Download - Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

Transcript
Page 1: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France During the 19th Century

GE. Berrios

Until the 1850s. obsessive-compulsive phenomena were considered to be a variant of the old

notion of insanity. Around this time they became a separate disease: first, as a member of the old class of the neuroses; then, briefly, as a variant of the newly formed notion of psychosis; and finally,

as a neurosis proper (in the post-1880s sense). These changes reflected theoretical shifts in the definition of the grand psychiatric categories. After 1860, organic causal hypotheses for OCD

included dysfunctions of the autonomic nervous system and cortical blood supply. Psychological hypotheses suggested the OCD might result from volitional, intellectual, or emotional impairment,

the last of which predominated after 1890. Issues relating OCD to personality types and hereditability were dealt with in terms of the degeneration theory. By the late 188Os, OCD achieved

full clinical and nosological definition. 0 1989 by W.8. Saunders Company.

0 BESSIVE COMPULSIVE DISORDER (OCD) includes interloping and iterative thoughts and/or actions that may interfere with or paralyze

behavior. OCD may be accompanied by declarations of distress, characterized by resistance, interference, and the presence of insight. In spite of revived interest in OCD,‘.’ its nosological status, natural history, and classification remain unclear.

Assuming that OCD-like behavior phenomena existed before the 19th century,* it is plausible to expect that words and concepts were marshalled to define them.9 If so, the history of the words (historical semantics) and concepts (conceptual history) require separate analysis. They were affected differently by the epistemological break that changed the psychiatric discourse during the early 19th century.”

The concepts involved in the current definition of OCD were tooled during the 19th century. This review focuses on the French psychiatrists responsible for its formation. German and British work is comparatively less important, and has been dealt with elsewhere.8~9*1’

HISTORY OF THE WORDS

Since the Medieval period, latin terms such as obsessio, compulsio, impulsio, and scrupulus were adopted by the European medical community to deal with OCD-like phenomena. A number of vernacular terms served a similar function in ordinary language.*

As part of the 19th-century drive for description, French psychiatrists coined the following noun and adjectival phrases to name clinical phenomena, which included OCD: manie sans dblire, I2 folie raisonnante, l3 monomanie raisonnante,14 kleptomanie, ” id&es fixes, I6 idt?e irresistible,” dilire aver conscience and d6lire sans dglire. ‘* id&es restrictives or mobiles,‘g pseudomonomanie,20 folie lucide,=’ folie or monomanie avec conscience, ” dilire de toucher,23 folie de doute,24 dklire

From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, England. Address reprint requests to G.E. Berries. M.A.. M.D.. F.R.C.Psych. Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Adden- brooke’s Hospital (Level 4), Hills Rd., Cambridge CB2 2QQ. U.K.

0 1989 by W.B. Saunders Company. 00i0-440X/89/3003-0002$03.00/0

Comprehensive Psychiatry, Vol. 30. No. 4 (July/August), 1989: pp 283-295 283

Page 2: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

284 G.E. BERRIOS

emotif,25 obsessions pathologiques,26 folie des htrkditaires dtg&?res,27 and crainte de souillure.2a

These categories also referred to clinical phenomena other than OCD. Since Pinel, manie sans dklire and folie raisonnante described forms of insanity unaccompanied by delusions (i.e., that escaped the conventional intellectual definition of insanity).29 Prichard’s concept of moral insanity was but an anglicized version of this category.30

The inclusion of OCD underfolie raisonnante proved unsuccessful. This category had no conceptual machinery to explain the presence of thoughts which, although persistent and disturbing, did not qualify as delusions (clinical observations demonstrated that some sort of insight seemed to be present).3’

After the 185Os, OCD was redefined within a new category, “folie avec conscience” (insanity with insight). The transfer was dealt with in a famous debate in the SociCtt Mtdico-Psychologique.32 By temporarily surrendering the lack- of-insight criterion (central to the concept of delusion), psychiatrists supporting this category allowed a number of disorders such as OCD, agoraphobia and panic disorder, and hypochondriacal and homicidal monomanias to be classified as insanities. Folie avec conscience disintegrated by the end of the century under the pressure of the advancing (and narrower) Kraepelinian view of psychosis33; by then, OCD was also given an alternative clinical and etiological interpretation.

