EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ...

36
342 EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADD A Viable Alternative to Traditional Medical Intervention? PAUL MICHAEL RAMIREZ, a DEBORAH DESANTIS, a AND LEWIS A. OPLER a,b a Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology at Long Island University (Brooklyn Campus), Brooklyn, New York 11201, USA b Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons and Research Division for the New York State Office of Mental Health, New York, New York 10032, USA ABSTRACT: Literature searches dating back to 1968 were conducted through Psychlit and Medline services to review the scientific literature on EEG bio- feedback treatment of ADD. While anecdotal and case reports cite promising evidence, methodological problems coupled with a paucity of research pre- cludes any definitive conclusions as to the efficacy of enhanced alpha and hemisphere-specific EEG biofeedback training. One of the more promising EEG biofeedback treatment paradigms involves theta/beta training. Studies have reported that academic, intellectual, and behavioral gains have been attained with this approach. Significant behavioral and cognitive changes have also been reported following SMR training. However, research into these treatment approaches has also been marred by methodological inadequacies and lack of sufficient follow-up studies. A number of recommendations for future research into this treatment approach are made. KEYWORDS: ADD; ADHD; Biofeedback; EEG biofeedback. INTRODUCTION In treating adults with residual Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) symptomat- ology, the more traditional treatment approaches have included stimulant medication and/or cognitive behavioral psychotherapeutic techniques. While predominantly focusing on children and young adolescents, over the past twenty-five to thirty years research has surfaced positing EEG biofeedback as yet another viable technique in the treatment of this disorder. 1–3 Biofeedback techniques use external instrumenta- tion to provide a subject with information regarding psychophysiological processes, which are usually outside of one’s awareness and ability to control. 4 The usual pro- cedure involves establishing reinforcer-response contingencies with rewards being supplied for desired changes in internalized physiologic responses. 5 Thus, the clini- cian sets desired thresholds on the biofeedback equipment which are based on treat- ment goals and, as the client’s physiologic changes approach and surpass those thresholds, the equipment provides either auditory or visual feedback which serves Address for correspondence: Paul Michael Ramirez, Ph.D., 5400 Fieldston Rd., #44-D, Riverdale, NY 10471. Voice & fax: 718-601-4161.

Transcript of EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ...

Page 1: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

342

EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADD

A Viable Alternative to Traditional Medical Intervention?

PAUL MICHAEL RAMIREZ,

a

DEBORAH DESANTIS,

a

AND LEWIS A. OPLER

a,b

a

Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology at Long Island University (Brooklyn Campus), Brooklyn, New York 11201, USA

b

Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons and Research Division for the New York State Office of Mental Health, New York, New York 10032, USA

A

BSTRACT

: Literature searches dating back to 1968 were conducted throughPsychlit and Medline services to review the scientific literature on EEG bio-feedback treatment of ADD. While anecdotal and case reports cite promisingevidence, methodological problems coupled with a paucity of research pre-cludes any definitive conclusions as to the efficacy of enhanced alpha andhemisphere-specific EEG biofeedback training. One of the more promisingEEG biofeedback treatment paradigms involves theta/beta training. Studieshave reported that academic, intellectual, and behavioral gains have beenattained with this approach. Significant behavioral and cognitive changes havealso been reported following SMR training. However, research into thesetreatment approaches has also been marred by methodological inadequaciesand lack of sufficient follow-up studies. A number of recommendations forfuture research into this treatment approach are made.K

EYWORDS

: ADD; ADHD; Biofeedback; EEG biofeedback.

INTRODUCTION

In treating adults with residual Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) symptomat-ology, the more traditional treatment approaches have included stimulant medicationand/or cognitive behavioral psychotherapeutic techniques. While predominantlyfocusing on children and young adolescents, over the past twenty-five to thirty yearsresearch has surfaced positing EEG biofeedback as yet another viable technique inthe treatment of this disorder.

1–3

Biofeedback techniques use external instrumenta-tion to provide a subject with information regarding psychophysiological processes,which are usually outside of one’s awareness and ability to control.

4

The usual pro-cedure involves establishing reinforcer-response contingencies with rewards beingsupplied for desired changes in internalized physiologic responses.

5

Thus, the clini-cian sets desired thresholds on the biofeedback equipment which are based on treat-ment goals and, as the client’s physiologic changes approach and surpass thosethresholds, the equipment provides either auditory or visual feedback which serves

Address for correspondence: Paul Michael Ramirez, Ph.D., 5400 Fieldston Rd., #44-D,Riverdale, NY 10471. Voice & fax: 718-601-4161.

Page 2: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

343RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

as reinforcement for the desired changes. As an example, as a client decreases thetaand increases beta waves during EEG biofeedback treatment of ADD, reinforcementis provided to encourage the client to become more aware of what he is doing toachieve this desired state and to continue in the same vein.

Given that research involving the application of EEG biofeedback training in thetreatment of either ADD and/or learning disability (LD) almost exclusively involveschildren, this chapter critically reviews some of the more pertinent studies of chil-dren, particularly those with potential relevance to the adolescent and adult popula-tions. Literature searches were conducted through Psychlit and Medline servicesdating back to 1968 in order to locate relevant articles.

This review is divided into four sections. The first section (after the introduction)describes the application of sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) training, which is generallyutilized in the treatment of hyperactivity. While this is certainly less of an issue whendealing with adult residual ADD phenomenology, this section is included to providean overview of the EEG biofeedback procedures most often utilized in dealing withADHD (Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder). The second section focuses ontheta/beta training. The third area involves a discussion of alpha training. Finally, wedescribe the utilization of hemisphere-specific EEG biofeedback training to amelio-rate the behavioral and/or cognitive difficulties typically associated with ADD andADHA (used somewhat interchangably). In reviewing the efficacy of each approach,four important questions are applied:

1. Did biofeedback training result in the intended EEG changes? 2. Did significant modifications in cognition and behavior ensue inside and

out of the laboratory? 3. Could these changes be reliably linked to biofeedback treatment? 4. And lastly, are these changes retained over time?

Owing to their extent and complexity, theories accounting for the results found inthese studies have been excluded other than to mention that EEG biofeedback is gen-erally hypothesized to affect the brain abnormalities associated with ADD and relat-ed disorders, and to promote those changes in maladaptive behavior and inattentiontypical of such disorders.

6

In EEG biofeedback training, feedback is typically furnished concerning the defi-ciency or overabundance of a specific amplitude and/or frequency of brain waveelectrical activity.

7

In the treatment of ADD, feedback is usually provided to promotea reduction in theta waves (4–8 Hz), typically evident when drowsy or dreaming,either alone or in combination with the enhancement of beta waves (16–20 Hz),which are associated with higher levels of arousal, problem-solving, and anxiety. Inhyperactive states, sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) (12–15 Hz), believed to be relatedto somatomotor inhibition,

8

are also an integral part of the treatment plan. In addi-tion, alpha rhythms (8–12 Hz), which are related to states of wakeful relaxation andhemispheric discrepancies, are also targeted.

9

The overall aim of these approachesis to produce desired changes in behavioral and cognitive states,

8

and are summa-rized in T

ABLE

1.

Research dating back 35 years has established the presence of various degrees ofEEG abnormalities in children with ADD and/or LD. Preliminary studies found that63

%

of children with LD showed EEG abnormalities as opposed to 20

%

of matched

Page 3: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

344 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

controls.

12

Later, other investigators specified these abnormalities. As an example,Winkler, Dixon and Parker

13

detected greater diffuse, slow wave theta activity, less fastwave beta SMR activity and more abnormal transient discharges in a sample of chil-dren evidencing scholastic and behavioral problems. In addition, Lubar, Bianchini,Calhoun, Lambert, Brody and Shasbin

14

reported that LD-ADHD children exhibitedsignificantly greater amounts of theta and more EMG activity (muscular tension) thannormal matched controls. They could also predict group membership (LD-ADHD vs.non-LD) by better than 97

%

when a combination of EEG parameters were utilized, andby more than 80

%

when restricting predictors to variables concerning increased thetaactivity in frontal-temporal locations alone. In another study, Mann, Lubar, Zimmer-man, Miller and Muenchen

15

found additional evidence that children with pure ADDexhibited increased theta activity in frontal and central locations, and decreased betaactivity in frontal and temporal regions as compared to matched controls. It has beensuggested, however, that at least some of the EEG abnormalities seen in youngerADHD children may not be generalizable to adults with ADD as they are pathogno-monic of a maturational lag in cerebral development which normalizes with advancingage, in particular during the latter stages of adolescence.

16

While intriguing, there arethose who believe that EEG abnormalities continue to be clearly evident in adolescentsand adults with ADD.

11

In addition to EEG abnormalities found in those individuals with ADD-relateddifficulties, numerous other investigations have yielded additional evidence of cere-bral dysfunction. As an example, Zametkin, Nordal, and Gross

17

studied and com-pared PET scans of adults with histories of childhood hyperactivity and persistingcomplaints of restlessness and inattentiveness with those of normal controls. Theseindividuals were naive to stimulant

medication and were the

biological parent of ahyperactive child. They found that the hyperactive adult group exhibited 8.1

%

lowerglobal cerebral glucose metabolism than did normal controls. They also reported sig-nificant hypometabolism in 30 of 60 brain regions, particularly, but not

exclusively,in areas of the frontal and central cortex, areas previously found to demonstrate thegreatest theta elevations and beta reductions in children with ADD.

18

T

ABLE

1. EEG biofeedback in ADHD

a

a

Note that EEG biofeedback training often involves the training of either increases ordecreases involving more than one type of brain wave activity.

b

Some debate continues as to whether alpha should be increased or decreased.

Rhythm Phenomenology Training

Sensorimotor rhythm (SMR)

Movement causes blockage of SMR

Increase SMR

Theta rhythm Abundant when drowsy Decrease theta

Beta rhythm Associated with attentional and/or memory processes

Increase beta

Alpha rhythm Associated with a relaxed, but alert state

Increase/decrease alpha

b

Hemispheric rhythm training

Still experimental Still experimental

Page 4: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

345RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

Further research has also shown that children with ADHD and LD exhibit lessalpha attenuation while engaging in cognitive activities, including listening andarithmetic

19

and immediate recall tasks,

20

in conjunction with more incorrectresponding, as compared with normal controls.

20

These findings are noteworthybecause of the inverse relationship previously cited between alpha, attention,

21

andcognitive activity.

19

Discrepancies have also been noted in the cerebral functioningof these populations.

22

In particular, right hemisphere dysfunction has been demon-strated in children and adolescents with ADD with and without hyperactivity

23,24

and in children with ADHD demonstrating comorbid LD.

25

SMR BIOFEEDBACK

The use of SMR biofeedback training for children with hyperactivity stemmed,in part, from a body of literature which demonstrated that conditioned increases inSMR brain wave activity achieved via biofeedback training produced markeddecreases in seizure activity in epileptics

26,27

as well as motor inhibition.

28–30

Wyricka and Sternman

31

found that cats reinforced for the production of increasedlevels of SMR obtained that reinforcement by assuming motionless positions. Chaseand Harper

28

also reported reduced muscular tension in cats following SMR train-ing. In humans, Sternman

et al

.

27

described heightened SMR activity in the EEGs ofparaplegics and quadriplegics and a decrement of the rhythm in epileptics, particu-larly when motor symptomatology was displayed. Following this work, Lubar andBahler

32

reported increased motor control in a 14-year-old subject who was both epi-leptic and hyperactive in conjunction with increased production of SMR anddecreased slow wave activity during SMR training. The investigators were tentativein drawing conclusions from these findings because of the high number of potentialconfounding factors, including maturational changes, which could account for suchresults.

In examining the efficacy of SMR biofeedback in individuals with historiesuncomplicated by seizure activity, Lubar and Shouse

33

investigated the applicationof SMR training with an 11-year, 8-month-old hyperkinetic male. The child under-went five phases of training, baseline periods of No Drug (I) and Drug only (II) con-ditions in the absence of EEG biofeedback, a feedback phase (III) during which theproduction of 12–14 Hz and the inhibition of 4–7 Hz EEG activity were reinforced,a contingency reversal condition (IV) in which the reinforcement paradigm wasreversed and finally a reinstatement of the original feedback phase (V). During con-ditions II to V, the child was on a steady regimen of methylphenidate (10 mg/day).Training occurred three times per week, 40 minutes each time, with sessions consist-ing of two 5-minute baseline periods and two 15-minute feedback phases. It wasfound that SMR values consistently increased as training progressed, returning topre-treatment levels only temporarily during the contingency reversal phase and fol-lowing a three-week lapse in training. Furthermore, a statistically significant inverserelationship between SMR and electromyography (EMG) (motor tension) was alsoreported during each training phase, fortifying the link between SMR and motorinhibition as well as baseline shifts in SMR activity, providing evidence for a carry-over effect of learning. Finally, thirteen behavioral categories adapted from Wahler’s

Page 5: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

346 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

category system were observed in the child’s classroom setting. Six, including outof seat behavior, cooperation, object play, sustained non-interaction, sustainedschool work and opposition, showed even greater improvement with SMR trainingand medication than with medication alone. Improvement was also noted in two oth-er behaviors (i.e., self-stimulation and sustained attention) not affected by drug ther-apy. As with SMR, these behavioral gains reversed in large part during thecounterconditioning phase. In a later paper, Lubar

18

reported that follow-ups con-ducted several years post treatment showed the subject to have maintained the gainsachieved during SMR training and to be performing well academically and withoutmedication.

Using a similar design and similar selection criteria, Shouse and Lubar

2,34

trainedfour hyperkinetic children with pronounced classroom misconduct in combinationwith substantial deficits in SMR and arousal. In addition to those of the designdescribed above, experimental conditions included a sixth phase, which consisted ofSMR training in the absence of medication. Relative to initial and training-phasebaselines, three of the four subjects demonstrated increases in SMR during training,including the final SMR alone condition, and temporary reinstatement of near pre-training levels of SMR during counterconditioning. These same subjects alsoshowed modest initial baseline shifts in SMR, providing evidence for the carry-overof training effects. One subject who, as his principal difficulty, evidenced the highestlevels of pre-treatment SMR and distractibility, as opposed to hyperactivity, failed toacquire the SMR task and was dropped from the study after six months. As in theirearlier study, all four subjects showed behavioral improvement in six of thirteenclassroom behaviors with medication.

33

Yet, it was solely those demonstratingincreases in SMR, who exhibited additional improvements in those same behaviorsin both the training and medication as well as training alone conditions. Ameliora-tion of two behaviors (i.e., self-stimulation and sustained attention) which had notbeen helped by medication alone, was also noted. In addition, there were trends forthese improvements to temporarily reverse during the counterconditioning phase.

Tansey and Bruner

1

applied both SMR biofeedback and EMG training with a10-year-old boy diagnosed as ADD with hyperactivity, a developmental readingdisorder and ocular instability (i.e., problems with saccadic fixation and ocularsmooth pursuit movements). Typically, in EMG training for general relaxation, areduction of muscular tension is achieved via EMG feedback monitored over thecentral forehead region. Such techniques have reportedly demonstrated some utilityin the treatment of hyperactivity.

35

After being weaned off Ritalin, the child firstunderwent 3 EMG training sessions (1 per week) and subsequently 20 weekly SMRsessions, in addition to daily practice of relaxation techniques in which he had beentrained. In conjunction with decreased EMG readings following training, subjectivereports showed him to be in greater control of his behavior and no longer diagnos-able as hyperactive. Gains superseded those achieved with medication alone. Fur-thermore, with an increase in SMR following biofeedback, the child’s ability to readand comprehend improved significantly and his ocular instability improved. Specif-ically, he was able to keep his head stationary while tracking smoothly along a hor-izontal axis as well as retaining focus on an object while moving his head from sideto side. While originally scheduled to be retained in a fourth grade class for the per-ceptually impaired prior to biofeedback, after training the child was enrolled in a

Page 6: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

347RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

normal fourth grade class where he attained better than passing grades in all areasthroughout the academic year. These improvements were maintained on 24- monthfollow-up as well as 10 years post-treatment, when the subject was reported as con-tinuing to exhibit normal social and academic functioning and EEG activity compa-rable to that of 24 children who had also undergone biofeedback training and wereno longer deemed as mixed ADHD and learning disabled.

36

In addition, Tansey

37

applied SMR biofeedback training with six LD boys using a reversal (A-B-A)design. Besides biofeedback, the subjects were not involved in any additional formof treatment for their learning difficulties other than academic training includingresource room and special class assignments. The investigator found mean increasesin SMR amplitudes ranging from 38–300

%

, with a mean group increase of 137.6

%

over baseline. He also reported that all of the subjects demonstrated improvementsof at least one standard deviation in their WISC-R Full Scale IQ scores. Those indi-viduals evidencing significant pre-treatment verbal/performance discrepancies (15points or greater) showed substantially greater improvement in the lower of their twoscores following training. Replicating their prior findings, those subjects with addi-tional oculomotor dysfunction exhibited improvement of these difficulties.

