lear

ly d

efin

ed in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m:

Art

Are

a Li

bra

ry A

rea

Blo

ck A

rea

Co

mp

ute

r/Li

sten

ing

Are

a D

uri

ng

pla

nn

ing,

C1

sai

d: "

I’m

go

ing

to b

lock

are

a.”

Page 25: PQA-R - HighScope

24

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Are

as o

f th

e ro

om

are

cle

arly

m

arke

d w

ith

lab

els

and

d

ivid

ed in

th

e sp

ace.

Ad

ult

s an

d c

hild

ren

use

th

e n

ames

. T:

“Le

t’s

mee

t at

th

e Sa

nd

Ta

ble

.”, “

I saw

yo

u w

ere

exp

erim

enti

ng

in D

isco

very

.”

Ch

: “I w

ant

to b

uild

wit

h t

he

Lego

s” (

po

inti

ng

to t

he

Bu

ildin

g Sp

ace)

. C2

to

T: “

Will

yo

u p

lay

rest

aura

nt

wit

h m

e in

Ho

use

kee

pin

g?”

Dis

cove

ry C

en

ter

Art

s &

Cra

fts

Au

tho

r’s

Spac

e Sa

nd

an

d W

ater

tab

les

Bu

ildin

g Sp

ace

H

ou

seke

ep

ing

and

Pre

ten

d

Pla

y

Ther

e ar

e a

vari

ety

of

inte

rest

ar

eas

set

up

fo

r ch

ildre

n. A

reas

in

clu

de

a h

ou

se a

rea,

tex

ture

ta

ble

, bo

ok

area

, blo

ck a

rea,

to

ys a

nd

gam

es, a

rt, a

nd

d

isco

very

. Th

rou

gho

ut

the

mo

rnin

g, t

each

ers

and

ch

ildre

n r

efer

to

th

e ar

eas:

C

h: “

J an

d I

will

pla

y a

gam

e in

th

e To

ys.”

T:

“A

rt n

eed

s m

ore

pai

nt.

” C

h: “

Can

I ad

d w

ater

to

th

e Te

xtu

re T

able

?”

T: “

Ou

r H

ou

se A

rea

is a

mes

s!”

All

area

s w

ere

mar

ked

wit

h a

p

ictu

re a

nd

wo

rd la

bel

.

The

follo

win

g in

tere

st a

reas

are

n

amed

an

d c

lear

ly d

efin

ed in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m:

Art

Are

a Sa

fe S

po

t Sa

nd

Tab

le

Wri

tin

g C

en

ter

Scie

nce

Are

a Li

bra

ry A

rea

Pla

y d

ou

gh A

rea

Kit

chen

Are

a M

usi

c A

rea

Co

mp

ute

r/Li

sten

ing

Are

a D

uri

ng

pla

nn

ing,

C1

said

: "I’

m g

oin

g to

blo

ck a

rea.

” C

2 sa

id: “

I’m

go

ing

to p

lay

do

ugh

are

a.”

C3

sai

d, "

I’m

go

ing

to s

afe

spo

t.”

T: “

Wh

at a

re

you

go

ing

to c

oo

k in

th

e ki

tch

en

area

?”

Page 26: PQA-R - HighScope

25

Item

I‐A

Th

e in

do

or

spac

e h

as a

var

iety

of

inte

rest

are

as t

hat

hav

e n

ames

an

d a

re in

ten

tio

nal

ly o

rgan

ized

.

Ro

w 2

: Mat

eria

ls in

inte

rest

are

as a

re o

rgan

ized

, gro

up

ed b

y fu

nct

ion

, an

d a

cces

sib

le t

hro

ugh

ou

t th

e d

ay.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Bin

s o

f m

ater

ials

wer

e st

ore

d

alo

ng

the

wal

l wit

h n

o

org

aniz

atio

n. O

ne

tub

co

nta

ined

dre

ssu

p c

loth

es,

coo

kin

g u

ten

sils

, blo

cks

and

m

usi

cal i

nst

rum

ents

. In

on

e co

rner

, 3 b

ins

wer

e st

ore

d o

n

top

of

on

e an

oth

er.

Som

e o

f th

e m

ater

ials

are

in

con

tain

ers

on

th

e sh

elve

s, b

ut

teac

her

s ke

ep

man

y m

ater

ials

u

p in

hig

h c

up

bo

ard

s. C

hild

ren

h

ave

to a

sk t

o h

ave

them

b

rou

ght

do

wn

. So

me

thin

gs t

o

bu

ild w

ith

are

sto

red

to

geth

er

on

sh

elve

s.

Mat

eria

ls a

re n

ot

gro

up

ed b

y fu

nct

ion

. Th

ere

are

cray

on

s an

d

mar

kers

mix

ed u

p in

a b

in w

ith

d

ress

-up

clo

thes

. Tea

cher

s u

se

cen

ters

at

free

pla

y an

d c

hild

ren

cy

cle

th

rou

gh e

ach

on

e.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

In t

he

art

area

, th

e ta

pe,

glu

e an

d s

tap

lers

wer

e o

n t

he

sam

e sh

elf.

In t

he

toy

area

, p

uzz

les

are

sto

red

nex

t to

b

ear

cou

nte

rs, a

lon

g w

ith

so

me

art

colla

gin

g m

ater

ials

. Th

e b

oo

ks a

re a

ll as

kew

in

the

bo

ok

area

, so

me

up

sid

e d

ow

n, o

ther

s to

rn a

nd

hal

f o

pen

, wit

h n

o in

ten

tio

nal

o

rgan

izat

ion

. C

hild

ren

hav

e ac

cess

to

m

ater

ials

all

day

.

Mat

eria

ls a

re s

tore

d o

n

shel

ves

in t

ub

s; t

he

tub

s ar

e n

ot

lab

eled

or

org

aniz

ed b

y fu

nct

ion

; cra

yon

s an

d m

arke

rs

are

mix

ed t

oge

ther

. In

th

e h

ou

se a

rea,

dre

ssu

p c

loth

es

are

mix

ed t

oge

ther

in a

bin

w

ith

pla

y fo

od

.

Ch

ildre

n h

ave

acce

ss t

o

mat

eria

ls a

t al

l par

ts o

f th

e d

ay.

In t

he

kitc

hen

, all

the

dis

hes

are

in

the

cup

bo

ard

s; f

ake

foo

d w

as in

th

e fr

idge

; an

d d

ress

up

item

s h

un

g o

n

ho

oks

. In

th

e B

lock

Are

a al

l th

e b

uild

ing

sets

are

sto

red

to

geth

er.

Teac

her

s u

se c

ente

rs a

t fr

ee p

lay;

ac

tivi

tie

s ar

e se

t u

p in

eac

h c

ente

r an

d c

hild

ren

cyc

le t

hro

ugh

eac

h

on

e.

Page 27: PQA-R - HighScope

26

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Mat

eria

ls w

ere

all a

cces

sib

le

to c

hild

ren

.

Mo

st m

ater

ials

wer

e gr

ou

ped

b

y fu

nct

ion

(b

lock

s se

par

ated

b

y ty

pe

and

all

in o

ne

area

).

Inte

nti

on

ally

org

aniz

ed a

rt

mat

eria

ls s

ort

ed b

y ty

pe,

etc

.

Pre

ten

d p

lay

mat

eria

ls w

ere

on

sh

elve

s: c

loth

ing

gro

up

ed

toge

ther

wit

h t

wo

ho

oks

: a

toy

bo

x h

eld

cel

l ph

on

es,

pu

rse

s, s

tuff

ed a

nim

als,

bab

y d

olls

, do

ll cl

oth

es, a

jum

p

rop

e, d

oct

or’

s ki

t, 3

pu

pp

ets,

tw

o c

on

stru

ctio

n h

ats,

an

d a

to

ol b

elt.

Mat

eria

ls w

ere

sto

red

in

rela

ted

bin

s: c

rayo

ns

wer

e in

o

ne

bin

an

d m

arke

rs in

an

oth

er in

th

e ar

t ar

ea; L

ego

s w

ere

in o

ne

tub

an

d L

inco

ln

Logs

in a

no

ther

tu

b in

th

e b

lock

are

a, e

tc. A

ll m

ater

ials

th

rou

gho

ut

the

spac

e w

ere

lab

eled

wit

h p

ictu

res

and

w

ord

s.

Du

rin

g ch

oic

e ti

me,

ch

ildre

n

wer

e as

sign

ed in

tere

st a

reas

to

pla

y in

; no

t al

l ch

ildre

n h

ad

acce

ss t

o a

ll in

tere

st a

reas

th

rou

gho

ut

this

tim

e.

Mat

eria

ls a

re o

rgan

ized

by

inte

rest

ar

ea a

nd

are

lab

eled

wit

h t

ext

and

p

ictu

res

or

dra

win

gs.

In t

he

ho

use

are

a, d

oll

item

s an

d

dre

ssu

p c

loth

es a

re s

tore

d n

ear

each

oth

er; a

cces

sori

es

such

as

cam

eras

, ph

on

es, a

nd

gla

sses

are

gr

ou

ped

to

geth

er; f

oo

d, d

ish

es a

nd

p

ans

are

gro

up

ed t

oge

ther

. In

th

e b

lock

are

a, u

nit

blo

cks

are

gro

up

ed

toge

ther

; far

m a

nd

ho

rse

s st

ore

d

on

to

p s

hel

f w

ith

th

e fa

rm a

nim

als;

fo

am b

lock

s gr

ou

ped

to

geth

er;

card

bo

ard

blo

cks

gro

up

ed t

oge

ther

. In

th

e ex

plo

rati

on

are

a,

mic

rosc

op

e, m

easu

rin

g w

orm

s, a

nd

sh

ells

sto

red

on

to

p s

hel

f;

mic

rosc

op

e sl

ides

, ru

lers

an

d

mag

net

ic w

and

s st

ore

d o

n t

he

seco

nd

sh

elf;

pu

zzle

s an

d g

ames

ar

e st

ore

d o

n b

ott

om

sh

elf.

In t

he

art

area

, tap

e an

d g

lue

sto

red

ap

art

fro

m o

ne

ano

ther

; co

llage

item

s st

ore

d (

con

fett

i, gl

itte

r an

d p

om

-p

om

s, e

tc.)

are

sto

red

on

on

e sh

elf;

ri

bb

on

, sti

cker

s, a

nd

yar

n s

tore

d o

n

ano

ther

sh

elf.

C

hild

ren

use

d a

ll m

ater

ials

th

rou

gho

ut

the

ob

serv

atio

n.

Page 28: PQA-R - HighScope

27

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Stap

lers

are

gro

up

ed

toge

ther

nex

t to

th

e ta

pe.

All

the

blo

cks

and

bu

ildin

g m

ater

ials

are

in t

he

blo

ck

area

. Car

db

oar

d b

lock

s ar

e o

n

the

sam

e sh

elf

and

gro

up

ed

by

colo

r. T

he

wo

od

en u

nit

b

lock

s ar

e gr

ou

ped

to

geth

er.

The

farm

an

imal

s ar

e n

ext

to

the

bar

n. T

he

foo

ds

(bre

ads,

fr

uit

, veg

etab

les,

mea

ts, e

tc.)

ar

e g

rou

ped

an

d lo

cate

d o

n

the

sam

e sh

elf.

Ch

ildre

n p

icke

d t

he

area

s th

ey w

ish

ed t

o p

lay

wit

h

du

rin

g p

lan

nin

g. T

wo

bo

ys

bu

ilt in

th

e b

lock

are

a; a

gir

l to

ok

cray

on

s o

ff t

he

shel

f in

th

e ar

t ar

ea, a

nd

an

oth

er g

irl

loo

ked

at

bo

oks

in t

he

bo

ok

area

.

Mar

kers

, cra

yon

s, a

nd

pen

cils

ar

e to

geth

er (

in s

ep

arat

e b

ins)

in

th

e ar

t ar

ea.

Var

iou

s ty

pes

of

blo

cks

are

adja

cen

t in

th

e b

lock

are

a.

Wri

tin

g u

ten

sils

an

d a

var

iety

o

f p

aper

s ar

e at

th

e w

riti

ng

area

des

k. P

ots

, pan

s, c

oo

kin

g u

ten

sils

, an

d t

able

war

e ar

e in

th

e cu

pb

oar

ds

in t

he

ho

use

ar

ea.

Ch

ildre

n u

sed

pla

y fo

od

an

d

mad

e th

e te

ach

er b

reak

fast

, co

lore

d p

ictu

res

in t

he

art

area

, bu

ilt w

ith

blo

cks

in t

he

blo

ck a

rea,

an

d d

id p

uzz

les

at

a ta

ble

.

All

mat

eria

ls a

re la

bel

ed (

incl

ud

es

bas

ket

or

con

tain

er a

nd

sh

elf)

.

Mat

eria

ls a

re s

tore

d b

y re

leva

nt

inte

rest

are

a. F

or

exam

ple

: St

amp

s ar

e lo

cate

d b

y th

e st

amp

p

ads.

D

ry-e

rase

mar

kers

are

loca

ted

nex

t to

dry

-era

se b

oar

ds.

M

arke

rs a

nd

pen

cils

are

nex

t to

ea

ch o

ther

in t

he

wri

tin

g ar

ea a

nd

ar

e lo

cate

d n

ext

to p

aper

an

d

enve

lop

es.

All

dre

ss-u

p c

loth

es a

re n

ext

to

each

oth

er.

All

wo

od

en b

lock

s ar

e n

ext

to e

ach

o

ther

.

All

mat

eria

ls a

re a

t ch

ildre

n’s

leve

l; P

ain

tbru

shes

, pai

nt,

sm

ock

s, a

nd

p

aper

are

at

the

ease

l; C

hild

ren

u

sed

th

ese

mat

eria

ls t

hro

ugh

ou

t th

e o

bse

rvat

ion

.

Ch

ildre

n u

sed

fo

am in

th

e ar

t ar

ea.

Ch

ildre

n u

sed

th

e sm

art

bo

ard

w

ith

ou

t ad

ult

hel

p b

y u

sin

g a

sto

ol.

Page 29: PQA-R - HighScope

28

Item

I‐B

Cla

ssro

om

mat

eria

ls a

re p

len

tifu

l. (

Use

Cla

ssro

om

Mat

eria

ls C

hec

klis

t fo

r sc

ori

ng.

) Le

vel 1

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

Le

vel 2

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

Le

vel 3

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

Le

vel 4

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

Ro

w 1

: Ple

nti

ful

liter

acy

mat

eria

ls.

CM

C T

ota

l = 6

C

MC

To

tal =

9

CM

C T

ota

l = 1

1 C

MC

To

tal =

12

Ro

w 2

: Ple

nti

ful

mat

hem

atic

s m

ater

ials

.

CM

C T

ota

l = 4

C

MC

To

tal =

8

CM

C T

ota

l = 9

C

MC

To

tal =

10

Ro

w 3

: Ple

nti

ful

per

cep

tual

, mo

tor,

an

d p

hys

ical

d

evel

op

men

t m

ater

ials

.

CM

C T

ota

l = 7

C

MC

To

tal =

8

CM

C T

ota

l = 1

0 C

MC

To

tal =

11

Ro

w 4

: Ple

nti

ful

soci

al s

tud

ies/

soci

al-

emo

tio

nal

mat

eria

ls.

CM

C T

ota

l = 2

C

MC

To

tal =

3

CM

C T

ota

l = 5

C

MC

To

tal =

6

Ro

w 5

: Ple

nti

ful

scie

nce

mat

eria

ls.

CM

C T

ota

l = 1

C

MC

To

tal =

2

CM

C T

ota

l = 3

C

MC

To

tal =

4

Ro

w 6

: Ple

nti

ful

div

ersi

ty o

f h

um

an

exp

erie

nce

s m

ater

ials

.

CM

C T

ota

l = 2

C

MC

To

tal =

3

CM

C T

ota

l = 4

C

MC

To

tal =

5

Page 30: PQA-R - HighScope

29

Item

I‐C

Th

ere

is a

saf

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

are

a w

ith

am

ple

sp

ace,

str

uct

ure

s, a

nd

mat

eri

als

to s

up

po

rt m

any

typ

es o

f m

ove

men

t.

Ro

w 1

: Th

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

area

is s

afe

and

th

ere

is s

pac

e an

d p

lay

stru

ctu

res

that

allo

w f

or

mo

vem

ent.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ther

e is

no

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea.

The

child

ren

are

tak

en f

or

a w

alk

du

rin

g o

uts

ide

tim

e in

stea

d o

f p

layi

ng

bec

ause

th

ere

isn

’t a

n o

uts

ide

area

.

The

child

ren

to

ok

a w

alk

aro

un

d t

he

bu

ildin

g an

d in

th

e p

arki

ng

lot

of

the

cen

ter.

Th

e w

eath

er w

as n

ot

incl

em

ent,

ho

wev

er, t

her

e w

as

som

e fl

oo

din

g o

n t

he

pla

ygro

un

d.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Per

ob

serv

atio

n, c

hild

ren

ju

mp

an

d c

limb

in a

nd

ou

t o

f th

e w

oo

den

ho

use

. Th

ere

are

no

str

uct

ure

s fo

r sw

ingi

ng

or

slid

ing.

The

ou

tdo

or

area

allo

ws

for

jum

pin

g an

d c

limb

ing.

Th

ere

are

no

op

po

rtu

nit

ies

for

swin

gin

g o

r sl

idin

g.

The

pla

ygro

un

d is

just

ou

tsid

e th

e cl

assr

oo

m. T

her

e ar

e n

o p

lay

stru

ctu

res.

Th

ere

is a

pav

ed a

rea

wit

h a

Lit

tle

Tike

s b

aske

tbal

l se

tup

an

d a

gra

ssy

area

wit

h b

adm

into

n.

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

All-

nat

ura

l ou

tdo

or

pla

y sp

ace

wit

h a

dre

amca

tch

er

clim

ber

, lar

ge s

and

are

a,

ced

ar b

alan

ce b

eam

, mu

d

kitc

hen

, wo

od

en b

rid

ge,

ced

ar s

tage

an

d s

eat

ing,

sl

ide,

an

d c

limb

ing

logs

.

Are

a fo

r ru

nn

ing

and

jum

pin

g,

and

a p

lay

stru

ctu

re c

hild

can

cl

imb

on

an

d s

lide

do

wn

. Th

ere

are

no

sw

ings

or

stru

ctu

res

for

swin

gin

g.

The

ou

tdo

or

pla

y ar

ea d

oes

no

t h

ave

a st

ruct

ure

on

it t

hat

allo

ws

child

ren

to

sw

ing.

Th

ere

is a

ho

use

th

at c

hild

ren

can

go

into

an

d s

mal

l sl

ides

th

at c

hild

ren

can

clim

b u

p

and

slid

e d

ow

n o

n. C

hild

ren

hav

e gr

assy

sp

ace

to r

un

an

d ju

mp

bu

t th

ere

are

no

str

uct

ure

s ch

ildre

n c

an

clim

b a

nd

jum

p f

rom

.