HISTORY OF THE CONCEPT

The First Halfof the 19th Century: Esquirol(1772-1840)

Esquirol14 opened a new clinical space for OCD when he classified the disease of Mlle. F as a form of volitional monomania (dklire partiel). He defined this category as “involuntary, irresistible, and instinctive activity”; the patient is “chained to actions that neither his reason or emotion have originated, that his conscience rejects and his will cannot suppress.” Esquirol noticed that Mlle. F described her thoughts as irresistible and hence seemed to have insight. He concluded that such irresistibility reflected a weakness in the volitional faculty. Diseases of the will remained a popular subject in French psychiatry until the end of the century.34.35

The view that the OCD was a partial insanity or monomania did not last long. The concept of monomania, never very popular, declined in the 1850~.~~ Critics claimed the following faults (1) it had resulted from a mechanical application of Faculty Psychology3’; (2) it encompassed too many clinical states and hence had little or nothing to say about individual phenomenology3*; (3) it was based on cross-sectional observation and had no conceptual machinery to handle longitudinal symptom- change$‘; (4) it handled subjective symptomatology poorly which, since the 1840s had become an important aspect of the definition of mental disorder29; and (5) it created legal difficulties.@ The category was sounded its death knel14i at the 1853-1854 debate of the Socitte Medico-Psychologique in Paris. Its disappearance set the category of OCD asunder.

The Second Halfof the Century: Morel (1809-1873)

Morel” described OCD as a member of a category he called dklire emotif, and which he considered not as “an insanity but as a neurosis, that is, a disease of the emotions.“25 He wrote: “what I call ‘delire emotif’ corresponds to a particular type

Page 3: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE IQTH CENTURY 285

of fixed idea and abnormal act whose existence, however, does not entail an involvement of intellectual faculties.” For example, the drive leading to the compulsion resulted from a heightened affective state. Morel’s use of the word dkfire was unconventional in that it allowed for the presence of insight.42

Analysis of his seven cases confirms the broad nature of the category that also included vasomotor and digestive symptomatology, phobias, dysphoria, unmoti- vated fears, fixed ideas, and impulsions, as long as there was no cognitive impairment. Janet’s concept of psychasthenia43*” followed this grouping very closely.

Three reasons explain the success of d&Ike emot$ first, the towering reputation of Morel; second, it provided an alternative to the German view that OCD was a thinking disorder similar to paranoia4’; and third, it allowed for the classification of a variety of anxiety-equivalents.46 Luys4’ suggested that since ideas, emotions, and actions had separate cortical localization, nervous excitation might give rise to bizarre ideas, involuntary emotions, or compulsive acts. Luys disagreed with Morel’s view that all vegetative functions were localized in the ganglionar system.47

Reclassification of the OCDs as a form of neuroses reopened the possibility that they might result from nervous involvement of areas of the brain related to cognition, emotions, or volition. Once again, it must be remembered that in 1866 the word “neurosis” was only half-way through its long development.48

Morel’s view has been echoed in the history of psychiatry, particularly in the 1960s by Beech and Perigault,3 and later by DSM-III-R,49 where OCD is classified as an emotional disorder. However, neither of these efforts has solved the objection raised at the time against Morel’s view: why the so-called insanities with insight, in spite of their common origin, are so clinically dissimilar. The belief that there was no need for a generic name for such a heterogeneous group of disorders won the day and the concept of fulie cfvec conscience faded way.

Dagonet (1823-1902) and the Concept of Impulsion

While obsessions were defined vis-a-vis the concept of delusion, compulsions required separation from impulsions, a class of symptoms including all manner of paroxysmal, stereotyped, and (apparently) involuntary actions.

The explanatory force of the term “impulsion,” (imported from the science of Mechanics),” resulted from its analogical use that alluded to vestigial behaviors, drives, cravings, and appetites. This helped psychiatrists to explain apparently unmotivated seizural and episodic behavioral phenomena. In addition to OCD, impulsive insanity also included phobias, homicidal and suicidal tendencies, manic behavior, hypochondriacal preoccupations, and even epileptic seizures.‘i3’*

The concept of impulse was ambiguously poised in two different dimensions. In one, it was used both as a description and as an explanation; in the other, it was considered as either reactive or endogenous. From a historical viewpoint, the earliest view was that it was of inner origin.52q53

Versions of impulsive or instinctive insanity, based on Faculty Psychology,30 were in circulation in European psychiatry as early as 1810. According to Faculty Psychology, the will was a separate mental function, and hence susceptible to independent impairment. 3o Pinel” and Georgets4 included cases with impetuositi de penchans in this category of manie sans dglire or non delirante. Esquirol’s’4*55

Page 4: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

286 G.E. BERRIOS

notion of monomania56 brought the disorders of the will into the mainstream of psychiatry.

After the decline of the notion of monomania, the monomanic impulsive syndromes were included in the category of impulsive insanity, thus named by Marc& and Foville.52 Dagonet offered a clinical and taxonomic analysis and concluded that any patient suffering from insanity might display “impulsions violentes, irresistibles.” The latter could be primary or secondary to delusion, pathological affect, or hallucinations. They could be of short duration or chronic.52 They “resulted from a failure of the will.” Dagonet included in this category clinical phenomena in which “the more one tries to discard an idea, the more it becomes imposed upon the mind, the more one tries to get rid of an emotion or tendency, the more energetic it becomes.”