Lubar and Lubar

38

also trained six males with specific learning disabilities and,in some cases, hyperactivity using a combination of SMR and subsequent beta bio-feedback in addition to academic training twice a week over a 10–27 month period.All children exhibited increased production of SMR and beta activity as well asdecreased EMG and slow wave (theta) activity. Subjects also evidenced improvedacademic performance, as demonstrated by achievement test scores or grades, andgreater behavioral control. Notably, five of the six children had previously been inacademic training and special resource classes prior to biofeedback training withoutsignificant improvement having being noted. Tansey

39

also utilized SMR trainingwith six neurologically impaired, one perceptually impaired, and one hyperkineticchild. Simultaneously recording five frequency bands of brain activity (5, 7, 10, 12,and 14), he found a mean group SMR amplitude change of 76

%

over baseline at theend of training in conjunction with a reduction in average slow wave (theta) activityfor those with an IQ between 76 and 85 and an increase across all frequency bandsfor those within the 102–116 range, with slow wave activity increasing the least. Allsubjects demonstrated WISC-R IQ score improvements of at least one standard devi-ation, with those exhibiting verbal/performance discrepancies of greater than 14points evidencing a greater increase in the lower of their two scores. As in their pastwork, amelioration of ocular motor dysfunction was also found. Lastly, Tansey

40,41

trained eleven neurologically impaired children, eleven perceptually impaired, andtwo with ADD for a mean of 27.9 sessions utilizing SMR biofeedback techniques.All subjects exhibited significant mean energy decreases in slow wave (5 and 7 Hz)and increases in fast wave (12 and 14 Hz) activity. In addition, elevations in FullScale IQ of at least (or close to) one standard deviation were achieved as well as nor-malization of verbal/performance discrepancies.

Overall, in all seven studies presented, increased production of SMR was demon-strated and, in some cases, coincided with increased beta and decreased EMGand theta activity. One subject

33

was reported as unable to increase SMR productionand four

39

to evidence elevations as opposed to decrements in theta activity, althoughsix of Tansey’s subjects were neurologically impaired in an unspecified manner.

Page 7: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

348 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

Behaviorally, subjects were noted to demonstrate less hyperactivity and greaterbehavioral control following SMR and EMG training as well as amelioration ofa number of maladaptive classroom behaviors and remediation of oculomotordysfunction. Cognitively, individuals evidenced improved abilities in reading andcomprehension, better grades, increased WISC-R IQ scores and resolution ofverbal/performance discrepancies. Presumably, the better grades and test scores areattributable to their ability to stay on-task for longer periods of time. While such find-ings are quite compelling, control groups were not utilized in any of the above studiesand designs, at times, mixed a variety of biofeedback techniques (such as SMR,EMG, and beta biofeedback) as well as academic training and medication. These fac-tors make a definitive link between biofeedback training and the resulting changesdifficult to forge. On the other hand, studies utilizing reversal designs

33

reported aremediation of EEG and behavioral gains upon counterconditioning and lapses intraining. Moreover, the gains achieved with SMR training were above and beyondthose resulting from medication and/or resource classroom instruction and weremaintained in the absence of other forms of treatment. Finally, the single subject

33

who failed to enhance SMR production also failed to attain the behavioral and elec-trophysiological improvements demonstrated by other subjects. Only two casestudies

18,36

included follow-up reports to ascertain whether these results were main-tained. In each, EEG, behavioral and academic gains were reported to have beenretained up to ten years post-treatment.

In general, while some of these findings are promising, these studies suffer frommethodological flaws consistently evident across many of the studies examined.Specifically, the use of control groups are lacking and subjects carrying differentdiagnoses as well as some with comorbid disorders are often lumped together, thusassuming that they share common neuropathological processes.

THETA/BETA BIOFEEDBACK

A number of studies were also found targeting theta and/or beta brain wave levelsto produce desired cognitive and behavioral changes in ADD and LD children.Lubar

18

described a demonstration project (conducted in Knox County, Tennesseeand surrounding area schools) in which 37 ADD children treated with EEG biofeed-back to increase beta and decrease theta (in addition to resource classroom training)were compared to 37 matched controls (who received resource classroom trainingalone for reading disabilities associated with ADHD). Those undergoing EEG bio-feedback demonstrated significantly greater improvements in Grade Point Average(GPA) and on their Metropolitan Achievement Test scores than controls and contin-ued to show improvements of better than 1.5 GPA levels over control subjects oneyear post-treatment. More recently, Lubar, Swartwood, Swartwood, and O’Don-nell,

42

trained 23 children and adolescents with ADHD to decrease theta waves byeither reinforcing decreased theta or increased beta activity over a two- to three-month summer program. All participants were trained under identical treatmentprotocols and none were on medication for the duration of the study. Twelve of nine-teen subjects showed significant decreases in theta across sessions with no significantdifferences having been found in pre-treatment levels of theta across subjects. In

Page 8: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

349RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

addition, those individuals who demonstrated theta reductions (EEG change group)evidenced a significantly greater improvement in TOVA test performance, a visualcontinuous performance task. Specifically, they showed improved functioning acrossthree of four TOVA scales as opposed to those who did not exhibit such EEG changes(no change group). The EEG change group also demonstrated a significant elevationin post-treatment WISC-R Verbal, Performance and Full-Scale IQ scores. The signif-icance of these findings though are compromised by the fact that no analyses werereported comparing the pre- and post-treatment WISC-R scores of the no changegroup. Finally, significant behavioral differences were not found between the EEGchange and no change groups possibly, according to the authors, because parents mayhave overemphasized the positive gains made by their children when responding onsubjective measures. Given that this was not a tightly controlled study, alternativeexplanations are also possible. As an example, perhaps the children comprising theno change group exhibited genuine behavioral improvement because of the individ-ualized attention they were getting within the context of the treatment, or the fact thatit was summer and they were relieved of school-related pressures. While the authorsmay indeed be correct in their explanation for the lack of behavioral differencesbetween the change and no change group, the lack of experimental controls certainlyopens up the possibility of alternative explanations.

Alhambra, Fowler and Alhambra

43

reported that 30 of the 36 ADD/ADHD chil-dren and adolescents they had trained with EEG biofeedback showed significantoverall improvements across behavioral measures, quantitative EEGs and TOVAscores, with significant correlations being found between changes in EEG and TOVAscore elevations. Moreover, of the 24 subjects treated with medication, five werecompletely removed from medication, eleven were maintained on a reduced doseand four, though on the same dose, showed greater overall improvement on that dosefollowing training. In another study, Pratt, Abel and Skidmore

44

trained 19 ADD orADHD subjects to decrease theta and increase beta in the presence or absence ofbackground music. They reported that even though pre- and post-test ratios of thetaand beta were not significant, to varying degrees, all subjects were subjectively ratedas having improved their focusing capability, impulsivity, social skills and control ofmoods, with 70

%

of individuals maintaining these gains six months post treatment.Furthermore, children with ADD trained in the presence of background music werereported to have achieved even greater gains in focusing behavior. Given that pre-and post-test ratios of theta and beta were not significant, it is unclear as to what theresults of this study signify other than the possible efficacy of background music inameliorating some of the symptoms which typify ADD and ADHD. Lastly, Linden

et al

.

6

compared two groups of children with ADD/ADHD and, in some cases, learn-ing disabilities, who were randomly assigned to either forty sessions of training tosuppress theta and enhance beta activity (over a six month period) or a waiting list/control group. None of the children were undergoing any other form of treatment forthe duration of the study. The authors found that while neither of the groups weresignificantly different in terms of IQ, inattention, overactivity or aggressive-defiantbehavior prior to treatment, the experimental group showed a significant increaseon their K-BIT IQ scores (average increase of 9 points), improved attention, anda non-significant decrease in hyperactivity as compared to controls followingtreatment. It should be noted that, while the average increase in K-BIT IQ scores

Page 9: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

350 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

represents statistical significance within the context of this study, it only representsan actual increase of less than one standard deviation, thus perhaps limiting its func-tional significance. In addition, because of methodological difficulties, the authorsdid not analyze their EEG data, thus precluding their ability to correlate the improve-ments found with conditioned brain wave changes.

Adding to the limited number of follow-up reports, Lubar

11

described a retro-spective telephone survey completed on 52 patients, 50

%

of which were assessedbetween one and ten years following treatment. Sixteen items from the Conner’sParent-Teacher Questionnaire were utilized to assess results. The greatest changesfound were in areas including homework, improved grades, family interactions andattitude. The authors also reported that virtually all parents interviewed had attribut-ed these changes to EEG biofeedback training, though no information was providedas to whether subjects had been treated with SMR or theta/beta training. Whileencouraging, we do not know whether the demand characteristics of a telephone sur-vey may have influenced parental response, nor whether the confounding effects ofhistory itself were contributory. Specifically, perhaps intervening remedial andbehavioral strategies which followed the original biofeedback treatment wereresponsible for the gains cited.

In the above reports, only four provided information concerning the attainment ofintended theta/beta alterations during training. One study

42

reported that twelve ofnineteen individuals were able to decrease theta, another described an improvementin EEG activity

43

and the last,

44

a non-significant difference between pre- and post-theta/beta ratios. In terms of behavioral and cognitive changes, increases in focusingcapabilities, social skills, family interactions and attitude as well as significantincreases in IQ and academic performance were evident. The authors also reportednon-significant decreases in hyperactivity. One investigation

43

also noted that a largeproportion (20 of 24) of the individuals in their study who were on medication hadtheir doses decreased or eliminated altogether following training.

Although the data are interesting, one cannot firmly establish a direct linkbetween theta/beta biofeedback and the changes reported because of the lack of EEGdata in some studies

6,18

as well as non-significant EEG changes cited in others.

44

Still, two controlled studies

6,18

demonstrated that experimental subjects showed sig-nificantly greater behavioral and cognitive changes than control subjects. In addi-tion, two of the works cited indicated that none of their subjects had been onmedication

42

or any other form of treatment

6

during biofeedback treatment.Three studies provided follow-up data

6,18,44

in which cognitive and behavioralchanges were reported to have been maintained up to six months, one year, and eventen years post-treatment for a proportion of subjects trained.

ALPHA BIOFEEDBACK

Four studies were located pertaining to the application of alpha biofeedback withchildren having ADD and LD, three of which trained subjects to increase alpha whilethe last trained them to decrease or attenuate alpha. O’Malley and Conners

45

traineda dyslexic adolescent to unilaterally increase alpha production in the left hemisphereand beta or theta in the right over a five-day period. While the subject did demonstrate

Page 10: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

351RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

increased levels of alpha as compared to baseline, ratings of corresponding changesin behavioral and cognitive functioning were not obtained. Unfortunately, whiledemonstrating the ability to train unilateral brain wave changes, the functional con-comitants of such changes was not addressed. In addition, Nall

3

investigated theassociation between increased alpha levels and amelioration of overactive and mal-adaptive behaviors and overall achievement in hyperactive children with learning dis-abilities. She utilized three matched groups (

n

=

16), an experimental group whichwas provided with correct alpha feedback, a control group given false feedback anda no-treatment control group. Following biofeedback, she reported that similar num-bers of subjects in the experimental and false feedback groups (9 of 16 and 7 of 16)achieved increased alpha levels and that, though there was a trend in favor of theexperimental group, no behavioral differences resulted between any of the threegroups. It was also reported that, in terms of achievement measures, the experimentalgroup significantly outperformed the other two groups in reading comprehensionalone.

Gracenin and Cook

46

also investigated the utilization of alpha biofeedback train-ing in achieving improved oral reading and reading comprehension scores in LDchildren. Experimental subjects (

n

=

9) received a ten-minute training session toincrease alpha productivity one time/week for ten weeks. The investigators reportedthat, following treatment, most subjects evidenced gains in alpha amplitudes andduration which were accompanied by slight improvements in hyperactive and mal-adaptive behaviors, as well as significant improvements in comprehension as com-pared to controls.

Lastly, Jackson and Eberly

47

trained five mentally challenged individualsbetween the ages of 22 and 29 to suppress alpha while engaging in a cognitive taskacross ten sessions spaced over two weeks time. A significant attenuation in alphaensued in conjunction with an increase in correctly answered problems and attentionas indexed by head turning responses. Though the population utilized in this exper-iment is a great deal older and diagnostically different than those usually studiedwithin this context, this study was included because it demonstrates that even thoseindividuals who may be mentally challenged can be trained to alter brain wave pat-terns within an EEG biofeedback training paradigm.

In reviewing these few studies of alpha biofeedback training, a proportion of indi-viduals were shown to have successfully increased alpha. Slight to no differences inhyperactive behaviors though were reported and enhanced reading comprehensionabilities proved to be the only significant cognitive change. Unfortunately, no follow-up data were supplied. Because of the scant number of studies found, the limitednumber of subjects (total

n

=

31 for all four studies combined), diagnostically mixedsubject populations, and the small though significant changes in alpha, no links orconclusions can be drawn about the efficacy of this approach with LD and/or ADDchildren at this time. Furthermore, in terms of alpha attenuation biofeedback, thoughall mentally challenged subjects involved in the one study cited

47

successfullydecreased alpha (in conjunction with increasing attention and correctly answeredquestions), assessing the efficacy of this approach needs to be delayed until additionalresearch with sufficient numbers of subjects and control groups accumulates which isspecific to the ADD and LD populations.

Page 11: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

352 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

HEMISPHERE-SPECIFIC BIOFEEDBACK

Five studies were also located which applied unilateral or bilateral biofeedback toalter brain waves in the cerebral hemispheres of LD and ADD children/adolescents.Murphy and Darwin

48

trained three groups of LD adolescents who manifested cere-bral dysfunction and accompanying depression and significant (15 points or greater)verbal-performance IQ score differences. Subjects were placed into either an alpha,beta or no training (control) group. Training occurred over ten sessions and targetedthe left hemisphere, specifically the left occipital-temporal region. It was reported thatalpha training resulted in such affective changes as reduced anxiety, improvementsin emotional disinhibition, and increased expressed warmth as well as enhancedarithmetic performance on the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT). These sameauthors also trained one individual who evidenced right cerebral dysfunction anda significantly depressed performance relative to verbal IQ score (30 points) withthirty-five sessions of varied EEG biofeedback which resulted in a reduction of righthemispheric frequencies, an elimination of his cerebral brain wave asymmetry, a sig-nificant performance IQ elevation, and academic improvement. Similarly, Patmonand Murphy

49

trained hyperactive and LD adolescents to increase or decrease domi-nant left occipital-temporal EEG frequencies in eight 21-minute sessions and foundthat increased frequency training was accompanied by corresponding dominant EEGalterations and reduced muscular tension (EMG), but no changes across hyperkineticor cognitive indices. On the other hand, while decreased frequency biofeedbackresulted in improved reading scores, no behavioral or physiological (including EEG)changes were noted. Cunningham and Murphy

50

tested the effectiveness of bilateralEEG biofeedback with twenty-four LD adolescent boys evidencing cerebral dysfunc-tion. They defined such dysfunction as verbal IQ scores that were at least twelvepoints lower than performance IQ scores. The authors divided the sample into threegroups, training one group to increase dominant EEG frequencies in the right hemi-sphere while decreasing dominant frequencies in the left and a second to decreasedominant frequencies in both hemispheres over eight weekly twenty-one minute ses-sions. A third group, which acted as a control group, received no training. Followingtreatment, both biofeedback groups showed decrements in dominant left hemisphereEEG frequencies, but no changes in dominant right hemisphere frequencies. In addi-tion, no cognitive changes ensued besides an improvement in arithmetic performanceevidenced in those individuals trained to increase dominant frequencies in the righthemisphere while decreasing those in the left hemisphere.

Lastly, Carter and Russell

9

utilized left hemisphere training to alternately pro-duce alpha or beta activity in three LD boys with depressed verbal WISC-R IQscores and one with depressed performance IQ scores in biweekly sessions over aperiod of eight weeks. Though statistical analyses were not attempted due to thesmall sample size and limited data, the authors reported that the three subjects withdepressed pretreatment verbal IQ scores showed an elevation in their verbal scoresas well as a reduction of their verbal/performance discrepancies. On the other hand,the single subject who demonstrated depressed performance IQ scores showed nochange following treatment. In addition, Russell and Carter

51

applied EMG andEEG hemisphere specific beta biofeedback to LD diagnosed individuals and found

Page 12: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

353RAMIREZ

et al

.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

significant elevations in verbal IQ accompanying left hemisphere training and per-formance IQ accompanying right hemisphere training.

In terms of attaining intended EEG changes with this approach, results are varied.For example, one study

48

reported a single case to have successfully decreased righthemispheric frequencies and eliminated cerebral asymmetry following biofeedback.Another

50 described both their experimental groups as having attained a decrementin dominant left hemispheric frequencies, but no intended alterations in right fre-quencies. Finally, another study49 reported attainment of increased dominant lefthemisphere frequencies and decreased EMG in their increased frequency group, butno physiological changes (EEG or EMG) for those in the decreased frequency group.

With respect to cognitive and behavioral changes, deceased anxiety and increasedexpressed warmth as well as elevated arithmetic, reading and verbal/performance IQscores have been reported following hemisphere-specific biofeedback training,although no follow-ups were reported. Though significantly positive results werereported in a number of these studies, links between achieved gains in hemispheric-specific biofeedback as well as conclusions regarding the efficacy of this approachcannot be made at this time because of the small number of studies found, the vari-ability in resulting EEG alterations, heterogeneity of treatment approaches utilized(i.e., area and EEG activity targeted), and limited sample sizes. It should be notedthat most of the work cited focused on adolescents as opposed to children with ADDand LD.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

While anecdotal and case study reports cite promising evidence, no definitiveconclusions can be drawn about the efficacy of enhanced alpha and hemisphere spe-cific biofeedback training as an effective technique in treating ADD, ADHD, or LD.While desired EEG alterations were achieved by a proportion of individuals trained,minimal to no corresponding behavioral or cognitive changes emerged, with somestudies even failing to measure such changes. This is further compounded by thesmall number of studies found utilizing this approach, the general lack of controlgroups, no follow-up data, and mixed diagnostic subject pools. In general, the effi-cacy of alpha biofeedback as a rehabilitative technique with ADD and LD individu-als has yet to be studied in a scientifically rigorous fashion.