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

On

e cl

imb

ing

stru

ctu

re h

as 3

sl

ides

, a t

un

nel

, an

d a

w

alkw

ay t

o t

he

clim

ber

wit

h

a sl

ide

; Clim

ber

2 h

as t

wo

sl

ides

an

d a

bri

dge

wal

kway

. Th

ere

are

2 b

alan

ce b

eam

s, 6

sw

ings

, ste

ps,

an

d le

vels

fo

r

Larg

e p

lay

stru

ctu

re w

ith

la

dd

ers/

slid

es, c

limb

er, s

tep

s,

bas

ketb

all h

oo

p, b

alan

ce

bea

m, r

ide

on

bu

s, s

win

gs, a

nd

lil

y p

ads.

Th

ere

is a

larg

e gr

assy

are

a w

her

e ch

ildre

n

Ch

ildre

n a

nd

ad

ult

s ac

cess

p

layg

rou

nd

fro

m d

oo

r in

hal

lway

th

at le

ads

dir

ectl

y o

uts

ide

. Th

ere

is

a la

rge

pav

ed a

rea

righ

t o

uts

ide

the

do

or.

Th

ere

is a

lso

a h

ill t

hat

lead

s d

ow

n t

o t

he

pla

ygro

un

d. O

utd

oo

r ar

ea h

as 8

sw

ings

, 3 s

lides

, 1

Page 31: PQA-R - HighScope

30

child

ren

to

jum

p f

rom

. Th

ere

is a

cem

ent/

pav

ed a

rea

wit

h

a 4

-sq

uar

e an

d h

op

sco

tch

.

pla

yed

“ta

g” d

uri

ng

the

ob

serv

atio

n.

clim

bin

g st

ruct

ure

, 4 b

ench

es, 1

ta

ble

, an

d a

so

ccer

net

. Th

ere

are

gras

sy a

reas

an

d a

reas

wit

h c

emen

t an

d w

oo

d c

hip

s.

Page 32: PQA-R - HighScope

31

Item

I‐C

Th

ere

is a

saf

e o

utd

oo

r p

lay

are

a w

ith

am

ple

sp

ace,

str

uct

ure

s, a

nd

mat

eri

als

to s

up

po

rt m

any

typ

es o

f m

ove

men

t.

Ro

w 2

: Ou

tdo

or

area

incl

ud

es p

ort

able

mat

eria

ls f

or

acti

ve p

lay.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

No

po

rtab

le m

ater

ials

wer

e u

sed

wh

ile c

hild

ren

pla

yed

o

uts

ide

.

2 To

nka

tru

cks

in t

he

san

db

ox

bu

t n

o o

ne

cou

ld u

se t

he

san

d

bo

x; it

was

to

o w

et.

Ch

ildre

n a

sked

fo

r si

dew

alk

chal

k.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Teac

her

s b

rou

ght

ou

t b

alls

an

d s

occ

er g

oal

s.

2 se

ts o

f st

ilts,

2 s

coo

ters

, an

d

3 b

alls

2

bal

ls, 3

sto

pw

atch

es, s

and

to

ys,

and

2 H

ula

- h

oo

ps

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ob

serv

ed p

ort

able

mat

eria

ls

incl

ud

e t

rike

s, p

last

ic c

ars,

b

alls

, ch

alk,

larg

e w

oo

den

b

lock

s, s

and

to

ys, t

ruck

s,

pla

stic

kit

chen

fu

rnit

ure

.

Som

e o

f th

e po

rtab

le

equ

ipm

ent

is b

roke

n a

nd

w

eath

ered

.

Ther

e ar

e b

oo

ks, w

ater

tab

le

toys

, sci

ence

bu

cket

s, b

ub

ble

s,

dra

win

g m

ater

ials

, sh

ove

ls a

nd

sa

nd

to

ys, a

nd

mu

sica

l in

stru

men

ts.

Ther

e ar

e tr

ikes

, 3 c

ozy

co

up

es a

nd

5

rid

e-o

n "

bik

es"

w/o

ped

als;

big

w

hee

l, b

-bal

l ho

op

, 1 b

all,

chal

k,

crat

es, a

nd

a k

itch

en.

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Shed

ou

tsid

e w

ith

po

rtab

le

mat

eria

ls t

hat

ch

ildre

n c

an

take

fro

m a

nd

pu

t b

ack

in t

he

shed

: —

bal

anci

ng

stilt

s —

mu

sica

l in

stru

men

ts

—la

rge

bal

ls w

ith

han

dle

s —

vari

ety

of

smal

l bal

ls

—co

nes

Hu

la-h

oo

ps

—b

ats

Ther

e ar

e sw

ings

, ham

mo

cks,

a

san

d a

rea

wit

h s

ho

vels

, tr

uck

s, b

uck

ets,

po

ts a

nd

pan

s,

bik

es, s

coo

ters

, wag

on

s, r

ock

s,

a tr

acto

r ti

re e

mb

edd

ed in

th

e d

irt,

tab

les,

a p

lay

ho

use

, w

ater

pu

mp

(p

itch

er p

um

p),

P

VC

pip

es, b

alls

, an

d b

oo

ks.

Ch

ildre

n s

coo

ped

dir

t, p

ulle

d

wag

on

s, r

od

e b

ikes

&

sco

ote

rs; c

limb

ed o

n t

he

clim

ber

; use

d p

vc p

ipes

as

—3-

pas

sen

ger

Tee

ter-

Tott

er

—B

ikes

Sco

ote

rs

—3-

pas

sen

ger

rou

nd

bik

e

—Ea

sel

—K

itch

en s

et

—R

olli

ng

san

d t

able

San

d t

oys

, sco

op

s, f

un

nel

s,

con

tain

ers,

liza

rds,

bo

ats,

co

lan

der

Dru

m a

nd

mu

sica

l in

stru

men

ts

—Fo

otb

alls

Ro

un

d b

alls

Page 33: PQA-R - HighScope

32

—b

alan

cin

g b

oar

ds

—fo

am n

oo

dle

s —

bea

nb

ags

wit

h b

ean

bag

to

ss

—p

om

po

ms

—p

arac

hu

te

—b

ow

ling

pin

s an

d b

alls

Vel

cro

pad

dle

s an

d b

alls

pla

stic

ho

rse

sho

es

—Fr

isb

ees

—p

last

ic s

ho

pp

ing

cart

ease

l —

bik

es

—1

sen

sory

tab

le f

illed

wit

h

pu

rple

san

d a

nd

1 s

en

sory

ta

ble

fill

ed w

ith

pin

e co

nes

ho

rns

and

tel

esco

pes

; sw

un

g o

n s

win

gs a

nd

in h

amm

ock

s;

mad

e sa

nd

cak

es f

or

a b

irth

day

; wal

ked

/bal

ance

d o

n

logs

; pla

yed

ho

use

; sat

on

a

big

tir

e in

th

e su

n; t

alke

d w

ith

a

frie

nd

; mo

ved

big

ro

cks

to

loo

ked

fo

r w

orm

s.

—B

ean

-bag

to

ss e

ase

l wit

h p

ock

ets

for

bea

nb

ags

—B

aske

tbal

l ho

op

w/b

alls

Smal

l tru

cks

wit

h 2

Lit

tle

Tyk

es

gara

ges

—Si

dew

alk

chal

k —

Bu

bb

les

—M

ou

nta

in r

ock

s —

Rin

g to

ss

—M

itts

an

d b

alls

(V

elcr

o)

—Sm

all p

last

ic b

alls

, 4”

—So

ccer

bal

l —

Vo

lleyb

all

—H

ula

-ho

op

s —

Ten

t —

Co

op

erat

ion

bal

l slid

e o

n r

op

es

—Sn

ow

sh

ove

ls

—Sl

ed

s —

Sno

w c

law

s —

Jum

p r

op

es

Page 34: PQA-R - HighScope

33

Item

I‐D

Ch

ildre

n’s

wo

rk a

nd

en

viro

nm

enta

l pri

nt

are

on

dis

pla

y.

Ro

w 1

: Ad

ult

s d

isp

lay

child

ren

’s w

ork

th

rou

gho

ut

the

lear

nin

g en

viro

nm

ent

in m

any

way

s.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

8 co

ffee

filt

er b

utt

erfl

ies

are

on

th

e w

all i

n t

he

bo

ok

area

at

ad

ult

eye

leve

l.

14 w

ater

colo

r p

ain

tin

gs in

th

e ar

t ar

ea a

t ad

ult

eye

leve

l. 5

pap

er-p

late

su

ns,

all

pai

nte

d

yello

w, o

n t

he

wal

l at

child

ren

’s

leve

l in

th

e b

oo

k ar

ea.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

On

th

e b

ulle

tin

bo

ard

(ad

ult

ey

e le

vel)

wer

e p

aper

-pla

te

spid

ers

and

fin

ger-

pai

nte

d

rain

bo

ws,

all

the

sam

e. T

her

e w

ere

two

exa

mp

les

of

emer

gen

t w

riti

ng

do

wn

low

in

th

e w

riti

ng

area

.

Ch

ildre

n's

dis

pla

ys w

ere

in t

he

art

and

blo

ck a

rea

and

in

clu

ded

dra

win

gs, c

olla

ges,

p

ain

tin

gs, s

tam

ps,

ph

oto

s o

f b

lock

bu

ildin

gs, a

nd

cla

ss-

mad

e b

oo

ks. D

isp

lays

are

at

bo

th c

hild

an

d a

du

lt e

ye le

vel.

All

wo

rk is

ad

ult

leve

l an

d o

nly

in

on

e co

rner

of

the

roo

m. A

ll w

ork

is

child

-in

itia

ted

an

d in

clu

des

p

ain

tin

gs, p

ictu

res,

wri

tin

g, a

nd

ch

arts

.

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

The

artw

ork

dis

pla

yed

th

rou

gho

ut

the

clas

sro

om

in

clu

des

ch

ild-i

nit

iate

d

dra

win

gs, s

amp

les

of

wri

tin

g,

colla

ge a

rt, p

ain

tin

gs, a

nd

st

amp

art

. Th

ere

are

also

p

ho

togr

aph

s in

cen

ters

of

child

ren

pla

yin

g (e

.g.,

d

raw

ing,

bu

ildin

g, e

tc.)

in

tho

se a

reas

. Art

wo

rk in

th

e

ho

use

are

a is

po

sted

at

adu

lt

eye

leve

l.

Ch

ildre

n's

wo

rk is

dis

pla

yed

on

th

e b

ack

of

shel

ves

in a

var

iety

o

f p

lace

s in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m.

Wo

rk in

clu

des

ph

oto

grap

hs,

d

raw

ings

, an

d q

uo

tes

fro

m t

he

child

ren

ab

ou

t th

eir

wo

rk.

Art

wo

rk in

clu

ded

ch

ildre

n’s

sc

rib

blin

gs, d

itto

co

lor

pag

es,

bla

ck c

on

stru

ctio

n p

aper

wit

h

chal

k in

dif

fere

nt

colo

rs, a

nd

d

eco

rate

d p

aper

bag

s m

ade

to

loo

k lik

e p

um

pki

ns.

Som

e w

ork

is a

t ch

ildre

n’s

eye

le

vel a

nd

so

me

is a

bo

ve. T

her

e ar

e p

ho

tos

of

child

ren

do

ing

vari

ou

s ty

pes

of

wo

rk in

all

inte

rest

are

as.

In t

he

art

area

an

d o

n b

ack

wal

l th

ere

is a

var

iety

of

dif

fere

nt

typ

es

of

art

dis

pla

yed

. Th

ere

are

pai

nti

ngs

, dra

win

gs, w

riti

ng

sam

ple

s, c

olla

ges,

an

d p

ho

tos.

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Dis

pla

yed

wo

rk in

clu

ded

co

llage

s, c

hal

k d

raw

ings

, d

raw

ings

of

child

ren

’s

fam

ilie

s, c

hild

ren

’s w

riti

ng

thei

r n

ames

, wat

erco

lor

Ch

ildre

n's

wo

rk c

on

sist

s o

f p

ain

tin

gs, d

raw

ings

, ph

oto

s o

f p

roje

cts,

co

llage

mat

eria

ls

pro

ject

, wri

tin

gs.

Ch

ildre

n’s

wo

rk w

as d

isp

laye

d in

all

inte

rest

are

as a

nd

incl

ud

ed c

hild

-in

itia

ted

dra

win

gs, p

ain

tin

gs,

colla

ges,

an

d p

ho

tos

of

child

ren

at

wo

rk. A

ll w

ork

was

dis

pla

yed

at

Page 35: PQA-R - HighScope

34

pai

nti

ngs

, an

d r

ecal

l gra

ph

s.

Fam

ily d

raw

ings

po

sted

in

the

bo

ok

area

; var

iety

of

child

ren

's a

rtw

ork

po

sted

in

the

blo

ck, h

ou

se, a

nd

to

y ar

eas;

ch

ildre

n's

ch

alk

dra

win

gs p

ost

ed b

y th

e

cub

bie

s n

ear

the

do

or;

ex

amp

les

of

child

ren

’s

rep

rese

nta

tio

ns

of

thei

r n

ames

in t

he

wri

tin

g ar

ea;

vari

ety

of

child

ren

's c

olla

ges

po

sted

in a

rt a

rea

at

child

ren

’s e

ye le

vel.

The

child

ren

's w

ork

is

dis

pla

yed

in d

iffe

ren

t p

arts

of

the

clas

sro

om

: dis

cove

ry a

rea,

th

e b

ath

roo

m, b

y th

e ch

ildre

n's

cu

bb

ies,

in t

he

stu

dio

, nea

r th

e dr

amat

ic p

lay

area

.

The

child

ren

's w

ork

is

dis

pla

yed

at

the

child

ren

's e

ye

leve

l.

child

ren

’s e

ye le

vel.

Ho

mem

ade

clas

s b

oo

ks w

ere

on

th

e b

oo

ksh

elf

in t

he

bo

ok

area

.

Page 36: PQA-R - HighScope

35

Item

I‐D

Ch

ildre

n’s

wo

rk a

nd

en

viro

nm

enta

l pri

nt

are

on

dis

pla

y.

Ro

w 2

: Man

y ex

amp

les

of

envi

ron

men

tal p

rin

t th

at e

nco

ura

ge c

hild

ren

to

wri

te le

tter

s, n

um

ber

s, n

ames

an

d w

ord

s ar

e in

ten

tio

nal

ly p

lace

d t

hro

ugh

ou

t th

e cl

assr

oo

m.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ther

e ar

e n

o w

ord

s o

r le

tter

s p

lace

d in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m.

Ther

e is

an

alp

hab

et s

trip

; it

is

pla

ced

alo

ng

the

ceili

ng.

Th

ere

is a

cu

rsiv

e al

ph

abet

str

ip.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ch

ildre

n’s

nam

es a

re o

n t

hei

r cu

bb

ies

and

th

ere

are

area

n

ames

(B

oo

k A

rea,

Kit

chen

, et

c.).

The

Dai

ly S

ched

ule

, wit

h t

he

par

ts o

f th

e d

ay, i

s p

ost

ed

nea

r th

e ca

rpet

use

d f

or

gree

tin

g ti

me

an

d in

clu

des

p

ho

tos

of

child

ren

en

gage

d in

th

e va

rio

us

dai

ly a

ctiv

itie

s.

Ther

e is

a s

ign

-up

sh

eet

and

a jo

b

char

t p

ost

ed n

ear

the

do

or

of

the

clas

sro

om

.

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

The

child

ren

's n

ames

are

d

isp

laye

d a

t ey

e le

vel.

Ther

e is

a c

hild

ren

's s

ched

ule

an

d

job

ch

art.

Th

ere

are

lab

els

on

m

ater

ials

, wri

tin

g m

ater

ials

in

all

area

s, a

nd

an

alp

hab

et

char

t.

Ther

e ar

e ch

ildre

n's

cu

bb

ies

wit

h n

ames

an

d p

ho

tos

and

h

oo

ks w

ith

nam

es o

n t

hem

. Th

ere

is a

vis

ual

sch

edu

le.

Inte

rest

are

as a

nd

mat

eria

ls

are

lab

eled

. Th

ere

are

han

d-

was

hin

g vi

sual

s w

ith

nu

mb

ers

on

th

em f

or

step

s. T

her

e ar

e w

riti

ng

mat

eria

ls in

inte

rest

ar

eas.

Ch

ildre

n s

ign

ed in

at

the

beg

inn

ing

of

the

day

. C

hild

ren

's p

ictu

res

and

nam

es,

and

en

viro

nm

enta

l pri

nt

po

sted

o

n a

n a

lph

abet

wo

rd w

all.

Inte

rest

are

as a

nd

mat

eria

ls a

re

lab

eled

. A

lph

abet

an

d n

um

ber

str

ips

are

loca

ted

th

rou

gho

ut

the

clas

sro

om

. C

hild

ren

nam

es a

re o

n c

ub

bie

s an

d m

ailb

oxe

s.

Han

d-w

ash

ing

(ju

st w

ord

s)

pro

ced

ure

po

sted

by

the

sin

k.

Page 37: PQA-R - HighScope

36

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ch

ildre

n s

ign

in d

aily

usi

ng

nam

e an

d s

ymb

ol;

Inte

rest

ar

ea s

ign

s in

clu

de

area

nam

e an

d p

ho

to; A

lph

abet

an

d

nu

mer

ical

str

ips

pla

ced

in t

he

wri

tin

g ar

ea a

nd

dra

mat

ic

pla

y ar

ea; C

hild

ren

's n

ames

vi

sib

le t

hro

ugh

ou

t ro

om

on

cu

bb

ies,

art

wo

rk, w

riti

ngs

, an

d “

go h

om

e”

fold

ers;

Clo

ck

in L

GT

area

an

d d

ram

atic

pla

y ar

ea; r

eal c

ale

nd

ar in

LG

T ar

ea a

nd

nea

r th

e d

oo

r;

han

d-w

ash

ing

char

t w

ith

p

ictu

res,

wo

rds,

an

d

nu

mb

ers

nea

r th

e si

nk;

wo

rd

card

s in

th

e w

riti

ng

area

, art

ar

ea a

nd

dra

mat

ic p

lay

area

.

Sign

-in

sh

eets

wit

h c

hild

ren

’s

nam

es, l

ett

er-l

inke

d s

ymb

ols

, an

d n

um

ber

s th

at m

atch

eac

h

child

's c

ot/

bag

; In

tere

st a

reas

h

ave

sign

s; S

hel

ves

and

to

y b

ins

are

lab

eled

; A

BC

str

ip

po

sted

at

child

ren

’s le

vel i

n

bo

ok

area

; Ch

ildre

n’s

nam

es

and

lett

er-l

inke

d s

ymb

ols

vi

sib

le o

n c

ub

bie

s; C

hild

ren

’s

nam

es o

n c

ots

; Co

mp

ute

r ke

ybo

ard

s; H

and

-was

hin

g re

min

der

s w

ith

wo

rds,

n

um

ber

s, a

nd

pic

ture

s lo

cate

d

by

bo

th s

inks

; La

bel

ed o

bje

cts;

C

lock

in h

ou

se a

rea

wit

h

nu

mb

ers;

Map

s in

ho

use

are

a

Sign

in s

hee

ts; I

nte

rest

are

a si

gns;

W

ord

wal

l wit

h c

hild

ren

's n

ames

an

d e

nvi

ron

men

tal p

rin

t; L

ette

rs

on

bat

hro

om

do

or;

Co

lor

nam

es

on

wal

l; N

um

ber

1 t

o 2

0 o

n w

all;

Sc

ien

ce c

har

ts w

ith

det

ails

of

exp

erim

ents

wit

h a

pp

les;

C

hild

ren

's n

ames

on

cu

bb

ies;

La

bel

ed o

bje

cts;

Clo

ck w

ith

n

um

eral

s; A

lph

abet

po

ster

; P

red

icti

on

ch

arts

, sh

ared

wri

tin

g,

po

ems;

Dai

ly r

ou

tin

e w

ith

nam

es

and

nu

mb

ers

Page 38: PQA-R - HighScope

37

Item

II‐A

Ad

ult

s Su

pp

ort

Ch

ildre

n’s

Un

der

stan

din

g o

f th

e C

on

sist

ent

Seq

ue

nce

of

Even

ts (

Dai

ly R

ou

tin

e).