Legrand du Saulle (1830-1886)

Consolidation of the clinical description of the OCDs was achieved in the work of Legrand du Saulle, 57 who reported 27 cases (11 his own) in 1875. He recognized that OCD patients were admitted to the hospital only when severely ill or complicated by psychotic symptoms or depression. He was aware of OCD’s fluctuating course, insidious onset, and tendency to symptom-change with time. He classified OCD as one of the varieties of insanity with insight, reporting that it had early onset, occurred more frequently in women, affected individuals from higher social classes, and occurred more frequently in fastidious and rigid personalities.

A believer in longitudinal analysis, he identified three stages in OCD. In the first there were “involuntary, spontaneous and irresistible thoughts on various subjects without illusions or hallucinations”; with a “feeling of doubt, of brooding,” and occasionally, “mental representations and images” of the repetitive thoughts. These symptoms engendered fears and anxieties and led to the establishment of rituals.

In the second the condition was characterized by unexpected revelations made to relatives or friends of the presence of symptoms often kept secret for years. Depression, anxiety, agitation, and suicidal ideas might also appear together with suicidal brooding, but rarely led to self-harm. Patients could also develop animal phobias, somatic (e.g., vasomotor) symptoms, rituals, fear of touching objects, abnormal cleanliness, hand washing, and eccentric behaviour. Insight was not lost and doubt became less bothersome. Symptoms followed a fluctuating pattern with a course punctuated by attenuations and remissions.

The third stage included major compromise of psychosocial competence, rituals, and obsessional paralysis. On follow-up, (often longer than 20 years), some patients had become house-bound and maintained only a semblance of insight; they showed a typical “double book-keeping” type of behavior and examination revealed a darker, almost psychotic side. There was no evolution towards dementia.

Although his work transits the old and new concepts of insanity, some confusion may be caused by Legrand du Saulle’s quaint usage of the terms neuroses (in its pre-Morel meaning), and dtlire, (one of whose renditions is delusion), to refer to obsessions.42 Legrand du Saulle wrote this book 9 years after Morel’s paper,25 and he accepted the latter’s view that OCD was primarily a problem of emotions. But, his great clinical acumen, applied to the analysis of 27 cases (the largest series published up to that period), led him to identify forms of behavior that went beyond what was considered acceptable for the neuroses of his time. So, although paying lip

Page 5: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE 19TH CENTURY 287

service to the neurosis theory, the book suggests something that has since become an important issue, i.e., the presence of psychotic symptoms in OCD.

There is yet another point that may confuse the reader; it relates to his inclusion of animal phobias, agoraphobia, vasomotor phenomena and other somatic symp- toms, panic disorder, and probably some cases of partial epilepsy with complex seizures. With regard to this, remember that Legrand du Saulle was writing in 1875, that these clinical states had not yet been separated from the nosological viewpoint, and that during this period somatic symptoms were freely included as part of the definition of mental disease.

This attitude vis-a-vis somatic symptoms changed when Freud’s theories became available. Psychiatrists then felt unable to include somatic symptoms among the primary symptoms of psychosis (e.g., the vasomotor changes of catatonia), and either ignored them or regarded them as secondary and accidental. Those who have studied Janet’s work44 have realized that he maintained the boundaries for OCD exactly where Legrand du Saulle had drawn them 30 years earlier, i.e., including phobias and epileptic states in OCD.

Ball (1834-I 893)

Ba115s suggested eight operational criteria for OCD: insight; sudden onset (patients may remember the very day the disease started); paroxysmal nature (severity increases in certain periods, and winter is a period of calm); fluctuating course (there are long periods of remission); no cognitive impairment; release of tension after the compulsion has been carried out; high frequency of somatic and anxiety symptoms; and family history, although acquired obsessions are common.

Ball classified OCD according to the following states: minor (obsession only), moderate (additional presence of anxiety and major hesitations), and major (compulsions also). As precipitants he listed tiredness and fatigue, major life events, puberty, sexual problems, pregnancy, puerperium, and menopause. Ball’s view was that OCD resulted from impaired brain circulation, but strongly advised against the use of morphine. Ball’s criteria became popular.

