Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

download Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

of 29

Transcript of Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    1/29

    Anthropology is philosophy with thc pcoplc in,- T'int Irtgold

    This book is an invitation to a journey which, in the aulhor's opinion, is oneof the most rewarding a human being can embark on - and it is dclinitelyone of the longest. It will bring the reader lrom the damp rainforests of theAmazon to the cold semi-deserl of the Arctic; from the skyscrapers ofManhattan to mud huts in the Sahel; lrom villages in the New Cuincahighlands to African cities.It is a long journey in a different se nse too, Socialrand cultural anthropol-ogy has the whole of human society as its field oi in[eresl-, etnd tries tc.runderstand the connections between the variouS aspects of Our existence.When, for example, we study the traditional economic system of Lhe Tiv ol'central Nigeria, an essential part ofthe exploration consists in understand-ing how their economy is connected with other aspects oltheir society' IIthisdimension is absent, Tiv economy becomes incomprehensible to anthropol-ogists. If we do not know that the Tiv traditionally could not buy and sellland, and that they have customarily not used money as a means ofpayment, it will plainly be impossible to understand how they themselvesinlcrpret their situation and how they responded to the economic changesimposed on their society during colonialism.Anthropology tries to account lor the social and cultural variation in theworld, but a crucial part of the anthropological project also consists in con-ceplualising and understanding similarities between social systems andhuman relationships. As one of the loremost anthropologists of the tu'entiethcentury, Claude L6vi-Strauss, has expressed it: 'Anthropology has humanitylike the other

    verse manileabout how dit can be said t

    in common.Another prominent anthropolo gist, Cliflord Geerf ' has exPresst9 :-tl-11."'view in an- essay which essentially deals with the differences be tweenhumans and animals:

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    2/29

    2 Snmll Places, Larqe lssucslf wc wirnt lt; discovcr what man amounts to, wc can only find it in whal" mcn are: andwhat mcn arc, abovc all oLhcr things, is various. lt is in undcrstanding thatvilriousncss - its ran8c. its naturc, its basis, and its implications - that wc shall cometo construct a conccpt ol humitn naturc that, morc than a statistical shadow and lessthan a prirnitivisI drcarn, has both substancc and truth. ((lccrtz ) 973, p.52)Although anLhropologistr^ havc n,idc-ranging and frequently highlyspecialised intcrests, they all share a common concern in trying tourrderstand both connccl"ions uril/rirr societies and connecti ons bctwaettsocicties. As will bccome clearcr as we proceed on this journey through thesubjcct-matter and theorics olsocial and cultural anthropology, there is amultitude of ways in which to approach these problems. Whether one rsinterested in undcrstanding why and in which sense the Azande of CentralAfrica believe in witches, why there is greater social inequality in Brazil thanin Sweden, how the inhabitants oI Mauritius avoid violent ethnic conflict.or what has happened to the traditional way of Iite ol-the Inuit (Eskimos) inrecent years, in most cases one or several anthropologists would have carriedout research and written on the issue, Whether one is interested in the studyof religion, child-raising, political power, economic life or the relationshipbetween men and rvomen, one may go to the professional anthropologicalliterature for inspiration and knowledge.

    The discipline is also concerned with accounting for the interrelationshipsbetween dillerent aspects of human existence , and usually anthropologistsinvestigate these interrclationships taking as their point of departure adetailed study oI local life in a parlicular society or a delineated sociatenvironment. One may therelore say that anthropology asks large questions,while at the samc timc it draws i[s most important insights from small places.It has been common to regard its traditional focus on small-scale non-industrial societies as a distinguishing feature of anthropology, comparedwith other subjccl-s dcaling with culturc and society. However, because oichanges in the world and in l-hc discipline itself, this is no longer an accuratedescription. Practically any social system can be studied anthropologicallyand contemporary anthropological research displays an enormous range,empirically as well as thematically.AN OUTLINE OF THE SUBJECTWhat, then, is anthropology? Let us begin with the etymology of the concept.It is a compound o[ two Greek words, 'anthropos' and 'iogos', which can betranslated as 'human' and 'reason', respectively. So anthropology means'reason about humans' or'knowledge about humans'. Social anthropologywould then mean knowledge about humans in societies. Such a definitionwould, o[course, cover the other social sciences as well as anthropology, butit may still be uselul as a beginning.

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    3/29

    Introduction: ComparisonandContext 3The word 'culture', which is also crucial to the discipline, originates lrom

    the Latin 'colere' , which means to cultivate. (The word 'colony' has the sameorigin.) cultural anthropology thus means 'knowledge about cultivatedhumans'; that is, knowledge about those aspects of humanity which are notnatural, but which are related to that which is acquired''Culture' has been described as one olthe two or three most complicatedwords in the English language (Williams 1981, p. 87). In the early 1950s'Clyde Kluckhohn and Alfred Kroeber (1952) presented 161 difl'erentdefinitions oIculture. It would not be possible to consider the maiority ofthese delinitions here; besides, many oI them were - fortunately - quitesimilar. Let us therefore, as a preliminary conceptualisation of culture' de{ineit as those abilities, notions and lorms of behaviour persons have acquiredas members of society. A definition of this kind, which is indebted to both theVictorian anthropologist Edward Tylor and to Geertz (although the latterstresses meaning rather than behaviour), is the most common one amonganthropologists.

    Culture nevertheless carries with it a basic ambiguity. On the one hand,every human is equally cultural; in this sense, the term relers to a basicsimilarity within humanity. On the other hand, people have acquireddifferent abilities, notions, etc., and are thereby dillerent because ofculture.Culture refers, in other words, both to basic similarities and to syslematicdillerences between humans.

    If this sounds slightly complex, some more complcxity is necessary alreadyat this point. Truth to tell, during the last decades of the twentieth century,the concept ofculture was deeply contested in anthropology on both sides oithe Atlantic. The intluential Geertzian concept of culture, which had beenelaborated through a series of erudite and elegant essays written in the I 9 6Osand 1970s (Geertz f973, 1983), depicted a culture both as an integratedwhole, as a puzzle where all the pieces were at hand, and as a system ofme anings that was largely shared by a population. Culture thus appeared asintegrated, shared in the group and sharply bounded. But what of variatiouswithin the group, and what about similarities or mutual contacts with neigh-bouring groups - and what to make of, say, the technologically andeconomically driven processes of globalisation (see Chapter 19), whichensure that nearly every nook and cranny in the world is, to varying degrees,exposed to ne ws about football world cups, to wagework and the concept ol'human rights? In many cases, it could indeed be said that a national or localculture is neither shared by all or most of the inhabitants, nor bounded - Ihave myself explored this myth regarding my native Norway, a countr)'usually considered 'culturally homogeneous' (Eriksen f 9 9 3 b). Many beganto criticise the overly neat and tidy picture suggested in the dominant conceptof culture, from a variety of viewpoints, some of which will be discussed inlater chapters. Alternative ways of conceptualising culture were proposed(e.g. as unbounded'cultural flows'or as'lields of dipcourse', or as 'traditionsof knowledge'), and some even wanted to get rid of the concept altogether

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    4/29

    4 S nlall places, Large lssues

    not just thc contcstcd meaning oIthc term culture, but also the fact thatculture concepts that are close kin to the classic anthropologicar one arebeing exploited politically, in identity politics (see Chapters 17_I9).The relationship between culture and society can be described in the[ollowing way. culturc relers to the acquired, cognitive and symbolic aspectsof existence, whereas society relers to the social organisation of human lil.e,patterns of intcraction and power relationships. The implications of thisanalytical distinction, which may scem bewildering, will eventuallv beevidcnt.A short definition of anthropology may read thus: ,Anthropology is thecomparative study of cultural and social life. Its most important method isparticipant observation, which consists in lengthy lieldwork in a particularsocial setting.' The discipline thus compares aspects of dillerent societies, and