The hemisphere specific biofeedback studies cited report variable degrees of pos-itive EEG, cognitive and behavioral findings. However, a paucity of studies employ-ing this technique, coupled many methodological shortcomings, severely limits anypositive conclusions as regards this therapeutic approach. Perhaps future rigidly con-trolled studies will prove hemispheric specific biofeedback to be useful, however, atpresent, it should be considered a highly experimental procedure.

A promising treatment technique for this population involves theta/beta biofeed-back training. Given evidence of the association between increased beta and cognitivefunctions such as attention52 as well as between theta activity and drowsiness,53,54 atreatment paradigm which involves increasing beta while decreasing theta activityhas inherent face validity. Studies have reported significant academic, intellectual andbehavioral gains having been attained with this approach, even in the absence of other

Page 13: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

354 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

treatment modalities, such as medication. Follow-ups have also provided some evi-dence for the longevity of results, although the number of follow-up studies are ratherlimited. In order to link these gains to biofeedback and to draw more definitive con-clusions, more controlled research utilizing these techniques is necessary. Moreover,this research needs to be guided by stricter methodologies including larger and morehomogenous populations, unified treatment protocols, valid and reliable outcomemeasures, follow-up reports, etc.

The approach with the most published research (n = 7) located through our Psy-chlit and Medline searches involves SMR biofeedback training. For the most part,subjects were shown to successfully increase SMR production and to exhibit signif-icant behavioral and cognitive changes during and following SMR training, with areversal of these gains being evident during a counterconditioning procedure. In sev-eral investigations, these changes were shown to be superior to those attained byalternative treatments including medication and resource classroom instruction andto be maintained even when these alternative treatments were discontinued. Whilethe links between achieved gains and SMR biofeedback training is the strongestamongst the treatment paradigms reviewed, a paucity of controlled studies utilizingthis approach continues to hinder wider acceptance of this procedure. Similarly totheta/beta biofeedback then, further research endeavors with stricter methodologiesneeds to be conducted within this treatment paradigm.

SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

Future work investigating the efficacy of SMR, theta/beta, alpha and hemispherespecific biofeedback techniques with ADD and LD children as well as adolescentsand adults is obviously necessary. Such work needs to include larger sample sizes,more homogeneous populations and treatment protocols, control groups and follow-up reports. The cost effectiveness of EEG biofeedback treatment relative to standardtreatment options, including medication, also needs to be investigated. Researchendeavors which utilize promising but, as of yet, unsubstantiated biofeedback pro-cedures including alpha attenuation training with ADD and LD individuals may alsoprove fruitful. Furthermore, as Linden et al.6 suggest, investigations consisting of amultiple group design which compare children with ADD or LD treated with EEGbiofeedback with a control group as well as those receiving other forms of treatment(e.g., behavior modification) would be of critical import in assessing the efficacy ofthese techniques versus other available treatment options. We would like to seefuture studies which employ double blind, placebo controlled designs with seriallongitudinal follow-up at one, three, five, ten, and even fifteen years in order to firmlyestablish the efficacy of EEG biofeedback training as a treatment technique in ame-liorating symptoms of ADD.

While random assignment to experimental versus control groups is logisticallyeasier to accomplish than matching samples, the very limited sample sizes in manyof the cited studies makes matched samples a more robust methodological approach.In addition to matched samples, such studies need to focus on children, adolescents,and adults, as the possibility of maturationally mediated brain wave changes needsto be considered. The establishment of consistent standardized procedures across

Page 14: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

355RAMIREZ et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

studies, including electrode placement, type of brain wave activity reinforced,response, as well as the number, frequency and length of treatment sessions criteriais also imperative for further research and clinical endeavors.

Yet another area in need of further investigation involves the underlying mecha-nisms of EEG brain wave formation. Influences on brain wave activity which havebeen proposed include excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials, thalamo-cortical relays, brainstem reticular formation relays, amongst others. The possibleeffects of individual variability on EEG biofeedback training must also be subjectedto further research as certain EEG patterns may portend different underlying pro-cesses across individuals.10 As Walsh55 states, significant variability exists in respectto subjective reports associated with EEG alpha biofeedback training.

It may very well be that EEG biofeedback treatment represents an efficacioustreatment option for individuals who suffer from ADD. However, its acceptance as aviable alternative to, or augmentation of, standard intervention with medicationremains to be seen. Unfortunately, EEG biofeedback is often “perceived” as esoteric,much in the same way that acupuncture was once frowned upon by the medicalestablishment. In order to assuage this perception, those proponents in the forefrontof the EEG biofeedback research movement must increase the amount of supportivedata obtained within the context of rigidly controlled studies. Once accomplished, itmay well lead to wider acceptance of EEG biofeedback as an efficacious treatmentmodality for individuals afflicted with ADD and ADHD.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors would like to thank Dr. L. Eugene Arnold for reading this manuscriptand providing helpful suggestions.

REFERENCES

1. TANSEY, M.A. & R.I. BRUNER. 1983. EMG and EEG biofeedback training in the treat-ment of a 10-year-old hyperactive boy with a developmental reading disorder. Bio-feedback Self-Regul. 8(1): 25–37.

2. SHOUSE, M.N. & J.F. LUBAR. 1979. Operant conditioning of EEG rhythms and ritalinin the treatment of hyperkinesis. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 4(4): 299–312.

3. NALL, A. 1973. Alpha training and the hyperkinetic child—is it effective? AcademicTher. 9(1): 5–19.

4. SCHWARTZ, G.E. & J. BEATTY. 1977. Biofeedback: Theory and Research. AcademicPress. New York.

5. LUBAR, J.F. & M.N. SHOUSE. 1977. Use of biofeedback in the treatment of seizure dis-orders and hyperactivity. Adv. Child Clin. Psychol. :203–265.

6. LINDEN, M., T. HABIB & V. RADOJEVIC. 1996. A controlled study of the effects ofEEG biofeedback on cognition and behavior of children with attention deficit disor-der and learning disabilities. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 2(1): 35–49.

7. BROWN, B.B. 1977. Stress and the Art of Biofeedback. Harper & Row. New York.8. COBB, D.E. & J.R. EVANS. 1981. The use of biofeedback techniques with school-aged

children exhibiting behavioral and/or learning problems. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.9(2): 251–281.

9. CARTER, J.L. & H.L. RUSSELL. 1981. Changes in verbal-performance IQ discrepancyscores after left hemisphere EEG frequency control training: a pilot report. Am. J.Clin. Biofeedback 4(1): 66–67.

Page 15: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

356 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

10. LUBAR, J.F. 1989. Electroencephalographic biofeedback and neurological applica-tions. In Biofeedback: Principles and Practice for Clinicians, 3rd edit. J.V. Basma-jian, Ed. Williams & Wilkins. Baltimore.

11. LUBAR, J.F. 1995. Neurofeedback for the management of attention-deficit/hyperactiv-ity disorder. In Biofeedback: A Practitioner’s Guide, 2nd edit. M.S. Schwartz, Ed.Guilford Press. New York.

12. MUEHL, S., J. KNOTT & A. BENTON. 1965. EEG abnormality and psychological testperformanced in reading disability. Cortex 1: 434–439.

13. WINKLER, A., J. DIXON & J. PARKER. 1970. Brain function in problem children andcontrols: psychometric, neurological, electroencephalogic comparisons. Am. J. Psy-chiatry 127: 94–105.

14. LUBAR, J.F., K. BIANCHINI, W. CALHOUN, et al. 1985. Spectral analysis of EEG differ-ences between children with and without learning disabilities. J. Learn. Disabil. 18:403–408.

15. MANN, C.A., J.R. LUBAR, A.W. ZIMMERMAN, et al. 1991. Quantitative analysis ofEEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: controlled study withclinical implications. Pediatr. Neurol. 8: 30–36.

16. WEISS, G. & L. TROKENBERG-HECHTMAN. 1993. Hyperactive Children Grown Up:ADHD in Children, Adolescents, and Adults, 2nd edit. Guilford Press. New York.

17. ZAMETKIN, A.J., T.E. NORDAHL, M. GROSS, et al. 1990. Cerebral glucose metabolismin adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset. N. Engl. J. Med. 323(20): 1361–1366.

18. LUBAR, J.F. 1991. Discourse on the development of EEG diagnostics and biofeedbackfor attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 16(3): 201–225.

19. CRAWFORD, H.J. & M. BARABASZ. 1996. Quantitative EEG magnitudes in childrenwith and without attention deficit disorder during neurological screening and cogni-tive tasks. Child Study J. 26(1): 71–86.

20. FULLER, P.W. 1978. Attention and the EEG alpha rhythm in learning disabled chil-dren. J. Learn. Disabil. 1(5): 303–312.

21. MULHOLLAND, T.B. 1969. The concept of attention and the electroencephalographicalpha rhythm. In Attention in Neurophysiology. C.R. Evans & T.B. Mulholland, Eds.Butterworths. London.

22. ROURKE, B.P. 1975. Brain-behavior relationships in children with learning disabilities:a research program. Am. Psychol. 3(9): 911–920.

23. GARCIA-SANCHEZ, C. & A. ESTEVEZ-GONZALEZ. 1997. Right hemisphere dysfunctionin subjects with attention-deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity. J. ChildNeurol. 12(2): 107–115.

24. HEILMAN, K.M., K.K. VOELLER & S.E. NADEAU. 1991. A possible psthophysiologicsubstrate of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Neurol. 6(Suppl.): S76–S81.

25. MALONE, M.A., J. COUITIS, J.R. KERSHNER & W.J. LOGAN. 1994. Right hemispheredysfunction and methylphenidate effects in children with attention-deficit/hyperac-tivity disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 4(4): 245–253.

26. FINLEY, W.W., H.A. SMITH & M.D. ETHERTON. 1975. Reduction of seizures and nor-malization of the EEG in a severe epileptic following sensorimotor biofeedbacktraining: preliminary study. Biol. Psychol. 2: 189–203.

27. STERNMAN, M.B., L.R. MACDONALD & R.K. STONE. 1974. Biofeedback training of thesensorimotor EEG rhythm in man: effects on epilepsy. Epilepsia 15: 395.

28. CHASE, M.H. & R.M. HARPER. 1971. Somatomotor and visceromotor correlates ofoperantly conditioned 12-14 c/sec sensorimotor cortical activity. J. Electroencephal.Clin. Neurophysiol. 31: 85–92.

29. ROTH, S.R., M.B. STERNMAN & C.D. CLEMENTE. 1967. Comparison of EEG correlatesof reinforcement, internal inhibition, and sleep. J. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophys-iol. 23: 509–520.

30. STERNMAN, M.B. & W.A. WYRWICKA. 1967. EEG correlates of sleep: evidence forseparate forebrain substrates. Brain Res. 6: 143–163.

Page 16: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

357RAMIREZ et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD

31. WYRWICKA, W. & M.B. STERNMAN. 1968. Instrumental conditioning of sensorimotorcortex EEG spindles in the waking cat. Physiol. Behav. 3: 703.

32. LUBAR, J.F. & W.W. BAHLER. 1976. Behavioral management of epileptic seizures fol-lowing EEG biofeedback training of the sensorimotor rhythm. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 1(1): 77–104.

33. LUBAR, J.F. & M.N. SHOUSE. 1976. EEG and behavioral changes in a hyperkineticchild concurrent with training of the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR). Biofeedback Self-Regul. 1(3): 293–306.

34. SHOUSE, M.N. & J.F. LUBAR. 1979. Sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) operant conditioningand methylphenidate in the treatment of hyperkinesis. Biofeedback Self Regul. 4:299–311.

35. HAMPSTEAD, W.J. 1979. The effects of EMG-assisted relaxation training with hyperk-inetic children: a behavioral alternative. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 4: 113–125.

36. TANSEY, M.A. 1993. Ten-year stability of EEG biofeedback results for a hyperactiveboy who failed fourth grade perceptually impaired class. Biofeedback Self-Regul.(1): 33–44.

37. TANSEY, M.A. 1984. EEG sensorimotor rhythm biofeedback training: some effects onthe neurologic precursors of learning disabilities. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1: 163–177.

38. LUBAR, J.O. & J.F. LUBAR. 1984. Electroencephalographic biofeedback of SMR andbeta for treatment of attention deficit disorders in a clinical setting. BiofeedbackSelf-Regul. 9(1): 1–23.

39. TANSEY, M.A. 1985. Brainwave signatures—an index reflective of the brain’s func-tional neuroanatomy: further findings on the effect of EEG sensorimotor rhythm bio-feedback training on the neurologic precursors of learning disabilities. Int. J.Psychophysiol. 3: 85–99.

40. TANSEY, M.A. 1990. Righting the rhythms of reason: EEG biofeedback training as atherapeutic modality in a clinical office setting. Med. Psychother. 3: 57–68.

41. TANSEY, M.A. 1991. Wechsler (WISC-R) changes following treatment of learning dis-abilities via EEG biofeedback training in a private practice setting. Austral. J. Psy-chol. 43: 147–153.

42. LUBAR, J.F., M.O. SWARTWOOD, J.F. SWARTWOOD & P.H. O’DONNELL. 1995. Evalua-tion of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical set-ting as measured by changes in T.O.V.A. scores, behavioral ratings, and WISC-Rperformance. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 20(1): 83–99.

43. ALHAMBRA, M.A., T.P. FOWLER & A.A. ALHAMBRA. 1995. EEG biofeedback: a newtreatment option for ADD/ADHD. J. Neurotherapy 1(2): 39–43.

44. PRATT, R.R., H.H. ABEL & J. SKIDMORE. 1995. The effects of neurofeedback trainingwith background music on EEG patterns of ADD and ADHD children. Int. J. ArtsMed. 4(1): 24–31.

45. O’MALLEY, J.E. & C.K. CONNERS. 1972. The effect of unilateral alpha training onvisual evoked response in a dyslexic adolescent. Psychophysiology 9: 467–470.

46. GRACENIN, C.T. & J.E. COOK. 1977. Alpha biofeedback and LD children. AcademicTher. 12: 275–279.

47. JACKSON, G.M. & D.A. EBERLY. 1982. Facilitation of performance on an arithmetictask as a result of the application of a biofeedback procedure to suppress alpha waveactivity. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 7(2): 211–221.

48. MURPHY, P.J., J. DARWIN & D. MURPHY. 1977. EEG feedback training for cerebraldysfunction: A research program with learning disabled adolescents. BiofeedbackSelf-Regul. 2: 288.

49. PATMON, R. & P.J. MURPHY. 1978. Differential treatment efficacy of EEG and EMGfeedback for hyperactive adolescents. Presented at the Ninth Annual Meeting of theBiofeedback Society of America, Albuquerque, NM, March 4, 1978.

50. CUNNINGHAM, M.D. & P.J. MURPHY. 1981. The effects of bilateral EEG biofeedbackon verbal, visuo-spatial, and creative skills in learning disabled male adolescents. J.Learn. Disabil. 14(4): 204–208.

51. RUSSELL, H. & J. CARTER. 1982. EEG and EMG hemispheric specific biofeedback.Presented at the Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Biofeedback Society in America,Chicago, IL, March 7, 1982.

Page 17: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

358 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES

52. SHEER, D.E. 1974. Electroencephalographic studies in learning disabilities. In TheLearning Disabled Child. H. Eichenwald & A. Talbot, Eds. University of TexasMed. Center, Dallas.

53. GREEN, E.E., A.M. GREEN & E.D. WALTERS. 1970. Voluntary control of internalstates: psychological and physiological. J. Transpersonal Psychol. 2: 1–26.

54. BROWN, B.B. 1971. Awareness of EEG-subject activity relationships detected within aclosed feedback system. Psychophysiology 7: 451–464.

55. WALSH, D.H. 1974. Interactive effects of alpha feedback and instructional set on sub-jective state. Psychophysiology 11: 428–435.

Page 18: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

ADHD AND EEGLOO & BARKLEY

Clinical Utility of EEG in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity DisorderSandra K. Loo and Russell A. Barkley

UCLA Neuropsychiatric Institute, Los Angeles, California, USA,and Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina, USA

Electrophysiological measures were among the first to be used to study brain processes in chil-dren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Diagnostic and Statistical Manualof Mental Disorders [4th ed.], American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and have been used assuch for over 30 years (see Hastings & Barkley, 1978, for an early review). More recently, elec-troencephalography (EEG) has been used both in research to describe and quantify the under-lying neurophysiology of ADHD, but also clinically in the assessment, diagnosis, and treat-ment of ADHD. This review will first provide a brief overview of EEG and then present some ofthe research findings of EEG correlates in ADHD. Then, the utility of EEG in making anADHD diagnosis and predicting stimulant response will be examined. Finally, and more con-troversially, we will review the results of the most recent studies on EEG biofeedback(neurofeedback) as a treatment for ADHD and the issues that remain to be addressed in the re-search examining the efficacy this therapeutic approach.