Magnan (1835-I 916)

Magnanz7 classified mental disorders into organic, psychosis proper, and states of mental retardation. The second group he subdivided into mania, melancholia, chronic delusional state, intermittent psychosis, and psychosis of degeneration (&lie des d6g&ztr&s). OCD was included in the latter category together with phobias, agoraphobia, sexual perversions, and hypochondriacal states. OCD and its variants, onomatomanie (repeating obsession)59 and erythromanie Cfacilit6 ex- treme ii rougir, i.e., blushing),@ together with the other disorders, were assumed to result from cerebrospinal pathology. They were also assumed to “develop only in subjects affected by degeneration . . . and merit to be considered as the psychologi- cal stigmata of degeneration psychosis. . . .” Following the same model, Ameline61 suggested cerebral entropy as an explanation for the disorder.

Ladame13 summarized Magnan’s contribution well:

“The majority of authors, following Morel, have considered heredity as an important factor in obsessions. None of them had, however, considered the symptoms themselves as a direct sign of pathological heredity. This is what, with great success, has been done by Magnan. The various

Page 6: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

288 G.E. BERRIOS

clinical presentations of obsessions and impulsions are for this distinguished alienist episodic syndromes of the psychosis of degeneration. They are true psychological stigmata. . .”

ETIOLOGICAL DISPUTES

As mentioned previously, there were three psychological hypotheses that ac- counted for OCD. The emotive and volitional views were more popular in France while the intellectual view predominated in Germany,62 where Westpha163 sug- gested that Zwangworstelfungen (obsessional presentations) resulted from a pri- mary dysfunction of intellect.* However, his operational definition of obsessions differed from that of the French; he refused to give up the lack-of-insight criterion for delusions (Wahn).8 Instead, he asserted that obsessions were an ego-dystonic variant of fixed ideas. Westphal’s intellectual view received varying degrees of support from Tuke, Magnan, Cramer, Wernicke, Ziehen, and Darnbluth.

The emotive view remained popular among the French, and toward the end of the century began to spread. (It was supported by Aschaffenburg, Kraepelin, Freud, Hartenberg, Bleuler, and Stocker.)

The volitional theory, started by Esquirol and Billod, decreased in popularity during the third quarter of the century, only to undergo limited revival in the work of Ribot, Arnaud, and Legrain. Thus, Arnaud64 suggested that aboulia, a severe volitional deficit, was the primary impairment in OCD:

“Amongst bbsessives, whatever their severity, aboulia is always present and always precedes the disease; its permanence explains the obsessional state. . . emotional accompaniments, and ideas play an important role but are secondary in the pathogenesis of obsessions. . .”

He stated that the error of the intellectual view resulted from the belief that ideas were primary ingredients in any form of behavior.29 Legrain, a follower of the physiological views of LuYs,~~,~~.~’ also proposed a version of the volitional hypothesis.

On the other hand, Haskovec, 68 better known for his description of pre- neuroleptic akatisia, considered the emotional aspects of OCD to be secondary and argued in favor of a version of the intellectual view. He believed that the emotive view resulted from the analysis of samples that had been contaminated by phobic and anxious patients. He proved his point by analyzing a sample of OCD patients (n = 100) that contained such confusing symptoms,6’ concluding that

“sometimes the visual image of the phobic object triggers the obsessional idea, and this, in turn, heightens the emotion; the patient may, for example, see an axe, and this will trigger the obsession of killing a loved one.“68

Among psychiatrists writing in French, Haskovec was the exception. By the turn of the century, when Janet was collecting both cases and hypotheses to write his book L’Obsession et la Psychasthenie,43 practically everyone supported the emotive hypothesis.

CONSOLIDATION OF THE EMOTIVE VIEW

The emotive view proved popular not only in relation to OCD, but also to clinical states such as abulia69 which, during the first half of the century, had been considered as a form of volitional disorder. Anxiety-based explanations became acceptable not only because great men were espousing them, but because the second

Page 7: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE t 9TH CENTURY 289

half of the century was a period of revival for all the aspects of affectivity and emotion, and their relationship to the autonomic nervous system.2g*70

This shift was encouraged not only by the internal logic of research in psychology and psychiatry, but also by the fact that the non-emotive explanations were found lacking. In addition, there was the Zeitgeist factor: in France this was a period of heightened social anxiety. Zeldin71 suggested that anxiety increased

‘1 . in the sense that traditional supports of behaviour were weakened, that people were left facing a larger world and a vastly greater range of problems, with far less certainty as to how they should treat them, and often with sharper sensibilities.”

In turn, this seems to have put a great deal of clinical pressure on physicians, as the type of complaints brought to them fell well outside the range of traditional insanity concepts. Related explanatory concepts such as neurasthenia, psychasthenia, surmenage, and phosphate deficiency thus became very popular.