Ro

w 1

: Ad

ult

s su

pp

ort

ch

ildre

n’s

un

der

stan

din

g o

f th

e co

nsi

sten

t ro

uti

ne

and

se

qu

ence

of

even

ts in

a s

cho

ol d

ay.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ther

e is

a t

yped

sch

edu

le

po

sted

nex

t to

th

e te

lep

ho

ne

abo

ve t

he

hea

ds

of

the

child

ren

.

No

sch

edu

le is

po

sted

. Du

rin

g ch

oic

e ti

me,

T s

aid

: “W

e n

eed

to

cle

an u

p f

or

snac

k.”

No

o

ther

par

ts o

f th

e d

ay w

ere

refe

ren

ced

.

Ther

e is

a s

ched

ule

po

sted

in t

he

LGT

area

at

child

ren

’s e

ye le

vel;

teac

her

s an

d c

hild

ren

did

no

t ta

lk

abo

ut

the

par

ts o

f th

e d

ay.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ther

e is

a p

ost

ed d

aily

sc

hed

ule

fo

r ch

ildre

n. T

he

sch

edu

le in

clu

des

tex

t an

d

imag

es.

At

the

star

t o

f th

e d

ay, T

sai

d:

“Co

me

mee

t m

e at

th

e ca

rpet

fo

r gr

ou

p.”

Vis

ual

Ro

uti

ne

(wit

h w

ord

s)

po

sted

un

der

th

e sm

art

bo

ard

.

T sa

id lo

ud

ly: “

5 m

inu

tes

till

gro

up

. Fin

ish

up

.”

T: “

Ok,

are

we

read

y? I

am

goin

g to

cal

l yo

ur

nam

e; y

ou

ar

e go

ing

to c

lap

yo

ur

sylla

ble

s an

d g

o t

o y

ou

r ta

ble

.”

CA

an

d T

sai

d lo

ud

ly: “

It’s

tim

e to

cle

an u

p.”

Sch

edu

le is

po

sted

at

child

leve

l w

ith

pic

ture

s an

d w

ord

s u

sin

g th

e H

igh

Sco

pe

sch

edu

le c

ard

s.

Du

rin

g th

e m

orn

ing

mee

tin

g, t

he

teac

her

an

no

un

ced

th

at in

stea

d o

f d

oin

g m

usi

c th

ey w

ou

ld g

o t

o t

he

gym

.

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ch

to

T: “

Wh

en a

re w

e go

ing

ou

tsid

e?”

T as

ked

th

e q

ues

tio

n b

ack:

“W

he

n a

re w

e

goin

g o

uts

ide

?”

TA: “

To t

ran

siti

on

to

go

o

uts

ide

, yo

u a

re g

oin

g to

do

sh

apes

. Yo

u a

re g

oin

g to

Dai

ly s

ched

ule

is a

t ch

ild e

ye

leve

l.

T: “

Wh

at d

o w

e d

o a

fte

r w

e ea

t? L

oo

k at

our

sch

edu

le u

p

ther

e.”

Po

inte

d t

o d

aily

sc

hed

ule

. Ch

: “N

ap.”

T:

“W

e d

o h

ave

rest

tim

e.”

A1:

"Th

at's

op

en d

uri

ng

bo

ok

tim

e."

C1

: "I t

hin

k w

e ca

n d

o t

hat

at

dis

mis

sal."

A1:

"O

h, y

ou

th

ink

may

be

at d

ism

issa

l, o

k."

Page 39: PQA-R - HighScope

38

com

e u

p a

nd

pic

k a

shap

e.”

Dai

ly r

ou

tin

e is

po

sted

on

th

e si

de

of

the

art

cab

inet

s;

clip

art

and

tex

t at

ch

ild le

vel.

T: “

Five

mo

re m

inu

tes.

An

d

then

wh

at?”

C

h:

“Th

en w

e go

insi

de

and

w

ash

ou

r h

and

s an

d g

o t

o t

he

carp

et a

nd

list

en t

o a

sto

ry

and

th

en s

mal

l gro

up

an

d t

hen

w

ork

tim

e.”

T:

“Yo

u r

emem

ber

ed o

ur

afte

rno

on

sch

edu

le.”

A1:

"So

meo

ne

said

th

at it

mig

ht

rain

, so

we

wo

uld

hav

e to

tak

e th

is o

ff if

we

can

't g

o o

uts

ide.

So

w

ork

tim

e w

ou

ld b

e lo

nge

r."

(po

cket

ch

art)

T: “

Go

was

h y

ou

r h

and

s; it

’s t

ime

for

lun

ch.”

Dai

ly s

ched

ule

is p

ost

ed v

erti

cally

o

n t

he

wal

l nea

r th

e la

rge

-gro

up

ar

ea; i

t co

nta

ins

wri

tten

tex

t (i

n a

p

ock

et c

har

t).

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Dai

ly s

ched

ule

is p

ost

ed a

t ch

ildre

n's

leve

l wit

h w

ord

s an

d H

igh

Sco

pe

sym

bo

ls.

T1: “

We

’re

goin

g to

larg

e gr

ou

p.”

C

2: “

Wh

at a

re w

e d

oin

g fo

r la

rge

gro

up

?”

T1:

“We

’re

do

ing

par

tner

d

anci

ng.

On

e p

aren

t ca

me

to p

ick

up

a

child

aft

er

sto

ry t

ime.

C

1: “

It’s

tim

e to

go

ho

me?

T1:

“We

’re

goin

g to

eat

sn

ack

and

th

en d

o la

rge

gro

up

.”

Dai

ly s

ched

ule

is p

ost

ed a

t ch

ildre

n’s

leve

l in

th

e so

cial

ar

ea w

her

e th

ey h

ave

larg

e gr

ou

p.

A3:

“Z,

I’m

wo

nd

erin

g if

yo

u

was

hed

yo

ur

han

ds?

” C

1: “

Yes.

” A

3: “

Ok,

th

en w

hat

is t

he

nex

t st

ep, w

hat

do

we

do

nex

t?”

C1

: "Si

gn in

; I d

id.”

A1

to C

h: “

Yes,

as

soo

n a

s w

e go

to

sm

all g

rou

p, t

hat

will

be

you

r ta

ble

.”

The

visu

al d

aily

sch

edu

le is

at

eye

leve

l fo

r ch

ildre

n. T

he

visu

al

sch

edu

le h

as s

imp

le p

ictu

res

and

w

ord

s fo

r ea

ch p

art

of

the

day

. Te

ach

ers

refe

rred

to

th

e ro

uti

ne

at m

ult

iple

tim

es t

hro

ugh

ou

t th

e d

ay t

alki

ng

abo

ut

wh

at c

ame

nex

t:

T: “

We

hav

e o

ne

mo

re m

inu

te t

o

fin

ish

bef

ore

mo

rnin

g m

essa

ge.”

T:

“It

's a

lmo

st t

ime

to c

lean

up

.”

T: “

We

are

mo

vin

g to

larg

e-g

rou

p

no

w.”

T:

“O

ff t

o r

ecal

l tim

e.”

Page 40: PQA-R - HighScope

39

C2

: “W

e a

lrea

dy

did

larg

e gr

ou

p.”

T1

: “W

e d

id la

rge

gro

up

on

e ti

me

tod

ay b

ut

rem

emb

er w

e d

o la

rge

gro

up

tw

o t

imes

a

day

.”

C2

: “B

ut

we

on

ly h

ave

on

e sm

all g

rou

p.”

T1

: “W

e d

o o

nly

hav

e o

ne

smal

l gro

up

.”

T1: “

Aft

er

sto

ry t

ime

is s

nac

k ti

me.

Ch

ild’s

job

was

to

ch

eck

the

sch

edu

le; s

he

aske

d t

o d

o t

his

d

uri

ng

free

ch

oic

e. S

he

wen

t to

rev

iew

it w

ith

A3.

T1: “

We

nee

d t

o f

inis

h s

nac

k so

we

can

go

ou

tsid

e; t

hen

it’ll

b

e t

ime

for

rest

tim

e.”

At

gro

up

, C1

mo

ved

th

e m

arke

r o

n t

he

sch

edu

le.

T: “

Loo

k, S

mo

ved

it t

o s

mal

l-gr

ou

p t

ime.

Th

at’s

wh

at's

nex

t.

Then

we

will

do

pla

nn

ing.

Page 41: PQA-R - HighScope

40

Item

II‐A

Ad

ult

s Su

pp

ort

Ch

ildre

n’s

Un

der

stan

din

g o

f th

e C

on

sist

ent

Seq

ue

nce

of

Even

ts (

Dai

ly R

ou

tin

e).

Ro

w 2

: Ad

ult

s ar

e th

ou

ghtf

ul a

bo

ut

lett

ing

child

ren

kn

ow

wh

en t

ran

siti

on

s to

a d

iffe

ren

t (w

ith

in a

nd

ou

tsid

e o

f th

e cl

assr

oo

m),

gro

up

, or

acti

vity

will

occ

ur.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ther

e w

as a

fiv

e-m

inu

te

war

nin

g b

efo

re c

lean

up

fro

m

cho

ice.

No

oth

er p

ort

ion

s o

f th

e d

ay w

ere

ann

ou

nce

d in

ad

van

ce.

Du

rin

g w

ork

tim

e, c

hild

ren

fl

ash

ed li

ghts

an

d lo

ud

ly

ann

ou

nce

d c

lean

up

.

T1: “

Pu

t it

aw

ay, s

mal

l gro

up

is

ove

r.”

No

oth

er t

ran

siti

on

war

nin

gs

wer

e o

bse

rved

.

Ch

ild’s

job

was

to

let

oth

er

child

ren

kn

ow

th

at t

her

e w

ere

five

min

ute

s le

ft t

o p

lay

at w

ork

ti

me.

N

o o

ther

war

nin

gs f

or

tran

siti

on

w

ere

ob

serv

ed.

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

T sa

id lo

ud

ly: “

We

just

fi

nis

hed

cle

anu

p; I

wo

nd

er

wh

at c

om

es n

ext

on

ou

r sc

hed

ule

?” T

wen

t o

ver

to

the

visu

al s

ched

ule

to

ch

ange

it

to

rec

all:

“Aft

er

reca

ll w

e w

ill r

ead

a b

oo

k an

d t

hen

go

o

uts

ide.

TA: “

E (f

ive

-min

ute

job

).”

E tu

rned

ligh

ts o

ff. T

A s

aid

lo

ud

ly: “

5 m

ore

min

ute

s to

cl

ean

up

. Sin

g th

e so

ng,

E.”

E

wen

t ar

ou

nd

th

e ro

om

b

angi

ng

a d

rum

.

T sa

id t

o c

hild

ren

in t

he

blo

ck

At

cho

ice

tim

e, c

hild

ren

wer

e gi

ven

a 5

-min

ute

war

nin

g w

ith

th

e lig

hts

off

.

Ch

ildre

n w

ere

qu

ietl

y aw

aken

ed in

div

idu

ally

fro

m

rest

tim

e. T

each

ers

op

ened

th

e b

lind

s fi

rst,

an

d t

hen

tu

rned

on

th

e lig

hts

.

Teac

her

an

no

un

ced

lou

dly

to

cl

ass:

“Fr

eeze

! Ev

eryo

ne

com

e to

th

e ru

g.”

Ther

e w

ere

visi

tors

fro

m t

he

den

tist

’s

off

ice.

At

cho

ice

tim

e, A

1 an

no

un

ced

: “O

ne

mo

re m

inu

te.”

A1

san

g a

cle

anu

p s

on

g an

d

join

ed c

hild

ren

on

th

e ca

rpet

to

b

egin

gro

up

.

Aft

er

lun

ch, c

hild

ren

wer

e re

dir

ecte

d t

o s

it b

ack

at t

he

tab

les

un

til t

her

e w

ere

less

th

an

3 ch

ildre

n in

lin

e. A

ch

ild w

aite

d

alo

ne

at t

he

tab

le; a

ll ch

airs

wer

e st

acke

d. T

he

teac

her

red

irec

ted

ch

ildre

n in

lin

e: “

You

’re

firs

t, h

e’s

nex

t in

lin

e, a

nd

th

en y

ou

’re

afte

r h

im…

.”

Page 42: PQA-R - HighScope

41

and

dra

mat

ic p

lay

area

s:

“Fo

ur

mo

re m

inu

tes.

T sa

id lo

ud

ly t

o a

ll ch

ildre

n a

t th

eir

smal

l-gr

ou

p t

able

s: “

We

are

goin

g to

mo

ve li

ke a

ve

hic

le o

f yo

ur

cho

ice

to r

ead

al

ou

d. “

Le

vel 3

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

T:

“I’

m g

oin

g to

sh

ow

yo

u

som

e p

ictu

res.

Wh

en y

ou

se

e yo

ur

pic

ture

, yo

u w

ill g

o t

o

you

r sm

all g

rou

p.”

“W

ho

's

this

gu

y? W

hat

's h

is la

st

nam

e?”

“M

rs. A

's t

able

. __

_ is

go

ing

to M

rs. B

's t

able

.”

“Wh

at's

__

_’s

last

nam

e?”

C

hild

ren

get

up

an

d h

ead

to

sm

all g

rou

p a

s th

ey s

ee

thei

r m

atch

ing

pic

ture

or

as t

hey

ar

e d

irec

ted

.

T: "

Get

in li

ne

.” C

hild

ren

get

in

lin

e fo

r gy

m. “

Oka

y ar

e w

e go

ing

to b

e n

ois

y as

el

eph

ants

or

qu

iet

as m

ice?

T w

alke

d a

rou

nd

to

ch

ildre

n

T st

and

s at

th

e fr

on

t o

f th

e ta

ble

s: "

You

gu

ys h

ave

a co

up

le m

ore

min

ute

s to

fin

ish

u

p w

ith

yo

ur

bre

akfa

st."

T: "

Nex

t w

e're

go

ing

to p

lan

b

efo

re w

e m

ove

to

wo

rk

tim

e. If

yo

u a

re in

my

gro

up

, m

ove

to

th

e b

lock

are

a. If

yo

u

are

in M

rs. W

's g

rou

p, y

ou

can

st

ay r

igh

t h

ere.

"

T: "

Bo

ys a

nd

gir

ls, l

oo

k at

th

e ti

me.

Wh

at d

oes

it s

ay?"

C

: "N

o t

ime!

" C

2: "

Cle

anu

p t

ime.

" T:

"A

lmo

st!

It s

ays

we

hav

e ab

ou

t o

ne

min

ute

."

T to

Ch

: “Yo

u a

re r

ead

y to

go

h

om

e. A

fte

r o

uts

ide

tim

e, it

’ll

At

the

end

of

larg

e gr

ou

p:

T1:

“C1

, ho

w s

ho

uld

we

get

to

the

sin

k to

was

h o

ur

han

ds?

” (f

or

snac

k)

C1

: “W

alk.

” T1

: “C

1 s

ays

we

sho

uld

wal

k to

th

e si

nk.

” C

2: “

I wan

t to

ho

p.”

T1

: “Y

ou

can

ho

p.”

Bre

akfa

st:

T1 q

uie

tly

said

, as

she

ate

bre

akfa

st w

ith

ch

ildre

n: “

5 m

ore

m

inu

tes

to f

inis

h b

reak

fast

.”

Seve

ral c

hild

ren

sai

d: “

OK

.”

Wo

rk T

ime:

T1

: “W

atch

th

e gr

een

(sa

nd

) ti

mer

. Wh

en t

he

gree

n t

imer

get

s d

ow

n t

o t

he

bo

tto

m, y

ou

pu

t th

e ta

ble

ts a

way

.”

Page 43: PQA-R - HighScope

42

in t

he

gym

: “2

mo

re m

inu

tes

at g

ym.”

T w

alke

d a

rou

nd

th

e cl

assr

oo

m: “

5 m

ore

min

ute

s at

ch

oic

e ti

me.

Aft

er

lun

ch a

ctiv

itie

s: “

You

ca

n g

o g

et y

ou

r st

uff

an

d

then

we

are

goin

g to

hea

d

ou

tsid

e.”

Cal

l on

ch

ildre

n a

fe

w a

t a

tim

e.

be

tim

e fo

r yo

ur

mo

m t

o p

ick

you

up

.”

T1 s

too

d in

on

e sp

ot

in t

oy

area

an

d s

aid

to

all:

“Te

n m

ore

m

inu

tes,

gu

ys.”

Tran

siti

on

to

larg

e gr

ou

p:

T1:

“Fri

en

ds,

bo

oks

aw

ay p

leas

e.”

(n

o w

arn

ing)

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Wo

rk T

ime:

T:

“A

ll th

is s

tuff

yo

u a

re p

ilin

g o

n m

e w

e're

go

ing

to h

ave

to

cle

an u

p a

t cl

ean

up

tim

e in

20

min

ute

s.”

A c

hild

fla

shed

th

e lig

hts

an

d

told

eve

ryo

ne

: “5

mo

re

min

ute

s.”

T: “

Let'

s h

ead

to

th

e ca

rpet

fo

r re

call;

th

en w

e'll

figu

re

ou

t w

her

e w

e're

go

ing

to

cle

an u

p.”

T: “

Aft

er

rece

ss, w

e'll

hav

e m

eeti

ng

circ

le.”

At

arri

val,

A2

wen

t ar

ou

nd

th

e ro

om

an

d t

old

ch

ildre

n a

nd

ad

ult

s p

layi

ng

that

th

ere

wo

uld

be

five

mo

re m

inu

tes

to

pla

y.

A2

gave

info

rmat

ion

ab

ou

t sm

all g

rou

p a

t th

e en

d o

f th

e la

rge

gro

up

: “A

t sm

all g

rou

p

tod

ay, w

e w

ill b

e ex

plo

rin

g m

agn

ets.

” B

efo

re s

mal

l gro

up

en

ded

ch

ildre

n w

ere

give

n a

2-

min

ute

an

d t

hen

1-m

inu

te

war

nin

g. T

hey

wer

e al

so t

old

th

at t

hey

co

uld

use

th

e m

ater

ials

at

free

ch

oic

e.

Ch

to

TA

: “T

he

clo

ck is

on

th

e fo

ur.

” TA

rep

eate

d: “

You

’re

righ

t.

Wh

at d

o w

e d

o w

hen

th

e cl

ock

is

on

th

e fo

ur?

” C

h: “

We

pic

k u

p

and

go

to

th

e ca

rpet

to

rea

d

bo

oks

.”

Wo

rk T

ime

: SA

gav

e 5

-min

ute

w

arn

ing

sign

s to

th

e sp

ecia

l h

elp

er. C

hild

wen

t ar

ou

nd

an

d

sho

wed

th

e 5-

min

ute

sig

n. (

Sign

h

ad t

he

nu

mb

er 5

on

it).