Key words: EEG biofeedback, diagnosis, treatment, neurotheraphy

Electroencephalography (EEG) measures reflect thecorrespondence between intracranial electrical currentsand the resulting voltages on the scalp reflecting certainfacets of brain electrical function and processing, suchas how electrically active various brain regions are andhow responsive they may be to stimuli or during cogni-tive tasks. Early EEG studies found that children withattention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) exhibitEEG abnormalities such as excess slow wave activityand epileptiform spike and wave activity (Satterfield,Cantwell, & Satterfield, 1974). These findings wereinterpreted as indicating abnormal brain processesamong children with ADHD, specifically a matura-tional delay marked by underarousal. Recent advancesin technology have resulted in more accurate quantifi-cation of EEG activity by allowing computation of am-plitude and power values for specific frequency bandsof activity, source localization, and brain electricalactivity mapping. Electrophysiological techniques(event-related potential [ERP] and EEG) are non-invasive, are less sensitive to movement artifact, do notinclude radioactive isotopes, and offer excellent tempo-

ral resolution (EEG can measure changes in the brain tothe millisecond). The spatial resolution (i.e., where theEEG signal is coming from), however, is sometimesdifficult to determine because electrical currents re-corded from the cortex do not always bear a direct rela-tion to any specific underlying brain structure and areaffected by many sources of electrical artifacts.

When examining EEG activity, scientists and clini-cians often look at the activity within a specific fre-quency band. Frequency refers to the number of oscilla-tions (or cycles) within a given time period (e.g., fourcycles per second). Note that EEG waveforms are amixture of several different frequency bands, which aretransformed and quantified for further analysis. In addi-tion, although it is possible to decompose the EEG sig-nal into different frequency bands, they are part of a dy-namic milieu that acts in concert. Thus, certaincognitive or behavioral characteristics have been asso-ciated with a frequency band, but it is also the relation-ship among frequencies in other areas of the brain thatproduce complex behaviors. Details about the specificfrequency bands are presented in the table below alongwith a brief summary of some findings concerningADHD and its subtypes.

In addition to looking at activity in the individual fre-quency bands, theta/beta and theta/alpha ratios havealso been examined and are thought to reflect level of

64

Applied Neuropsychology2005, Vol. 12, No. 2, 64–76

Copyright 2005 byLawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Requests for reprints should be sent to Sandra K. Loo, UCLADepartment of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, 760 West-wood Plaza, NPI 47–406, Los Angeles, CA 90024, USA. E-mail:[email protected]

Page 19: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

cortical arousal and maturational delay, respectively.Though these may seem redundant measures of the in-dividual frequency bands, they have been proposed tobe a better way to capture the relative levels of these off-setting brain activation patterns (Monastra, Lubar, &Linden, 2001).

EEG Correlates in ADHD Children

Comparisons of ADHDand Normal Children

Early reviews of studies of electrophysiologicalmeasures collected on hyperactive or ADHD childrenconcluded that the disorder was most likely associatedwith problems of underreactivity to stimulation andtask demands with less evidence supporting restingunderarousal in the disorder (Hastings & Barkley,1978; Rosenthal & Allen, 1978). Recent studies havehelped to support, clarify, and further refine these earlystudies (for a comprehensive review of EEG findings,see Barry, Clarke, & Johnstone, 2003). Current re-search findings show that most children with ADHDdisplay fairly consistent EEG differences in brain elec-trical activity when compared to normal children, par-ticularly regarding frontal and central theta activity,which is associated with underarousal and indicative ofdecreased cortical activity (Chabot & Serfontein, 1996;Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, & Selikowitz, 1998, 2001a;El-Sayed, Larsson, Persson, & Rydelius, 2002;Lazzaro et al., 1998). In the largest EEG study ofADHD to date (with a sample of over 400 children),Chabot and Serfontein (1996) found that children withADHD displayed increased theta power, slight eleva-tions in frontal alpha power, and diffuse decreases inbeta mean frequency. Increased theta power is the most

consistent finding in this ADHD EEG literature, indi-cating that cortical hypoarousal is a commonneuropathological mechanism in ADHD.

It has also been suggested that the theta/beta ratio isassociated with cortical arousal that has also beenshown to consistently differentiate between ADHD andnormal samples (Bresnahan & Barry, 2002; Clarke etal., 2001a; Monastra et al., 2001). Furthermore, thismeasurement has been shown to be stable over time;EEG recording from a single electrode at the vertex(Cz) yielded that a 1-month reliability of the theta/betaratio was .96, p < .05 (Monastra et al., 2001). Thetheta/beta ratio has been found to discriminate betweenindividuals with ADHD and normal controls across theage range (Bresnahan, Anderson, & Barry, 1999) andtheoretically makes sense given that frequency bandsare part of a milieu rather than occurring in isolation.

Aside from the findings for theta and the theta/betaratio, results for other frequency bands such as beta andalpha have been more variable among children withADHD. The findings for beta (indicative of heightenedcortical arousal) activity have been less consistent, withseveral studies finding decreased beta activity in frontaland central regions (Chabot & Serfontein, 1996; Clarkeet al., 1998, 2001a; Lazzaro et al., 1998; Mann, Lubar,Zimmerman, Miller, & Muenchen, 1992) and othersnot (Janzen, Graap, Stephanson, Marshall, & Fitz-simmons, 1995; Kuperman, Johnson, Arndt, Lindgren,& Wolraich, 1996; Satterfield, Schell, Backs, &Hidaka, 1984). There also appears to be a small groupof ADHD children (~15%) who show an excess of betaactivity (Chabot & Serfontein, 1996; Clarke, Barry,McCarthy, & Selikowitz, 2001c). With the exception ofslightly elevated levels of temper tantrums and moodi-ness, this group of children appears to be behaviorallysimilar to other ADHD children, although they aremore likely to be ADHD–Combined Type (Clarke et

65

ADHD AND EEG

Table 1. Summary of EEG Frequency Bands and ADHD

Frequency Band Delta Theta Alpha Beta

Cycles per second (Hz) < 4 4–7 8–12 >13Associated feeling states Sleep, unconscious Drowsiness, unfocused Eyes closed; relaxed,

but alertMental activity,

concentrationFindings in ADHD Mixed findings: Increased

in some ADHD, normalor decreased levels inothers

Increased in frontal andcentral area, continuesinto adulthood

Mixed findings: Maydepend on age,gender, or subtype

Decreased in some but notall ADHD children,may normalize with inadults

ADHD subtype Increased inADHD–CombinedType

Increased inADHD–CombinedType

Increased inADHD–InattentiveType

Decreased inADHD–CombinedType

Note: ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Page 20: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

al., 2001c). Similarly, studies have found mixed resultsfor alpha power, with some studies showing it to be in-creased (Chabot & Serfontein, 1996; Clarke et al.,2001a), others showing it to be decreased (El-Sayed etal., 2002), and still other studies finding it to be similar(Bresnahan et al., 1999) between children with ADHDand normal controls. Gender and age may play a role inthese discrepant findings, as well as differences inmethods such as sampling (i.e., community vs. clinicalsamples), diagnostic procedures, and EEG data collec-tion and processing.

EEG Differences AmongADHD Subtypes

Another possible explanation for the variability inEEG findings may lie in patterns of EEG activity ac-cording to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual ofMental Disorders (4th ed. [DSM–IV], American Psy-chiatric Association, 1994) ADHD (Inattentive, Hyper-active–Impulsive or Combined) subtypes. Subtype dif-ferences, however, have not been well studied, withonly a handful of groups examining this question.Chabot and Serfontein (1996) looked at differentgroups of children with attention disorders (normal, at-tention problems [but subthreshold ADHD], ADHD)and found that differences were more quantitative thanqualitative, with the children having the most severesymptoms showing the greatest EEG abnormality. An-other group that has systematically studied subtype dif-ferences found that children with ADHD–CombinedType (CT) exhibited more absolute and relative theta,and higher theta/alpha and theta/beta ratios when com-pared to those with ADHD–Inattentive (I) type (Clarkeet al., 1998, 2001a; Clarke et al., 2003). The differentia-tion between ADHD subtypes by the theta/beta powerratio was not independently replicated (Monastra et al.,2001); thus more study is required. Nonetheless, theseresults suggest that it is the ADHD–CT children whoshow the classic pattern found in earlier studies ofgreater underarousal and maturational delay thanADHD–I children. In contrast, ADHD–I children ex-hibited more relative alpha in the posterior regions thanthose with ADHD–CT, which is consistent with reportsof slower cognitive processing and increased rates ofdaydreaming among these kids. Developmental studiessuggest that there are actually two distinct componentsin ADHD that may be quantifiable with EEG. The firstis a hyperactive–impulsive component that appears tonormalize with increasing age and the second is an inat-tentive component that does not normalize with in-creasing age and therefore represents a deviation from

normal development (Barry et al., 2003). This hypothe-sis is consistent with the clinical phenomenology ofADHD, where the hyperactive symptoms often de-crease substantially with age, but inattention and disor-ganization remain problematic longer into develop-ment (Hart, Lahey, Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995).

It is likely, however, that the use of the DSM–IV ap-proach to ADHD subtyping will not be the most fruitfulmeans of clinical description since diagnosis does notinform treatment or predict treatment response (Pel-ham, 2001). Furthermore, the different ADHD sub-types are based on behavioral criteria with no consider-ation of underlying pathophysiology. An alternate wayof classifying subgroups of ADHD children may be ac-cording to their EEG patterns, which may reflect CNSabnormality. In studies examining whether there aredistinct EEG defined subgroups of ADHD children,studies have found that both the ADHD–CT andADHD–I have at least two distinct clusters with similarEEG profiles: a hypoaroused group and a maturationallag group (Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, Selikowitz, &Brown, 2002). Further work is needed to replicate thesesubgroups in other ADHD samples and to determinewhether this approach to ADHD diagnosis can aid intreatment response and prediction. These findings maybe consistent with research that suggests that a subset ofADHD children (perhaps 30%–50%) now classified asInattentive Type may have qualitatively different prob-lems with attention, cognitive, social, and academicfunctioning as well as treatment response profiles(Milich, Ballentine, & Lynam, 2001). This subset is re-ferred to as having Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT;McBurnett, Pfiffner, & Frick, 2001) and some havesuggested that it may constitute a separate, distinct dis-order from ADHD. More work is needed to determinewhether one of these EEG defined subgroups is associ-ated with the SCT subgroup.

EEG Findings Among Adolescentsand Adults With ADHD

Studies that have examined developmental trendsamong normal and ADHD individuals have also docu-mented EEG abnormalities across the lifespan inADHD samples. Developmental studies of EEG amongnormal samples have found decreasing theta and in-creasing beta activity with age, with alpha activity ini-tially increasing into adolescence and then decreasinginto adulthood (Bresnahan & Barry, 2002; Gasser,Verleger, Bacher, & Sroka, 1988; John et al., 1983). Thetheta/beta ratio also decreases with increasing ageamong normal samples (Bresnahan et al., 1999;

66

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 21: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Monastra et al., 2001). Similar to children with ADHD,adolescents with ADHD had higher levels of theta ac-tivity and higher theta/beta ratios across development,which remained abnormally high into adulthood. Betaactivity was also significantly reduced among adoles-cents with ADHD when compared to normal samples;however, this normalized into adulthood, except in pos-terior locations (Bresnahan et al., 1999; El-Sayed et al.,2002; Hermens et al., 2004). Thus, these findings haveled some to hypothesize that frontal–central theta activ-ity is related to impulsivity and frontal–central beta ac-tivity with hyperactivity. This might mirror the clinicalphenomenology of decreased gross motor activity asADHD children get older; however, at the level of be-havior ratings, hyperactivity and impulsivity form asingle dimension of child behavior, not two distinct do-mains as suggested by these results (Achenbach, 2001;DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & Reid, 1999).Attentional systems in the posterior regions of the brain(Levy & Swanson, 2001; Mirsky, 1996; Posner &Dehaene, 1994) may be related to EEG abnormalities inthe alpha or beta bands seen in parietal regions.

Diagnostic Utility of EEG in ADHD

Sensitivity and specificity of EEG. To study thediagnostic utility of any new instrument (in this caseEEG), one should compare its ability to correctly iden-tify those with a diagnosis (made with the “gold stan-dard”) and those with no diagnosis. Sensitivity tells youwhat percent of ADHD children have an abnormal EEGand specificity tells you what percent of non-ADHDchildren have a normal EEG. In several studies, theEEG has demonstrated good sensitivity (90%–97%)and specificity (84%–94%; Chabot, Merkin, Wood,Davenport, & Serfontein, 1996; Monastra et al., 2001;Monastra et al., 1999). This means that, when you havea group of children with ADHD, a high percentage ofkids will have a corresponding abnormal EEG(increased theta or high theta/beta ratio); in a compari-son group of children with no ADHD, a high percent-age of those children will have a normal EEG (lowerlevels of theta or lower theta–beta ratio). But more im-portant from the standpoint of clinical diagnosis is posi-tive (PPP) and negative predictive power (NPP). PPPtells you whether an abnormal EEG can correctly pre-dict which children will receive a diagnosis of ADHDand NPP tells whether a normal EEG correctly predictswho will be normal or non-ADHD. The PPP and NPPfor EEG have been reported to be 98% and 76%, re-spectively (Monastra et al., 2001), meaning that whenthere is an abnormal EEG (high theta/beta ratio in this

case) it is highly likely that the child is ADHD. How-ever, when the EEG is within the normal range, 24% ofthose children go on to be diagnosed as ADHD usingother clinical methods. Most clinicians would considerthis to be an unacceptably high rate of misdiagnosis forclinical purposes. Furthermore, a two-group compari-son (ADHD vs. normal) of EEG diagnostic validity isnot the most appropriate way to examine predictivepower because most ADHD children referred to clinicshave at least one if not two other comorbid disorders.Thus, the issue facing the clinician is not whether thereferred case is disordered or normal, but rather, whichdisorder or set of disorders the case manifests amongthe various possible disorders (e.g., learning, depres-sion, anxiety, etc.) occurring in a clinical practice.

Differentiating ADHD and other comorbiddisorders. More instructive, therefore, are studiesexamining whether EEG can discriminate amongADHD, learning disorders, and other psychiatric disor-ders. Chabot and Serfontein published two papers(Chabot et al., 1996; Chabot & Serfontein, 1996) re-porting discrimination between normal children andthose with learning disabilities (LD) and ADHD. Whencomparing an ADHD sample to a normative databaseof normal and LD children (John et al., 1983), EEG wassensitive (93%–97%) and fairly specific (84%–90%) indifferentiating ADHD from LD. Including the possibil-ity that children may be normal lowers these valuesconsiderably with correct classification rates of 76% ofnormals, 89% of ADHD–ADD, and 70% of LD chil-dren (Chabot et al., 1996). There are also methodologi-cal issues that may affect the generality of these results.Children were diagnosed ADHD using only parent andteacher behavior rating scales, which may have led tomisdiagnosis. In addition, the ADHD sample was com-pared to a normative database where LD children werenot specifically screened for ADHD and included avery broad and heterogeneous group of children withlearning difficulties (either low IQ or normal IQ with anachievement score less than 90).

When large samples of ADHD and LD children weredirectly compared, the discriminant validity of EEGappear high enough to be potentially useful (Chabot &Serfontein, 1996). Though the classification of ADHDalone and LD alone was good (97% and 84%, respec-tively), classification of ADHD children with and with-out learning disorders was not reliable (i.e., a split halfreplication was less than 60%). The best initial classifi-cation they obtained was 65% of ADHD only and 70%of ADHD + LD children. Although these classification

67

ADHD AND EEG

Page 22: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

rates are significantly higher than chance, they still re-sult in unacceptably high rates (20%–35%) of mis-classification and therefore misdiagnosis. These stud-ies do not support the clinical utility of EEG alone indifferentiating between ADHD patients with and with-out learning disabilities.

In addition to learning disorders, most ADHD chil-dren referred to clinics have at least one if not two othercomorbid psychiatric disorders, such as other disrup-tive behavior disorders, depression, anxiety, and sub-stance abuse disorders (Barkley, 1998). It is hard to an-ticipate how these other disorders might affect the EEGmeasures and their capacity to discriminate ADHDfrom normal cases as well as those involving other dis-orders. Unpublished data (V. Monastra, personal com-munication, August, 2004) suggests that EEG has asensitivity of 78% and specificity of 95% (with an over-all classification rate of 86%) when differentiating be-tween ADHD and oppositional, anxiety, and mood dis-orders. More systematic work in this area is needed. Todate, all of the studies conducted thus far have exam-ined children with ADHD who do not have othercomorbid psychiatric conditions or where the propor-tion of comorbid disorders goes unspecified. Similarly,none of the studies to date have examined whether EEGcan differentiate or accurately classify children havingthe different ADHD subtypes. Until EEG research ad-dresses its utility in this context of diagnosticcomorbidity, it should not be used clinically in the diag-nosis of ADHD.