Therefore, research by psychologists (who were often philosophers, also) took place against a background of emotivitk and was encouraged by changing philosophical winds blowing in favor of the emotive view.72 Conditions in which affective symptoms featured prominently were a natural target for this change. From the methodological viewpoint, the following belief started during this period: The best way to learn about normal function was to study it during disease. Towering among researchers during this period were Luys, Ribot, F&t. and the other members of the School of Paris (Paulhan, Janet, and Binet).73

There soon developed a philosophical reaction against the materialism and intellectualism propounded by psychiatrists such as Baillarger and Morel. This reaction, in a country that, despite Pasteur and Bernard, never ceased to worry about its spiritual needs, was strong, and the emotive hypothesis rode high on this yearning. The reaction was led by Lemoine and Janet (1823-1899) in particular. It began in 1867 with the publication of the latter’s Le Cerveau et la Pens.5e.72 Janet, who greatly influenced the work of his nephew, Pierre,74 was the standard-bearer of the emotive tradition in French philosophical psychology, started by Maine de Biran,73 and kept alive by illustrious philosophers such as Royer-Collard (whose brother was a psychiatrist), Cousin, Jouffroy, and Ravaisson.74 In his popular Paris lectures,75 Janet insisted that feelings and emotions were at the center of psycholog- ical organization.

ISSUES OF CLASSIFICATION

The clinical phenomena covered by the French categories d6fire emotif and folie avec conscience were the same as those presently covered by the sections “Anxiety Disorders” (or “Anxiety and Phobic Neuroses”) and “Impulse Control Disorders Not Otherwise Specified” in DSM-III-R,4g except that they also included vasomo- tor phenomena (e.g., Reynaud’s disease). After the 186Os, these two categories absorbed most of the clinical states included under folie raisonnante, which was consequently narrowed down until it reached its final definition in the work of Serieux and Capgras.76*77

After 1900, the gradual teasing out of folie avec conscience into OCD, panic disorder, anxiety disorder, phobic anxiety, and vasomotor phenomena, took place in France under the influence of historical factors such as the narrowing and psychologization of the neuroses (a la Freud),48V78 and relentless clinical research.

Page 8: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

290 G.E. BERRIOS

The analysis of the process of disintegration of folie avec conscience is beyond the scope of this review, but can be followed in the work of Brissaud, Souques, Devaux, Logree, Guede, Claude, and Levy-Valensi.79-84

PSYCHOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

Ribot (1839-l 916). Fkrt. and Paulhan

Of the French psychologists of the second half of the 19th century, Thiodule Ribot is probably the best known and certainly the most influential.85386 With regard to OCD he wrote:

“The mental state called ‘insanity of doubt’ or ruminative mania is to the irresolute character, what abulia is to the apathetic character. It consists in hesitation over futile issues, and incapacity to make decisions. The hesitation affects the intellectual sphere and the patient faces endless self-questioning . . If ‘psychological rumination’ (as Legrand du Saulle states) was all that there is, we should have nothing else to say; but the perturbation of intellect translates itself into acts. The patient dares not do anything without taking endless precautions. If he writes a letter, he reads it many times, egged on by the fear of having forgotten or misspelt a word . . . This is a disease of the will resulting from weakness of character and accompanied by pathology of the intellect. In a way, it is to be expected that obsessional ideas will become translated into empty acts, not adapted to reality; in this process, however, individual factors play a role; and we have found a decrease in vitality (‘abaissement du ton vital’). Evidence for this factor is to be found in the nature of the diseases which may cause it (hereditary neuropathies, debilitating illnesses), and in the fainting that may follow efforts to overcome the problem etc. . . ‘.“35

In his La Pathologie des Emotions,*’ written under the influence of Ribot, Charles F&t (1852-1907) undertook the description of all clinical phenomena related to morbid affectivity, including OCD. His list was large, as he believed that “there existed no mental activity unaccompanied by sentiment of some kind.” However, he used the term “emotion” in a general sense to name all affective states.** His central claim was that, generally, intellectual and volitional symptoma- tology were secondary to primary morbid affect.

He offered a fourfold classification of clinical phenomena falling within the category of morbid emotivity, according to whether or not arousal and emotivity were diffuse or systematic (focused on a particular trigger or area), and whether or not the functions were increased or decreased.*’ In the first group (diffuse and increased), he included all general or free-floating anxiety states; in the second (diffuse and decreased), the stupors; in the third (systematic and increased), specific phobias and OCD; and in the fourth (systematic and decreased), sexual deficits. He reported a number of cases that demonstrated frequent overlapping of symptomatol- ogy. He parried the obvious criticism that his systematic and increased group was resurrecting the affective monomanias by claiming that patients in this category always present intellectual pathology, i.e., abnormal mental contents acting as triggers for the emotional response. This was not the case for the monomanias.