Ch

ild

pre

ten

ded

to

be

a ca

t w

hile

ta

kin

g th

e si

gn a

rou

nd

th

e ro

om

.

T w

alke

d a

rou

nd

th

e ro

om

: “It

lo

oks

like

L is

tel

ling

us

that

we

hav

e 5

mo

re m

inu

tes

left

to

Page 44: PQA-R - HighScope

43

T: “

If y

ou

're

fin

ish

ed w

ith

sm

all g

rou

p, y

ou

can

pu

t yo

ur

thin

gs a

way

an

d g

et r

ead

y fo

r lu

nch

.”

T: “

On

e m

ore

per

son

nee

ds

to f

inis

h in

th

e b

ath

roo

m a

nd

th

en w

e'll

shu

t th

e lig

hts

off

fo

r re

st t

ime.

Wh

ile o

uts

ide,

th

e te

ach

er

mo

ved

aro

un

d t

he

spac

e an

d

told

ch

ildre

n: “

We

hav

e ab

ou

t fi

ve m

ore

min

ute

s u

nti

l we

line

up

fo

r in

sid

e.”

Du

rin

g w

ork

tim

e, T

to

ld a

ch

ild: “

We

on

ly h

ave

abo

ut

five

mo

re m

inu

tes

to w

ork

on

th

is. T

hen

it’ll

be

tim

e to

cle

an

up

an

d h

ead

fo

r re

call.

pla

y.”

Spec

ial H

elp

er t

urn

ed t

he

ligh

ts

off

an

d s

aid

: “Ti

me

to c

lean

up

.”

T m

ove

d a

rou

nd

th

e ro

om

: “I s

ee

the

ligh

ts a

re o

ff n

ow

; it

mu

st b

e ti

me

to c

lean

up

. Let

’s c

lean

up

an

d g

et r

ead

y fo

r gr

ou

p.”

La

rge

Gro

up

: C

hild

hel

per

mo

ved

th

e cl

ip o

n t

he

dai

ly s

ched

ule

. T

aske

d: “

Wh

at c

om

es n

ext?

” C

hild

ren

: “W

ork

tim

e!”

T: “

You

’re

righ

t; a

nd

th

en w

e w

ill c

lean

up

an

d d

o o

ur

smal

l gro

up

s.”

Page 45: PQA-R - HighScope

44

Item

II‐B

Th

ere

is t

ime

each

day

fo

r ch

ild‐i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m a

nd

du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me.

R

ow

1: A

du

lts

allo

w c

hild

ren

to

car

ry o

ut

thei

r in

ten

tio

ns

usi

ng

all a

cces

sib

le m

ater

ials

dur

ing

clas

sro

om

ch

ild-i

nit

iate

d

acti

vity

fo

r an

ext

end

ed p

erio

d o

f ti

me.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Pla

y ti

me

was

fro

m 9

:30

– 10

:10

am. T

each

ers

pla

ced

m

ater

ials

on

4 t

able

s:

1.C

rayo

ns

and

co

lori

ng

bo

oks

2.M

agn

a-Ti

les

3.5

bo

ard

pu

zzle

s4.

Bin

go s

tam

per

s an

d p

aper

T as

sign

ed t

he

child

ren

to

ta

ble

s: “

OK

, yo

u c

an p

lay

wit

h t

he

mat

eria

ls in

fro

nt

of

you

fo

r 10

min

ute

s. T

hen

we

will

mo

ve t

o t

he

nex

t st

atio

n.”

Eve

ry 1

0 m

inu

tes

T sa

id: “

Tim

e to

mo

ve t

o t

he

nex

t st

atio

n.”

At

wo

rk t

ime

, a c

om

mu

nit

y re

ader

sto

pp

ed b

y to

rea

d t

o

the

child

ren

. T1

had

ch

ildre

n

all l

eav

e th

eir

acti

viti

es

and

sit

o

n t

he

carp

et t

o b

e re

ad t

o a

s a

gro

up

. Th

e vi

sito

r w

aite

d

abo

ut

10 m

inu

tes

un

til a

ll th

e ch

ildre

n w

ere

qu

iet.

Th

e ch

ildre

n h

ad o

nly

bee

n

pla

yin

g fo

r ab

ou

t 10

min

ute

s.

Wh

en t

he

read

ing

was

fin

ish

ed

they

wen

t o

n t

o s

nac

k ti

me.

At

wo

rk t

ime

, T2

sto

pp

ed

ever

yon

e to

let

them

kn

ow

th

ey

wer

e ge

ttin

g to

o lo

ud

: “M

y, o

h

my,

yo

u a

re a

ll b

ein

g w

ay t

oo

lo

ud

. Qu

iet

do

wn

no

w o

r p

lay

tim

e w

ill b

e d

on

e.”

She

sto

pp

ed

them

3 t

imes

an

d t

hen

dec

ided

th

ey w

ere

do

ne

afte

r 20

m

inu

tes:

“Th

at is

all!

We

are

do

ne

her

e. It

is c

lean

up

tim

e.”

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Two

gir

ls c

olo

red

wit

h

cray

on

s at

th

e ar

t ta

ble

. W

hen

th

ere

wer

e a

lot

of

bu

bb

les

in t

he

wat

er t

able

, th

e te

ach

er c

lose

d t

he

tab

le

and

th

e b

lock

are

a n

ext

to

the

wat

er t

able

. A c

hild

was

to

ld: “

Kee

p t

he

tru

cks

on

th

e

Pla

y ti

me

was

sch

edu

led

fo

r 45

m

inu

tes.

Ch

ildre

n p

laye

d in

th

e h

ou

se a

rea,

bu

ilt w

ith

b

lock

s, a

nd

did

pu

zzle

s at

th

e ta

ble

. Tw

o b

oys

ran

an

d

chas

ed

eac

h o

ther

. T1

told

th

em t

o s

top

. Wh

en t

hey

co

nti

nu

ed r

un

nin

g, T

1 ga

ve

them

a t

imeo

ut,

mak

ing

them

Wo

rk t

ime

was

ob

serv

ed f

rom

9:

20–9

:55

AM

. Ch

ildre

n g

ot

ou

t th

e ca

rdb

oar

d r

amp

s to

ext

en

d

thei

r p

lay

wit

h t

he

cars

. T2

was

in

th

e d

ress

up

are

a p

layi

ng

sto

re a

nd

ord

erin

g fo

od

. Oth

er

child

ren

pla

yed

wit

h c

ars

on

th

e le

dge

, rac

ing

them

an

d t

alki

ng

abo

ut

wh

ich

car

s ar

e th

e fa

stes

t

Page 46: PQA-R - HighScope

45

carp

et, p

leas

e.”

A b

oy

loo

ked

at

a b

oo

k in

th

e b

oo

k ar

ea.

sit

ou

t th

e re

st o

f th

e p

lay

tim

e, a

no

ther

10

min

ute

s.

and

slo

wes

t. T

1 u

ses

wo

rk t

ime

to a

sses

s ch

ildre

n, c

allin

g th

em

ove

r o

ne

at a

tim

e fo

r ab

ou

t 10

m

inu

tes

each

. Le

vel 3

Exa

mp

le

Evid

ence

C

hild

ren

pre

ten

d t

o c

oo

k in

th

e h

ou

se a

rea

wit

h T

2 m

akin

g “b

run

ch”

at T

2’s

req

ues

t. C

hild

ren

bu

ild w

ith

Le

gos

in t

he

blo

ck a

rea.

Th

ree

ch

ildre

n p

lay

at t

he

wat

er t

able

. A c

hild

pu

ts

som

e w

ater

in a

n e

mp

ty

con

tain

er a

nd

tak

es it

to

th

e h

ou

se a

rea.

An

oth

er c

hild

go

es t

o d

o t

he

sam

e. T

1:

“Yo

u c

an o

nly

tak

e th

e w

ater

if

yo

u a

re g

oin

g to

th

e w

ater

ta

ble

.” C

h p

oin

ts t

o t

he

ho

use

are

a. T

1: “

You

can

’t

hav

e w

ater

in t

he

ho

use

ar

ea;

if y

ou

wan

t to

pla

y w

ith

w

ater

, tak

e it

to

th

e w

ater

ta

ble

.”

Ch

: “C

an I

pla

y o

n t

he

com

pu

ter?

” T:

“Th

ere

is o

nly

o

ne

th

at w

ork

s, s

o w

hen

so

meo

ne

is d

on

e yo

u c

an

hav

e a

turn

.”

Thre

e c

hild

ren

wen

t an

d u

sed

la

rge

blo

cks

to b

uild

st

ruct

ure

s. O

ne

child

go

t en

velo

pes

an

d a

rt m

ater

ials

, sa

t at

a t

able

wri

tin

g, a

nd

pu

t p

aper

in e

nve

lop

es. T

wo

ch

ildre

n w

ent

to t

he

toy

area

to

bu

ild. O

ne

child

fo

un

d it

ems

that

wer

e m

enti

on

ed in

th

e re

ad-a

lou

d s

tory

an

d t

old

his

o

wn

ver

sio

n o

f th

e st

ory

(th

e te

ach

er w

as in

vite

d t

o li

sten

an

d w

atch

). T

hre

e c

hild

ren

p

laye

d w

ith

do

lls (

som

e ch

ildre

n d

ress

ed

up

). T

1 an

no

un

ced

cle

anu

p t

ime

wit

ho

ut

war

nin

g. C

hild

ren

had

30

min

ute

s o

f fr

ee t

ime.

A c

hild

sat

in t

he

loft

an

d p

laye

d

wit

h p

up

pet

s. T

wo

gir

ls d

ress

ed

b

aby

do

lls in

th

e h

ou

se a

rea

and

car

rie

d t

hem

aro

un

d t

he

clas

sro

om

. T1

hel

ped

a c

hild

w

rap

mas

kin

g ta

pe

aro

un

d

dif

fere

nt

pie

ces

of

pap

er. T

he

san

d/w

ater

tab

le w

as c

lose

d

(T1

ann

ou

nce

d t

hey

wo

uld

no

t b

e p

layi

ng

ther

e to

day

bec

ause

o

f th

e b

ub

ble

pro

ble

m t

he

day

b

efo

re).

Page 47: PQA-R - HighScope

46

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ch

oic

e ti

me

was

ob

serv

ed

fro

m 9

:15

–10:

05 A

M. O

ne

child

to

ok

the

trai

n f

rom

th

e b

lock

are

a to

th

e h

ou

se a

rea

to s

et it

up

. Ch

ildre

n m

ade

sou

p w

ith

lett

ers;

a c

hild

u

sed

th

e b

ean

bag

to

ss; t

hre

e

child

ren

use

d s

tick

s an

d

bla

nke

ts t

o b

uild

a t

ent,

ad

din

g ta

pe

to h

old

th

e b

lan

kets

to

th

e p

ole

s. C

h 1

sa

id: “

I am

th

e d

ad a

nd

I’m

go

ing

to c

han

ge t

he

bab

y.

You

be

the

un

cle

an

d g

et t

he

bo

ttle

.” C

h2:

“I a

m g

oin

g to

go

pla

y w

ith

G a

nd

th

e p

up

pet

s.”

Wo

rk t

ime

was

ob

serv

ed f

rom

9:

05 –

10:0

0 A

M. T

wo

gir

ls

mad

e st

raw

ber

ry s

ou

p f

or

T1

in t

he

ho

use

are

a. A

bo

y m

ade

a “r

ock

et s

hip

” w

ith

th

e M

agn

a-Ti

les.

An

oth

er b

oy

pu

shed

a T

on

ka t

ruck

aro

un

d

the

clas

sro

om

wit

h p

lay

foo

d

in t

he

bed

. Ch

to

ld T

1 in

th

e ar

t ar

ea: “

I’m

mak

ing

a ca

rd

for

my

mo

m.”

Wo

rk t

ime

was

ob

serv

ed f

rom

9:

16 –

10:0

9 A

M. C

hild

ren

fill

ed

mu

ffin

tin

s w

ith

pla

y d

ou

gh.

They

car

rie

d p

rete

nd

fo

od

to

o

ther

are

as. A

gir

l mix

ed p

ain

ts

at t

he

ease

l. C

hild

ren

pu

t b

ead

s an

d s

hel

ls in

pla

y d

ou

gh. T

wo

gi

rls

pac

ked

pu

rse

s w

ith

a

vari

ety

of

item

s an

d c

arri

ed

th

em in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m. C

hild

ren

w

ore

dre

ssu

p c

loth

es w

hile

w

ork

ing

in o

ther

are

as. T

hre

e

bo

ys b

uilt

ram

ps

wit

h b

lock

s.

Page 48: PQA-R - HighScope

47

Item

II‐B

Th

ere

is t

ime

each

day

fo

r ch

ild‐i

nit

iate

d a

ctiv

itie

s in

th

e cl

assr

oo

m a

nd

du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me.

R

ow

2: A

du

lts

allo

w c

hild

ren

car

ry o

ut

thei

r in

ten

tio

ns

usi

ng

all a

cces

sib

le e

qu

ipm

ent

and

mat

eria

ls d

uri

ng

the

ou

tdo

or

child

-in

itia

ted

act

ivit

y fo

r an

ext

en

ded

per

iod

of

tim

e.

Leve

l 1 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ch

ildre

n d

id n

ot

go o

uts

ide

to p

lay.

C

hild

ren

wen

t o

uts

ide

to p

lay,

b

ut

wer

e to

ld t

o s

tay

on

th

e b

lack

top

wit

h t

he

bal

ls (

2

bas

ketb

alls

, 4 p

layg

rou

nd

b

alls

) b

ecau

se “

It is

to

o m

ud

dy

to u

se t

he

pla

ygro

un

d.”

Ch

1 an

d C

h2

ran

aft

er

a p

layg

rou

nd

bal

l th

at r

olle

d

on

to t

he

gras

s. T

: “C

om

e b

ack

to t

he

bla

ckto

p. N

o

pla

ygro

un

d t

od

ay.”

Du

rin

g o

utd

oo

r ti

me

the

teac

her

s sp

lit t

he

gro

up

into

tw

o. T

1 p

laye

d “

Du

ck, D

uck

, G

oo

se”

wit

h h

er g

rou

p a

nd

T2

pla

yed

“R

ed

Lig

ht,

Gre

en

Lig

ht.

” A

fte

r 15

min

ute

s T1

an

no

un

ced

, “T

ime

to g

o b

ack

insi

de.

Let

’s

go!”

Leve

l 2 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Du

rin

g o

uts

ide

tim

e th

e ch

ildre

n r

ide

tric

ycle

s, p

lay

wit

h b

alls

, an

d r

un

on

th

e gr

ass

chas

ing

each

oth

er.

Aft

er

abo

ut

10 m

inu

tes

the

teac

her

s ca

ll to

ch

ildre

n t

o

join

in t

he

par

ach

ute

gam

es.

Wh

en s

om

e ch

ildre

n s

ay n

o,

they

are

to

ld t

hey

mu

st jo

in

in t

he

gam

e.

As

child

ren

pre

par

e to

go

o

uts

ide,

get

tin

g th

eir

jack

ets

on

, T1

ann

ou

nce

s th

at t

hey

w

ill n

ot

be

allo

wed

to

pla

y o

n

the

clim

ber

bec

ause

it is

wet

fr

om

rai

n o

vern

igh

t. T

hey

go

o

ut

and

pla

y fo

r 30

min

ute

s.

At

ou

tsid

e ti

me

child

ren

pla

yed

w

ith

bal

ls, s

wu

ng

on

sw

ings

, an

d c

has

ed

eac

h o

ther

. T2

join

ed in

th

e ga

me

of

chas

e,

dec

ided

wh

o w

as “

it”

and

tri

ed

to

mak

e th

e ch

ildre

n f

ollo

w h

er

rule

s to

th

e ga

me:

“Lo

ok,

it’s

his

tu

rn; y

ou

hav

e to

ru

n a

nd

ch

ase

h

im.”

Leve

l 3 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ou

tsid

e ti

me

last

ed 3

5 m

inu

tes.

So

me

child

ren

wer

e

sho

velin

g sn

ow

wit

h s

ho

vels

an

d o

ther

s w

ere

bu

ildin

g a

sno

wm

an. O

ne

child

pu

lled

Ch

ildre

n p

laye

d o

n s

win

gs,

ran

, ro

de

bik

es, a

nd

ran

bac

k an

d f

ort

h o

n t

he

clim

ber

’s

bri

dge

. T2

pla

yed

wit

h c

hild

ren

in

th

e p

layh

ou

se p

rete

nd

ing

to

Ch

ildre

n p

laye

d t

ag, p

rete

nd

ed

to b

e w

olv

es, d

ug

in t

he

san

db

ox,

an

d s

om

e c

hild

ren

m

ade

a ca

r u

nd

er t

he

pla

y st

ruct

ure

wit

h a

ste

eri

ng

wh

eel

Page 49: PQA-R - HighScope

48

ano

ther

ch

ild o

n a

sle

d. T

wo

gi

rls

laid

on

th

eir

bel

lies

on

th

e sw

ings

. On

e ch

ild

dra

gged

ou

t al

l th

e ju

mp

ro

pes

an

d le

ft t

hem

in a

pile

o

f sn

ow

. T2

sho

ute

d t

o h

er,

“Bef

ore

yo

u p

lay

wit

h t

he

sno

w s

ho

vels

, yo

u m

ust

cle

an

up

all

the

jum

p r

op

es y

ou

left

o

ut

ove

r th

ere.

coo

k. A

fte

r ab

ou

t 20

min

ute

s,

T1 g

ave

a 5

-min

ute

war

nin

g to

go

insi

de.

and

ch

airs

. So

me

child

ren

ro

de

tr

icyc

les

then

sw

itch

ed

acti

viti

es

to p

lay

tag.

T1

sto

pp

ed t

hem

in t

he

mid

dle

of

thei

r ga

me

of

tag

and

ask

ed

them

to

par

k th

eir

trik

es a

gain

st

the

wal

l bef

ore

th

ey s

tart

ed

ano

ther

act

ivit

y.

Leve

l 4 E

xam

ple

Ev

iden

ce

Ou

tsid

e ti

me

was

ob

serv

ed

fro

m 1

0:45

–11:

25 A

M. S

ix

child

ren

th

rew

bal

ls a

nd

hit

th

em w

ith

bat

s o

r p

add

les.

Tw

o c

hild

ren

bal

ance

d o

n

bal

anci

ng

stilt

s o

n t

he

sid

ewal

k. O

ne

child

str

app

ed

a d

rum

ove

r h

is s

ho

uld

ers

and

wal

ked

aro

un

d o

uts

ide

pla

yin

g th

e d

rum

s. C

hild

ren

d

ipp

ed c

hal

k in

wat

er a

nd

d

rew

on

ch

alkb

oar

d e

ase

l. O

ne

child

pla

yed

a d

rum

on

to

p o

f th

e p

lay

stru

ctur

e. O

ne

child

pic

ked

flo

wer

s an

d s

aid

, “I

’m p

icki

ng

flo

wer

s fo

r m

y m

om

.” T

1 sa

id, “

You

’re

pic

kin

g fl

ow

ers

for

you

r m

om

. Oka

y.”