EEG and medication response. Relatively fewEEG studies have examined medication responseamong ADHD children. ADHD children who are medi-cation responders have been reported to have excessiveslow wave activity (Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, &Selikowitz, 2002; Satterfield et al., 1984), supportingthe theory that ADHD children are cortically hypo-aroused. In addition, stimulant medication appears to“normalize” the EEG patterns and evoked potentials ofchildren with ADHD (Jonkman et al., 1997; Verbaten etal., 1994; Winsberg, Javitt, & Silipo, 1997) and to de-crease slow wave EEG (theta) activity and increase fastwave (beta) activity depending on the task and elec-trode location (Clarke, Barry, Bond, McCarthy, &Selikowitz, 2002; Loo, Teale, & Reite, 1999; Lubar,White, Swartwood, & Swartwood, 1999; Swartwood etal., 1998). Using EEG alone or a combination of behav-ioral and EEG measures, several studies have reportedcorrect identification of 70%–80% of stimulant re-sponders (Chabot, di Michele, Prichep, & John, 2001;

Chabot et al., 1996; Prichep & John, 1992; Suffin &Emory, 1995). Similar predictive power rates (PPP andNPP ~70%) have been reported using ERP componentssuch as the P3 (Sangal & Sangal, 2004). Though thismay seem to be a relatively impressive predictive powerof the EEG for predicting medication response, it is ac-tually no better than the base rate one would haveguessed in the absence of any EEG information. Re-search repeatedly finds that ~70% of ADHD childrenplaced on a single stimulant demonstrate a positive re-sponse (Barkley, DuPaul, & McMurray, 1991;Cantwell, 1996; Findling, Short, & Manos, 2001). Un-less the EEG can significantly surpass the predictionfrom the base rate, its utility in this respect is unimpres-sive.

Summary

Collectively, the EEG findings in children, adoles-cents, and adults with ADHD are increased slow-waveactivity in frontal regions, suggesting cortical hypo-arousal, especially in the ADHD Combined subtype.Several researchers have reported that EEG measuresdiscriminate well between children with and withoutADHD and others have asserted that the EEG workswell in determining medication responders from non-responders. There is preliminary evidence that EEGcan differentiate ADHD subtypes, at least at the grouplevel of comparison, but the requisite information onaccuracy of individual classification is lacking.

Our conclusion, then, is that EEG alone, if used fordiagnosis or prediction of treatment (i.e., stimulant)response, results in unacceptably high rates of mis-diagnosis and misclassification. Although rates of70%–80% classification are interesting at the researchlevel and may be comparable to other assessment toolsalone (e.g., rating scales or computerized tests), in aclinical setting, it means that 20%–30% of children willnot receive the correct diagnosis or treatment. Thissuggests that the use of diagnostic instruments such asa structured or semistructured clinical interview,well-standardized behavior rating scales of ADHDsymptoms, and information collected from multiplesources (parent, teacher, child) are still required. Be-cause such measures must still be collected in evaluat-ing anyone for ADHD, regardless of whether an EEGhas been conducted, the EEG findings remain an inter-esting but nonessential piece of information in the diag-nostic process. Though these findings indicate somepromise for EEG as a diagnostic tool, additional sys-tematic research to empirically validate its classifica-tion accuracy is needed.

68

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 23: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

EEG Biofeedback (Neurofeedback):The EEG as Treatment Device

Given the excess of theta and decreased beta activityobserved among children with ADHD, it is easy to un-derstand the theoretical basis for examining whether al-tering these problems through treatment would result inimprovements in ADHD symptoms. This is the basicgoal of EEG biofeedback, neurofeedback, or neuro-therapy—to train the patient to decrease their slowwave activity and/or increase their fast wave EEG activ-ity, often using behavioral principles such as operantconditioning (i.e., positive reinforcement) in the pro-cess. Typically, a neurofeedback therapist places one tothree electrodes on the patient’s head, which are con-nected to a computer. The computer detects the EEG in-formation and provides a visual or auditory display ofactivity in the targeted frequency band(s). When theperson is producing the desired EEG pattern (there aredifferential training programs for alpha or theta reduc-tion and sensorimotor rhythm [SMR] or beta increase),the computer will give a positive response or reward,usually in the form of points earned. The person is thengiven a reward (e.g., money or other reinforcers) forearning a certain amount of points within each session.After many sessions of training, between 20 and 50 ascurrently practiced, it is hypothesized that a person willbe able to produce the desired EEG brain waves on theirown through increased awareness of their own physio-logical processes. Such conditioned EEG changes havebeen reported to be associated with improved or nor-malized symptoms of ADHD, to generalize outside thetreatment setting (such as at home, school, or work)even when the treatment is withdrawn (Monastra,Monastra, & George, 2002) and to be maintained intoadulthood in most treated cases (Lubar, 1991). Of noteis the fact that no other treatment approach for ADHDhas been able to demonstrate such generalization ormaintenance effects (Pelham, Wheeler, & Chronis,1998; Smith, Barkley, & Shapiro, in press).

This treatment has stirred up quite a controversy be-tween the clinical and scientific communities workingwith ADHD. Recent reviews of EEG biofeedback havegenerally concluded that preliminary studies of EEGbiofeedback are promising, but require further study inrigorous scientifically controlled studies (Arnold,2001; Nash, 2000; Ramirez, Desantis, & Opler, 2001).Proponents of EEG biofeedback feel that their studieshave been overly criticized and that the scientific com-munity has been unfairly biased against neurofeedbacktreatment, despite large numbers of participants whohave reportedly experienced positive outcomes. Critics

of EEG biofeedback, however, contend that the pub-lished studies have suffered from significantmethodological weaknesses that make interpretation ofthe results and conclusions about the actual effect ofEEG biofeedback impossible.

Many of these flaws were identified a decade ago byBarkley (1992) and the same problems with scientificmethodology that existed then continue to exist withthese newer studies. The flaws included no controlgroups, the confounding of several different treatmentswithin the EEG biofeedback group, use of small num-bers of participants, diagnostic uncertainty about thechildren in the study, lack of placebo control proce-dures, absence of blindness of the evaluators to thetreatment received by the cases, and practice effectswith the measures being used to evaluate the ADHDchildren. Crucial yet lacking in most studies of EEGbiofeedback has been the randomized assignment ofcases to treatment and no-treatment (or placebo)groups. Instead, treatment groups are often constructedretrospectively from a series of clinical cases that havebeen previously treated or not with EEG biofeedback.Furthermore, there may exist a conflict of interest inthese findings because EEG biofeedback studies aretypically conducted by clinicians who are being paid toprovide the treatment and are published inneurotherapy journals that do not have rigorous peer re-view. As Chambless and Hollon (1998) pointed out intheir guidelines for defining empirically supportedtherapies, treatment efficacy must be demonstrated incontrolled research where it is “reasonable to concludethat benefits observed are due to the effects of the treat-ment and not to chance or confounding factors such asthe passage of time, the effects of psychological assess-ment, or the presence of different types of clients in thevarious treatment conditions.” Thus, these are not pettyor simply annoying issues that can be ignored. They arecentral to any demonstration of treatment efficacy. Inthe following paragraphs, we will review the most re-cent controlled studies of EEG biofeedback and offer asummary of where the state of EEG biofeedback liescurrently.

EEG Biofeedback Versus No Treatment

The first controlled study was completed by Linden,Habib, and Radojevic (1996) and utilized small sam-ples of ADHD patients; nine cases were randomly as-signed to receive EEG biofeedback and nine wereplaced on a wait-list. Importantly, no other treatmentwas provided simultaneously including stimulant med-ication. The dependent measures included an IQ test

69

ADHD AND EEG

Page 24: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

and two parent rating scales of ADHD symptoms andaggression. The neurofeedback group showed a signifi-cant increase in IQ and a significant decrease in parentratings of inattention. There was no significant effect ofEEG biofeedback on hyperactive–impulsive or aggres-sive behavior ratings.

This study is often cited as support for EEG biofeed-back and does incorporate some important method-ological controls such as random assignment, wait-listcontrol, and treatment integrity. Noteworthy, however,is that no pre- and posttreatment comparisons in EEGpower were reported to show that the treatment had al-tered the EEG parameters associated with ADHD. Alsoimportant to consider is that (a) no placebo controlgroup was used to control for therapist time, attention,and other demand characteristics of the treatment envi-ronment; (b) parents evaluating the children before andafter therapy were not blind to the treatment condition(nor were the children); and (c) the improvement on theIQ test is irrelevant to the demonstration of efficacy ofthis treatment for ADHD. IQ is not a measure ofADHD. Just as important, the overall or omnibus statis-tical analysis (multivariate analysis of variance, orMANOVA) of IQ and behavior ratings reported in thisarticle did not find a significant effect of treatmentgroup but rather a nonsignificant “trend” for time (pre-to posttreatment), meaning that all children, regardlessof whether they had treatment or not, showed similarlevels of improvement from the pretreatment toposttreatment evaluations. Though this may have beendue to low power because of the small sample size, fol-low-up univariate tests of a nonsignificant MANOVAare not recommended and increase the risk of Type 1(false positive) error (Weinfurt, 1998).

EEG Biofeedback VersusPlacebo Biofeedback

Most EEG biofeedback studies suffer a glaring over-sight and that is the failure to incorporate a placebo con-trol condition. There have been many reasons put forthfor not using a placebo control, such as difficulty de-signing a sham biofeedback that is not detectable by cli-nicians and patients, ethics of giving a placebo treat-ment for 6 months when other effective treatments areavailable, and feasibility of doing a placebo controlcondition within the context of a private clinical prac-tice, which is where these studies have been conducted.Nonetheless, there is no other way to control for the ef-fects of patient–therapist time, expectations generatedby applying electrodes and being connected to a com-

puter, ancillary support given to parents, and motivationand investment needed to complete treatment.

Only one study has used a placebo control group andis noteworthy for the degree of scientific rigor in itsdesign. This was the unpublished paper by Fine,Goldman, and Sandford presented at the American Psy-chological Association meeting in 1994. In this study,71 patients were randomly assigned to biofeedback, ano-treatment wait-list control group, or a placebo con-trol group involving computerized cognitive trainingprotocol. The authors collected 51 different measures,including 30 lab measures and parent ratings. Exam-iners doing the testing were blind to the treatment groupassignment of these children; however parents werenot. There were significant group differences on 12measures, eight of which came from parent ratings. Ofthe four lab measures, just one favored the biofeedbackgroup whereas the other groups did better on the re-maining three. On the parent ratings, both treatmentgroups exceeded the wait-list control group on eightsubscales from the three global rating scales. The bio-feedback group was slightly better than the placebogroup on two scales whereas the opposite was the caseon the third rating scale, that being the Child BehaviorChecklist (Fine, Goldman, & Sandford, 1994). In whatis the most methodologically sound study on EEG bio-feedback treatment outcome, using random assignmentto groups and a placebo control group with examinerblindness to treatment assignment, no compelling evi-dence of efficacy for EEG biofeedback was evident.

Additionally, Heywood and Beale (2003) employeda single-subject design with a placebo control conditionapplied to a small sample of children (N = 7). The ef-fects of EEG biofeedback were contrasted with a pla-cebo (noncontingent) feedback condition. Outcomemeasures included parent and teacher behavior ratingsas well as several cognitive tests (auditory and visualcontinuous performance tests, or CPTs, paired associ-ate learning task, and verbal fluency task) during eachof the conditions. Behavioral ratings and performanceon cognitive tasks during active and placebo feedbackconditions were compared and the results appear tosupport the effects of active EEG biofeedback on thedependent measures. These effects disappear, however,when controlling for overall trend of the data (whichhelps to account for maturation and nonspecific treat-ment effects) and including treatment noncompleters(known as an intent-to-treat design). Furthermore, theeffects of active and placebo biofeedback do not resultin changes in the treatment outcome measures that dif-fer significantly from baseline measures. Thus, onemight mistakenly conclude that there is a significant

70

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 25: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

treatment effect of EEG biofeedback only if maturationand nonspecific effects as well as treatment noncom-pleters are ignored.

Overall, of the three treatment outcome studies com-paring EEG biofeedback to either no-treatment or pla-cebo control conditions, two fail to support an activetreatment effect. These studies are, methodologicallyspeaking, the three strongest. Though the small samplesizes in the Linden et al. study may have limited statisti-cal power for comparisons, the Fine et al. study hadlarge sample sizes providing sufficient statistical powerto detect differences between conditions had they beenpresent.

EEG Biofeedback VersusOther Treatments (Medicationand Psychological)

There have been three studies that have comparedneurotherapy to other treatments, all including psycho-stimulant medication that is the gold standard in treat-ment for ADHD. If EEG biofeedback treatment dem-onstrated treatment effects that are similar (or notsignificantly inferior) to stimulant medication treat-ment, this might be taken as an indicator of equivalencein efficacy (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). Unfortu-nately, none of these studies used random assignment.Instead, they reconstructed their treatment groups afterthe fact of treatment (months or years) using samples ofclinically treated patients or allowed patients toself-select into the treatment they preferred. Also, thesestudies failed to report psychiatric or learning disordersthat often are comorbidities with ADHD, and did notincorporate evaluators who were blind to the patient’streatment condition. In addition, only one study testedEEG biofeedback by itself without confounding it withadditional treatments.

Rossiter and LaVaque (1995) were the first to com-pare EEG biofeedback to stimulant medication ingroups (23 in each group) of children and adults withADHD (ages 8 to 21 years). Rather than randomly as-signing cases to each treatment group, the authorsmatched the cases of those who previously receivedEEG treatment against those who had received stimu-lant therapy (ages 5–45 years) using age as a matchingcriterion. Again, the absence of random assignment totreatment groups is an important methodological over-sight here because such randomization helps to mini-mize inherent biases such as self-selection into the vari-ous treatments and experimenter bias in choosing thepatients in treatment group assignment that confoundefforts to draw conclusions from group comparisons.

The patients were assigned to treatment groups based inpart on their preferences, in part on whether they hadpreviously failed stimulant therapy, and in part on in-surance coverage for biofeedback. Furthermore, medi-cation (to five of the EEG cases) and additional treat-ments were provided to all cases, confounding the twotreatment groups and making interpretation of individ-ual treatment effects (medication and EEG biofeed-back) impossible. The authors reported using the Testof Variables of Attention (TOVA), a continuous perfor-mance test assessing inattention and impulsiveness, anda parent rating scale of behavioral problems, though notthe same one for all participants. Cases and their par-ents were not blind to their treatment condition, norwere the examiners testing the cases on the lab mea-sures blind to such assignment. Also, no information isprovided as to just how these cases were selected fromthe larger pool of clients likely treated in this practice.Were all available cases within a specified period oftime reclassified into these post hoc treatment groups orjust some? If not all, why were some chosen for inclu-sion in these analyses and others not?

From pre- to posttest, the EEG biofeedback groupshowed significant improvement on the TOVA and onparent ratings of inattention, hyperactivity, and inter-nalizing symptoms. So did the medication group, withno differences between them in the degree of changeshown. Important to note is that, here again, pre- andposttreatment EEG measures were not reported so as toshow that the biofeedback had changed the importantparameters of the EEG believed to mediate the changesin ADHD symptoms. Although the authors concludethat, for children who do not respond to medications,EEG biofeedback is a good treatment choice, the signif-icant scientific design problems (i.e., absence of ran-dom assignment to treatments, confounding of treat-ments, and lack of reported EEG changes) prohibitmaking such a conclusion.

In their study on EEG biofeedback, Monastra et al.(2002) reported results from samples of 51 ADHD chil-dren (6 to 19 years old) who received comprehensiveclinical care (CCC; medication, parent counseling, aca-demic support) with EEG biofeedback for 1 year and 50who had received CCC alone (no biofeedback). Again,patients were not randomly assigned to the treatmentgroups. The fact that groups were found to not differ onpretreatment scores on either the measures of ADHD(ratings) or EEG measures is not very reassuring giventhat many other variables can operate to bias treatmentstudies such as this one absent random assignment totreatment groups before initiating therapy. Results ofthe study indicate that children in the CCC + EEG bio-

71

ADHD AND EEG

Page 26: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

feedback (CCC + B) group were better at posttreatmenton behavior ratings of attention and hyperactiv-ity–impulsive behaviors (on and off medication), aswell as on the TOVA (only when tested off medication),when compared to the CCC group. In addition, at post-treatment those in the EEG group had lower theta/betaratios than the CCC group. These results indicate im-proved functioning in the CCC + B group even whenoff medication; however, the significant differences areprimarily due (surprisingly) to virtually no improve-ment in the CCC group. Close examination of the pre-and posttreatment behavior rating scale scores (on andoff medication) indicate that the CCC alone groupappear to have received a degraded version of the CCCor are treatment nonresponders. This atypical patientgroup as a comparison coupled with the lack of randomassignment, variation in individual treatment compo-nents, failure to control for the amount of time spentwith a therapist, and lack of information as to just howpatients were chosen from the larger treated pool pro-hibits interpretation concerning efficacy of any specifictreatment component (EEG biofeedback without all ofthe other treatments).

The Fuchs et al. (Fuchs, Birbaumer, Lutzenberger,Gruzelier, & Kaiser, 2003) study is the only studythus far that involves a direct comparison betweenEEG biofeedback (N = 22) and stimulant medication(N = 11) where the treatments are not confounded(i.e., stimulants given to the EEG biofeedback group).As with the previous studies, the sample descriptionlacks important information regarding ADHD subtypeand psychiatric or learning disorder comorbidity. Inaddition, the readministration of the WISC intelli-gence test within such a short time period (12 weeks)invalidates the results of the posttreatment test. Meth-odological issues (no random assignment, no controlfor additional therapist time, small sample size, no in-formation on the larger pool of treated patients fromwhich these cases were selected and why) notwith-standing, these results may suggest that EEG biofeed-back and methylphenidate result in similar levels ofshort-term change in ADHD behaviors. Yet thosemethodological issues are crucial to being able to sayanything about such a treatment effect. Replication ofthese findings with increased scientific controls andlarger sample sizes (at least 25–30 respondents percondition; Chambless & Hollon, 1998) will be a nec-essary step toward establishing EEG biofeedback asan equivalent treatment to medication. To demonstratethat EEG changes are responsible for treatment ef-fects, reporting of actual EEG changes and correlationwith treatment outcome must be shown.