Frederic Paulhan’s main work includes an analysis of the concept of character” (on which Ribot based the chapter on the same topic in his book on emotions), and of the emotions.” On the latter, like Ribot, he espoused an epiphenomenic view and conceived of the emotions as psychophysiological phenomena subject to the laws of evolution. ” As Hoffding 91 s howed, Paulhan’s” hyp othesis that elementary feelings can be combined into composite ones, could also be used to account for the phenomenon of doubt. For example, when hope and fear are present in equal

Page 9: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE 19TH CENTURY 291

strength, he believed that “doubt results, whose central feature is anxious restlessness that may lead to rash decisions to stop the anxiety caused by the hestiation. . . .”

FCrC and Paulhan, with their enthusiastic support for the psychological and physiological analysis of the emotions, spoke a language that psychiatrists could understand. They made sure that the emotive view of OCD reigned supreme.

Pitres (I 848-l 928) and Rkgis (1855-l 918)

Pitres and RBgisp2 defined obsessions as

“a morbid syndrome characterised by the anxious experiencing of parasitical thoughts and feelings which force themselves upon the self and lead to a form of psychical dissociation whose final stage is a splitting of the conscious personality . . . we do not hesitate to consider emotions as the primitive and fundamental element of the obsession.”

They defended the emotive view at the Twelfth International Congress of Medicine in Moscow in 1897, but rejected the Freudian sexual hypothesis.

In their book, which deals with obsessions, impulsions, phobias, and panic disorder, Pitres and Regis rejected the view that panic disorder (&rose dbngoisse) was a separate entity; instead, they considered it as a syndrome that could become grafted upon OCD, anxiety disorder, or depression. They reported 110 cases (the largest series until then) in which 63% had clear emotional precipitants, and 80% had relatives suffering from some form of mental disorder. Age of onset was before 30 years, and the disease, more common in women, was divided in ideational and compulsive varieties.

The Aftermath: Janet (1859-1947)

Janet owes much to the work of his predecessors, including Pitres and RCgis. His contribution, as his coworker Raymondp3 once said,p4 must be considered to be more theoretical than clinical. For Janet, OCD resulted from a dislocation of function; they were an engourdissement of the mind that required no anatomical substratum.p5 Obsessions were but the experiential or subjective concomitant of a feeling of incompletion that resulted from a defect in the “function of the real.” This amounts to no more than a metaphorical redescription of how patients often respond in relation to their inability to bring tasks to completion. Janet carved out psychasthe- nia from the already inflated category of neurasthenia.p6~p7

Psychasthenia, itself an over-inclusive category, soon became the new giant of neuropathology. Pa In I’Automatisme Psychologique,” Janet considered obsession as an id&e _fixe which, together with hallucinations, constituted “simple and rudimentary forms of mental activity.“43 Psychasthenia was essentially an etiologi- cal term and hence could not have clear-cut descriptive clinical boundaries.‘00 Rather, it relied on theoretical mechanisms such as a reduction of psychological tension”’ and incompletion (inachLvement).74*‘02 Schwartzlo was right in referring to it as “a cluster of symptoms artificially demarcated to which the predominance of a ‘typical (causal) mechanism’ conferred a particular aspect.”

Indeed, analysis of the 236 cases reported by Janet shows that psychasthenia included, in addition to OCD, panic, phobic and tic disorders, hypochondriacal and confusional states, and some forms of epilepsy. Janet’s psychological approach firmly placed OCD in the territory of the neuroses, which also included at the time

Page 10: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

292 G.E. BERRIOS

neurasthenia, hysteria, and psychasthenia. 96-98 This was to be reinforced by Freud.‘04*‘05 (See references 10 and 103 for a history of the psychodynamic hypothesis with regard to OCD.)

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

In conclusion, this review has demonstrated the following: 1. Between the 1830s and 1900, OCD was successively classified as insanity

(monomania), neurosis (old definition), psychosis (new definition), and, finally, as a member of the newly formed class of neuroses.

2. The component symptoms of OCD also went through changes. Obsession had to be separated from delusion (&5&e), and both of them from their common ancestor, idiefixe. This process was completed by the 1870s. However, it must be remembered that the French word dklire means more than the English term “delusion.” Compulsion, in turn, had to be separated from the term “impulsion,” i.e., the name for a parent class constituted by all forms of paroxysmal, episodic, stereotyped, and irresistible behavior. This second process took longer to be completed.

3. Three theories accounted for the psychogenesis of OCD: volitional, emotive, and intellectual. The former two were more popular in France, and eventually the emotive hypothesis predominated. By considering OCD to be a form of pathological or distorted emotion, it was possible to retain it as a functional disorder, at the same time maintaining its relationship to the autonomic nervous system. Very impor- tantly, it was also possible to satisfy the ongoing fashion (which also influenced Freud) of interpreting most mental disorders as an impairment of emotional mechanisms.