On

e ch

ild r

od

e

Ou

tdo

or

pla

y to

ok

pla

ce f

rom

11

:15–

12:0

0. C

hild

ren

ro

de

on

b

ikes

, use

d t

he

swin

gs, p

laye

d

wit

h H

ula

-ho

op

s to

geth

er, a

nd

ro

lled

do

wn

a h

ill.

A b

oy

kick

ed a

bal

l wit

h t

he

teac

her

, th

en w

ent

to t

he

clim

ber

. Ch

ildre

n u

sed

si

dew

alk

chal

k o

n t

he

cem

ent;

a

girl

ask

ed t

he

teac

her

to

d

raw

ho

psc

otc

h f

or

them

.

Ch

ildre

n w

ere

ou

tsid

e fr

om

10

:50

–11:

28 A

M. C

hild

ren

use

d

bal

loo

ns

and

Hu

la-h

oo

ps

at t

he

bal

ance

bea

m. S

om

e ch

ildre

n

hel

d t

he

ho

op

s an

d w

alke

d

acro

ss t

he

bea

m. O

ne

child

tr

ied

to

ro

ll th

e h

oo

p d

ow

n t

he

bea

m. T

1 an

d c

hild

ren

th

rew

b

allo

on

s th

rou

gh t

he

ho

op

s. C

: “T

he

flo

or

is la

va.”

T: “

Wh

at

do

es t

hat

mea

n?”

C: “

You

will

tu

rn in

to t

he

lava

mo

nst

er.

Scre

am!”

On

e ch

ild w

alke

d

acro

ss t

he

bea

m, a

nd

th

en r

an

to t

he

bas

ketb

all h

oo

p. T

wo

b

oys

du

g in

th

e sa

nd

bo

x. A

gir

l w

as s

win

gin

g an

d t

hen

ran

to

th

e cl

imb

er.

Page 50: PQA-R - HighScope

49

a b

ike

wh

ile h

old

ing

a co

nta

iner

fu

ll o

f ch

alk.

Page 51: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 51

Preschool Program Quality Assessment — Revised (PQA-R)

Research Foundation

Introduction

The HighScope Educational Research Foundation developed the Preschool Program

Quality Assessment-Revised (PQA-R) in response to research that shows high-quality early

learning is directly related to academic and life success (Schweinhart et al., 2005; Snow, Burns,

& Griffin, 1998). There is growing evidence that educators who use data-driven instruction have

a greater impact on student learning outcomes (Datnow, Park, & Wohlstetter, 2007; Petrides &

Nodine, 2005). Researchers have consistently found that two interrelated sets of indicators —

process and structure — influence the quality of the educational experiences for children,

especially our most vulnerable children (National Institute for Early Education Research,

2002). HighScope based the PQA-R on process and structure indicators identified in the

research as most relevant to providing a high-quality preschool experience, including learning

environment, teaching-learning routines, adult-child interactions, language, literacy,

mathematics, critical thinking, curriculum, planning, assessment, and parent engagement

(Ehrlich et al., 2018). We conducted an extensive review of the extant literature to provide the

research base for the items chosen to assess three essential domains: the Learning

Environment; Teaching and Learning Routines and Adult-Child Interactions; and Curriculum,

Planning, Assessment, and Parent Engagement.

Page 52: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 52

Domain 1: Learning Environment

Young children are affected by their surroundings but, more importantly, the preschool

environment influences the teaching and learning process (e.g., Anders et al.,

2012). Intentionally organizing and preparing the learning environment to help support

children’s development is critical. Specifically, it is important that the learning environment is

inclusive, provides a variety of classroom materials, and includes different interest

areas. Additionally, there should be a safe outdoor area with permanent structures and

portable materials that allow for diverse kinds of movement and play.

Item I-A: The indoor space has a variety of interest areas that have names and are

intentionally organized.

The utility of interest areas in promoting specific desirable child behaviors has been

identified in the research literature. Although there is no set rule for the number of interest

areas, it is recommended by Vogel (2012) to have basic interest areas such as creative,

construction, dramatic play, inquiry-based, and literacy areas. Having a number of different

areas enables teachers to provide diverse activities and tailor activities to individual children’s

interests (Inan, 2009).

A study conducted in 1980 found that the presence of partitioned special play areas was

associated with increased rates of fantasy play and peer interaction (Field, 1980). Additional

studies have found that organization into learning zones is associated with attention and active

engagement with peers (Dunst, McWilliam, & Holbert, 1986). Researchers have also identified

the presence of clearly identified and specific areas throughout the classroom containing

Page 53: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

53

conceptually or schematically clustered materials as potentially supportive of language learning

and use (Roskos & Neuman, 2001).

Item I-B: Classroom materials are plentiful.

The six learning areas addressed in this item (i.e., literacy; mathematics; science;

socioemotional skills; perceptual, motor, and physical development; and human diversity) are

all important parts of the preschool curriculum. Early development of skills in these areas,

particularly literacy and math, supports later school achievement (Yoshikawa et al., 2013).

Language and literacy are present in all areas of children’s education; math, science, and social

studies all draw on language and literacy competencies, particularly as children enter the higher

grades. Print materials and a literacy-rich environment that make literacy artifacts, such as

functional signs, literacy tools, and literacy-enriched play tools (e.g., menus, coupons, pens and

stationary, shopping list pads, recipe cards), available so children can enhance their literacy

activity in play (Neuman & Roskos, 1992).

Early math outcomes are also important for children’s future achievement and life

outcomes; research finds that early number competence predicts math achievement and the

rate of growth in math achievement through the third grade and math achievement in the 10th

grade (Jordan, Kaplan, Ramineni, & Locuniak, 2009; Stevenson & Newman, 1986). Additionally,

persistent math deficiencies can limit student career options (Eccles, 1997). A review of

research on the use of manipulatives in math instruction for students from the kindergarten to

college level found that, overall, manipulative use was associated with better learning

outcomes than instruction without manipulatives (Carbonneau, Marley, & Selig, 2013). These

positive effects, however, were moderated by the instructional techniques employed in each

Page 54: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 54

study, indicating that manipulatives alone, without intentional use and instruction by teachers,

are insufficient to support math learning.

Although less commonly integrated in preschool classrooms, science presents an

opportunity to explore critical thinking skills as children observe, manipulate, and talk about

their scientific experiences, and science supports language, literacy, and vocabulary

development (Brenneman, Stevenson-Boyd, & Frede, 2009). Science in the classroom should

be treated as more than learning lists of facts (Brenneman et al., 2009). Rather, children need

hands-on experiences that allow them to handle, observe, and talk about concepts in order to

understand what is new and different to them (Rivera, 1998).

Developing socioemotional skills is another central area of learning in preschool, and is

beneficial to children’s school performance as they grow older (Wentzel & Asher,

1995). Sociodramatic play is supported by research for developing and promoting children’s

socioemotional skills, including improving their role-taking ability, which is necessary for

developing communication and empathy skills (Hughes, 2009). Many objects in a preschool

classroom can be incorporated into sociodramatic play, particularly materials in areas that

support dramatic play, such as the house area.

Motor skills are also important to children’s success in school. Better overall motor

skills in preschool have been found to be associated with third-grade reading achievement

(McPhillips & Jordan‐Black, 2007). Additionally, fine motor skills at kindergarten entry predict

later achievement (Grissmer, Grimm, Aiyer, Murrah, & Steele, 2010), which is unsurprising as

fine-motor tasks are prevalent in school environments. To illustrate, one study found that

kindergarten children are engaged in tasks that require fine-motor skills 46% of the time (Marr,

Page 55: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

55

Cermak, Cohn, & Henderson, 2003). Motor skills have even been found to be more strongly

associated with decoding, reading comprehension, and overall reading gains during

kindergarten than executive function skills (Cameron et al., 2012). Beyond school achievement,

fundamental motor-skills mastery contributes to other life outcomes, as it is associated with

higher cardiovascular fitness and lower weight as children grow older (Lubans, Morgan, Cliff,

Barnett, & Okely, 2010). Finally, perceptual skills including better musical skills and better

musical sensitivity, which can be developed through the use of instruments and materials such

as rhythm sticks, are significantly associated with better reading development and phonological

awareness (Anvari, Trainor, Woodside, & Levy, 2002; Huss, Verney, Fosker, Mead, & Goswami,

2011).

Introducing children to human diversity and a diverse array of cultures is another

important part of the preschool experience because children develop an awareness of

differences in language, skin color, abilities, and customs by the age of three. In addition, they

begin to develop attitudes towards others based on these differences (Grant & Haynes, 1995).

The preschool classroom may be the first setting where children are interacting with children

from a different cultural background than their own. Introducing classroom materials that

allow children to interact with and learn about different cultures can create opportunities for

meaningful interactions and instruction around diversity. The dramatic play area is a part of the

classroom where such materials can be introduced (Kendall, 1983). Culturally relevant dramatic

play centers that include items that represent the cultures of children in the classroom also give

children an opportunity to draw from their home experiences to enhance play (Kirova, 2010;

Salinas-Gonzalez, Arreguín-Anderson, & Alanís, 2018). Teachers should take care to ensure that

Page 56: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

56

the materials they introduce for dramatic play, along with all other classroom materials, do not

reflect cultural biases and are accessible to children of all abilities (Rettig, 2002). In addition to

providing learning opportunities for the class, integrating materials that relate to the individual

uniqueness of the children in the classroom provides environmental clues that children and

their culture are seen as valuable within their classroom community (Cooper-Marcus &

Sarkissian, 1986; Weinstein, 1987).

However, the introduction of materials representing human and cultural diversity into

the classroom, which creates an opportunity for learning about diversity, is not in itself

sufficient for learning without adult support (Salinas-Gonzalez et al., 2018; Souto-Manning,

2013). Children can interact with such materials in ways that act out their existing stereotypes

and biases, therefore intentionality in instruction and in taking advantage of learning moments

that arise during play on the part of the teacher is imperative (Salinas-Gonzalez et al.,

2018). Materials must be integrated into the classroom and their introduction and culturally

appropriate use guided by teachers in intentional ways to promote learning (Kirova, 2010;

Salinas-Gonzalez et al., 2018). This intentional and appropriate use of materials is also true for

other content areas. While abundant materials in the classroom addressing all the above-

mentioned important areas of learning can support preschool children’s development, it is

important to note that the presence of materials is not enough. Children need the opportunity

to use such materials in the context of meaningful interactions with teachers or peers or during

an intentional instructional exercise to fully benefit from their availability (Williford, Vick

Whittaker, Vitiello, & Downer, 2013).

Page 57: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

57

Item I-C: There is a safe outdoor play area with ample space, structures, and materials

to support many types of movement.

Being overweight early in childhood has been found to significantly predict adult obesity

and health issues such as coronary heart disease (Baker, Olsen, & Sørensen,

2007). Furthermore, the development of gross-motor skills in childhood is associated with the

level of physical activity a child engages in, with improved motor skills being associated with

increased physical activity (Laukkanen, Pesola, Havu, Sääkslahti, & Finni, 2014). Improved

motor skills in children have also been found to correlate with maintaining higher levels of

physical activity as children age, as well as with higher health-related fitness levels (Barnett,

Van Beurden, Morgan, Brooks, & Beard, 2008, 2009; Lopes, Rodrigues, Maia, & Malina,

2011). In contrast, delays in motor-skill development are associated with lower perceived

physical competence (Robinson, 2011) and weaker academic achievement in later grades

(Kantomaa et al., 2013). As such, providing opportunities for physical activity and motor

development is important for a number of preschool children’s long-term outcomes.

Research has shown that children prefer to play on playground structures rather than in

open field spaces when such structures are provided (Farley, Meriwether, Baker, Rice, &

Webber, 2008). As the number of structures available to children increases, so do observed

rates of physical activity (Nielsen, Bugge, Hermansen, Svensson, & Andersen, 2012; Sugiyama,

Okely, Masters, & Moore, 2010). Research has demonstrated that both fixed and portable

playground equipment encourage increased rates of activity (Bower et al., 2008). Indeed, the

provision of balls and other portable equipment along with ample open space in which to use

such equipment was found to be more strongly associated with increased rates of moderate to

Page 58: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 58

vigorous physical activity than fixed equipment (Brown et al., 2009). Thus, to support the

development of motor skills and encourage physical activity in young children, outdoor play

spaces should be large enough for all children to engage with equipment and should contain

both fixed structures and portable materials.

Item I-D: Children’s work and environmental print are on display.

Hanging children’s work of all kinds, along with materials that relate to their individual

uniqueness, provides environmental clues that children are seen as important and matter

within their classroom community (Cooper-Marcus & Sarkissian, 1986; Weinstein, 1987). Such

displays have also been found to improve children’s performance on measures of self-esteem

(Maxwell & Chmielewski, 2008). Additionally, hanging children’s work can be used to make

print visible in the classroom environment.

Classrooms that provide a print-rich environment provide children with opportunities to

see print and engage in print-related activities (Phillips, Clancy-Menchetti, & Lonigan,

2008). Recognizing and being exposed to environmental print is a key element of the

development of print awareness, the knowledge that print can be used to represent an object

(Neuman & Roskos, 1990, 1993; Pullen & Justice, 2003). In turn, this is prerequisite knowledge

to reading (Pullen & Justice, 2003). Exposure to environmental print can also improve

children’s ability to read those words in context (Vukelich, 1994). One way to expose children

to print is by using labeling, for example, labeling of interest areas and objects, as it has been

shown that labeling is an important language-stimulation strategy for supporting child language

development (Bunce, 1995; Girolametto & Weitzman, 2002). Exposing children to

environmental print can be used in teaching as well; children whose teachers showed them

Page 59: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

59

examples of letters in familiar environmental print (e.g., S on a stop sign, M on McDonald’s

branding) improved the letter writing and letter recognition more than peers who were only

shown examples in standard manuscript form (Neumann, Hood, & Ford, 2013).

It is important, however, for teachers to note that environmental print does not lead to

word reading on its own (Masonheimer, Drum, & Ehri, 1984; Vukelich, 1994). Likewise, while

high-quality writing and literacy areas did improve alphabet knowledge, they did not improve

name writing without teacher support (Guo, Justice, Kaderavek, & McGinty, 2012).

Nevertheless, print materials and a literacy-rich environment that makes literacy artifacts, such

as functional signs, literacy tools, and literacy-enriched play tools available to children, can

provide opportunities for adult interaction that encourages children to think, speak, and

interact in literate ways (Neuman & Roskos, 1993) besides enhancing their literacy activity in

play (Neuman & Roskos, 1992). Thus, while print materials and a literacy-rich environment are

not a sufficient condition for improvement in children’s literacy skills, they are integral to

facilitating teacher interactions, which can help children build their skills and interact with

environmental print in new ways.

Domain 2: Teaching and Learning Routines and Adult-Child Interactions

Teaching and learning routines that match children’s stage of development are crucial as they

help to build classroom community, ensure efficient use of the available time, help to organize

transitions, create predictability, and allow time for all the necessary elements of the preschool

curriculum in the school day as well as time for spontaneous learning (Williams & Williams,

2001). Within the preschool classroom, high-quality adult-child interactions are important for

Page 60: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 60

learning as they are the crux of children’s social and academic development (e.g., Williford et

al., 2013).

Item II-A: The classroom follows a consistent sequence of events during the school

day.

As young children transition from home to school, they must learn an entirely new set

of routines and expectations. These changes may feel confusing and overwhelming for some

young children (Berk, 1994). When a child feels overwhelmed by a task (such as remembering

what part of the day comes next), he or she may disengage (Bailey & Brooks, 2003) or engage in

challenging behaviors (Stone, 1978). Teachers can support young students by sequencing their

daily routine, displaying the routine in a visual way, and guiding children through transitions

(Bailey & Brooks, 2003). When children know what to expect and can anticipate what part of

the day comes next, they are developing the ability to navigate their classroom environment,

and by doing so, gain independence and self-control (Osborn & Osborn, 1981).

Item II-B: There is time each day for child-initiated activities in the classroom and

during outdoor time.

Young children typically come to school naturally curious and motivated to learn. Yet,

research has shown that, for many, motivation decreases as they enter formal K–12 schooling

(Cordova & Lepper, 1996). One strategy for enhancing motivation, is to allow children to make

choices within the school day (Cordova & Lepper, 1996), for example, what and who to play

with and in the way they use materials. When children are given choices, they are more

motivated to persist in the face of challenges and are more likely to remain engaged in a task

(Cordova & Lepper, 1996). A natural result of student choice is student interest. In turn,

Page 61: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

61

children are better able to learn and retain content that is taught within a context interesting to

them (Hidi, 1990; Lepper & Cordova, 1992). Moreover, when children are given the

responsibility of making choices, they perceive themselves as more competent and aspire to

greater challenges (Cordova & Lepper, 1996). Providing time for children to play is one type of

activity in which children are provided with choice. Therefore, spontaneous play should form

an important part of the preschool curriculum, which may lead to the development of a higher

level of children’s cognitive functions (Almy, 1967; Umek & Musek, 2001). To sustain

sociodramatic and constructive play, children need sufficient, uninterrupted time for at least 30

minutes (Ward, 1996). Children need this time to generate ideas, assign roles, find materials,

communicate, negotiate, and enact dramatizations.

Another part of the day that is especially ripe with opportunities for children to pursue

their interests is unstructured outdoor playtime. Playing outside provides children with deep

and authentic opportunities for learning (Bilton, 2002). Given the right physical environment

and collective expectations, a variety of play types emerges during outdoor play (Maxwell,

Mitchell, & Evans, 2008). Fantasy play, functional play, constructive play, and rule-based games

benefit children’s cognitive, social, and emotional development (Maxwell et al., 2008). Experts

at the Society of Health and Physical Educators recommend that children spend 60 minutes

engaging in unstructured play outside in order to receive its benefits (Committee on Physical

Activity and Physical Education in the School Environment, Food and Nutrition Board, Institute

of Medicine, Kohl & Cook, 2013). In order to maximize the benefits of unstructured play,

teachers and school staff should structure the physical environment in such a way as to

encourage physical activity behaviors (Bower et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2009) by maximizing

Page 62: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 62

opportunities for children to be active and utilizing a variety of appropriate equipment,

interactional strategies, and high-quality training for staff (EPAO, as cited in Bower et al., 2008).

Item II-C: Adults support children’s ideas, actions, and developmental levels during

child-initiated activities.

Allowing children time to engage in child-initiated activities bolsters child outcomes

(Graue, Clements, Reynolds, & Niles, 2004). During child-initiated activities, children have

discretion in how they complete activities and choose peers to work with, and they can opt out

of activities (Stipek, Daniels, Galluzzo, & Milburn, 1992). Play is an example of a child-initiated

activity and Vygotsky viewed play, when scaffolded, as being important for learning.

Scaffolding occurs when an adult or more knowledgeable other (e.g., a peer) assists the child in

performing at a higher level than would be possible without support and, in this way, extends

his or her knowledge and learning during play (Han, Moore, Vukelich, & Buell, 2010). Adult

scaffolding during play is important as it encourages children to learn self-regulation,

cooperation, memory, language use, and literacy (Bodrova & Leong, as cited in Han et al.,

2010).

Item II-D: There is time each day for adult-initiated, large-group activities that support

each child’s developmental level and Item II-E: There is time each day for adult-initiated,

small-group activities that support each child’s developmental level.