    Further' the discipline emphasises the importance of ethnographiclieldwork, which is a thorough close-up study ol"a particular social andcultural environment, where the researcher is normally required to spend ayear or more.clearly, anthropology has many leatures in common with other socialsciences and huma.ities. Indecd, a difficult question consists in decidingwhether it is a science or one o[the humanities. Do we search for generallaws, as the natural scientists do, or do we instead try to understand anointerpret different societies? E.E. Evans-prilchard in Britain and Alfred

    some of the implications of this divergence in views will be discussed inlater chapters. A few important defining features of anthropology are never-theless common to all practitioners of the subject: it is comparative andempirical; its most important method is fierdwork; and it has a trury grobal

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    5/29

    Introduction: Cornparison and ConLext 5focus in that it does not single out one region, or one kind of society, as beingmore important than others. Unlike sociology proper, anthropology does notconcentrate its attention on the industrialised world; unlike philosophy, itstresses the importance olempirical research; unlike history, it studies societyas it is being enacted: and unlike linguistics, it stresses the social and culturalcontext o[ speech when looking at language. Definitely, there are greatoverlaps with other sciences and disciplines, and there is a lot to be learntfrom them, yet anthropology has its distinctive character as an intcllectualdiscipline, based on ethnographic fieldwork, which tries simultancously toaccount for actual cultural variation in the world and to develop a theoreticalperspective on culture and society.THE UNIVERSAL AND THE PARTICULAR'If each discipline can be said to have a central problem', writes MichaelCarrithers (1992, p. 2),'then the central problem o[ anthropology is thcdiversity of human social li[e.' Put dillerently, one could say that anthropo-logical research and theory tries to strike a balance between similaritics anddi{Ierences, and theoretical questions have olten revolved around the issueof universality versus relativism: To what extent do all humans , cultures orsocieties have something in common, and to what extent is each ol'themunique? Since we employ comparative conceptb - that is' supposedlyculturally neutral tcrms like kinship system, gender role, system o[inheritance, etc. - it is implicitly acknowledged that all or nearly all societieshave several features in common. However, many anthropologis[s challengethis view and claim the uniqueness o[each culture or society. A strong uni-versalist programme is found in Donald Brown's book Hurnan Universnis(Brown 1991), where the author claims that anthropologists have forgenerations exaggerated the differences between societies, neglecting thevery substantial commonalities that hold humanity together. In hisinfluential, ifcontroversial book, he draws extensively on an earlier study oI'human universals', which included:age-grading, athtetic sports, bodily adornment, calendar, clcanliness training'community organization. cooking, cooperative labor, cosmology, courtship, dancing,dccorative art, divination, division of labor, drctrm interpretation, cducation,eschatology, ethics, ethnobotany, etiqucttc, faith hcaling, family, icasting. firemaking, tolklore, food taboos, funeralritcs, games, gesturcs, gift giving' governmcnt,greetrngs ...And this was just the a-to-g segment of an alphabetical 'partial list' (Murdock1945, p. 124, quoted from Brown 1991' p' 70). Several arguments could beinvoked against this kind of list: that it is trivial and that what matters is tocomprehend the unique expressions of such 'universals'; that phenomenasuch as 'tamily' have totally dillerent meanings in different societies, andthus cannot be said to be'the same'everywherei and that this piecemeal

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    6/29

    6 Snlall Places, Large Issueseipproach ;-o society and culture removes the very hallmark o[ good anthro-pology, namely the ability to sce isolated phenomena (like age-grading orfood taboos) in a broad context. An institution such as arranged marriagemcans somcthing fundamentally different in the Punjabi countryside thanin the French upper class. Is it still the same institution? Yes - and no. Brownis right in accusing anthropologists of having been inclined to emphasise theexotic and unique at the expense of neglecting cross-cultural similarities, butthis docs not mean that his approach is the only possible way of bridging thegap between societies. In later chapters, several other alternatives will bediscussed, including structural-functionalism (all societies operate accordingto the same general principles), structuralism (the human mind has acomrnon archilecture cxpressed lhrough myth, kinship and other culturalphcnomena), transactionalism (the logic of human action is the sameeverywhere ) and materialist approaches (culture and society are determinedby ecological and/or technological factors).

    Thc tension between the universal and the particular has been immenselyproductive in anthropology, and it remains an important one. It iscommonly discussed, inside and outside anthropology, through the conceptof ethnocentrism.THI PIt0BLEM OT ETHNOCXNTRISMA society or a culture, it wirs remarked above, must be understood on its or,vnterms. In saying this, we warn against the application ola shared, universalscale to be used in the evaluation o[every society. Such a sca]e, which is oftenuscd, could be defined as longevity, gross national product (GNP), democraticrights, literacy rates, etc. Until quite recently, it was common in Europeansociety to rank non-Europeans according to the ratio o[ their populationwhich was admittcd into the Christian Church. Such a ranking of peoples isuttcrly irrelevant to anthropology. In order to pass judgement on the qualityof lile in a foreign society, lt'e rrust first try to understand that society fromthe inside; otherwise our judgement has a very limited intellectual interest.What is conceived of as 'the good life' in the society in which we live maynoL appear at.t.racLive at all if it is seen liorn a dillerent vantage-point. In orderto understand people's lives, it is therefore necessary to try to grasp thetotality of lheir experiential world; and in order to succeed in this project, itis inadequate to look at selected'variables'. Obviously, a concept such as'annual income' is meaningless in a society where neither money norwagework is common.

    This kind of argument may be read as a warning against ethnocentrism.This term (from Greek 'ethnos' , meaning 'a people') means evaluating otherpeople from one's own vantage-point and describing them in one's ownterms, One's own'ethnos', including one's cultural values, is literally placedat:the centre. Within this frame of thought, other peoples would necessarily

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    7/29

    Introclttction: Contparisortnnd Corttc^-t , 7appcar as in[erior imitations o[ oneto get a mortgagc [o buY a house'society than ourselves. If the KwaAmerica lack electricity, they seemthe Kachin olupper Burma reject conversion to christianity, they arc lesscivilised than we are, and itthe San ('Bushmen') of the Kalahari are illiterate,they appear less intelligent than us. Such points of view express an ethno-centric attitude which fails to allow other peoples to be diflerent lromourselves on their own terms, and can be a serious obstacle to understand-ing. Rather than comparing strangers with our own society and placingourselves on top of an imaginary pyramid, anthropology calls for an under-standing of diflerent societies as they appear lrom thc insirle. Anthropologycannot provide an answer to a question of which societies are better thanothers, simply because the discipline does not ask it. If asked what is the goodlife, the anthropologist will have to answer that every society has its or'r'ndelinition(s) of it.Moreover, an ethnocentric bias, which may be less easy to detect thanmoralistic judgements, may shape the very concepts we use in describingand classifying the world. For example, it has been argued that it may bcinappropriate to speak oI politics and kinship when referring to socictieswhich themselves lack concepts of 'politics' and 'kinship'. Polil"ics, pcrhaps,belongs to the ethnographer's society and not to the pociety under study. wercturn to this fundamental problem later.cultural relativism is sometimes posited as the opposite oIethnocentrism.This is the doctrine that societies or cultures are qualitatively different andhave their own unique inner logic, and that it is therefore scientificallyabsurd to rank them on a scale. IIone places a San group' say, at thc bottomof a ladder wherc Lhe variables are, say, literacy and annuttl incomc, thislacldcr is irrelevant to thcrn if it turns out that the San do not placc a highpriority on money and books. It should also be evident that one cannot,within a cultural relativist lramework, argue that a society with manl'carsis 'better' than one with fewer, or that the ratio of cinemas to population isa useful indicator of the quality of life.cultural relativism is an indispensable and unquestionable theoreticalpremiss and methodological rule-of-thumb in our attempts to undcrstandalien societies in as unprejudiced a way as possible. As an ethical principle.however, it is probably impossible in practice, since it seems to indicate thateverything is as good as everything else, provided it makes sense iu aparticular society. It may ultimately lead to nihilism. For this reason, it mayte timely to stress that many anthropologists are impeccable culturalrelativists in their daily work, while they have dehnite, frequently dogmaticnotions about right and wrong in their private lives. In Western socelsewhere, curren[ debates over minority rights and multiindicate both the need [or anthropological knowledge and the