Is EEG Conditioning the ActiveIngredient in Biofeedback?

The reason we come back over and over again to sci-entific methodology is that proper experimental con-trols makes it possible to discern whether training EEGpatterns is the active ingredient in the treatment. In fact,one of the biggest issues that the EEG biofeedbacktreatment literature needs to address is whether or not itis actually the training of the EEG patterns that leads toimprovement in ADHD symptoms. Though the goal ofEEG biofeedback is the “unconscious conditioning ofunderlying neurological systems [to] learn balancethrough reinforced practice” (Monastra, 2004), none ofthe studies thus far have demonstrated that the EEGchanges are the actual mediator of treatment outcome.In an earlier EEG biofeedback study (Lubar, Swart-wood, Swartwood, & O’Donnell, 1995), ~60% of chil-dren showed EEG changes with biofeedback treatment.The children who showed EEG changes (decreasedtheta) also exhibited significantly greater improvementon the TOVA (three of four scales improved) whencompared to those whose EEG did not change (one offour scales improved). And yet, there are significantoverall treatment effects in several studies. This indi-cates that other nonspecific or unintentional factors arepresent in the treatments that are helping bring aboutbehavioral and cognitive improvement. But if it isn’tEEG change, what else might be at work to elicit the be-havioral and cognitive improvements reported in thesestudies?

First, there are several nonspecific factors that mayresult in ADHD symptom improvement. Children inEEG biofeedback conditions, based on the study de-scriptions, received additional time with a therapistranging from 17 to 40 hours across studies than didcases not receiving biofeedback. Failure to control forthe amount of treatment time means that the EEG bio-feedback group may have improved simply becausethey spent more time with a therapist, are more investedin treatment and therefore more motivated to change, ormay have more stability and support from mental healthprofessionals rather than the EEG biofeedback per se,or may simply have been more likely to want to pleasethe therapist.

Another possibility is that biofeedback is simply an-other form of cognitive–behavioral training that justhappens to employ the use of electrodes placed on thehead. Under this scenario, it is not anything to do withthe electrodes or EEG that necessarily produces thetreatment effect. Instead, it is whatever conscious cog-nitive or behavioral actions the individual is actively

72

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 27: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

employing to alter the EEG activity that is being condi-tioned. For instance, in some studies, children are toldto focus their concentration on some object or on someimagined condition, such as being a heavy rock. In oth-ers they are told to find some mental activity that resultsin a change in their performance of the videogame be-ing used to give them feedback about their EEG status.In others, they are told simply to try to do better at thevideogame. Even advocates of EEG biofeedback con-cede that “attentional training through behavioral meth-ods cannot be ruled out” (Linden et al., 1996) and thatthe “factors that are essential in teaching attention/con-centration remain an empirical question” (Monastra,2004). Noteworthy here is that cognitive behavioraltraining has not been found to be effective in treatingADHD (DuPaul & Eckert, 1997; Pelham et al., 1998).Yet, rarely have children had to perform this sort of sus-tained practice (for 30–50 hr) and received such salientrewards (up to $150) for successful performance. Re-search studies have repeatedly shown that ADHD chil-dren’s performance on cognitive tests can be normal-ized with immediate and salient reinforcers (Firestone& Douglas, 1975; Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1998). Thisis also likely to lead to stimulus generalization whenchildren come into the lab for posttreatment EEG andTOVA assessments (Heywood & Beale, 2003). Thus,when children are completing the posttreatment ses-sion, they may be expecting similar rewards for perfor-mance; these behaviors may not continue, however, inother settings and if performance is not continually re-warded. Similarly, it has been suggested that alteredbreathing patterns may minimize theta activity, whichmay be a separate but correlated mechanism for treat-ment effects thought to be the result of EEG biofeed-back (Heywood & Beale, 2003). This is consistent withsome neurotherapy treatment protocols that encouragethe patient to relax, which most likely leads to deeperbreathing and increased oxygenation of blood cells inthe brain. Perhaps it is the conditioning of deeperbreathing and therefore increased cerebral perfusionthat improves ADHD symptoms.

If it is not the reinforced conditioning of EEG activ-ity per se, then the use of computers, electrodes, andamplifiers are unnecessary, similar to what was foundfor the EMG treatments in the 1970s—teaching musclerelaxation proved sufficient. This should lead to a lessexpensive, more targeted treatment focused on the ac-tive ingredient of this treatment. If, as proponents ofEEG biofeedback state, it is the EEG conditioning thatproduces the balance among the underlying neurologi-cal systems, then additional studies are needed to dem-onstrate this specific effect. While EEG biofeedback

studies with seizure patients have demonstrated corre-lation between EEG changes and clinicalsymptomatology (Sterman, 2000), this has not yet beendemonstrated in ADHD.

Summary

Although the existing studies of EEG biofeedbackclaim promising results in the treatment of ADHD, thepromise of EEG biofeedback as a legitimate treatmentcannot be fulfilled without studies that are scientificallyrigorous. Undoubtedly, treatments for ADHD wouldbenefit greatly from a nonmedication alternative that isefficacious and cost effective. But there is much work tobe done to demonstrate that EEG biofeedback providesthat alternative and that actually changing the EEG isthe mechanism of change in ADHD symptoms (as op-posed to just more time with a therapist). Without suchdemonstrations, the changes in behavior cannot in factbe attributed to this specific treatment mechanism. Itmust also be shown that treatment effects can general-ize to nontreatment settings and can persist over time.Even with such demonstrations it must also be shownthat treatment is cost effective in managing the symp-toms of ADHD relative to the prevailing empiricallysupported approaches.

Future Directions for Research on theClinical Utility of EEG in ADHD

If EEG is to be used as a diagnostic tool for ADHD,there has to be much greater clarity on its ability to dif-ferentiate ADHD from normal children, ADHD sub-types from each other, and to assess for differential di-agnoses as well as ADHD comorbidities. Workdocumenting correlations between EEG and ADHDsymptoms and subtypes is needed. Two studies (Chabot& Serfontein, 1996; Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, &Selikowitz, 2001b) have identified EEG-defined sub-types within ADHD, with one group exhibiting a higherthan normal beta power in both samples. Replication ofthese subtypes and greater description of how they re-late to current diagnostic subgroups and treatment out-come seems warranted, particularly among the excessbeta group. Though some work has been done to exam-ine the diagnostic utility of EEG in ADHD, more sys-tematic study needs to be done using rigorous diagnos-tic procedures (i.e., structured or semistructureddiagnostic interviews), careful identification of comor-bid diagnoses (including specific learning disorders)and impact of these disorders on EEG characteristics.In addition, studies examining EEG correlates of stimu-

73

ADHD AND EEG

Page 28: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

lant response should incorporate double-blind medica-tion titration and reporting of EEG differences accord-ing to varying doses of medication.

As for EEG biofeedback as a treatment for ADHD,there are clearly many issues that need to be addressedadequately in future research. The first and foremost isaddressing the methodological problems that haveplagued this treatment outcome research from the start.Proper scientific controls are crucial to demonstratingthat there is a real treatment effect due to EEG biofeed-back and that EEG conditioning is the effective ingredi-ent within the treatment. This will require clinical trialsthat incorporate random assignment to treated and un-treated groups, placebo conditions, larger sample sizes,evaluators that are blind to treatment condition, clearand comprehensive sample description (particularlywith regard to psychiatric and learning disorder comor-bidity), appropriate data analytic (statistical) proce-dures, and documentation of EEG changes that corre-late with treatment outcome. These methodologicaldifficulties compromise the internal validity of most ofthe studies reviewed here, making interpretation of theresults and conclusions about the actual effect of treat-ment impossible.

Finally, side effects of EEG biofeedback must bemonitored systematically and reported in studies. Alltruly effective treatments produce some side effects insome percentage of the population. This has to be so be-cause individuals differ in their physiological makeup,particularly brain organization and functioning. Thoseindividual differences are sufficient to result in thetreatment producing adverse effects in a subset of thepopulation. Moreover, clinical ineptitude in the deliv-ery of the treatment in some cases and as a consequenceof comorbid disorders in other cases always ensuresthat some patients will not respond well to the interven-tion as delivered. This is as true for behavioral interven-tions as it is for medications. Hence any claim that atreatment is effective yet has absolutely no associatedside effects is oxymoronic. The former cannot existwithout the latter. This may be a telling piece of infor-mation about whether neurofeedback is actually effec-tive for the management of ADHD.

Conclusion

The clinical utility of EEG in ADHD has yet to beproven. Though there are some promising results thatrequire further study, the threshold for using EEG clini-cally has not been met. Of the possible uses reviewed

here (diagnostic utility, prediction of stimulant re-sponse, and EEG biofeedback), the diagnostic utility ofEEG appears most promising although considerablework is needed for this promise to be realized. The EEGbiofeedback studies with the most rigorous methodolo-gies to date have not supported the efficacy of EEG bio-feedback when compared to no-treatment control orplacebo feedback. Methodological flaws of previousEEG studies have hampered firm conclusions regardingits usefulness and precision. Though the field of ADHDwould benefit greatly from a single diagnostic test andan effective nonmedication treatment alternative, wecannot recommend the use of EEG in a clinical settingbased on the current empirical data.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (2001). Manual for the Revised Child BehaviorProfile and Child Behavior Checklist. Burlington, VT: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statisti-cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Au-thor.

Arnold, L. E. (2001). Alternative treatments for adults with atten-tion-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Annals of the NewYork Academy of Science, 931, 310–341.

Barkley, R. A. (1992). Is EEG biofeedback treatment effective forADHD children? Proceed with much caution. Attention DeficitDisorder Advocacy Group newsletter.

Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.Scientific American, 279(3), 66–71.

Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1991). Attentiondeficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: clinical re-sponse to three dose levels of methylphenidate. Pediatrics, 87,519–531.

Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., & Johnstone, S. J. (2003). A review ofelectrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I.Qualitative and quantitative electroencephalography. ClinicalNeurophysiology, 114, 171–183.

Bresnahan, S. M., Anderson, J. W., & Barry, R. J. (1999).Age-related changes in quantitative EEG in atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 46,1690–1697.

Bresnahan, S. M., & Barry, R. J. (2002). Specificity of quantitativeEEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity dis-order. Psychiatry Research, 112(2), 133–144.

Cantwell, D. P. (1996). Attention Deficit Disorder: A Review of thePast 10 Years. Journal of the American Academy of ChildAdolescent Psychiatry, 35, 978–987.

Chabot, R. J., di Michele, F., Prichep, L., & John, E. R. (2001). Theclinical role of computerized EEG in the evaluation and treat-ment of learning and attention disorders in children and ado-lescents. Journal of Neuropsychiatry Clinical Neuroscience,13, 171–186.

Chabot, R. J., Merkin, H., Wood, L. M., Davenport, T. L., &Serfontein, G. (1996). Sensitivity and specificity of QEEG in

74

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 29: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

children with attention deficit or specific developmental learn-ing disorders. Clinical Electroencephalography, 27, 26–34.

Chabot, R. J., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitativeelectroencephalographic profiles of children with attentiondeficit disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 951–963.

Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empiricallysupported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-chology, 66, 7–18.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Bond, D., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz,M. (2002). Effects of stimulant medications on the EEG ofchildren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psycho-pharmacology (Berl), 164, 277–284.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (1998).EEG analysis in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Acomparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 81,19–29.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2001a).Age and sex effects in the EEG: differences in two subtypes ofattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophy-siology, 112, 815–826.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2001b).EEG-defined subtypes of children with attention-deficit/hy-peractivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 2098–2105.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2001c).Excess beta activity in children with attention-deficit/hyperac-tivity disorder: An atypical electrophysiological group. Psy-chiatry Research, 103, 205–218.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2002).EEG differences between good and poor responders tomethylphenidate and dexamphetamine in children with atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology,113(2), 194–205.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., & Brown,C. (2002). EEG evidence for a new conceptualization of atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology113, 1036–1044.

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., Clarke, D.C., & Croft, R. J. (2003). Effects of stimulant medications onchildren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and ex-cessive beta activity in their EEG. Clinical Neurophysiology,114, 1729–1737.

DuPaul, G. J., & Eckert, T. L. (1997). The effects of school-basedinterventions for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Ameta-analysis. School Psychology Digest, 26, 5–27.

DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (1999).The ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and clinicalinterpretation. New York: Guilford.

El-Sayed, E., Larsson, J. O., Persson, H. E., & Rydelius, P. A.(2002). Altered cortical activity in children with atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during attentional load task.Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychi-atry, 41, 811–819.

Findling, R. L., Short, E. J., & Manos, M. J. (2001). Developmentalaspects of psychostimulant treatment in children and adoles-cents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal ofthe American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 40,1441–1447.

Fine, A. H., Goldman, L., & Sandford, J. (1994, August). Innovativetechniques in the treatment of ADHD: An analysis of the im-

pact of EEG biofeedback training and a cognitive computergenerated training. Paper presented at the American Psycho-logical Association, Los Angeles, CA.

Firestone, P., & Douglas, V. (1975). The effects of reward and pun-ishment on reaction times and autonomic activity in hyperac-tive and normal children. Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 3, 201–216.

Fuchs, T., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Gruzelier, J. H., &Kaiser, J. (2003). Neurofeedback treatment for atten-tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children: A comparisonwith methylphenidate. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeed-back, 28, 1–12.

Gasser, T., Verleger, R., Bacher, P., & Sroka, L. (1988). Develop-ment of the EEG of school-age children and adolescents. I.Analysis of band power. Electroencephalography and ClinicalNeurophysiology, 69, 91–99.

Hart, E. L., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B., & Frick, P. J.(1995). Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactiv-ity disorder in boys: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 23, 729–749.

Hastings, J. E., & Barkley, R. A. (1978). A review of psycho-physiological research with hyperkinetic children. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 6, 413–447.

Hermens, D. F., Williams, L. M., Lazzaro, I., Whitmont, S.,Melkonian, D., & Gordon, E. (2004). Sex differences in adultADHD: a double dissociation in brain activity and autonomicarousal. Biological Psychology, 66, 221–233.

Heywood, C., & Beale, I. (2003). EEG biofeedback vs. placebotreatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A pilotstudy. Journal of Attention Disorders, 7, 43–55.

Janzen, T., Graap, K., Stephanson, S., Marshall, W., &Fitzsimmons, G. (1995). Differences in baseline EEG mea-sures for ADD and normally achieving preadolescent males.Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20, 65–82.

John, E. R., Prichep, L., Ahn, H., Easton, P., Fridman, J., & Kaye, H.(1983). Neurometric evaluation of cognitive dysfunctions andneurological disorders in children. Progress in Neurobiology,21, 239–290.

Jonkman, L. M., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., Koelega, H. S.,Camfferman, G., v.d. Gaag, R. J., et al. (1997). Effects ofmethylphenidate on event-related potentials and performanceof attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder children in auditoryand visual selective attention tasks. Biological Psychiatry, 41,690–702.

Kuperman, S., Johnson, B., Arndt, S., Lindgren, S., & Wolraich, M.(1996). Quantitative EEG differences in a nonclinical sampleof children with ADHD and undifferentiated ADD [see com-ments]. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adoles-cent Psychiatry, 35, 1009–1017.

Lazzaro, I., Gordon, E., Whitmont, S., Plahn, M., Li, W., Clarke, S.,et al. (1998). Quantified EEG activity in adolescent attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Electroencephalogra-phy, 29, 37–42.

Levy, F., & Swanson, J. M. (2001). Timing, space and ADHD: Thedopamine theory revisited. Australian and New Zealand Jour-nal of Psychiatry, 35, 504–511.

Linden, M., Habib, T., & Radojevic, V. (1996). A controlled studyof the effects of EEG biofeedback on cognition and behavior ofchildren with attention deficit disorder and learning disabili-ties. Biofeedback & Self Regulation, 21, 35–49.

75

ADHD AND EEG

Page 30: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Loo, S., Teale, P., & Reite, M. (1999). EEG correlates ofpsychostimulant effects among children with ADHD: A pre-liminary report. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1657–1660.

Lubar, J. F. (1991). Discourse on the development of EEG diagnos-tics and biofeedback for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-ders. Biofeedback & Self Regulation, 16, 201–225.

Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., & O’Donnell, P.(1995). Evaluation of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedbacktraining for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changesin T.O.V.A. scores, behvioral ratings, and WISC-R perfor-mance. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 20, 83–99.

Lubar, J. F., White, J. N., Jr., Swartwood, M. O., & Swartwood, J. N.(1999). Methylphenidate effects on global and complex mea-sures of EEG. Pediatric Neurology, 21, 633–637.

Mann, C. A., Lubar, J. F., Zimmerman, A. W., Miller, C. A., &Muenchen, R. A. (1992). Quantitative analysis of EEG in boyswith attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Controlled studywith clinical implications. Pediatric Neurology, 8(1), 30–36.

McBurnett, K., Pfiffner, L. J., & Frick, P. J. (2001). Symptom prop-erties as a function of ADHD type: an argument for continuedstudy of sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 29, 207–213.

Milich, R., Ballentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. (2001). ADHD com-bined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are dis-tinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Scienceand Practice, 8, 463–488.

Mirsky, A. F. (1996). Disorders of attention: A neuropsychologicalperspective. In R. G. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Atten-tion, memory, and executive function (pp. 71–96). Baltimore:Paul H. Brookes.