In a recent study, conducted by researchers at the Institute for Social Research, findings

suggest that one of the biggest predictors of elementary students’ success in math and reading

is his or her teacher (RAND Education, 2012). Similarly, high-quality early education can

significantly erase or minimize the achievement gaps that exist for many of our children

Page 63: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

63

(Barnett, 2011; Camilli, Vargas, Ryan, & Barnett, 2010; Pianta, Barnett, Burchinal, & Thornburg,

2009) and one way to maximize the benefits of these early education experiences is by

purposefully planning for instruction that meets the needs of each learner. In fact, teacher-

directed activities, used in conjunction with child-initiated activities, have been shown to

further support children’s development and contribute to child outcomes (Graue et al., 2004).

Teacher-directed activities include large-group and also small-group activities or settings

in which learning can take place. The competence of teachers to organize children in various

settings has a large impact on children’s learning (Sheridan, Williams, & Samuelsson, 2014) and

may be more important than the nature of the setting itself. In these teacher-directed or adult-

initiated activities, as in child-initiated activities, adults can scaffold children’s

learning. Scaffolding and modeling by adults help children to learn new knowledge when the

task, skills, or concept that is introduced is moderately challenging rather than being too easy

or too difficult (Acar, Hong, & Wu, 2017).

The use of a variety of formats in preschool is advocated in the position statement of

the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) on developmentally

appropriate practice, which recommends the use of large-group instruction as well (NAEYC,

2009). DiCarlo, Pierce, Baumgartner, Harris, and Ota (2012) emphasize that various teacher

behaviors, such as acknowledging children’s responses and providing relevant materials during

large-group instruction, predict preschool children’s attentiveness and results in more effective

large-group instruction.

Lou, Abrami, and Spence (2000) make the case that planning for small-group instruction

is a highly effective way to support each student. Children in groups of between four and five

Page 64: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 64

were shown to learn more than children in larger groups. In addition to the academic benefits

small groups provide, small groups also provide opportunities for students to receive more

individualized attention from their teacher. When teachers listen to and know their students as

individuals, they are better able to adapt their responses to fit that particular child’s needs

(Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). By tracking the children’s needs, teachers can follow their

development and intentionally plan for ways to provide moderate challenges (Tomlinson,

2001). Brain research has shown that engaging in tasks just outside of one’s zone of proximal

development, as defined by Vygotsky (1978), stimulates enduring understanding.

Item II-F: Adults create a sensitive and responsive learning environment for all

children.

High-quality interactions are those in which the teacher responds to the child’s needs in

a timely and appropriate manner (Howes & Hamilton, 1992). The teacher remains gentle and

warm and finds ways to personalize the interaction to fit the individual needs of the child

(Hamre & Pianta, 2005; Pianta, Hamre, & Stuhlman, 2003). Such relationships are formed

through consistently positive interactions between children and adults. A high-quality

relationship with a teacher in early childhood supports the development of social, emotional,

and cognitive skills (Goosens & Van IJzendoorn, 1990). Moreover, a warm, positive relationship

between teachers and children can predict whether a child is sufficiently well adjusted later in

school (Pianta, Steinberg, & Rollins, 1995) and results in children having greater academic

success throughout elementary school (Hamre & Pianta, 2001). In addition, social-emotional

advantages can be gained from high-quality relationships with teachers, including engaging in

Page 65: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

65

less destructive behavior (Howes, Matheson, & Hamilton, 1994) and being better liked by peers

(Hamre & Pianta, 2001).

Providing positive comments is one way through which teachers can establish positive,

high-quality relationships. However, praise is commonly given when a task is well done or

completed, for example, “What a beautiful painting!” This kind of praise lowers children’s

confidence, their expectations of success when working on difficult tasks, and leads to

dependency of children on an authority figure to tell them what is right or wrong (Hitz &

Driscoll, 1988). Instead, teachers can use encouragement, which refers to positive

acknowledgement that focuses on specific student efforts or specific attributes of the work

completed, for example, “You worked for a long time on that painting!” As visible in those

examples, encouragement does not place judgement on student work or give information

regarding its value. Rather, encouragement avoids labeling or interpreting the work and

focuses on the process rather than the evaluation of a finished product (Hitz & Driscoll, 1988).

This helps children develop an appreciation of their own behaviors and achievements.

Item II-G: Adults encourage and support children to make plans and reflect upon their

work.

Planning is a key component of executive functioning and is an important metacognitive

process since it increases students’ success in learning (Crook & Evans, 2014; Fisher,

1998). Hudson and Fivush (1991) state that age is a factor affecting children’s planning abilities.

As children grow to be five years of age, they are better able to think flexibly about their plans

and adapt them when needed (Hudson, Shapiro, & Sosa, 1995). When teachers support

children in making plans and scaffold their individual attempts to plan, they are providing the

Page 66: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 66

child with opportunities to practice and become better planners (Hudson et al., 1995). This is

particularly important for children from low-income households who may experience greater

family turmoil, live in households that are noisy or crowded, and have fewer structured

routines and rituals. The household chaos that may result from these circumstances, coupled

with overburdened or stressed parents, can interfere with the development of young children’s

planning skills (Crook & Evans, 2014).

Recall or reflection time is the practice of mediating children’s thinking in order to

reflect on a past event. In the sequence of the daily routine, recall time follows work or

playtime, which is preceded by planning time. In this way, children plan and reflect around the

events of their playtime. By virtue of play time’s highly interesting and personalized nature,

children are increasingly motivated to think more deeply (Cordova & Lepper, 1996). However,

while children in preschool learn and retain a great deal of information, they are less capable at

retrieving relevant information independently, even within a familiar context (Perlmutter,

Sophian, Mitchell, & Cavanaugh, 1981). Teachers and other supportive adults must then

facilitate thoughtful opportunities for children to reflect on past experiences. Using cues such

as colors, areas of play, or other descriptions, teachers can help children generate more

comprehensive reflections than an independent or “free” recall might (Perlmutter et al., 1981).

Teachers might also support children by modeling the reflective thinking process using a “think

aloud” for children to view the internal process of reflection in context (Lindfors, 1999).

Page 67: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

67

Item II-H: Adults support children’s language and literacy development throughout the

day.

The preschool years are crucial to the development of children’s emergent literacy skills

that will ensure a smooth transition into formal reading (Pullen & Justice, 2003). Moreover, the

development of emergent literacy skills is imperative, as various aspects of emergent literacy

have been found to predict later academic achievement (e.g., Bleses, Makransky, Dale, Højen,

& Ari, 2016). Therefore, preschool teachers have the critical task to foster children’s emergent

literacy. Important interrelated aspects of emergent literacy are letter knowledge,

phonological awareness, vocabulary development, comprehension skills, and writing skills.

Children’s letter knowledge, the most basic skill of knowing the names of letters

(Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998), as well as their letter-sound knowledge, are emergent literacy

skills that are important for later literacy (e.g., Lonigan, Burgess, & Anthony, 2000). Teachers

can support the development of alphabetic knowledge and letter-sound associations by adult-

mediation during play aimed at focusing on letters and their sounds (Pullen & Justice, 2003).

Related to letter and letter-sound knowledge is phonological awareness. Phonological

awareness, or the knowledge that spoken words are comprised of various phonological units, is

important for decoding, which children need for learning to read (Pullen & Justice, 2003). At

preschool age, children’s phonological awareness may be supported by using rhyme and

alliteration during instruction or while playing as this helps children focus on the phonological

structure of spoken language (Pullen & Justice, 2003). Blending (e.g., blending syllables into

words) and segmenting activities (e.g., counting syllables in words) are other ways to support

children’s phonological awareness (Pullen & Justice, 2003).

Page 68: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 68

Children’s vocabulary development is also related to later reading and can be supported

in many ways, for example, teachers can provide an ongoing description of their own activities

or thoughts during storybook reading, or repeat or expand upon children’s utterances (Pullen &

Justice, 2003). To illustrate, Dickinson and Smith (1994) revealed the effects of preschool

teachers’ book reading on low-income children’s vocabulary as well as on their comprehension.

Children’s comprehension skills, as result of storybook reading, can be improved if teachers ask

open-ended, high-level questions during reading and encourage children’s active participation

(McGee & Schickedanz, 2007). Additionally, letting children retell stories supports their

comprehension skills (Morrow, 1985).

Early or emergent writing is another foundational literacy skill that is important for

children’s later reading success (National Early Literacy Panel [NELP], 2008) and contributes to

the development of letter knowledge and phonological awareness (Cabell, Tortorelli, & Gerde,

2013). Children’s writing provides teachers with a window into what they know in regard to

phonological awareness (Cabell et al., 2013). Teachers can use that knowledge to inform their

instruction around writing and support each child’s writing efforts. Teachers can incorporate

writing into play activities, prompt children to draw and write about themselves, and point out

the letters in children’s names. Moreover, teachers can model writing skills by explicitly stating

each step they make in constructing a text, for example, pointing out how a letter should begin

with a greeting like “Hi” or “Hello”, which is useful for children’s acquisition of writing skills

(Mayer, 2007). In sum, providing preschool children with rich writing experiences can

contribute to laying a foundation for literacy learning (Cabell et al., 2013).

Item II-I: Adults support children’s mathematics development throughout the day.

Page 69: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

69

Young children instinctively compare quantities, observe, make patterns, and problem-

solve in their play interactions with objects and peers (Linder, Powers-Costello, & Stegelin,

2011). A preschool teacher can act as a facilitator to make the child’s informal connections to

mathematics more explicit. Scaffolding and supporting early numeracy skills and general

mathematics development in young children is important given the strong association between

those early skills and later mathematical achievement (Raghubar & Barnes, 2017).

One aspect of early numeracy is subitizing, which means that a young child can

determine the number of small sets without counting or, in other words, instantly (Linder et al.,

2011). Preschool teachers may support children’s subitizing skills through modeling. While the

number of small sets can be determined by subitizing, counting is the only way to determine

the exact number of small sets (Le Corre, Van de Walle, Brannon, & Carey, 2006). For this,

children need to manage one-to-one correspondence, the basic skill of mapping each number

word onto each item in a set (Raghubar & Barnes, 2017) which can be supported through

counting together with children (Linder et al., 2011). One-to-one correspondence is central to

the concept of cardinality (Sophian, 1988). Cardinality represents children’s understanding that

the last number word used when counting a set indicates the number of objects in that

particular set (Raghubar & Barnes, 2017). Children who demonstrate cardinality therefore have

a more sophisticated understanding of numbers than children who only count a set using one-

to-one correspondence (Linder et al., 2011). Acquiring the cardinality principle is important as it

is related to mathematical achievement at the end of preschool (Chu, Van Marle, & Geary,

2015) and is necessary for knowing the meaning of symbols and number words (Raghubar &

Page 70: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 70

Barnes, 2017). In turn, symbolic number knowledge in the preschool years has been reliably

associated with later mathematics achievement (Göbel, Watson, Lervåg, & Hulme, 2014).

Using measurement attributes to compare objects, such as quantity and length, is

another fundamental component of children’s mathematics development (Gerde, Schachter, &

Wasik, 2013). Preschool teachers can teach children about measurement by providing them

opportunities to learn about the physical properties of objects. One way to do this is by letting

children make comparisons and by asking them guiding questions such as which of a set of

objects is the tallest (Notari-Syverson & Sadler, 2008). The use of tools such as blocks and

containers can be useful in this process. Another aspect of children’s mathematics development

in which teachers can take a supporting role is the knowledge of a variety of shapes. For

example, teachers can talk with children about whether or not shapes roll and, in this process,

elicit more information from children by asking questions (Gerde et al., 2013).

Item II-J: Adults support children’s reasoning and problem-solving throughout the day.

Problem solving, a way to reach a goal that is not immediately attainable, is natural to

young children because the world is new to them and, accordingly, they exhibit curiosity,

intelligence, and flexibility as they encounter new situations (Tarim, 2009). It is important to

build upon children’s innate problem-solving tendencies by supporting and scaffolding

children’s reasoning in the problem-solving process.

One way in which teachers can support children’s reasoning is by asking open-ended

questions that allow children to participate fully in the co-construction of an activity (French,

2004). This may help children to think about scientific concepts. While posing open-ended

questions, preschool teachers can introduce scientific language and model forms for asking,

Page 71: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

71

planning, describing, and explaining (French, 2004). Additionally, it is important that teachers

engage children in and teach them how to observe, question, predict, and summarize to draw

conclusions (Gerde et al., 2013), which may help improve their problem-solving skills. Also,

letting children talk about this scientific process can increase their ability to talk about a range

of scientific concepts (French, 2004).

Item II-K: Adults encourage thoughtful social interaction among all children

throughout the day.

Children’s social development, the ability to form positive relationships with adults and

peers, is important to children’s readiness for school and is correlated with positive academic

outcomes (Hair, Halle, Terry-Humen, Lavelle, & Calkins, 2006; La Paro & Pianta, 2000). Peer

interactions are of particular importance in preschool and teachers can support them by

providing children with positive feedback about their social interactions and fostering

cooperative play (Brown & Odom, 1995). When children spend too much time interacting with

teachers or other adults, they have fewer opportunities to benefit from interaction with peers

(Johnson, Ironsmith, Snow, & Poteat, 2000).

Item II-L: Adults diffuse conflicts and support all children in resolving conflicts.

Disagreements between peers represent a critical developmental challenge. The

teacher’s role in resolving children’s conflicts is crucial since learning to deal with conflict

promotes socially competent behaviors (Vestal & Jones, 2004). Teachers can diffuse conflict

situations to prevent them from escalating but teachers can also act as facilitators by helping

children to resolve their own conflicts (Chen, 2003). The latter involves assisting children in

identifying the problem, articulating feelings, and generating and accordingly implementing

Page 72: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 72

mutually agreeable solutions (Bayer et al., in Chen, 2003). In fact, it has been shown that

children in preschool classrooms of teachers who used conflict resolution strategies had a

higher number of relevant conflict solutions — which suggests they were able to think of more

ways to solve interpersonal conflicts — than children whose teachers were not trained to enact

conflict resolution strategies (Vestal & Aaron Jones, 2004). This study showed that children can

be taught to think of more relevant conflict solutions when confronted with an interpersonal

conflict.

Domain 3: Curriculum, Planning, Assessment and Parent Engagement

Preschool teachers’ tasks and responsibilities are not limited to providing daily routines

and engaging children in high-quality learning experiences but extend to professional

responsibilities beyond the classroom. These professional responsibilities entail collaboratively

planning teaching based on evidence-based teaching approaches, recording child progress and

using it to inform instructional planning, and planning for and engaging parents in their

children’s educational efforts. Taken together, these responsibilities positively impact child

development and future outcomes.

Item III-A: Adults use a comprehensive, evidence-based educational

model(s)/approach(es) to guide teaching practices.

A promising strategy for increasing the impact of early childhood education involves

implementing in preschool classrooms evidence-based teaching strategies and curricula that

build skills progressively in various specific learning domains (Bierman, Heinrichs, Welsh, Nix, &

Gest, 2017). To illustrate, an evidence-based preschool curriculum has been found to foster

language and literacy as well as social-emotional skills (Nix, Bierman, Domitrovich, & Gill, 2013).

Page 73: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

73

In addition, to achieve successful inclusion of all children, teachers should adapt the preschool

curriculum for children with special needs to their developmental level (Ahmetoğlu, 2015;

Sucuoğlu, Bakkaloğlu, İşcen Karasu, Demir, & Akalin, 2013).

Item III-B: Adults document the developmental progress of each child using measures

validated for preschool-aged children.

The need for high-quality assessment in every preschool should not be questioned. To

this aim, it is important that preschool children’s assessments are developmentally appropriate

and sensitive to the developmental characteristics of young children that relate to testing, such

as the ability to understand instructions (Teale, 1988). Observations are one way to assess

children’s development and growth and various measures can be used to guide child

observations. In fact, observations may be an even more appropriate assessment method for

preschool-aged children than standardized testing, as they are intrusive and often performed

under artificial testing conditions (Teale, 1988). In turn, assessment information is necessary to

inform instructional decisions and may allow for early intervention for the children who are in

need of this (Bayoglu, Bakar, Kutlu, Karabulut, & Anlar, 2007; Teale, 1988).

Item III-C: Adults record and use anecdotal notes to create lesson plans that are

connected to learning goals and focused on learning through developmentally appropriate

practices (play).

Anecdotal notes are written descriptions that provide a short, factual, objective,

nonjudgmental account of an event or incident and do not include a teachers’ interpretation of

the event (Wortham, Barbour, & Desjean-Perrotta, 1998). These anecdotal notes can be used

to document the skills and development they observe in the preschool classroom (Ratcliff,

Page 74: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

74

2001; Wortham et al., 1998). An advantage of these notes over checklists is the richness and

depth that could be documented in them. This type of information can be used to provide

insights that may help the teacher more fully understand a child’s behavior, development, or

use of skills (Ratcliff, 2001). Specifically, this form of embedded, authentic assessment provides

information about children’s strengths and challenges and provides insight into how children

learn and what they are learning (Wortham et al., 1998). Such information may be used to

support summative assessment decisions. In addition, the information contained in these notes

may be used to guide the selection of certain activities or teaching approaches (Rhodes &

Nathenson-Meija, 1992; Wortham et al., 1998).

Preschool teachers may document the planning of classroom activities in lesson plans. In

lesson plans, goals and objectives are chosen from yearly plans that state the objectives for

children to attain during a whole year (Erden, 2010). Accordingly, these lesson plans should

involve objectives appropriate for the age of preschool children as well as various activities,

which can be either teacher- or child-directed, aimed to achieve these objectives (Erden, 2010;

Justice, 2004).

The development of lesson plans, which can be done in advance, for example per week,

is a responsibility of the entire team in preschool (Justice, 2004). The entire team must be

knowledgeable about the structure and content of the plan. Furthermore, it is important that

lesson plans and the objectives included in them are developmentally appropriate for

preschool-aged children. Lesson plans can be used to enhance school readiness, but it should

be realized that young children learn differently from older children or adults and their ways of

making sense of the world rely heavily on play (Nicolopoulou, 2010). It is developmentally

Page 75: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

75

inappropriate to enact an exclusively a top-down, directive approach for preschool-aged

children rather than incorporating play in lesson plans. Instead, incorporating play is

appropriate as play is not simply trivial, but is an intensely absorbing activity that serves as a

source for learning and development (Nicolopoulou, 2010).

Item III-D: Adults provide many family engagement options, encourage two-way

sharing of child information, and support families with program transitioning.

Parent engagement in preschool is essential for children’s development and

achievement through its influence on parent-child interactions as well as for parent-teacher

relationships (e.g., Ma, Shen, Krenn, Hu, & Yuan, 2016; Mendez, 2010; Nix, Bierman, Motamedi,

Heinrichs, & Gill, 2018). In fact, the meta-analysis of Ma et al. (2016) showed that the role of

parental involvement in influencing learning outcomes was more important than the role of

communities and schools. This home-school connection is particularly important for children

and families from disadvantaged backgrounds who often lack exposure to high-quality

resources (Mendez, 2010). Parental engagement involves participation of parents in classroom

activities and the exchange of information between parents and teachers about a child’s

learning, behavior, and accomplishments (Fantuzzo et al., 2013; Ma et al., 2016). Also, parents

may learn from teachers how to help their child at home, for example, by using resources and

materials that are shared with parents (Fantuzzo et al., 2013; Mendez, 2010; Nix et al.,

2018). Sharing information about children’s development is of importance during transitions to

enhance continuity between preschool, school, and home. In turn, this is important for the

prevention of difficulties in transitions. For example, Alatalo, Meier, and Frank (2017) illustrate

Page 76: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 76

that sharing information about children’s literacy learning during the transition from preschool

to elementary school is perceived by teachers as being important in this transition.