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    8/29

    8 Snldll Plnccs' Lnrg;e Issttcsof finding a simple solution to these complex problems' which will naturally

    n,hat anthropologl, is about: to disco\er both the uuiqtleness o[ eacl't stx'iirland cultural setting nnd the n'ays in rn'hich humanity is one'SUGGLSTIONS FOR FURTHER READINGL,L Eran'IYirch;r.f: Sr:id j.r:l-..r..,.rr, t-,rr.- < :*'[':e:: ]o: -Cl-dlordGeenz:ltre\ rtro{Uilersnr In 4rvrs:no tu::u'ol {rJ:ro:'l;o eC R'-hc.t-,Bct{*r

    Neu' \ork: lt{cGrart-Hill I 994Adam Kuper: Antltroltology anrl Antltropologlists: T,rc Modcnr British School (lrd edition)London: Routlcdgc 1996.

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    9/29

    2 A BRIEF HISTORY OF ANTHROPOLOGY

    lhavespentover8monthsinonevillageintheTrobriandsandthisprovedtome'how even a poor observer like myself can get a certain amount o[reliable inlormation'it he puts himsetf into the proper conditions for observation '

    - Bronislaw Malinowski (Iette r Lo A'C' Harklon' May 191 6)Like the other social sciences, anthropology is a lairly recent discipline' It\\,as BiveD tts prcsent shapc durirrg, the twer-rtieth celltLlrv, but it hasiurlNlti\r\t lrrrrtutrurtrs i1 t\t' tristori0 .1'ilph\, gt'ogl't\ph\'. tt'itt'c[ lt't'itirtg'philosophy and iurispr-udcucc o[ cuflirr tiuri]s. 'l'ltct't ttt't. itt ttttl' tttst, tttitttt'ways of writing the history of anthropology, iust as, itl ally givctr socict)',there may exist competing versions of [ational history or origitl lnyths.promoted b_.- groups or individuals with diverging interests. History is ttotpdmarih a pnrlu.r o{ the prr:rr itsN. but L: r.rther shuS*ri b5 tht (ctutrus tr[rbe prneot- \ rhde r-L)or-ErrLi changt- pa:. errns anJ [erNLr shi-h. r-\trruuforeground and background. and rsill be understood alrd er-aluated In ne\\ways. In an important book on the state o[ the art iu (chiefly) Atuericartrcultural anthropology, Bruce Knauft (1996) distinguishes between at leastfour 'genealogies of the prescnt' - four different ways of accounting for thepresent situation. This ambiguity of the past not only has a bearing on thewriting of our own professional history, but is itself a subject of anthropo-logical inquiry to be dealt with in a later chapter.ln other words. there can be no neutral history of anthropology (or o[ansthins.r. but rr-hat fo\Ioss trlort Ls neserthelers an attempt to provide abrief and - 35 [31 6 pcssible - unconro\-ersial description o[ the deseiopmento[ the subiect.PROTO.ANTHROPOLOGYII anthropology is the study o[ cultural variation, its roots rnay be tracecl aslar back in history as the ancient Greeks. The historian Herodotus (5thcentury BC) wrote detailed accounts of'barbarian' peoples to the east andnorth of the Greek peninsula, comparing their customs and belie[s to thoseof Athens, and the group of philosophers known as the Sophists were perhapsthe first philosophical relativists, arguing (as many twentieth-centuryanthropologists have done) that there can be no absolute truth because, aswe would put it today, truth is context-bound. Yet their interest in human

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    10/29

    I 0 Srnall places, Large Issuesvariation and differing cultural values fell short of being scientific, chietlybecause Hcrodotus lacked theory lt'hile the Sophi;^ts lacked empiricalmaterial.

    A more crcdible ancestor is thc Tunisian intellectual Ibn Khaldun(1332-1406), a remarkablc man who anticipated the social sciences byseveral centuries. His main work, the Muqaddintah ('An introduction tohistory'), was wri[ten in thc years following 1375, and contains aremarkablc wealth of obscrvations on law, education, politics and theeconomy. Khaldun's main achievement nevertheless lies in his non-religious, theoretical lramework, where he stresses diflering [orms of socialcohesion as a key variablc in accounting for historical change and the rise ofluew groups [0 powcr.In Europe, scholarly intcrest in cultural variation and human nature re-cmerged in the following century as a consequence of the new intellectualfreedom of the Renaissance and, perhaps even more importantly, increasingEuropean cxplorations and conquests ofdistant lands. Illustrious intellectu-als such as Michel de Montaigne (sixteenth century), Thomas Hobbes(seventeenth century) and Giambattista Vico (eighteenth century) belongedto the first generations of European thinkers who tried to account for culturalvariability and global cultural history as well as, in the case of Montaigne,taking on the challenge lrom relativism. In the eighteenth century, theoriesof human nature, moral philosophies and social theories developed, takinginto account an awareness of deep cultural diffcrences dividing humanity.David Humc (I7II-76), along with Adam Smith the most important thinl

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    11/29

    A Brie[ History of Anthropology I Ithat there existed a single, universal, global civilization. Herder argued thateach people (Volk) had its own Gcist or'soul'and therefore a right toretaining its own, unique values and customs - in a manner reminiscent oflater cultural relativism. Indeed, by the end o[ the eighteenth century,several o[ the theoretical questions still raised by anthropologists hadalready been defined: universalism versus relativism (what is commoo tohumanity; what is culturally specific), ethnocentrism versus culturalrelativism (moral judgements versus neutral descriptions of other peoplcs),and humanity versus (the rest o0 the animal kingdom (culture versusnature). Twentieth-century anthropotogy teaches that these and otheressentially philosophical problems are best investigated through therigorous and detailed study of actual living people in existing societies, andby applying carefully dcvised methods of comparison to the bewilderingvariety of 'customs and beliets'. It would take several generations alter Mon-tesquieu's comparative musings about Persia and France untilanthropology achieved this mark of scientihc endeavour'