4. The natural history of OCD, with its episodic course and varied symptomatol- ogy, was fully worked out before the turn of the century. Janet confirmed rather than created these findings.

5. The concept of degeneration played an important role in the formation of OCD. It provided a basis for the claim that a form of pathological personality might underlie OCD. It also provided a mechanism for the biological transmission of the disease. In the work of Magnan, OCD even became a psychological stigma of degeneration.

6. OCD, agoraphobia, panic disorder, and other anxiety disorders were traveling companions up to the end of the century. Their final teasing out was made more on theoretical (etiological) than clinical or epidemiological grounds.

7. The historical problem posed by OCD seems to have been that, although recognized since the 1830s as a sui generis cluster of symptoms, it did not elicit an imaginative response from psychiatrists; most tried to pigeonhole it into one of the ongoing clinical categories. This Occamian attitude might have encouraged great economy of thought, but has probably done little to increase the understanding of this fascinating and elusive condition.

REFERENCES

1. Pujol R, Savy A: Le Devenir de I’Obstdt. Paris, Masson, 1968 2. Monserrat-Esteve S, Costa JM, Ballds C (eds): Patologia Obsesiva. Mblaga, Graficasa, 197 1 3. Beech HR (ed): Obsessional States. London, Methuen, 1974 4. Nagera H: Obsessional Neuroses. New York, Aronson, 1976

Page 11: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE 19TH CENTURY 293

5. Rachman SJ, Hodgson RJ: Obsessions and Compulsions. New York, Prentice-Hall, 1980 6. Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello WE (eds): Obsessive Compulsive Disorders. Theory and

Management. Littleton, MA, PSG, 1986 7. Vallejo J (ed): Estados obsesivos. Barcelona, Spain, Salvat, 1987 8. Berrios GE: Obsessional disorders during the 19th century: Terminological and classificatory

issues, in Bynum WF, Porter R, Shepard M (eds): Anatomy of Madness, vol 1. London, Tavistock, 1985, pp 166-187

9. Berrios GE: Historia de 10s trastornos obsesivos, in Vallejo J (ed): Estados Obsesivos. Barcelona, Spain, Salvat, 1987, pp 1-16

10. Berrios GE: Henri EY: Jackson et les idles obsedantes. L’Evolution Psychiatr 12:687-699, 1977 11. Berrios GE: Descriptive psychopathology: Conceptual and historical aspects. Psycho1 Med

14:303-313, 1987 12. Pine1 Ph: Trait6 Medico-Philoosophique sur 1’Alienation Mentale (ed 2). Paris, Brosson, 1809 13. Ladame PL: La folie du Doute et le Delire du Toucher. Ann Med Psycho1 (Paris) 12:368-386,

1890 14. Esquirol JED: Des Maladies Mentales ConsiderCes Sous les Rapports Mtdical, Hygienique et

Medico-Legal (2 ~01s). Paris, Baillitre, 1838 15. Marc CCH: De la Folie, Consid6rte dans ses Rapports Avec les Questions Medico-Judiciaires, vol

1. Paris, Bailliere, 1840 16. Parchappe JBM: Symptomatologie de la folie. Ann Med Psycho1 (Paris) 2: l-20, 1850 17. Brierre de Boismont AJF: De l’etat des facultes dans les dtlires partiels ou monomanies. Ann Med

Psycho1 (Paris) 5:567-591, 1851 18. Guislain J: Lecons Orales sur les Phrtnopathies ou Trait6 Thtorique et Pratique des Maladies

Mentales (3 ~01s). Ghent, Hebbelynck, 1852 19. Renaudin LFE: Observations medico-legales sur la monomanie. Ann Med Psycho1 (Paris)

6~236-249, 1854 20. Delasiauve LJF: De la monomanie. J Med Ment 1:348-364.1861 21. Trelat U: La Folie Lucide. Paris, Delahaye, 1861 22. Ritti A: Folie avec conscience, in Dechambre A (ed): Dictionnaire Encyclopedique des Sciences

M6dicales (4th series, ~013). Paris, Asselin and Mason, 1879, pp 307-320 23. Ritti A: Folie de doute avec dClire de toucher, in Dechambre A (ad): Dictionnaire Encyclopedique

des Sciences Mtdicales, series 4, ~013. Paris, Asselin and Mason, 1879, pp 339-348 24. Mar& LV: Trait& Pratique des Maladies Mentales. Paris, Bailliire, 1962 25. Morel BA: Du d6lire emotif. Nevrose du systeme nerveux ganglionaire visceral. Arch G&n M6d

71385-402, 1866 26. Luys J: Des obsessions pathologiques dans leur rapports avec l’activit6 automatique des elements

nerveux. Endphale 3:20-6 1, 1883 27. Magnan V: Considerations g&&ales sur la folie (des htreditaires ou dCgtnCres). Prog Med