Monastra, V. J. (2004). EEG and neurofeedback findings in ADHD:An empirical response. The ADHD Report, 12, 5–8.

Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., & Linden, M. (2001). The developmentof a quantitative electroencephalographic scanning process forattention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Reliability and valid-ity studies. Neuropsychology, 15(1), 136–144.

Monastra, V. J., Lubar, J. F., Linden, M., VanDeusen, P., Green, G.,Wing, W., et al. (1999). Assessing attention deficit hyperactiv-ity disorder via quantitative electroencephalography: an initialvalidation study. Neuropsychology, 13, 424–433.

Monastra, V. J., Monastra, D. M., & George, S. (2002). The effectsof stimulant therapy, EEG biofeedback, and parenting style onthe primary symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-der. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 27, 231–249.

Nash, J. K. (2000). Treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disor-der with neurotherapy. Clinical Electroencephalography,31(1), 30–37.

Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Effects of reward and re-sponse cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anx-ious, and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-chology, 26(3), 161–174.

Pelham, W. E., Jr., Wheeler, T., & Chronis, A. (1998). Empiricallysupported psychosocial treatments for attention deficit hyper-activity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(2),190–205.

Pelham, W. E. (2001). Are ADHD/1 and ADHD/c the same or dif-ferent? Does it matter? Clinical Psychology: Science andPractice, 8, 502–506.

Posner, M. I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trendsin Neuroscience, 17(2), 75–79.

Prichep, L. S., & John, E. R. (1992). QEEG profiles of psychiatricdisorders. Brain Topography, 4, 249–257.

Ramirez, P. M., Desantis, D., & Opler, L. A. (2001). EEG biofeed-back treatment of ADD. A viable alternative to traditionalmedical intervention? Annals of the New York Academy ofSciences, 931, 342–358.

Rosenthal, R. H., & Allen, T. W. (1978). An examination of atten-tion, arousal, and learning dysfunctions of hyperkinetic chil-dren. Psychology Bulletin, 85, 689–715.

Rossiter, T. R., & La Vaque, T. J. (1995). A comparison of EEGbiofeedback and psychostimulants in treating Atten-tion-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders. Journal of Neurotherapy,Summer, 48–59.

Sangal, R. B., & Sangal, J. M. (2004). Attention-deficit/hyperactiv-ity disorder: Cognitive evoked potential (P300) topographypredicts treatment response to methylphenidate. ClinicalNeurophysiology, 115(1), 188–193.

Satterfield, J. H., Cantwell, D. P., & Satterfield, B. T. (1974).Pathophysiology of the hyperactive child syndrome. Archivesof General Psychiatry, 31, 839–844.

Satterfield, J. H., Schell, A. M., Backs, R. W., & Hidaka, K. C.(1984). A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of age effectsof electrophysiological measures in hyperactive and normalchildren. Biological Psychiatry, 19, 973–990.

Smith, B., Barkley, R. A., & Shapiro, C. (in press). Attention deficithyperactivity disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.),Treatment of childhood disorders (3rd ed.). New York:Guilford.

Sterman, M. B. (2000). Basic concepts and clinical findings in thetreatment of seizure disorders with EEG operant conditioning.Clinical Electroencephalography, 31, 45–55.

Suffin, S. C., & Emory, W. H. (1995). Neurometric subgroups inattentional and affective disorders and their association withpharmacotherapeutic outcome. Clinical Electroencephalogra-phy, 26(2), 76–83.

Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., Lubar, J. F., Timmerman, D.L., Zimmerman, A. W., & Muenchen. (1998). Methyl-phenidate effects on EEG, behavior, and performance in boyswith ADHD. Pediatric Neurology, 18, 244–250.

Verbaten, M. N., Overtoom, C. C., Koelega, H. S.,Swaab-Barneveld, H., van der Gaag, R. J., Buitelaar, J., et al.(1994). Methylphenidate influences on both early and lateERP waves of ADHD children in a continuous performancetest. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 561–578.

Weinfurt, K. P. (1998). Multivariate analysis of variance. In L. G.Grimm & P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understandingmultivariate statistics (pp. 245–276). Washington, DC: Ameri-can Psychological Association.

Winsberg, B. G., Javitt, D. C., & Silipo, G. S. (1997).Electrophysiological indices of information processing inmethylphenidate responders. Biological Psychiatry, 42,434–445.

76

LOO & BARKLEY

Page 31: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.
Page 32: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Please cite this article in press as: H.N.A. Logemann, et al., The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedbackcontrolled study, Neurosci. Lett. (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

ARTICLE IN PRESSGModel

NSL-27068; No.of Pages5

Neuroscience Letters xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

C o nte nts lists a v aila ble at Scie nce Direct

Neuroscience Letters

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / n e u l e t

The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A shamfeedback controlled study

H.N. Alexander Logemanna,b, , Marieke M. Lansbergenc, Titus W.D.P. Van Osd,Koen B.E. Böckerb, J. Leon Kenemansa,b

a Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlandsb Department of Psychopharmacology, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80082, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlandsc Department of Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlandsd GGZ Friesland and University Medical Center, Groningen, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 4 February 2010Received in revised form 7 May 2010Accepted 8 May 2010

Keywords:NeurofeedbackEEGDouble-blindShamImpulsivityAttentionADHD

a b s t r a c t

EEG-feedback, also called neurofeedback, is a training procedure aimed at altering brain activity, andis used as a treatment for disorders like Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Studies havereported positive effects of neurofeedback on attention and otherdependent variables.However, double-blind studies including a sham neurofeedback control group are lacking. The inclusion of such group iscrucial to control for unspecific effects. The current work presents a sham-controlled, double-blind eval-uation. The hypothesis was that neurofeedback enhances attention and decreases impulsive behavior.Participants (n=27)were students selected on relatively high scores on impulsivity/inattentionquestion-naires (Barrat Impulsivity Scale and Broadbent CFQ). They were assigned to a neurofeedback treatmentor a sham group. (sham)Neurofeedback training was planned for 15 weeks consisting of a total of 30sessions, each lasting 22min. Before and after 16 sessions (i.e., interim analyses), qEEGwas recorded andimpulsivity and inattention was assessed using a stop signal task and reversed continuous performancetask and two questionnaires. Results of the interim analyses showed that participants were blind withrespect to group inclusion, but no trend towards an effect of neurofeedback on behavioral measures wasobserved. Therefore in line with ethical guidelines the experiment was ceased. These results implicate apossible lack of effect of neurofeedback when one accounts for non-specific effects.However, the specificform of feedback and application of the sham-controlled double-blind design may have diminished theeffect of neurofeedback.

© 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Neurofeedback is a method with which individuals can learn toalter their brain activity by means of feedback of this activity andrewarding of appropriate brain activity. Neurofeedback may be apotential treatment for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder(ADHD) [23]. However, no solid sham feedback (fake feedback)controlled studies have been conducted aimed at evaluating theeffectiveness of neurofeedback on alleviating core symptoms ofADHD. The inclusion of a sham-control group is crucial to controlfor unspecific factors and to be able to estimate the effectiveness ofneurofeedback above placebo.

Different neurofeedback protocols do elicit specific EEG changesand positive effects on behavior [2]. In healthy subjects, Egnerand Gruzelier [12] showed that uptraining Sensory Motor Rhythm

Corresponding author at: Department of Experimental Psychology,Utrecht Uni-versity, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands. Tel.: +31 30 253 3386;fax: +31 30 2534511.

E-mail address: [email protected] (H.N.A. Logemann).

(SMR) (12–15Hz) was associated with increased perceptual sen-sitivity and a decline in commission errors on the Connor’sContinuous Performance Test (CPT), suggesting increased sus-tained attention and decreased impulsivity. High beta uptraining(15–18Hz) resulted in the opposite effect. In a second study, Egnerand Gruzelier [11] also found protocol specific effects. They con-cluded that increasing SMR results in a general attention enhancingeffect and 16–20Hz training results in an arousal enhancing effect.In a study of Vernon et al. [27], SMR uptraining and theta train-ing were investigated in healthy volunteers. The effect of SMRtraining on attention was replicated; however, in contrast to SMRtraining, theta training did not affect the EEG, working memoryperformance, or measures of attention.

Neurofeedback has been investigated as an alternative treat-ment for ADHD. In these patients, anomalies have been found onEEG measures of brain activity. In short, one group of patientsshows decreased fast wave beta (12–22Hz), less alpha (8–12Hz),and increased slow wave theta (4–7Hz) activity [6,8]. The othergroup presents an excess of beta activity [7]. The effectiveness of

0304-3940/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

Page 33: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Please cite this article in press as: H.N.A. Logemann, et al., The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedbackcontrolled study, Neurosci. Lett. (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

ARTICLE IN PRESSGModel

NSL-27068; No.of Pages5

2 H.N.A. Logemann et al. / Neuroscience Letters xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

neurofeedback in reducing ADHD symptoms has been assessedin several studies. A review of Monastra et al. [24] summarizingresearch on (1) SMR enhancement and theta (4–8Hz) suppression,(2) SMR enhancement and high beta suppression (22–30Hz), and(3) beta-1 (16–20Hz) enhancement and theta suppression showsresults comparable tomethylphenidate [23].Using fMRI in childrenwith ADHD, Lévesque et al. [18] showed normalization of activityin the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) during a cognitive controltask following neurofeedback as compared to a waiting list. TheACC is an area implicated in deficiencies in the control of atten-tion, Furthermore, two recent studies showed that neurofeedback,using theta/beta and SCP feedback, was superior to a computer-ized attention skills training in reducing ADHD symptomatology[14,15].

Although previous studies strongly suggest a positive effect ofneurofeedback in ADHD [2], these studies did not implement adouble-blind sham-controlled design, which is necessary to assesswhetherneurofeedback training outperformssham feedback train-ing. Indeed, there are ethical concerns for implementing a shamcontrol in clinical research if a standard treatment exists [16,28].Several alternatives have been proposed [28]. One possibility is toinvestigate the effects of neurofeedback on a variable implicated inpathology using a sample of healthy subjects. In this way, a sham-controlled design does not violate ethical principles. In one suchstudy, albeit not directly related to attention and impulsivity, theeffect of neurofeedback on an EEG component (alpha/theta ratio),targeted in the adjuvant neurofeedback therapy for alcoholism,was investigated [13]. Specifically, a sample of healthy participantswas used and a sham-controlled design was implemented to testthe idea that alpha/theta feedback significantly modulates powerwithin these frequency bands. Comparing real feedback with mockfeedback, an effect was indeed found on EEG, but not on subjectivemeasures.

The goal of the current study was to investigate with a sim-ilar sham-controlled design the specific effect of neurofeedbackon measures of impulsivity and attention in healthy volunteerswho scored relatively high on self-reported impulsivity and inat-tention. First, it was expected that neurofeedback would decreaseinattentiveness and impulsivity as assessed by the adult self-reportquestionnaireand the state impulsivityquestionnaire, respectively.Secondly, it was expected that a decrease in inattentiveness andimpulsivitywould alsobe evident usingobjectivemeasures. Specif-ically,neurofeedback, relative to the shamgroup, should reduce thestandard deviation of reaction time (SDRT) in the R-CPT (reflectingsustained attention [19,20]), the stop signal reaction time (SSRT)in the Stop Signal Task (SST), and the false-alarm rate in the R-CPT (both related to impulsivity). Lastly, it was expected thatneurofeedback would have an effect on the power in the trainedfrequency bands.

This study was a collaboration of Utrecht University with theNeurofeedback Instituut Nederland B.V. To account for electro-physiological heterogeneity, individualized protocols were used. Adouble-blind, sham-controlled between-subjects design was cho-sen. The study was approved by the Utrecht Medical Center,Medical Ethical Committee and conducted in accordance with thedeclaration of Helsinki.Medical Ethical Committee (IRB) guidelineswere to cease the experiment if no trend towards improvement inbehavior was found at the interim analyses (after 16 sessions).

To estimate the sample size for this study group × time effectsizes (cohen’s d) were calculated based on a study by Egner andGruzelier [11]. For behavioral variables, the group × time effect sizewas 1.25. Power analysis with d at 1.25, alpha at 0.05 and an aver-age power of 80% renders a sample size of at least 24 participants(12 in each group) sufficient. Furthermore, to ensure a sample withenough room for improvement on attention and impulsivity vari-ables, we selected healthy volunteers, who scored relatively high

on rating scales of impulsivity (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale ques-tionnaire; BIS [4]) and inattention (Broadbent’s Cognitive Failurequestionnaire; CFQ [5]), from a sample of 455 psychology stu-dents at Utrecht University. Specifically, for both questionnairesa higher score denotes more problems and participants with ascore of <1 S.D. below the average sample score on both ques-tionnaires were excluded, to ensure a large enough sample size(>N =24)andasamplewith roomfor improvement.Otherexclusioncriteria were (a history of) psychiatric disorders, epilepsy, and useof psychoactive drugs. Eventually, 29 participants with relativelyhigh scores on the questionnaires (BIS: M=66.14 S.D.=6.66, CFQ:50.52, S.D.=9.08) were included in the present study. After base-linemeasurements, two participantswere excluded, one fell asleepduring data acquisition, the other showed epileptiform activity inthe EEG. Participants were randomly assigned to either the treat-ment (n=14) or sham group (n=13). To prevent group differences,participants were matched on handedness, age, gender, IQ, StateImpulsivityQuestionnaire (STIMP) score, SSRTon the SST, and SDRTon the R-CPT task. One participant from the sham group droppedout after 14 sessions for personal reasons not related to this study.In the final sample (n=26) the mean age in the sham group was20.7 years (S.D.=2.72), one was male. For the treatment group themean age was 21.1 years (S.D.=2.30), two were male. Participantsreceived 3.50 euro per hour for participation.

TheAdult Self-Report (ASR) [1], State Impulsivity (STIMP)Ques-tionnaire and two computer tasks were used for baseline andinterim assessment. The ASR questionnaire yields two scores, oneforattentionalproblems (ATT score) andoneas indication forADHDsymptoms (ADHDscore). The STIMPwasused to assess impulsivity.On all questionnaires, a higher score indicates more dysfunction.The computer tasks consisted of the R-CPT and stop signal task(SST). The R-CPT consisted of one uniquely randomized block of608 trials comprised out of 16 letters (A, B, C, D, E, F, H, I, L, M,N, O, T, Y, Z, and X); each black letter was presented at the cen-ter of the screen on a gray background for 1000ms at a 1000msinterval, and each letter occurred in total 38 times. The goal wasto react as rapidly and accurately as possible by pressing a buttonon a pad after presentation of each letter except the X. Variables ofinterest were the standard deviation of mean reaction time (SDRT)on Go trials and the proportion of false alarms (i.e., responses tothe X divided by total number of X stimuli). In the SST, participantswerepresented sequencesof square-waveblack-white gratingpre-sented on a gray background. The gratings were 6 × 6 in size andconsisted of high or low fundamental spatial frequency, 3.62 cpdand 0.46 cpd respectively. After a fixation cross was presented for500ms a grating was presented for 750ms. The duration from theend of presentation of a grating to the presentation of the fixa-tion cross was variable between 1000ms and 1250ms. The taskconsisted of two types of trials, go trials and stop trials. In a gotrial the grating was not followed by a tone and a response had tobe made. Participants had to respond by pressing the right but-ton when presented with a high frequency grating and the leftbutton when presented with a low frequency grating. Stop trialswere trials in which the grating was followed by a binaural tone(1000Hz, 80dB, and 400ms in duration), and a response had to bewithheld. In total 630 trials were presented in five blocks of 126trials. Blocks consisted of a unique randomized sequence of trialsper participant and contained 76 go trials and 50 stop trials. Eachblock had an equal amount of high and low frequency gratings.No more than three stop trials were presented consecutively. Thetask started with a practice block followed by the four experimen-tal blocks. For reliable SSRTs, about 50% correct inhibitions needto be made, therefore the Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA; i.e.,go/stop interval) was adjusted after each block using a trackingalgorithm [9,22]. Only in the first block the SOA was set at 250ms.The Go-stop interval was jittered within a 250ms range to prevent

Page 34: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Please cite this article in press as: H.N.A. Logemann, et al., The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedbackcontrolled study, Neurosci. Lett. (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

ARTICLE IN PRESSGModel

NSL-27068; No.of Pages5

H.N.A. Logemann et al. / Neuroscience Letters xxx (2010) xxx–xxx 3

Table 1For the treatment group: neurofeedback protocols based on QEEG comparisons inNeuroguide.

STOP GO LOC

2–7Hz 12–15Hz Cz18–25Hz 11–13Hz Cz4–7Hz 12–14Hz Cz4–7Hz and 20–25Hz Pz11.5–12.5Hz and 22–26Hz 15–18Hz C311.5–14.5Hz and 23–25Hz Pz2–5Hz 12–15Hz P33–7Hz 11–14Hz Pz2–4Hz 12–15Hz C46.5–7.5Hz and 23–28Hz Cz25–30Hz 14–17Hz Cz3.5–4.5Hz and 20–24Hz Cz2–5Hz 12–15Hz Cz25–30Hz 12–15Hz Pz

Stop: inhibition of power in frequency band; GO: enhancement of power in fre-quency band; LOC: location.

response strategies [25].Due to apparatus failure, the toneoccurredunilateral for 12 participants; therefore this data was not used inthe analysis of the SST. The SST main outcome is the Stop SignalReaction Time (SSRT) which is thought to be a measure of motorinhibition [3,10]. The SSRT was calculated in accordance with DeJong et al. [10] and Tannock et al. [26].