    VICTORIAN ANTHROPOLOGYA characteristic of the anthropology of the nineteenth century was the belieiin social evolution - the idea that human societies developed in a particulardirection - and the related notion that European societies wcre the erld-product oI a long developmental chain which began with 'savagery" Thisidea was typical of the victorian age, dominated by an optimistic beliel irrtechnological progress and, simultaneously, European colonialism, whichwas frequently justified with reference to what Kipling wrote of as 'the whiteman's burden'; the alleged duty of the European to'civilise the savages'. Thefirst general theories of cultural variation to enjoy a lasting influence werearguably those of two men traincd as lawyers; Henry Maine (1822-88) inBritain and Ler,r'is Henry Morgan (18f 8-82) in the United States. True tothe spirit oI the times, both presented evolutionist models of variation andchange, where west European societies were seen as the pinnacle oIhumandevelopment. In his Ancient Law ( I 8 6l ), Maine distinguished between whathe called status and contractsocieties, a divide which corresponds roughly tolater dichotomies between traditional and modern societies. or, in the Iatenineteenth-century German sociologist Ferdinand Tonnies' tern]inology,Gemeinscha[t (community) and Gesellscha/t (society): status societies areassumed to operate on the basis of kinship and myth, while individual meritand achievement are decisive in contract societies. Although simplecontrasts ofthis kind have regularly been severely criticised, they continueto exert a certain inlluence on anthropological thinking'

    Morgan's contributions to anthropology were wide-ranging' and' amongmany other things, he wrote a detailed ethnography of the Iroquois. His evo-Iutionary scheme, presented in Ancient society (187 7), distinguished betweeD

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    12/29

    |2 Snrali plncss, Large lssuesscven staS,es (front lower savagery to civilization), and the typology wasmainly based on technological achievements. His materialist account oIcultural change immcdiatcly attracted Marx tLnd Engels, whose laterwritings on non- (or pre-) capitalist societies were clearly influenced byMorgan. Among Morgan's othcr achievements, his concern with kinshipmusl. be mentioncd. Dividing human kinship systems into a limited numberof typcs, and secing kinsbip terminology as a key to undcrstanding society,he is widely credited with making thc study of kinship a central preoccupa-tion o[anthropology, which it has indeed remained to this day. Writing in thesame period, the historian of religion Robertson Smith and the lawyer J.J.Bachofen oflered, respcctively, thcories of monotheistic religion and of the(wrongly) assur.ned historical t.ransition lrom matriliny to patriliny.An untypical scholar in the otherwise evolutionist Victorian era, theGerman ethnologist Adolf Bastian (1826-1905) reacted against what hesaw as simplistic typological schcmata. Drawing inspiration lrom bothHerderian Romanticism and Lhe humanistic tradition in German academra,Bastian wrote prolificalll' qr cultural history, taking great care to avoidunwarranted generalisations, yet hc held that all humans have the samepattern of thinking. This idea would later be developed independently, togreat sophisticat.ion, in Claude Levi-Strauss's structuralism.The leading British anthropologist of thc late Victorian era was EdwardTylor (1832-1917), who influenced Darwin's thinking about culture, andwhose voluminous writings include the famous definition of culturementioned in the first chapter: 'culture or civilization, taken in its widestethnographic scnsc, is that complcx whole which includes knowledge, belief,

    art, morals, custom, and any other capabilities and habits acquired by manas a member of society' (Tylor 1968 [1871]). This definition is still seen asuseiul by many anthropologists. Tylor's student James Frazer (l 8 54 194I),who rvould cclipsc his teachcr in tcrms of [ame and who held the first chairin Social Anthropology in Britain, urrote lhc massive Golden Bough (1g90,rev. edr.r i911-1 5), an ambiLious comparative study of myth and religion.Both Tylor and Frazer werc evolutionists, and Frazer's main theoreticalproject consisted in demonstrating how thought had developed from themagical via the rellgious to the scientillc.Neither Tylor nor Frazer carried out detailed field studies, although Tylorspent several years in Mexico and wrote a book there. A famous anecdotetells of a dinner party where William James, the pragmatist philosopher,asked Frazer whether he had ever become acquainted with any of thosesavages he wrote so much about. Frazer allegedly replied, in a shocked toneof voice, 'Heaven torbid ' (Evans-Pritchard 19 51).Important intellectual developments outside anthropology in the secondhalf of the nineteenth cen[ury also had a powerful impact on the lield.Darwin's theory of natural selection, first presented inhis origin o[species froml8 59, would both be seen as a condition for anthropology (positing, as it did,that all humans are closely related) and, later, as a threat to the discipline

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    13/29

    ABrielHistory ol Anthropologu 1)(arguing, as it seemed to do, the primacy of the biologicalovcr the cul[urali secIngold 1986). The emergence of classic sociological theory in the works ofComte, Marx and Tcinnies, and later Durkheim, Weber, Pareto and Simmcl,provided anthropologists with general theories of society, although the irapplicability to non-European societies continues to be disputed.The quality ofthe ethnographic data used by the early anthropologistswas variable. Most of the scholars mentioned above relied on the writtensources that were available, ranging from missionaries' accounts totravelogues o[ varying accuracy. The need [or more reliable data began tomake itself felt, Expeditions and systematic surveys - among the most lamouswere the British Torres Straits expedition led by W.H.R. Rivers and the large-scale American explorations of the Indians of the north-western coast -provided researchers around the turn of the century with an improvedunderstanding of the compass of cultural variation, which would eventuallylead to the downfall of the ambitious theories of unilineal evolution charac-teristic of nineteenth-century anthropology.An Austro-Cerman speciality proposed both as an alternative and acomplement to evolutionist thinking, was dilfusionism, the doctrine oI thehistorical diffusion of cultural traits. Never a part of the mainstream outsideof the German-speaking world (but counting important supporters in theEnglish-speaking world, including Rivers), elaborate theories oI culturaldiflusion continued to thrive, particularly in Berlin and Vienna, until after theSecond World War. Nobody denied that di[Iusion took place, but there wereserious problems of verification associated with the theory. Within anthro-pology, diffusionism went out of fashion when, around the time o[ thc lirstWorld War, researchers began to study single societids in great detail withouttrying to account for their historical development. However, a theoreticaldirection reminiscent oldiffusionism returned in the 1990s, under the labelotglobalisation theory (see Chapter 19), which is an attempt to understandand account for the ways in which modern mass communica[ions,migration, capitalism and other 'global' phenomena interact with localconditions,

    In spite of these and other theoretical developments and methodologicalrefinements, the emergence of anthropology, as the discipline is knowntoday, is usually associated with lour outstanding scholars working in threecountries in the early decades of the twentieth century: Franz Boas in theUSA, A,R. Radcliffe-Brown and Bronislaw Malinowski in the UK, and MarcelMauss in France.BOAS AND CULTURAL RELATIVISMBoas (1858-1942), a German immigrant to the United States who hadbriefly studied anthropology with Bastian at Heidelberg, carried outimportant research among Eskimo and Kwakiutl Indians in the 1890s. In