Page 77: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

77

References

Acar, I. H., Hong, S.-Y., & Wu, C. (2017). Examining the role of teacher presence and scaffolding

in preschoolers’ peer interactions. European Early Childhood Education Research

Journal, 25, 866–884. doi:10.1080/1350293X.2017.1380884

Ahmetoğlu, E. (2015). Inclusion at preschool period. In I. Koleva, R. Efe, Z. Blagoeva Kostova, &

E. Atasoy (Eds.), Education in the 21st century: Theory and practice (pp. 278–296). Sofia,

BG: St. Kliment Ohridski University Press.

Alatalo, T., Meier, J., & Frank, E. (2017). Information sharing on children’s literacy learning in

the transition from Swedish preschool to school. Journal of Research in Childhood

Education, 31, 240–254. doi:10.1080/02568543.2016.1274926

Almy, M. (1967). Spontaneous play: An avenue for intellectual development. Young Children,

22, 264–277. Retrieved from https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

Anders, Y., Rossbach, H.-G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehr, S., & Von Maurice, J. (2012).

Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of

early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 231–244.

doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003

Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., & Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among musical skills,

phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 83(2), 111–130. doi:10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00124-8

Bailey, B., & Brookes, C. (2003). Thinking out loud development of private speech and the

implications for school and self-control. Young Children, 58(5), 46–52. Retrieved from

https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

Page 78: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 78

Baker, J. L., Olsen, L. W., & Sørensen, T. I. (2007). Childhood body-mass index and the risk of

coronary heart disease in adulthood. New England Journal of Medicine, 357(23), 2329–

2337. doi:10.1016/s0084-3741(08)79187-x

Barnett, W. S. (2011). Effectiveness of early childhood intervention. Science, 333, 975–978.

doi:10.1126/ science.1204534

Barnett, L. M., Van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., & Beard, J. R. (2008). Does

childhood motor skill proficiency predict adolescent fitness? Medicine & Science in

Sports & Exercise, 40(12), 2137–2144. doi:10.1249/mss.0b013e31818160d3

Barnett, L. M., Van Beurden, E., Morgan, P. J., Brooks, L. O., & Beard, J. R. (2009). Childhood

motor skill proficiency as a predictor of adolescent physical activity. Journal of

Adolescent Health, 44(3), 252–259. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.07.004

Bayoglu, B. U., Bakar, E. E., Kutlu, M., Karabulut, E., & Anlar, B. (2007). Can preschool

developmental screening identify children at risk for school problems? Early Human

Development, 83, 613–617. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.12.001

Berk, L. (1994). Vygotsky’s theory: The importance of make-believe play. Young Children 50(1),

30. Retrieved from https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

Bierman, K. L., Heinrichs, B. S., Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., & Gest, S. D. (2017). Enriching preschool

classrooms and home visits with evidence-based programming: Sustained benefits for

low-income children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 129–137.

doi:10.1111/jcpp.12618

Bilton, H. (2002). Outdoor play in the early years. London, UK: David Fulton.

Page 79: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

79

Bleses, D., Makransky, G., Dale, P. S., Højen, A., & Ari, B. A. (2016). Early productive vocabulary

predicts academic achievement 10 years later. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 1461–1476.

doi:10.1017/S0142716416000060

Bower, J. K., Hales, D. P., Tate, D. F., Rubin, D. A., Benjamin, S. E., & Ward, D. S. (2008). The

childcare environment and children’s physical activity. American Journal of Preventive

Medicine, 34(1), 23–29. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.022

Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood

programs (Rev. ed.). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young

Children:

Brenneman, K., Stevenson-Boyd, J., & Frede, E. C. (2009). Math and science in preschool:

Policies and practice. Preschool Policy Brief, 19, 1–12. Retrieved from www.nieer.org/

Brown, W. H., & Odom, S. L. (1995). Naturalistic peer interventions for promoting preschool

children’s social interactions. Preventing School Failure, 39, 38–43. doi:10.1080/

1045988x.1995.9944641

Brown, W. H., Pfeiffer, K. A., McIver, K. L., Dowda, M., Addy, C. L., & Pate, R. R. (2009). Social

and environmental factors associated with preschoolers’ nonsedentary physical activity.

Child Development, 80(1), 45–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01245.x

Bunce, B. H. (1995). Building a language-focused curriculum for the preschool classroom.

Volume II: A Planning Guide. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

Cabell, S., Tortorelli, L., & Gerde, H. (2013). How do I write…? Scaffolding preschoolers’ early

writing skills. The Reading Teacher, 66, 650–659. doi:10.1002/TRTR.1173

Page 80: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

80

Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., & Morrison,

F. J. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten

achievement. Child Development, 83(4), 1229–1244. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2012.01768.x

Camilli, G., Vargas, S., Ryan, S., & Barnett, W. S. (2010). Meta-analysis of the effects of early

education interventions on cognitive and social development. Teachers college record,

112(3), 579–620. Retrieved from https://www.tcrecord.org/

Carbonneau, K. J., Marley, S. C., & Selig, J. P. (2013). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of teaching

mathematics with concrete manipulatives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2),

380–400. doi:10.1037/a0031084

Chu, F. W., Van Marle, K., & Geary, D. C. (2015). Early numberical foundations of young

children’s mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132,

205–212. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.006

Chen, D. W. (2003). Preventing violence by promoting the development of competent conflict

resolution skills: Exploring roles and responsibilities. Early Childhood Education Journal,

30, 203–208. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/journal/10643

Committee on Physical Activity and Physical Education in the School Environment., Food and

Nutrition Board., Institute of Medicine., Kohl H. W. III., & Cook H. D. (2013). The

effectiveness of physical activity and physical education policies and programs:

Summary of the evidence. In H. W. Kohl III & H. D. Cook (Eds.), Educating the student

body: Taking physical activity and physical Education to school (pp. 311–365).

Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

Page 81: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

81

Cooper-Marcus, C., & Sarkissian, W. (1986). Housing as if people mattered: Site guidelines for

medium-density family housing. Berkeley: CA: University of California Press.

Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning:

Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 88, 715–730. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.4.715

Crook, S. R., & Evans, G. W. (2014). The role of planning skills in the income-achievement gap.

Child Development, 85, 405–411. doi:10.1111/cdev.12129

Datnow, A., Park, V., & Wohlstetter, P. (2007). Achieving with data: How high-performing school

systems use data to improve instruction for elementary students. Los Angeles, CA:

Center on Educational Governance – University of Southern California.

DiCarlo, C. F., Pierce, S. H., Baumgartner, J., Harris, M. E., & Ota, C. (2012). Whole-group

instruction practices and children’s attention: A preliminary report. Journal of Research

in Childhood Education, 26, 154–168. doi:10.1080/02568543.2012.657744

Dickinson, D. K., & Smith, M. W. (1994). Long-term effects of preschool teachers’ book readings

on low-income children’s vocabulary and story comprehension. Reading Research

Quarterly, 29, 104–122. doi:10.2307/747807

Dunst, C. J., McWilliam, R. A., & Holbert, K. (1986). Assessment of preschool classroom

environments. Diagnostique, 11(3-4), 212–232. doi:10.1177/073724778601100306

Eccles, J. (1997). User-friendly science and mathematics: Can it interest girls and minorities in

breaking through the middle school wall. In D. Johnson (Ed.), Minorities and girls in

school: Effects on achievement and performance (pp. 65–104). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Page 82: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

82

Ehrlich, S. B., Pacchiano, D., Stein, A. G., Wagner, M. R., Park, S., Frank, E., … Young. C. (2018).

Early education essentials: Validation of surveys measuring early education

organizational conditions. Early Education and Development, 30, 540–567. doi:10.1080/

10409289.2018.1556969

Erden, E. (2010). Problems that teachers face in the curriculum implementation. (Unpublished

master’s thesis). Middle East Technical University: Ankara, TU

National Institute for Early Education Research. (2002, November). High-Quality Preschool: Why

we need it and what it looks like (Policy Brief No. 1). New Brunswick, NJ: Espinosa, L.M.

Fantuzzo, J., Gadsen, V., Li, F., Sproul, F., McDermott, P., Hightower, D., & Minney, A. (2013).

Multiple dimensions of family engagement in early childhood education: Evidence for a

short form of the Family Involvement Questionnaire. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly, 28, 734–742. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.001

Farley, T. A., Meriwether, R. A., Baker, E. T., Rice, J. C., & Webber, L. S. (2008). Where do the

children play? The influence of playground equipment on physical activity of children in

free play. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 5(2), 319–331. doi:10.1123/jpah.5.2.

319

Field, T. M. (1980). Preschool play: Effects of teacher/child ratios and organization of classroom

space. Child Study Journal, 10, 191–205. Retrieved from

https://catalyst.library.jhu.edu/catalog/bib_3471542

Fisher, R. (1998). Thinking about thinking: Developing metacognition in children. Early Child

Development and Care, 141, 1–15. doi:10.1080/0300443981410101

Page 83: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

83

French, L. (2004). Science as the center of a coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum.

Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 138–149. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.004

Gerde, H. K., Schachter, R. E., & Wasik, B. A. (2013). Using the scientific method to guide

learning: An integrated approach to early childhood curriculum. Early Childhood

Education Journal, 41, 315–323. doi:10.1007/s10643-013-0579-4

Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions

with toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

33(4), 268–281. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2002/022)

Göbel, S. M., Watson, S. E., Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2014). Children’s arithmetic development:

It is number knowledge, not the approximate number sense that counts. Psychological

Science, 25, 789–798. doi:10.1177/0956797613516471

Goossens, F. A., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1990). Quality of infants’ attachments to professional

caregivers: Relation to infant‐parent attachment and day‐care characteristics. Child

Development, 61(3), 832–837. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02825.x

Grant, D., & Haynes, D. (1995). A developmental framework for cultural competence training

with children. Children & Schools, 17(3), 171–182. doi:10.1093/cs/17.3.171

Graue, E., Clements, M. A., Reynolds, A. J., & Niles, M. D. (2004). More than teacher directed or

child initiated: Preschool curriculum type, parent involvement, and children’s outcomes

in the child-parent centers. Education Policy Analysis Archives, 12(75), 1–38.

doi:10.14507/epaa.v12n72.2004

Page 84: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

84

Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills

and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators.

Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017. doi:10.1037/a0020104

Guo, Y., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., & McGinty, A. (2012). The literacy environment of

preschool classrooms: Contributions to children’s emergent literacy growth. Journal of

Research in Reading, 35(3), 308–327. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01467.x

Hair, E., Halle, T., Terry-Humen, E., Lavelle, B., & Calkins, J. (2006). Children’s school readiness in

the ECL-K: Predictions to academic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early

Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 431–454. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.09. 005

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of

children's school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72(2), 625–638.

doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00301

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional and emotional support in the first‐grade

classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Child Development,

76(5), 949–967. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00889.x

Han, M., Moore, N., Vukelich, C., & Buell, M. (2010). Does play make a difference? How play

intervention affects the vocabulary learning of at-risk preschoolers. American Journal of

Play, 3, 82–105. Retrieved from https://www.journalofplay.org/

Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of

Educational Research, 60(4), 549–571. doi:10.2307/1170506

Page 85: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

85

Hitz, R., & Driscoll, A. (1988). Praise or encouragement? New insights into praise: Implications

for early childhood teachers. Young Children, 43, 6–13. Retrieved from

https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1992). Children’s relationships with child care teachers: Stability

and concordance with parental attachments. Child Development, 63(4), 867–878.

doi:10. 2307/1131239

Howes, C., Matheson, C. C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1994). Maternal, teacher, and child care history

correlates of children’s relationships with peers. Child Development, 65(1), 264–273.

doi:10.2307/1131380

Hudson, J. A., & Fivush, R. (1991). Planning in the preschool years: The emergence of plans from

general event knowledge. Cognitive Development, 6(4), 393–415. doi:10.1016/ 0885-

2014(91)90046-g

Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., & Sosa, B. B. (1995). Planning in the real world: Preschool children’s

scripts and plans for familiar events. Child Development, 66(4), 984–998.

doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00917.x

Hughes, F. P. (2009). Children, play, and development (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Huss, M., Verney, J. P., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Goswami, U. (2011). Music, rhythm, rise time

perception and developmental dyslexia: perception of musical meter predicts reading

and phonology. Cortex, 47(6), 674–689. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.010

Inan, H. Z. (2009). The third dimension in preschools: Preschool environments and classroom

design. European Journal of Educational Studies, 1(1), 55–66. Retrieved from

https://oapub.org/edu/index.php/ejes

Page 86: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 86

Johnson, C., Ironsmith, M., Snow, C. W., & Poteat, G. M. (2000). Acceptance and social

adjustment in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood Education Journal, 27, 207–

212. doi:10.1023/b:ecej.0000003356.30481.7a

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters:

Kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental

Psychology, 45(3), 850–867. doi:10.1037/a0014939

Justice, L. M. (2004). Creating language-rich preschool classroom environments. Teaching

Exceptional Children, 37, 36–44. doi:10.1177/004005990403700205

Kantomaa, M. T., Stamatakis, E., Kankaanpää, A., Kaakinen, M., Rodriguez, A., Taanila, A., . . .

Tammelin, T. (2013). Physical activity and obesity mediate the association between

childhood motor function and adolescents’ academic achievement. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences, 110(5), 1917–1922. doi:10.1073/pnas.1214574110

Kendall, F. E. (1983). Early Childhood Education Series: Diversity in the classroom: A

multicultural approach to the education of young children. New York, NY: Teachers

College Press.

Kirova, A. (2010). Children’s representations of cultural scripts in play: Facilitating transition

from home to preschool in an intercultural early learning program for refugee children.

Diaspora, Indigenous, and Minority Education, 4(2), 74–91. doi:10.1080/

15595691003635765

La Paro, K. M., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). Predicting children’s competence in the early school

years: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 70, 443–484. doi:10.

3102/00346543070004443

Page 87: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

87

Laukkanen, A., Pesola, A., Havu, M., Sääkslahti, A., & Finni, T. (2014). Relationship between

habitual physical activity and gross motor skills is multifaceted in 5- to 8-year-old

children. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 24(2), 102–110.

doi:10.1111/sms.12116

Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E. M., & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the

competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles. Cognitive

Psychology, 52, 130–169. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.07.002

Lepper, M. R., & Cordova, D. I. (1992). A desire to be taught: Instructional consequences of

intrinsic motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 16(3), 187–208. doi:10.1007/bf00991651

Linder, S. M., Powers-Costello, B., & Stegelin, D. A. (2011). Mathematics in early childhood:

Research-based rationale and practical strategies. Early Childhood Education Journal, 39,

29–37. doi:10.1007/s10643-010-0437-6

Lindfors, J. W. (1999). Language and Literacy Series: Children’s inquiry: Using language to make

sense of the world. Williston, VT: Teachers College Press.

Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and

early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal

study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613. doi:10.1037//OOI2-1649.36.5.596

Lopes, V. P., Rodrigues, L. P., Maia, J. A., & Malina, R. M. (2011). Motor coordination as

predictor of physical activity in childhood. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in

Sports, 21(5), 663–669. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.2009.01027.x

Page 88: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 88

Lou, Y., Abrami, P. C., & Spence, J. C. (2000). Effects of within-class grouping on student

achievement: An exploratory model. The Journal of Educational Research, 94(2), 101–

112. doi:10.1080/00220670009598748

Lubans, D. R., Morgan, P. J., Cliff, D. P., Barnett, L. M., & Okely, A. D. (2010). Fundamental

movement skills in children and adolescents. Sports Medicine, 40(12), 1019–1035.

doi:10.2165/11536850-000000000-00000

Ma, X., Shen, J., Krenn, H. Y., Hu, S., & Yuan, J. (2016). A meta-analysis of the relationship

between learning outcomes and parental involvement during early childhood education

and early elementary education. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 771–801. doi:10.

1007/s10648-015-9351-1

Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Fine motor activities in Head Start and

kindergarten classrooms. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57(5), 550–557.

doi:10.5014/ajot.57.5.550

Masonheimer, P. E., Drum, P. A., & Ehri, L. C. (1984). Does environmental print identification

lead children into word reading? Journal of Reading Behavior, 16(4), 257–271. doi:10.

1080/10862968409547520

Maxwell, L. E., & Chmielewski, E. J. (2008). Environmental personalization and elementary

school children’s self-esteem. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(2), 143–153.

doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.009

Maxwell, L. E., Mitchell, M. R., & Evans, G. W. (2008). Effects of play equipment and loose parts

on preschool children’s outdoor play behavior: An observational study and design

Page 89: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

89

intervention. Children, Youth & Environments, 18(2), 36–63. Retrieved from

https://www.jstor.org/journal/chilyoutenvi

Mayer, K. (2007). Emerging knowledge about emergent writing. Young Children, 62, 34–40.

Retrieved from https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

McGee, L. M., & Schickedanz, J. A. (2007). Repeated interactive read-alouds in preschool and

kindergarten. The Reading Teacher, 60, 742–751. doi:10.1598/rt.60.8.4

McPhillips, M., & Jordan‐Black, J. A. (2007). The effect of social disadvantage on motor

development in young children: a comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 48(12), 1214–1222. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01814.x

Mendez, J. L. (2010). How can parents get involved in preschool? Barriers and engagement in

education by ethnic minority parents of children attending Head Start. Cultural Diversity

and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 26–36. doi:10.1037/a0016258

Morrow, L. M. (1985). Retelling stories: A strategy for improving children’s comprehension,

concept of story structure, and oral language complexity. The Elementary School

Journal, 85, 647–661. doi:10.1086/461427

National Association for the Education of Young Children (2009). Developmentally appropriate

practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8 (Position

Statement). Retrieved from: https://www.naeyc.org/sites/default/files/globally-

shared/downloads/PDFs/resources/position-statements/PSDAP.pdf

National Early Literacy Panel (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early

Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy.

Page 90: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

90

Neumann, M. M., Hood, M., & Ford, R. M. (2013). Using environmental print to enhance

emergent literacy and print motivation. Reading and Writing, 26(5), 771–793.

doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9390-7

Neuman, S., & Roskos, K. (1990). The influence of literacy-enriched play settings on

preschoolers’ engagement with written language. National Reading Conference

Yearbook, 39, 179–187.

Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1992). Literacy objects as cultural tools: Effects on children's

literacy behaviors in play. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(3), 203–225. doi:10.2307/

747792

Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1993). Access to print for children of poverty: Differential effects of

adult mediation and literacy-enriched play settings on environmental and functional

print tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 30(1), 95–122. doi:10.2307/

1163191

Nicolopoulou, A. (2010). The alarming disappearance of play from early childhood education.