For QEEG assessment, the Deymed Truescan 32 acquisitiondevice was used. Recordings were done at 19 channels and a rateof 128 samples per second was chosen. 10/20 EEG caps were usedand electrodes were referenced to linked ears. Impedances werekept around or under 5k . Each recording lasted approximately9min (eyes closed since norm data also consisted of eyes closedrecordings). QEEG analyses needed for determining the individualtreatment-protocols were conducted by the Neurofeedback Insti-tuut Nederland B.V. (NIN). Segments with eye movements andartifacts were removed. By comparing the individual QEEGs (afterartifact rejection) to the Neuroguide database deviations from thenorm in the EEG were identified (FFT maximal z scores). Individu-alized treatment-protocols (STOP/GO, Table 1) were based on thisassessment and aimed at normalizing power within the frequen-cies at one specific location of interest. For each participant, theprotocol could consist of feedback based on the real EEG signal or asimulated EEG signal. The simulated EEG signal consisted of a ran-dom signal relatively similar to real EEG and simulation providedsimilar feedback experience.

15 weeks of neurofeedback training consisting of 30 sessions,each lasting 22min, were planned, but the experiment was ceasedafter 16 sessions (since no trend towards an effect of neurofeed-back on behavior was evident in the interim analyses, and ethicalguidelines were to cease the experiment). Before (Baseline) andhalfway (interim,after16sessions)participantswereassessedwithrespect to measures of attention and impulsivity and qEEG wasrecorded. Prior to the experiment and after receiving adequateinformation, participants were asked to sign the informed consentform. Afterbaselinemeasurements, (sham)neurofeedback sessionswere planned and proceeded as follows. Participants entered theirplanned twice a week attendance in an online calendar. It wasadvised to have one day between sessions. If a participant couldonly attend for one session in a week, they received three ses-sions the following week. Upon first arrival, the participant wasinstructed that the brightening of the movie screen and the audioclicks are good signs and that the learning process is mostly uncon-scious so no specific effort is needed. Afterwards the participantsat down on a comfortable chair in front of a 17-in. monitor, andlocations were prepped with Nuprep. The Brainmaster Atlantis IImodule in combination with Brainmaster 2.5SE software was used

for 22min of (uninterrupted) (sham)neurofeedback.. All electrodeimpedanceswere kept under 6k .One electrodewas placed at thescalp at the location of interest (Table 1) using Ten20 paste and ahair band. The reference was placed on the mastoid contralateralto the electrode at the scalp or at the right mastoid if the scalpelectrode was placed on the middle of the scalp. The ground wasplaced on the opposite mastoid. EEG was recorded at 256 sam-ples per second with a 50Hz Notch filter. Online artifact rejectionanalysiswas conductedwith a thresholdof 240 V. The17-in.mon-itor and speaker sets were used for video/audio feedback. If powerwithin the specific frequencies was above or below threshold, par-ticipants were rewarded with audio clicks and a brightening asopposed todarkeningof themovie. Feedback thresholdswereauto-matically and dynamically adjusted every 30 s to keep power 80%of time above or below threshold (depending on whether feedbackconsisted of up or down training). After 16 weeks of neurofeed-back, the interim analysis took place within two weeks after thelast session, subsequently, the experimentwas ceased. Finally, par-ticipants were asked to indicate whether they thought they hadreceived sham or real treatment.

Except for initial assessment by the NIN, eyes closed EEGsrecorded at baseline and interim, were analyzed in Brain VisionAnalyzer. A high pass filter of 0.5Hz, 12dB/oct, notch filter of 50Hz,and low pass filter of 40Hz, 12dB/oct was used. The first andlast parts of the EEG was discarded to remove possible artifacts;the segment of 20–500 s was used for analyses. This segment wasdivided in 2 s epochs resulting in a resolution of 0.5Hz. Automaticartifact rejection was used with a difference criterion of 100 V,effectively removing artifacts and possible remaining eye move-ment (even though the recordingswere eyes closed). A full segmentbaseline correction was performed to remove DC drift, and epochswere transformed to the frequencydomainwith Fast FourierTrans-form. For each participant and session (baseline/interim), powerwithin each specific frequency band (see Table 1) was calculatedfor each 2 s epoch. Since individualized protocols were used, effectsizes were calculated for the treatment group using the mean andstandard deviation of power across segments for each individ-ual participant, frequency band, and time point (baseline versusinterim). Effect sizes were recoded (a negative value to positiveand vice versa) for frequency bands in which powerwere expectedto be reduced. Since multiple frequency bands were often targetedwithin participants, effect sizes were then averaged yielding oneglobal effect size per participant (Table 1).

A Repeated Measures General Linear Model was chosen to testthe interaction between the effect of the within-subjects factortime and that of the between-subjects factor group. Dependentvariables were SSRT, SDRT and proportion false alarms, the STIMPscore, and the ADHD and ATT score from the ASR. A nonparamet-ric (chi2) test was used to test whether participants could guessto what group they were assigned. A one-sample t-test was usedto test whether the effect sizes in the treatment group deviatedsignificantly from zero.

With respect to behavioral data, there were no significanttime × group interactions, or trends towards significance, for anyof the dependent variables (SSRT, (F(1,12) <1); SDRT, (F(1,24) <1);false alarms, (F(1,24) <1); STIMP, (F(1,24) <1); ATT score on ASR,(F(1,24) =1.790), ADHD score on ASR, (F(1,24<1)). Furthermore,neurofeedback treatment did not seem to affect EEG. After theexclusion of one outlier (>2 S.D.), only a trend towards significancewas evident (t(1,12) =2.098, p=0.058).

Irrespective of group inclusion, most participants thought theywere in the sham group. For the treatment group, 10 out of 14participants thought they received sham feedback. 10 out of 12participants in the sham group thought they were in the shamgroup. Inclusion in group did not significantly influence guessing(chi2(1,26) =0.516, P =0.473).

Page 35: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Please cite this article in press as: H.N.A. Logemann, et al., The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedbackcontrolled study, Neurosci. Lett. (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

ARTICLE IN PRESSGModel

NSL-27068; No.of Pages5

4 H.N.A. Logemann et al. / Neuroscience Letters xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

In summary, the findings from the present double-blind,randomized, sham-controlled neurofeedback study in healthy vol-unteers could not confirm our hypothesis that neurofeedbackoutperforms sham feedback treatment in enhancing attention anddecreasing impulsive behavior. Previous studies do suggest aneffect of neurofeedback on measures of attention and impulsivity[2], but none of the studies conducted a double-blind, sham-controlled trial. This study incorporated a sham group-controlleddouble-blind design to control for unspecific effects and to investi-gate theeffect of neurofeedbackaboveplacebo. Sinceno interactionproved to be significant at the behavioral level, there may be notreatment effect at all, or the effect is rather small and irrelevant.In other words, the present findings suggest that neurofeedbackmay have no effect on behavior when accounting for unspecificfactors. This is in marked contrast to previous studies accountingfor unspecific effects by applying different protocols within onesample [11,12,27].

Several factors may account for the contrasting results. Oneissue concerns the use of the double-blind sham-controlled design.In our study, most participants thought they received sham feed-back irrespective of group inclusion. Part of the instruction toparticipants was that no specific effort was necessary for feed-back to have an effect. In combination with quite liberal andcontinuously changing feedback thresholds, it may be possi-ble that participants merely watched the movie. Similarly themovie itself could have distracted participants from the feedbackprocess. Awareness of feedback may be necessary for neurofeed-back to have an effect. In short, the specific application of thedouble-blind sham-controlled design may have diminished theeffectiveness of the treatment. Future research should elucidatewhich format of feedback is most effective and whether aware-ness of treatment is necessary for training to have an effect.If future research indeed suggests that the double-blind sham-controlled design diminishes the effectiveness of neurofeedback,other designs should be considered. Indeed, several alternativeshave been put forward [28]. If awareness is important in neu-rofeedback training, we would suggest a more direct and simpleform of feedback devoid of distracting elements such as presum-ably in our movie-feedback. For example, Egner and Gruzelier[11] used simply an altering size of geometrical figures to rep-resent power in a specific frequency band. In order to maintaina sufficient amount of awareness of an effect, the control groupcould receive feedbackonmuscle activity (electromyographic feed-back).

Another limitation concerns the sample. Even though the par-ticipants scored relatively high on measures of inattention andimpulsivity, the sample consisted of academic students. Perhapsinsufficient room for improvement couldpartly account for the lackof effect. Also, since our sample consisted of healthy subjects, theresults cannot be generalized to the clinicalADHD population. Sub-clinical impulsivity and problems with attention may differ fromthat of the clinical ADHD population. Lijfijt et al. [21], show that incontrast to the ADHD population, subclinical impulsivity does notcorrelate positively with decreased motor inhibition. Lansbergenet al. [17] found a lack of association between subjective impul-sivity and motor inhibition as measured by the SST. Moreover,they showed that opposite to what is seen in ADHD patients, longSSRTS (indication of decreased motor inhibition) were associatedwith low theta/beta ratios. The authors argue that participantswithhigher theta/beta ratiosmay bemoremotivated to increase perfor-mance on the task.

Another issue is the use of individualized protocols in ourelectrophysiologically heterogeneous sample. Use of one standardprotocol would have been better in terms of standardization, butwould have necessitated, with respect to ethical principles, partic-ipant selection based on qEEG. For practical reasons it was chosen

not to include this procedure. It could be that the effect of standardprotocols used in previous studies is superior to our heterogeneityof protocols. Indeed, the effectiveness of many of our implementedprotocols has not been evaluated yet in standardized research.Furthermore, we incorporated protocols aimed at inhibiting andprotocols aimed at both inhibiting and enhancing power. It maybe argued that there is a difference in effectiveness between thesetypes of feedback. Unfortunately in our dataset it is not possibleto separately assert the effect of specific protocols or only proto-cols aimed at inhibiting power, since the samplewould become toonarrow. There are two possibilities future studies could consider toresolve the issue with individualized protocols, and to successfullycombine an individual approach with standardization. One possi-bility is to assert the effect of (limited) individualized protocolson an electrophysiologically heterogeneous sample of sufficientsize, making it possible to assert the effect of specific protocolsin associated electrophysiologically similar subsamples. Secondly,an electrophysiologically homogeneous sample may be selectedthrough qEEG selection, and the effect of one specific protocol canbe evaluated on that specific sample.

To conclude, the current results suggest a lack of effect of neuro-feedback above placebowhenone accounts fornon-specific effects.However, the format of feedback in combination with the specificapplication of the double-blind sham-controlled design may havediminished the effect of neurofeedback.

Acknowledgments

This researchwas done in collaborationwith theNeurofeedbackInstituut Nederland B.V. I thank Drs. G. Loots, the psychophysiolo-gist of the NIN for his help with the neurofeedback equipment andfor providing the training in neurofeedback intervention.

I also thank the master students of Psychology under supervi-sion of Prof. Dr. L. van Doornen: Z. Molnar-Logemann, BSc., Drs.M. Halfhuid, M. Laub, BSc., S. Leeuwen, BSc., for providing the(sham)neurofeedback sessions and research on the clinical ques-tionnaires as part of their internships.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data associatedwith this article can be found, inthe online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026.

References

[1] T.M. Achenback, L.A. Rescorla, Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms and Pro-files, University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth and Families,Burlington, VT, 2003.

[2] M. Arns, S.deRidder,U. Strehl,M. Breteler, A. Coenen, Efficacy of neurofeedbacktreatment in ADHD: the effects on inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity:a meta-analysis, Clin. EEG Neurosci. 40 (2009) 180–189.

[3] G.P. Band, M.W. van der Molen, G.D. Logan, Horse-race model simulations ofthe stop-signal procedure, Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 112 (2003) 105–142.

[4] E.S. Barrat, The biological basis of impulsiveness: the significance of timing andrhythm disorders, Pers. Indiv. Diff. 4 (1983) 387–391.

[5] D.E. Broadbent, P.F. Cooper, P. FitzGerald, K.R. Parkes, The Cognitive FailuresQuestionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates, Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 21 (Pt 1) (1982) 1–16.

[6] R.J. Chabot,G. Serfontein,Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of chil-dren with attention deficit disorder, Biol. Psychiatry 40 (1996) 951–963.

[7] A.R. Clarke, R.J. Barry, R. McCarthy, M. Selikowitz, EEG-defined subtypes ofchildren with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Clin. Neurophysiol. 112(2001) 2098–2105.

[8] A.R. Clarke, R.J. Barry, R. McCarthy, M. Selikowitz, EEG analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparative study of two subtypes, PsychiatryRes. 81 (1998) 19–29.

[9] R. De Jong, M.G. Coles, G.D. Logan, Strategies and mechanisms in nonselectiveand selective inhibitory motor control, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.21 (1995) 498–511.

[10] R.De Jong,M.G. Coles,G.D. Logan,G.Gratton, In search of the point of no return:the control of response processes, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16(1990) 164–182.

Page 36: EEG Biofeedback Treatment of ADDimg2.timg.co.il/forums/1_157623192.pdf · 1982-03-07  · RAMIREZ 343 et al.: EEG BIOFEEDBACK TREATMENT OF ADD as reinforcement for the desired changes.

Please cite this article in press as: H.N.A. Logemann, et al., The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: A sham feedbackcontrolled study, Neurosci. Lett. (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.026

ARTICLE IN PRESSGModel

NSL-27068; No.of Pages5

H.N.A. Logemann et al. / Neuroscience Letters xxx (2010) xxx–xxx 5

[11] T. Egner, J.H. Gruzelier, EEG biofeedback of low beta band components:frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brainpotentials, Clin. Neurophysiol. 115 (2004) 131–139.

[12] T. Egner, J.H. Gruzelier, Learned self-regulation of EEG frequency componentsaffects attention and event-related brain potentials in humans,Neuroreport 12(2001) 4155–4159.

[13] T. Egner, E. Strawson, J.H. Gruzelier, EEG signature and phenomenology ofalpha/theta neurofeedback training versus mock feedback, Appl. Psychophys-iol. Biofeedback 27 (2002) 261–270.

[14] H. Gevensleben, B. Holl, B. Albrecht, D. Schlamp, O. Kratz, P. Studer, S. Wangler,A. Rothenberger, G.H. Moll, H. Heinrich, Distinct EEG effects related to neuro-feedback training in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial, Int. J.Psychophysiol. 74 (2009) 149–157.

[15] H. Gevensleben, B. Holl, B. Albrecht, C. Vogel, D. Schlamp, O. Kratz, P. Studer, A.Rothenberger,G.H.Moll,H.Heinrich, Is neurofeedback an efficacious treatmentfor ADHD? A randomised controlled clinical trial, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry50 (2009) 780–789.

[16] T.J. LaVaque, T. Rossiter, The ethical use of placebo controls in clinical research:the Declaration of Helsinki, Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 26 (2001) 23–37,discussion 61–25.

[17] M.M. Lansbergen,D.J. Schutter, J.L. Kenemans, Subjective impulsivity and base-line EEG in relation to stopping performance, Brain Res. 1148 (2007) 161–169.

[18] J. Levesque, M. Beauregard, B. Mensour, Effect of neurofeedback trainingon the neural substrates of selective attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study,Neurosci. Lett. 394 (2006) 216–221.

[19] E.D. Levin, C.K. Conners, D. Silva, S.C. Hinton, W.H. Meck, J. March, J.E. Rose,Transdermal nicotine effects on attention, Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 140(1998) 135–141.

[20] M. Lijffijt, J.L. Kenemans, M.N. Verbaten, H. van Engeland, A meta-analyticreview of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:deficient inhibitory motor control? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114 (2005) 216–222.

[21] M.B. Lijfijt, E.M., E.H. Quik, J. Bakker, J.L. Kenemans, M.N. Verbaten, Differencesbetween low and high trait impulsivity are not associated with differences ininhibitory motor control, J. Atten. Disord. 8 (2004) 25–32.

[22] G.D. Logan, R.J. Schachar, R. Tannock, Impulsivity and inhibitory control, Psy-chol. Sci. 8 (1997) 60–64.

[23] V.J.Monastra, S. Lynn,M. Linden, J.F. Lubar, J.Gruzelier, T.J. LaVaque, Electroen-cephalographic biofeedback in the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder, Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 30 (2005) 95–114.

[24] V.J. Monastra, D.M. Monastra, S. George, The effects of stimulant therapy,EEG biofeedback, and parenting style on the primary symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 27 (2002)231–249.

[25] S.R. Pliszka, M. Liotti, M.G. Woldorff, Inhibitory control in children withattention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: event-related potentials identify theprocessing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism, Biol. Psychiatry 48 (2000) 238–246.

[26] R. Tannock, R.J. Schachar, R.P. Carr, D. Chajczyk, G.D. Logan, Effects ofmethylphenidateon inhibitorycontrol inhyperactive children, J. Abnorm. ChildPsychol. 17 (1989) 473–491.

[27] D. Vernon, T. Egner, N. Cooper, T. Compton, C. Neilands, A. Sheri, J. Gruzelier,The effect of training distinct neurofeedback protocols on aspects of cognitiveperformance, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 47 (2003) 75–85.

[28] D. Vernon, A. Frick, J. Gruzelier, Neurofeedback as a treatment for adhd: amethodological review with implications for future research, journal of neu-rotherapy: investigations in neuromodulation, Neurofeedback Appl. Neurosci.8 (2004) 53–82.