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    14/29

    j 4 Snrall places, Large Issueshis tcaching and prolessional le adership, he strengthened the 'four-lieldapproach' in Ame rican anthropology, which still sets it apart lrom Europeananthropology, as it encompasses not only cultural and social anthropology,but also physical anthropology, archaeology and linguistics. In spite oIthrs,Boas is chiefly remembered for his ideas. Although cultural relativism hadbeen introduced more than a century before, it was Boas who madc it acentral premise for anthropological research. Reacting against the grandevolutionary schemes oITylor, Morgan and others, Boas took an early stancein [avour o[ a more particularist approach. He argued that each culture hadto be understood on its own terms and that it would be scientilicallymisleading to judge and rank other cultures according to a Western, ethno-centric typology gauging'levels of devclopment'. Accordingly, Boas alsopromoted lilstorical particularism, the view that all societies or cultures hadthcir own, unique history thaL could not be reduced to a category in someuniversalist scheme of development. On related grounds, Boas arguedagainst the un[ounded claims olracist pseudo-science, which were supportedby most of the leading biologists of the time. Boas's insistence on themeticulous collection of empirical data was not only due to his scientificviews, but also the realisation that cultural change quickly obliterated whathe saw as unique cultures, particularly in North America. Already in TheMind of Pritnitivc Man ( I 9 1 1), Boas argued that anthropology ought to beengaged on behalf of threatened indigenous populations.Perhaps because o[his particularism, Boas never systematised his ideasin a theoretical treatise. Several ofhis students and associates neverthelessdid develop general theories of culture, notably Ruth Benedict, ArfredKroeber and Robert Lowie. His most famous student was Margaret Mead(1901-78)" Although her bestselling books from various pacific societieshave been criticised for being ethnographically superlicial (see Chapter 4),they skilfully used material from non-western societies to raise questionsabout gender relations, socialisation and politics in the West. Mead,s workshows, probably better than that of any other anthropologist, the potentialof cultural criticism inherent in the discipline.One of Boas's most remarkable associates, the linguist Edward Sapir(1884-1939), formulated, with his student Beniamin Lee Whorf, the so-called Snprir-WltorJ hypothcsis, which posits that language determinescognition, and that the world's languages difler enormously (see Chapter15). consistent with a radical cultural relativism, the hypothesis implies that,

    lor example, Hopi Indians see and perceive the world in a fundamentallydifferent way from westerners, due to differences in the structure of therrrespective languages.Due to Boas's influence, the materialist tradition from Morgan fell into thebackground in the USA during the first half of the twentieth century. It wouldlater re-emerge as cultural ecology and neo-evolutionism, and Morgan,slegacy would also be acknowledged by many Marxist anthropologists. But for

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    15/29

    ABrieJHistory ol Anthropology I )now, Morgan's evolutionism was firmly sidetracked, as was any potentialinfluence lrom Darwin's theory of evolution'

    THETWO BRITISH SCHOOLS IWhile modern American anthropology had been shaped, on the one hand bythe Boasians and their relativist concerns, and on the other hand by theperceived need to record native cultures before their feared disappearance,the situation in the major colonial power, Great Britain, was very different.The degree of complicity between colonial agencies and anthropologistsworking in the colonies is debatable (Goody 1995), but the very fact olimperialism was an inescapable, if usually implicit, premise lor Britishanthropology at least until de-colonisation.

    The man who is often hailed as the founder of modern British socialanthropology was a Polish immigrant, Bronislaw Malinowski (18 84-1942),whose two years of fieldwork in the Trobriand Islands (between I 9 14 and1918)set a standard for ethnographic data collection that is still largelyunchallenged. Malinowski stressed the need to learn the local languageproperly and to engage in everyday life in the society under scrutiny, in orderto learn its categories 'from within', and to understand the often subtle inter-connections between the various social institutions and cultural notions.Malinowski also placed an unusual emphasis on the acting individual, seeingsocial structure not as a determinant of but as a framework lor action, andhe wrote about a wide range of topics, from garden magic, economics,technology and sex to the puzzling kula trade (see Chapter 12), oftenintroducing new issues. Although he dealt with pany topics of generatconcern, he nearly always took his point of departure in his Trobriandethnography, demonstrating a method of generalisation very di[Ierent fromthat of the previous generation, with its more piecemeal local knowledge.Malinowski regarded all institutions oIa society as intrinsically linked to eachother, and stressed that every social or cultural phenomenon ought to bestudied in its full context. He also held that inborn human needs were thedriving force in the development of social institutions, and thereiore hisbrand of [unctionalism is often described as 'biopsychological functionalism'.

    The other leading light in inter-war British social anthropology, A,R.Radclifle-Brown ( I 8 8 1-1 9 5 5 ), had a stronger short-term infl ucnce than hisrival, although it faded rapidly after the Second World War. An admirer oiEmile Durkheim's sociology, Radcliffe-Brown did relatively little fieldn'orkhimself, but aimed at the development o[ a 'natural science of socie[y' irrthe spirit o[ the Encyclopedists - where the universal laws of socialintegration could be lormulated. His theory, known as structural-function-alism, saw the acting individual as theoretically unimportant, emphasisinginstead the social institutions (including kinship, norms, politics, elc,). Mostsocial and cultural Dhenomena, according to this view, could be seen as

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    16/29

    16 Smallplaces, Largelssuesfunctional in the sense that they contributed to the maintenance of theoverall social structure. Some of his most important essays are collected inStructure and Function ilr Prinitive Society (1952)' where he shows howsocieties, in his view, are intcgrated, and how social institutions reinforceeach ol"her and contribute to the maintenance of society.

    Radclifle-Brown's scien[ific idcals were taken from natural science, andhe hoped to develop'gcneral laws oIsociety'comparable in precision [o thoseof physics and chemistry. This programme has been abandoned by mostanthropologists - like structural-lunctionalism in its pure form - but manyof the questions raised by contemporary anthropologists, particularly inDurope, wcrc origiually franrcd by lladclilTe-Brown.

    Despite their diflerences in emphasis, both British schools had a sociologi-cal concern in common (which they did not share with most Americans),and tended to see social institutions as functional. Both distanced themselvesfrom the ra,'idc-ranging claims oI dillusionism and evolutionism, and by thenext generation olscholars, the influences o[the two [ounding fathers maybe said to have merged (Kuper 1996), although the tension betweenstructural explanations and actor-centred accounts remains strong andproductive in anthropology even today (see Chapter 6).

    Malinowski's students included important names such as Raymond Firth,Audrey Richards and Isaac Schapera, while Radcliffe-Brown, in addition toenlisting E.E, Evans-PriLchard and Meyer Fortes - arguably the mostpowerlul British anthropologists in the 1950s - on lris side, taught widelyabroad and introduced his brand ofsocial anthropology to several colonialuniversities (notably Sydney and Cape Town) as well as Chicago. Britishanthropology, as typified by the first generation after Malinowski andRadcliffe-Brown, was characterislically orie nted towards kinship, politicsand economics, with Evans-Pritchard's masterpiece The Nuer (1940) demon-strating, perhaps better than any other monograph of the period, theintellectual power of a discipline combining detailed ethnography,comparison and elegant models. (Later, his models would be criticised forbeing too elegant. to l'it thc facts on the ground - a very Malinor.t'skianobjection to be examined in Chapte r I 1.)MAUSSAlthough anthropology and ethnology were still important subjects in theGerman-speaking region, they were set back seriously after the Second WorldWar. With France, it is different, and along with the UK and the USA, Francewas a major centre of anthropological thought and research throughout thetwentieth century. Already, in 1903, Durkheim had published' with hisnephew Marcel Mauss (18 72-i 9 50), an important treatise on knowledgesystems, Primitive ClassiJication (Durkheim and Mauss 1963). In 1909'Arnold van Gennep published Les Ritcs d.e passage, a strikingl.v original