Human Development, 53, 1–4. doi:10.1159/000268135

Nielsen, G., Bugge, A., Hermansen, B., Svensson, J., & Andersen, L. B. (2012). School playground

facilities as a determinant of children’s daily activity: A cross-sectional study of Danish

primary school children. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 9(1), 104–114.

doi:10.1123/jpah.9.1.104

Nix, R. L., Bierman, K. L., Domitrovich, C. E., & Gill, S. (2013). Promoting children’s social-

emotional skills in preschool can enhance academic and behavioral functioning in

Page 91: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

91

kindergarten: Findings from Head Start REDI. Early Education & Development, 24, 1000–

1019. doi:10.1080/10409289.2013.825565

Nix, R. L., Bierman, K. L., Motamedi, M., Heinrichs, B., & Gill, S. (2018). Parent engagement in a

Head Start home visiting program predicts sustained growth in children’s school

readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 45, 106–114. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.

06.006

Notari-Syverson, A., & Sadler, F. H. (2008). Math is for everyone: Strategies for supporting early

mathematical competencies in young children. Young Exceptional Children, 11(3), 2–16.

doi:10.1177/1096250608314589

Osborn, K., & Osborn, J. D. (1981). Discipline and Classroom Management. Athens, GA:

Education Associates.

Perlmutter, M., Sophian, C., Mitchell, D., & Cavanaugh, J. (1981). Semantic and contextual cuing

of preschool children’s recall. Child Development, 52(3), 873–881. doi:10.2307/ 1129089

Petrides, L. A., & Nodine, T. (2005). Anatomy of school system improvement: Performance-

driven practices in urban school districts. San Francisco, CA: NewSchools Venture Fund.

Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2008). Successful phonological awareness

instruction with preschool children: Lessons from the classroom. Topics in Early

Childhood Special Education, 28(1), 3–17. doi:10.1177/0271121407313813

Pianta, R. C., Barnett, W. S., Burchinal, M., & Thornburg, K. R. (2009). The effects of preschool

education: What we know, how public policy is or is not aligned with the evidence base,

and what we need to know. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(2), 49–88.

Retrieved from https://www.psychologicalscience.org/publications/pspi

Page 92: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 92

Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B., & Stuhlman, M. (2003). Relationships between teachers and children.

In W. M. Reynolds., G. E. Miller., & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (pp. 199–

234). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, K. B. (1995). The first two years of school: Teacher-child

relationships and deflections in children’s classroom adjustment. Development and

Psychopathology, 7(2), 295–312. doi:10.1017/s0954579400006519

Pullen, P. C., & Justice, L. M. (2003). Enhancing phonological awareness, print awareness, and

oral language skills in preschool children. Intervention in School and Clinic, 39(2), 87–98.

doi:10.1177/10534512030390020401

Raghubar, K. P., & Barnes, M. A. (2017). Early numeracy skills in preschool-aged children: A

review of neurocognitive findings and implications for assessment and intervention. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 31, 329–351. doi:10.1080/13854046.2016.1259387

RAND Education. (2012). Teachers matter: Understanding teachers’ impact on student

achievement. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation.

Ratcliff, N. J. (2001). Using authentic assessment to document the emerging literacy skills of

young children. Childhood Education, 78, 66–69. doi:10.1080/00094056.2002.10522706

Rettig, M. A. (2002). Cultural diversity and play from an ecological perspective. Children &

Schools, 24, 189–199. doi:10.1093/cs/24.3.189

Rhodes, L. K., & Nathenson-Meija, S. (1992). Anecdotal records: A powerful tool for ongoing

literacy assessment. The Reading Teacher, 45, 502–509. Retrieved from

https://ila.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/19362714/

Page 93: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

93

Rivera, M. (1998). Creating a Science Area in a Preschool Classroom. New York, NY: Lehman

College, University of New York. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 420438.

Robinson, L. E. (2011). The relationship between perceived physical competence and

fundamental motor skills in preschool children. Child: Care, Health and Development,

37(4), 589–596. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01187.x

Roskos, K., & Neuman, S. B. (2001). Environment and its influences for early literacy teaching

and learning. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy

research (pp. 281–294). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Salinas-Gonzalez, I., Arreguín-Anderson, M. G., & Alanís, I. (2018, December/January).

Supporting language: Culturally rich dramatic play. Teaching Young Children, 11(2).

Retrieved from NAEYC:

https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/tyc/dec2017/supporting-language-culturally-

rich-dramatic-play

Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S., Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005).

Lifetime effects: The HighScope Perry Preschool study through age 40 (Monographs of

the HighScope Educational Research Foundation, 14). Ypsilanti, MI: HighScope Press.

Sheridan, S., Williams, P., & Samuelsson, I. P. (2014). Group size and organizational conditions

for children’s learning in preschool: A teacher perspective. Educational Research, 56,

379–397. doi:10.1080/00131881.2014.965562

Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.

Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Page 94: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

94

Sophian, C. (1988). Early developments in children’s understanding of number: Inferences

about numerosity and one-to-one correspondence. Child Development, 59, 1397–1414.

doi:10. 1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01508.x

Souto-Manning, M. (2013). Multicultural teaching in the early childhood classroom: Approaches,

strategies, and tools, preschool-2nd grade. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes

in mathematics and reading. Child Development, 57, 646–659. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1986.tb00234.x

Stipek, D., & Daniels, D., Galluzzo, D., & Milburn, S. (1992). Characterizing early childhood

education programs for poor and middle-class children. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly, 7, 1–9. doi:10.1016/0885-2006(92)90015-q

Stone, J. G. (1978). A Guide to Discipline (Rev. ed.). Washington, D.C.: National Association for

the Education of Young Children.

Sucuoğlu, B., Bakkaloğlu, F. I., Karasu, F.I., Demir, Ş., Akalin, S. (2013). Inclusive preschool

teachers: Their attitudes and knowledge about inclusion. International Journal of Early

Childhood Special Education, 5, 107–128. doi:10.20489/intjecse.107929

Sugiyama, T., Okely, A. D., Masters, J. M., & Moore, G. T. (2010). Attributes of child care centers

and outdoor play areas associated with preschoolers’ physical activity and sedentary

behavior. Environment and Behavior, 44(3), 334–349. doi:10.1177/ 0013916510393276

Tarim, K. (2009). The effects of cooperative learning on preschoolers’ mathematics problem-

solving ability. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 72, 325–340. doi:10.1007/s10649-

009-9197-x

Page 95: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

95

Teale, W. H. (1988). Developmentally appropriate assessment of reading and writing in the

early childhood classroom. The Elementary School Journal, 89, 172–183.

doi:10.1086/461571

Tomlinson, C. A. (2001). How to differentiate instruction in mixed-ability classrooms (2nd ed.).

Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Umek, L. M., & Musek, P. L. (2001). Symbolic play: Opportunities for cognitive and language

development in preschool settings. Early Years: An International Journal of Research and

Development, 21, 55–64. doi:10.1080/09575140125077

Vestal, A., & Jones, A.N. (2004). Peace building and conflict resolution in preschool children.

Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 19, 131–142. doi:10.1080/

02568540409595060

Vogel, N., (2012). Arranging the active learning environment: Setting up the preschool

classroom. Ypsilanti, MI: HighScope.

Vukelich, C. (1994). Effects of play interventions on young children’s reading of environmental

print. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(2), 153–170. doi:10.1016/0885-

2006(94)90003-5

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society the development of higher psychological processes.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Ward, C. (1996). Adult intervention: Appropriate strategies for enriching the quality of

children’s play. Young Children, 51, 20–25. Retrieved from

https://www.naeyc.org/resources/pubs/yc

Page 96: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 96

Weinstein, C. S. (1987). Designing preschool classrooms to support development. In C. S.

Weinstein & T. G. David, Spaces for children (pp. 159–185). New York, NY: Springer.

Wentzel, K. R., & Asher, S. R. (1995). The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular, and

controversial children. Child Development, 66(3), 754–763. doi:10.2307/1131948

Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child

Development, 69, 848–872. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.00848.x

Williams, P. (2001). Preschool routines, peer learning and participation. Scandinavian Journal of

Educational Research, 45, 317–339. doi:10.1080/ 00313830120096743

Williford, A. P., Vick Whittaker, J. E., Vitiello, V. E., & Downer, J. T. (2013). Children's

engagement within the preschool classroom and their development of self-regulation.

Early Education and Development, 24(2), 162–187. doi:10.1080/10409289.2011.628270

Wortham, S. C., Barbour, A., & Desjean-Perrotta, B. (1998). Portfolio assessment: A handbook

for preschool and elementary educators. Olney, MD: Association for Childhood

Education International.

Yoshikawa, H., Weiland, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., Burchinal, M. R., Espinosa, L. M., Gormley, W. T., . .

. Zaslow, M. J. (2013). Investing in our future: The evidence base on preschool education

(Vol. 9). Washington, DC: Society for Research in Child Development and Foundation for

Child Development.

Page 97: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

97

Preschool Program Quality Assessment – Revised (PQA-R)

Technical Report

Preschool PQA-R Overview

HighScope developed the PQA-R to evaluate the quality of early childhood classrooms for four-

to five-year-olds as well as identify the training needs of the classroom staff. The assessment is

intended for classroom evaluation in that a trained independent evaluator rates the classroom

characteristics. Alternatively, the tool can be used as a self-assessment tool by administrators

or instructional staff for classroom planning and monitoring. The PQA-R can be used for

research and evaluation as well as to provide information to policymakers, program

administrators, families, and researchers. Each section comprises items that describe a broad

array of classroom characteristics and rows that describe the quality of the classroom

characteristics.

Assessors assign a level from 1 (no or poor quality) to 4 (highest quality) for each row. The rows

are summed to produce an aggregate score for each section. Each section is then assigned a

performance level of Low, Medium Low, Medium High, or High. The PQA-R defines quality

along a continuum. Teachers and program administrators are able to see where each classroom

lies along the continuum of quality and use the results to systematically plan school

improvement goals and objectives. Based on classroom observations and structured interviews

with teaching staff, PQA-R assessors rate classrooms on the quality of the learning

environment, teaching and learning routines, adult-child interactions, curriculum, planning and

assessment, and family engagement.

The assessment is made up of one form across three Sections: 1. Learning Environment; 2.

Teaching and Learning Routines and Adult-Child Interactions; 3. Curriculum, Planning,

Assessment, and Family Engagement. The form comprises 20 items and 61 rows across the

three sections.

Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation

The PQA-R is an observational assessment and thus the reliability of the results and the

effectiveness of its use relies on the ability of the observer to both observe accurately and score

reliably. The administration of the PQA-R, for accountability purposes, requires a trained and

reliable observer to spend at least 3 hours and up to an entire day in the preschool classroom

to complete the form, as evidence for each row must be entered before the observer assigns

scores. The trained observer completes the Materials Checklist and keeps a running log of all

the activities and adult-child interactions observed during the observation period. Sections 1

and 2 contain rows that can be scored through observation only, and Section 3 contains rows

that can only be scored by gathering additional information from classroom staff using the

guiding questions provided. Each row is scored on a 4-point scale. Indicators for all scores 1–4

Page 98: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 98

are described to assist assessor scoring. The scoring guide provides examples of evidence for

each level to help the assessor determine the quality level for that row on a scale from 1 to 4.

The row scores are then totaled to get a Section Score. The Section Score is then interpreted

into a Performance Level (i.e., Low, Medium Low, Medium High, or High). The overall results

are reported as three Section Scores and three Performance Levels.

Development of the PQA-R

HighScope developed the PQA-R by revising and refining the original Preschool Program Quality

(PQA) based on the extant research about how and what to measure to determine preschool

classroom quality. Content experts reviewed the revised version to determine that the breadth

and depth of quality indicators were included in the revised assessment. We subsequently

revised the draft version based on the content experts’ recommendations for improvement for

use for the validation study to determine the validity, reliability, and fairness of the PQA-R

results.

Validation Study

HighScope conducted three iterative rounds for the validation study. We conducted the first

two studies to gather information through psychometric analyses and assessor feedback to

revise and refine the instrument. We conducted the third study to determine the psychometric

properties of the instrument (including cut scores) and further revise and refine the PQA-R for

use during the 2018–2019 school year.

During the third study, 110 experienced assessors (with at least 2 years of experience using the

prior version of the PQA-R) completed 196 PQA-R assessments of preschool classrooms. The

assessors were from varied backgrounds and programs and had varied levels of education and

experience in assessing classrooms using observation-based instruments. The preschool

classrooms were a mix of state-funded preschools, federally funded preschools, and private

preschools, including Montessori preschools. The classrooms were located in urban, suburban,

and rural locations in nine states, and the teachers and students were of varied races,

ethnicities, and income levels.

Results of the Validation Study

Scale Reliabilities (Cronbach's Alpha) (n = 196)

The scale reliabilities show good to excellent internal consistency. The Section 1 scale (Learning

Environment) consists of 15 items (α = .84), the Section 2 scale (Teaching and Learning Routines

and Adult-Child Interactions) consists of 40 items (α = .94), and the Section 3 scale (Curriculum,

Planning, Assessment, and Family Engagement) consists of 14 items (α = .82). Removal of any

item within a section does not increase or decrease the internal consistency.

Results of the test retest reliability for section 1 and section 2 showed high test-retest

reliability. The correlation between test and retest was around .94 in both cases.

Page 99: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

99

Interrater Reliability The effects of training on interrater agreement were determined through a study conducted by presenting trained assessors with 15 scenarios that described a classroom or adult-child interactions (e.g., The Daily Routine is posted in words on the upper part of door for adults. Children review the question of the day and review the weather. T1 says they will go outside). The assessors used the PQA-R to rate each scenario. A proprietary survey tool, KeySurvey, was used to administer the interrater reliability assessment and to gather the data. The assessor rater scores were compared to scores which a team of HighScope raters, who were familiar with the instrument, agreed on after rating the scenarios individually and accordingly agreed on the most appropriate score. Using the data collected from 110 assessors (raters) and 15 scenarios, three measurements of interrater agreement were used: exact and adjacent agreement percentages, Kappa for polytomous responses, and intra-class correlation using a two-way ANOVA random model. (Both the scenarios and assessors are random samples of their respective populations.) The average exact percentage among the 15 scenarios was about 87%, while the adjacent percentage was about 99%. The average Kappa for multiple response options is 0.87; levels above .80 imply an excellent amount of agreement. The ICC (reliability of the mean) is 0.994 (95% CI: 0.989 - 0.997), implying an excellent degree of agreement among raters and suggesting that the scenarios, on average, were coded similarly among the 110 assessor raters. Implementation Study

The implementation study was conducting during the 2018–2019 school year to determine the

psychometric properties of the instrument (including cut scores) and further revise and refine

the PQA-R to develop a fully operationalized assessment for the 2019–2020 school year.

During the implementation study, 117 trained assessors completed 889 PQA-R assessments of

preschool classrooms. The assessors were from varied backgrounds and programs and had

varied levels of education and experience in assessing classrooms using observation-based

instruments. The preschool classrooms were a mix of state-funded preschools, federally funded

preschools, and private preschools, including Montessori preschools. The classrooms were

located in urban, suburban, and rural locations, and the teachers and students were of varied

races, ethnicities, and income levels.

Results of the Implementation Study

Model Fit, Wright Maps, and Item Curve Characteristics (RASCH) The weighted and unweighted Mean-Square fit statistics for 57 of the 59 items were all below 1.5, meaning that we did not observe misfit for any of those 57 items beyond that expected within the Partial Credit Rasch model. Two items were misfitting (above 2.0), indicating more noise than information. We determined that the two items were too important to delete so we reworded the items for clarity.

Page 100: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation

100

The Wright Map shows that our a priori predictions about the relative difficulty of items were

correct. Items that are more difficult are at or near the top and items that are less difficult are

at or near the bottom. Our Wright Map of MLE Estimates and Thresholds shows the general

orderliness with which the polytomous item steps group with developmental level 1 at the

bottom, followed by levels 2 and levels 3, with level 4 at the top. The polytomous Item Curve

Characteristics across the items provide evidence of an orderly successive progression of

developmental levels.

Scale Reliabilities (Cronbach's Alpha) (n=889)

The scale reliabilities show good to excellent internal consistency. The Section 1 scale (Learning Environment) consists of 16 items (α = .83), the Section 2 scale (Teaching and Learning Routines and Adult-Child Interactions) consists of 44 items (α = .95), and the Section 3 scale (Curriculum, Planning, Assessment, and Family Engagement) consists of 15 items (α = .86). Removal of any item within a section does not increase or decrease the internal consistency. Correlations (Rasch): We show the intercorrelations among the sections of the PQA-R in Table 1. Table 1 PQA-R: Section Correlations PQA-R Sections LE Curriculum/Assessment Learning Environment T/L Routines, A/C Interactions .72 .73 Curriculum/Assessment .70 Interrater Reliability of the Implementation Study

The effects of training on interrater agreement were determined as well in the implementation study by presenting trained assessors with 20 scenarios that described a classroom or adult-child interactions (e.g., Teachers are scheduled to plan on Fridays, but many times meetings are scheduled, absorbing the time set for teacher planning. Both teachers report that about once a month they are able to use the COR Advantage data to plan for children). The assessors used the PQA-R to assess each scenario. A proprietary survey tool, KeySurvey, was used to administer the interrater reliability assessment and gather the data. The assessor rater scores were compared to scores that a team of HighScope raters, who were familiar with the instrument, assigned after rating the scenarios individually and together. Using the data collected from 117 assessors (raters) and 20 scenarios, three measurements of interrater agreement were used: exact and adjacent agreement percentages, Kappa for polytomous

Page 101: PQA-R - HighScope

© 2019 HighScope Educational Research Foundation 101

responses, and intra-class correlation using a two-way ANOVA random model. (Both the scenarios and assessors are random samples of their respective populations.)

The average exact percentage among the 20 scenarios was 73.1%, while the adjacent

percentage was 99.2%. The average Kappa for multiple response options is 0.833 (95% CI 0.773,

0.888); levels above .80 imply an excellent amount of agreement. The ICC (reliability of the

mean) is 0.999 (95% CI 0.998, 0.999), implying an excellent degree of agreement among raters.

Developing Cut Scores for the PQA-R

Although the PQA-R reports the score for each row, it also aggregates the scores for all rows in

a section. These aggregated scores are hard to interpret unless cut scores are identified that

divide the scale into performance levels. Dividing the scale into meaningful segments allows for

reporting performance levels for each section of the PQA-R as Low, Medium Low, Medium

High, and High as well as reporting of the percentage of classrooms at each performance level.

We analyzed the results using the Rasch Rating Scale Model and then used an application of

Item Response Theory to identify the cut scores using Winsteps. This approach yields criterion-

based cut scores without the need for subjective expert judgement.

This approach was used to identify cut scores to divide the scale, for each section, into four

performance levels that can be described as follows:

Performance Level 1 High — Across the Section, assessors are more likely to endorse Level 4

than other response options, indicating a high level of quality overall.

Performance Level Medium High — Across the Section, assessors are more likely to endorse

Levels 3–4 than other response options, indicating a medium-high level of quality overall.

Performance Level Medium Low — Across the Section, assessors are more likely to endorse

Levels 2–3 than other response options, indicating a medium-low level of quality overall.

Performance Level Low — Across the Section, assessors are more likely to endorse Levels 1–2

than other response options, indicating a low level of quality overall.

Summary

The implementation study results show that the PQA-R assessment provides reliable and valid

psychometric measurement of the quality of preschool classrooms in three key dimensions.

Expected levels of intercorrelations between sections are evident in the data.