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    17/29

    ABrielHistorY ol AnthroPologg 17

    and duly praised in theMuusr;s theoretical stronglY insystematic comparlson rns in social

    life at all times and in al tivist note inhis reasoning about similaritics and diflerences betwecn societies LikeRadcliffe-Brown, Mauss was inspired by Durkheim, but in a vcry dif|erentway. Rather than developing 'a natural science of society' complcte n'ith'laws', his projcct consisted in describing and classifying greaily differentsocieties in order to look for structural similarities' In this way' he hopcd tode olsocial li[e' Mauss neverac hiq famous Thc Grli (19 54t1 'Ann(c Sociolo,girlrrc' n'hichM 9r7.Mauss also tlcver carricd out cthnographic tieldwork' but his vastknowledge of languagcs and culttlral historl' enabled him to present somcof the most penetrating analyses to date of phenomena such as sacrifice' gift-giving, personhood and the nation' Much of his energy in the intcr-rn'ar yearsnevertheless was spent on completing and publishing unfinished work lelt by

    tions in techniques of observation.The transition from evolutionist theory and grand syntheses [o more specific'detailed and empirically tounded work, which in dillerent ways took placeintheUK.theUsAandFranceduringthefirstdecadesofthetwentiet'lr

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    18/29

    I f.i Sntall places, Large Issuesccntury, amounted to nothing short of an intellectual revolution. In thespace ola lew years, the work ofTylor, Morgan and even Frazer had beenrelegated to the mists of history, and the discipline had in reality been takenover by small groups of scholars who saw intensive fieldwork, culturalrelativism, the study of single, small-scale societies and rigorous comparisonas the essence olthe new discipline. Today, the academic institutions, theconlerences and the learned journals all build on a view ofanthropology asa discipline that came into its own witb Boas, Malinowski, Radcliffe-Brownand Mauss. To a greater or lesser extent, this is also true of the anthropo-logical traditions o[ other countries (see Vermeulen and Rolddn 1995),including India, Australia, Mcxico, Argentina, the Netherlands, Spain,Scandinavia and, partly, the German-spe aking world. Soviet/Russian andEast European anthropologies have followed diflerent itineraries, and haveretained a connection with the older German volkskunde tradition, which ismore descriptive.

    Later developments in anthropology, to which we now turn briefly, revealboth continuity with and reactions against the foundations that were laidbefore the Second World War.THE SECOND HALF OF THE TWENTIETH CENTURYThe number of professional anthropologists and institutions devoted toteaching and research in the l'ield grew rapidly after the Second world war.The discipline also diversified, partly because of 'population pressure'. Newspecialisations such as psychological anthropology, political anthropologyand the anthropology of ritual emerged, and the geographical foci of thediscipline multiplied: whereas the pacific had been the most fertile area fornew theoretical developmcnts in the 1920s and Alrica had played a similarpart in the 19l0s and 1940s, trnd the American preoccupation with NorthAmerican Indians had been stable throughout, the 1950s saw a growinginterest in the 'hybrid' (or'rrrcstrzo')societies olLalin America as well as theanthropology of lndia and South-East Asia, while the New Guineanhighlands became similarly important in the 1960s. Such shifts in geo-graphical emphasis could be consequential in theoretical developments, aseach region raises its own peculiar problems.

    From the L 9 50s onwards, the end of colonialism has also affected anthro-pology, both in a banal sense - it has become more difllcult to obtainresearch permits in Third World countries - and more profoundly, as therclationship between [he obscrver and the observed has become problematicsince the traditionally 'observed' peoples increasingly have their own intel-lectuals and spokespersons, who frequently object to western interpretationsof their way of life. Anthropology has grown not only in size but in intellec-tual and academic importance, but the current situation also Doses its ownpeculiar challenges.

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    19/29

    A B r iel Histo r y ol An tl v o p olo 91 y

    STRUCTURALISMThe first major theory to emerge after the Second World War was Claudel;ivi-Strauss's structuralism. An admircr of Mauss and' like him' not a maiorfieldworker, [,vi-Strauss (190S- )developed an original theory olthe hurnanmind, based on inspiration from structural linguistic5, Mauss's theory oIexchange and Lvy-Bruhl's theory olthe primitive mind (which L6vi-Straussrejected). His first major work, Les Structures dtnrcntaires de In parent( (ThcElernentar y S tr u c tur c s o[ Kinship, 19 69 [I9 49]), i ntroduced a grammatica Lformal way of thinking about kinship, with particular re lerence to systemsof marriage (the exchange oI women between groups). L6vi-Strauss Iatcrexpanded his theory to cover totemism, myth and art, Never uncontroversial,structuralism had an enormous impact on French intellectual life [ar beyondthe confines of anthropology, and many leading contemporary Frenchanthropologists have been students ofLdvi-Strauss. In the English-speakingworld, the reception of structuralism was delayed, as Lvi-Strauss's majorworks were not translated until the 1960s, but they had both maior admirersand detractors from the beginning. Structuralism was criticised [or beinguntestable, positing as it did certain unprovable and unfalsi['iable propertiesof the human mind (most lamously the propensity to think in terms ofcontrasts or binary oppositions), but many saw L6vi-Strauss's work, alu'ttyscommitted to human universals, as an immense source of inspiration in thestudy of symbolic systems such as knowledge and myth.

    A rather different, and for a long time much less influential, brand of struc-turalism was developed by another student of Mauss, namely Louis Dumont(19i 1-99), an Indianist and Sanskrit scholar who did fieldwork both in theAryan north and the Dravidian south. Dumont, closer to Durkheirn'steachings on social cohesion than L6vi-Strauss, argued for a holisticperspective (as opposed to an individualistic one) in hts major work on theIndian caste system, Hortto Hiernrchicrr.s (1 9 80 [1 9 69 ]), claiming that Indiarrs(and by cxtension, many non-modern peoples) saw themse lves uot as 'freeindividuals' but as actors irretrievably cnmeshed in a web oIcomtnitmentsand social relations, which in the Indian case was clearly hierarchical.Most major French anthropologists o[ later generations have beenassociated with either Ldvi-Strauss, Dumont or Balandier, the Africanistwhose work in political anthropology simultaneously bridged gztps betu'eet.tFrance and the Anglo-Saxon world and inspired both neo-Marxist researchand applied anthropology devoted to development.REACTIONS TO STRUCTURAL-FUNCTIONALISMIn Britain and the colonies, the structural-[unctionalism now associatedchiefly with Evans-Pritchard and Fortes was under increased pressure alterthe war. Indeed, Evans-Pritchard himsellrepudiated his [ormer views in the

    tL)

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    20/29

    2O Sntall places, Large Issnesl9 5(i.1, arguing that the search lor'natural laws o[society'had been shownto bc futile and that anthropology should fashion itself as a humanitiesdiscipline rather than a natural science. Retrospcctively, this shift has oftenbeen quotcd as marking a shift 'frorn function to meaning' in the discipline'spriorities; and a lcading American anthropologist of the period, AlfredKroeber, expressed similar views in the usA. others found their own pathsaway lrom what was increasingly seen as a conceptual straitjacket, lorexample Malinowski's student Edmund R. Leach, whose poiitic al systents ofHigltlancl Burtna (1954) suggested a departure from certain orthodoxies,notably Radclifle-Brown's dictum that social systems tend to be rnequilibrium and Malinowski's view of myths as integrating 'social charters'.Latcr, Leach, always a controversial and unpredictable thinker, would be amain promoter and critic of structuralism in Britain. A few years earler,Leach's contemporary Raymond Firth had proposed a distinction betweensocial slructure (the sets ofstntuses in society) and social organisation (Firth1951), which he saw as the actual process of social life, where choice andindividual whims were seen in a dynamic relationship to structuralconstraints. Later in the 1 9 50s and I 9 60s, several younger social anthro-pologists, notably F.G. Bailey and Fredrik Barth, lollowed Firth's lead as vyellas the theory of games (a recent development in economics) in refining anactor-ccntred pcrspe c[ivc on social lifc, wherc thc formerly paramount levelo[norms and social institutions were re-framed as contextual variables (oreven, as in a programmatic statement by Barth, as unintended consequencesof intentional action). Following a difterent itinerary, Max Gluckman, aforrner pupil of Radclille-Brown and a close associate of Evans-pritchard, alsoincreasingly abandoned the strong holist programme of the structural-func-tionalists, reconceptualising social structure as a rather loose set ofconstraints, while emphasising thc importance of individual actors.cluckman's colle agues included a number oIimport.ant Africanists, such asA.L. Epstein,l. clyde Mitchell, Victor Turner and Elizabeth colson. worki.gin Southern Africa, this group pioneered both urban anthropology and tne

    study of cthnicity in the 1950s and 1960s.NEO-EVOI,TJTIONISM, CUL'I'I]IiAI, ECOI,O(IY AND NEO-MARXISMThe number o[ practising anthropologists has alw.ys been larger in theUnited statcs than anywhere else, and the discipline has also been verydiverse there. Although the influence from the Boasian cultural relativistschool remains strong to this day, other groups o[scholars have also madetheir mark. From the late 1940s oni,r,ards, a resurgent interesL in Morgan'sevolutionism as well as Marxism led to the formulation of several non-Boasian, cvolur-ionist and materialist research programmes. Julian sr.e ward,a student of Robert Redfield at chicago (who had himsell been a student olRadclifle-Brown), proposed a theory ol'cultural dynamics where he distin-

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    21/29

    A Brief History of Anthropology 2lguished between 'the cultural core' (basic institutions such as the divisionof labour) and'the rest of culture' in a way strongly reminiscent of Marx, aninfluence which could not be acknowledged openly at the time lor politicalreasons. Steward led research proiects and supervised work among LatinAmerican peasants as well as North American Indians, and encouraged arenewed focus on the relationship between culture, technology and theenvironment. His contemporary Leslie White held vidws that were moredeterministic than Steward's (who allowed for major local variations), butalso - perhaps oddly - saw symbolic culture as a largely autonomous realm(see Chapter 13). Among the major scholars influenced by White' MarvinHarris has retained the materialist determinism in his own theory, whichhe calls cultural materialism, while Marshall Sahlins in the 1960s made themove from neo-evolutionism to a symbolic anthropology influenced bystructuralism.Cultural ecologl', largely a North American speciality, sprang from theteachings o[ Steward and White, and represented a rare collaborationbetween anthropology and biology. Especially in the 1960s, many suchstudies were carried out; the most famous is doubtless Roy Rappaport's Pigsfor tlrc Ancestors (1968), an attempt to account lor a recurrent ritual in theNew Guinean highlands in ecological terms. However, the upsurge olMarxist peasant research, especially in Latin America, in the 1970s, r'r'asclearly also indebted to Steward.

    The advent of radical student politics in the late 1960s, which continuedto have an impact on academia until the early 1980s, had a slrong, i[passing, influence on anthropology virtually everywhere. Of the more lastingcontributions, apart lrom the string of peasant studies initiated by Stewardand furthered by Eric Wolf, Sidney Mintz and others, the French attempt atsynthesising L6vi-Straussian structuralism, Althusserian Marxism andanthropological relativism must be mentioned here,, Emmanuel Terray,Claude Meillassoux and, probably most importantly, Maurice Godelier wereamong those who tried to combine a concern with local conditions and auniversalist, ultimately evolutionist theory o[ society. Although bothMarxism and structuralism eventually became unfashionable, scholarsparticularly those engaged in applied work - continue to draw inspirationfrom Marxist thought.SYMBOLIC AND COGNITIVE ANTHROPOLOGYMore true to the tenor olthe Boasian legacy than the materialist approaches,studies of cognition and symbolic systems have developed and diversilledenormously in the decades after the second world war. A leading theoristis Cliflord Geertz, who wrote a string of influential essays advocatinghermenerrtics (interpretive method) in the 1960s and 1970s. while hisoriginality as a theorist can be questioned (possible precursors include the

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    22/29

    22 Srrrall places, Large Issuesplrirosopher Paul ILiccur, whosc influcn ce Oeertz acknowledges, as well asllvans-Prilchard and Malinowsl

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    23/29

    A Brie[ History oJ' Anthropology 21would suggest. These and other publications contributed to a sense oIcrisisin the discipline in the 19ttOs and early 1990s, as somc o[ its ccnlralll ain'b culturallY uniquen conceals a deePere ring the latter halfof the twentieth century was Noam chomsky's generative grammar, whichstressed the similarities between all languages. Eluen strong relativistpositions need a notion of the universal in order to make comparisons. Thisuniversal is ultimately located to the human mind in structuralism andmany varieties o[cognitive anthropology (see D'Andrade 199 5), and, fromthis perspective, it can even be said that [he relaLivity of culture s is mere ly asurface phenomenon since the mind works in the same way everywherc'Anthropology at the beginning of the new millennium is a sprawling andvaried discipline with a strong academic foothold in all continents, althoughits intellectual centres remain in the English- and French-speaking parts ofthe world. It is still possible to discern differences between American culturalanthropology, British social anthropology and French ethnologie, but theperhaps, but in itstudied intensivelY

    he r.t'orld changes,ate, ranging lrom

    studies of ethnomedicine and the body to urban consumer culture,advertising and cyberspace. Although the grand theories ofthe nineteenthand twentieth centuries - lrom unilinear evolutionism to structuralism -

    havcby and large been abandoned, new theories claiming to provide a unified viewof humanity are being proposed; lor example, new advances in evolutionarytheory and cognitive science offer ambitious general accounts oI social li[eand the human mind, respectively. The puzzles and problems confrontingearlier generations of anthropologists, regarding, for pxample, the nature o[social organisation, of knowledge, of kinship, ol myth and ritual, rcuaitlcentral to the discipline, although they are explored in ne\ I empirical settirlgsby scholars who are more specialised than their predecessors'SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER READINGAfan Barnard: Historg andThtory fu Anthropologg, Cambridge: Cambridge [Jnivecsity Prcss2000.AdamKuper: AnthropologyandAnthropologists:TheModentBrjtishSchool,3rdcdn'lnndon:

    Routledgc 1996.f erry D. Moore: Visions ol Culture: An lntrotlut:tion to Anthrttpologit'rtl Tltr:orics arrd T/tcori.sts.Walnut Creck, CA: AltaMira Press 1997.Han F. Vcrmeulcn and Arturo Alvarez Roldi{n, cds: Fit'ldrvork antl l:ooLrtoLas: .l'tridi/s itt l/tc

    HisLoru oJ F'rtropean Anthroltology' [,ondon: Routledge 199 5

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    24/29

    FIELDWORK AND ITS INTERPRETATION

    'l'hcory without data is empty, but data without l-heory arc blind,- C. Wriaht MillsFIELDWORKAnthropology distinguishes itselIfrom the other social sciences through thegreat emphasis placed on ethnographic fieldwork as the most importantsource o[new knowledge about society and culture. A field study may last lora lew months , a year, or even two years or more, and it aims at developingas intimate an understanding as possible of the phenomena investigated.Many anthropologists return to their lield throughout their career, to deepentheir understanding further or to record change. Although there aredifferenccs in field methods between different anthropological schools, it isgenerally agreed that the anthropologist ought to stay in the field longenough lor his or her presence to be considered more or less 'natural' by thepermanent residents, the inlormants, although he or she will always to someextent remain a stranger.Many anthropologists involuntarily take on the role of the clown whenin the lleld. They may speak strangely wifh a flawed grammar; they asksurprising and sometimes tactless questions, and tend to break many rulesregarding how things ought to be done. Such a role can be an excellentstarting-point for lieldwork, even if it is rarely chosen: through discoveringhow the locals react to one's own be haviour, one obtains an early hint abouttheir way of thinking. We are all perceived more or less as clowns inunfamiliar surroundings; there a re so many rules of conduct in any societythat onc will necessarily break some of them when one tries to take part insocial life in an alien society. In BriLain, for example, it is considereduncultured to wear white socks with a dark suit; still, it happeLrs that peoplcwho are not fully conversanl with the local dress code do so. In the field,anthropologists have been known to commit much more serious mistakesthan this.A diflerent, and somctimes more problematic, role that can be assume d bythe anthropologist in the field, is tbe expert role. Many fieldworkers are treatedwith great deference and respect by their hosts, are spoken to in extremelypolite ways and so on, and can thus run the risk ofnever seeing aspects olsociety which the locals are ashamed of showing to high-ranking strangers.

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    25/29

    Fieldwork and its InLerprctation 25No malter which role one takes on in the field - most ethnogretphers arcprobably partly expert, partly clown, at least in the early stages - fieldworris extremely demanding, both in professional and in human terms. The tidy,

    systematic and well-rounded texts written by anthropologisls are more oltenthan not the end-product of long periods in the I'ield characterised byboredom, illness, personal privations, disappointments and frustration: fewanthropologists can state squarely that their fieldwork was a continuouslyexciting journey of exploration, full of pleasant experiences. In a loreignsetting, one will usually master the Ianguage and tHe codes [or behaviourpoorly at the beginning, and one will feel helpless in many situations, Besides,one runs the risk o[ encountering suspicion and hostility, and it can beprofoundly unpleasant for the body to have to cope with an unfamiliarclimate, strange food and a different hygienic standard than one isaccustomed to. Last but not least, it can be very trying for people with amiddle-class'Western'background (which is that of most anthropologists) toadapt to societies where being alone is considercd a pitiful or pathologicalcondition. Plainly put, in many village settings onc is ncver left alone. Thisproblem does not usually exist for the growing number oI anthropologistswho carry out their fieldwork in modern urban settings. In their case, Lheproblem may be the opposite: in societies where people have TV sets and cars,and where time is considered a scarce resource, an ethnographer may quickrydiscover that his or her presence creates neither excitement nor curiosityamong the natives, and that continuous immersion in local lile is ditlicult.Urban fieldwork tends to be more discontinuous than village fieldwork, andolten depends on more formal methods, such as the structured interview.Even fieldwork in 'exotic' settings should not be unduly romanticised.Among some Native American peoples in North America, a new professionhas emerged in recenI years: that of thc professional ethnographicinformant. Some cultural specialists may thus charge handsome fees forspending their tirne explaining the intricacics o[ myths and customs tovisiting ethnographers.IN THE FIELDA principal requirement in ['ield',t'ork nonctheless consists ol trying to takepart in local liie as much as possible. AnthropologisLs also use a varicty olspecified, formal techuiques for the collection o[data (see, for example, Peltoand Pelto 1970; Ellen 1984). Depending on the kind oilleldwork one isengaged in, structured interviews, statistical sampling and other techniquesmay be required to varying degrees. Most anthropologists depcnd orr acombination of formal techniques and unstrucIured participant observationin their fieldwork.Participant observation relers to the inlormal field methods which formthe basis lor most fieldwork, lt,hether or not it is supplemented wiLh other

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    26/29

    26 Snnll pktces, Large lssuas

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    27/29

    Fieldwork and its lnterpretation 27terms, yet hc personally regarded them as unwashed savages and frequcr-rily.he reports, had to force himself to leave his hut in thc morning for fieldwork.The questiorr which has bcen raised in this context is whethcr il. is possiblcto carry out good fieldwork among people one has this little respect for. .fheanswer is obviously yes; and as long as one does not molest one's hosts rnthe field, there can be no rules against negative attitudes on thc part of theanthropologist. At the end of the day, the value of participant observationlies in the quality of the empirical data one has collected, not in the numberof close friends one has acquired in the lield.common problems in fieldwork can be rimited l|nowledge of the Ilerdlanguage, gender bias (see chapter 9) or the lact that one's main informantsfail to be representative of the society as a whole, concerning thc latter point,it may be true that anthropologists have tended to pay too much attcntionto the elite of the community (although elites in complex societies are unocr-studied, partly because access to l.hem is dill'icult). Frequently chiefs, leachersand othcr untypical individuals are most elficient in offering their servicesto a visiting anthropologist, and the anthropologist may also unrivittingly beattracted to this kind of people because they resemble him- or herself, ceraldBerreman (1962) once wrote a conlcssionar piece on his own fielclexperiences in North India. He depended on an interpreter, and only after hehad been working, rather unsuccessfully, lor a while did it becoml evidenrthat the interpreter was a significant source o[distortion in his lleldwork: notbecause he lied or was inadequate as an interpreter, but because o[ hrsposition in the caste hierarchy. people did not talk as openll, to him as the1,

    would to somconc likc Rcrreman himselt, who was cnste-neutral b,v virtue ofbe ing foreign, or l-o a local r,r'ith a differenl caste meurbershipFieldwork does not have to be either capital-intensive or labour-intensive;as a rese arch process it is cheap, since the only scientific instruments involvedare the fieldu'orkers themselves and possibly a few assistants. However, andthis is perhaps the main point about fieldwork as a scientific mcthod. it rstime-intensive. Ideally, one should stay in the field long enough to bc ablc tosee the lt'orld as the locals see it. Even if this may be impossible, among otherreasons because one cannot entirely get rid of one's own culturalbackground, it can bc a n,orthwhile aim to purru.. The strength of theanthropologist's knoruledge can thus be said to lie in his or her mastcry ofboth the local culture and a different culture (his or her own), and oitools olanalysis, which makes it possible to give an analytical, comparative:rccountof both.

    The strength o[cthnographic field method can also be its weakness: it isdemanding, and rewarding, partly because the ethnographer invests notonly professional skills in it, but also interpersonal skills. The ethnographerdraws on his or her entire personality to a greater extent than any otherscientist. For this reason, many emerge from the field exhausted, but with irmaterial of extraordinary richness and depth. At the same time, this degreeof personal involvement has important ethical implications. Are friendships

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    28/29

    -Zs . Srrrall /']/rtr:r,s. Lnrcc l.ssticsirrd orhcr- coLrlidcntial lclal-ionslrips clcvclopcd in l-hc licld'rcal' or'Ial

  • 8/12/2019 Eriksen Comparison Fieldwork

    29/29

    lticlrlv,ork uul iLs lttLupt'cLr.tLiotr 29specilic hypo[hcscs Onc wou]d virl-Lrtrlly clrown in dctails withoLrt bcing ablclo present patterr)s and rcgulurities. l'hc dcr-cription ol socicty would bc ar^complex anci ambiguous as society itsel[ and there lore supcrlluous. Thcanthropological projcct consrsts, to a greaI cxletrl-, ol irr-rposing rtrdcringpatterns and rcgularitics ctn to Lhe obr^erved mal_erial, ancl wc dcpencl onour own theoretical abstmctions in order to do so. 1'he challcnge Iies insaying something signilicant about culture ancl social lile throush thcscabstractions.

    The choice oian precise lopic lor investigation, thereforc, is ern intportantpart ol the prcparation ior llcldr,,,orli. One shoulcl, al_ thc vcry least, l