14:1089-1090 28. Eeden V: Les obsessions. Rev Hypnotism 6:5-14, 1892 29. Berrios GE: The psychopathology of affectivity: Conceptual and historical aspects. Psycho1 Med

15:745-758, 1985 30. Berrios GE: Historical background to abnormal psychology, in Miller E, Cooper PJ (eds): Adult

Abnormal Psychology. London, Churchill, 1988, pp 26-51 3 1. Socittt Medico-Psychologique, Paris (minutes): Debate on folie raisonnante. Ann Med Psycho1

241382-426, 1866 32. So&t6 Medico-Psychologique, Paris (minutes): Debate on folie avec conscience. Ann Med

Psycho1 33:434-435,1875 33. Berrios GE, Hauser R: The early development of Kraepelin’s ideas on classification: A conceptual

history. Psycho1 Med 18:813-821, 1988 34. Billod E: Maladies de la volonte. Ann Med Psycho1 10: 15-35, 1847 35. Ribot T: Les Maladies de la Volont6. Paris, Alcan, 1904 36. Winslow F: On monomania. J Psycho1 Med 9:501-521, 1856 37. Falret JP: Des Maladies Mentales et des Asiles d’Alienes. Lecons Cliniques et Considerations

Gtnbrales. Paris, Bail&e, 1864, pp 425-448 38. Kageyama J: Sur l’histoire de la monomanie. I’Evolution Psychiatr 49: 155- 162, 1984

Page 12: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

294 G.E. BERRIOS

39. Linas A: Monomanie, in Dechambre A (ed): Dictionnaire Encyclopedique des Sciences Mbdicales, series 2, ~015. Paris, Asselin and Mason, 187 1, pp 146- 195

40. de Saussure R: The influence of the concept of monomania on French medico-legal psychiatry (from 18251840). J Hist Med 1:365-397,1946

41. Socittt Mtdico-Psychologique, Paris (minutes): Debate on monomanie. Ann Med Psycho1 6:99-l 18, 1854

42. Berrios GE: Historical aspects of the psychoses: 19th century issues. Br Med Bull 43:484-498, 1987

43. Janet P: Les obsessions et la Psychasthenic. Paris, Alcan, 1903 44. Pitman RK: Pierre Janet on obsessive compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:226-232,

1987 45. Meyer A: The relation of emotional and intellectual functions in paranoia and in obsessions.

Psycho1 Bull 3:255-274, 1906 46. Doyen E: Quelques considerations sur les terreurs morbides et la dtlire emotif en general.

Enctphale 4:418-438,1885 47. Semelaigne R: Les Pionniers de la Psychiatric Francaise (Apr6s Pinel), vol 2. Paris, Bailhere,

1932 48. L6pez Pitiero JM: Historical Origins of the Concept of Neurosis. Cambridge, Cambridge, 1983 49. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (ed 3,

revised). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1987 50. Littre E: Dictionnaire de la Langue Francaise. Paris, Hachette, 1877 51. Baldwin JM: Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology. New York, MacMillan, 1901 52. Dagonet H: Des impulsions dans la folie et de la folie impulsive. Ann Med Psycho1 4:5-32, 1870 53. Porot A: Manuel alphabCtique de psychiatric. Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1975 54. Georget E: De la Folie. Paris, Crevot, 1820 55. Esquirol JED: Note sur la monomanie-homicide, in Holfbauer JC (ed): M6decine L6gale Relative

aux Ali6nCs et aux Sourds-Muets. Paris, Baillibre, 1827 56. Goldstein J: Console and classify. The French psychiatric profession in the nineteenth century.

Cambridge, Cambridge University, 1987 57. Legrand du Saulle H: La Folie du Doute (Avec Ddlire du Toucher). Paris, Delahaye, 1875 58. Ball B: Des obsessions en pathologie mentale. Ann Psychiatr Hypnolog 2: l- 15, 1892 59. Ballet G: Contribution a I’ttude de p&at mental des htr6ditaires dbg6ntrbs. Arch Gen Med

21:257-275,188l 60. Boucher L: Note sur une forme particuliere d’obsession chez une htrtditaire. Normandie Med

5:285-286,309-3 10, 1890 61. Ameline D: Considerations sur la Psycho-Physiologie des Obsessions et Impulsions Dtg6ntratives.

4th International Congress of Psychologie. Paris, Comptes Rendus, 1901, pp 572-578 62. Lugaro E: La psichiatria tedesca nella storia e nell’attualita. IV. La psicosi funzionali. Riv Patol

Nerv Ment 21:449-501.1916 63. Westphal K: Uber Zwangsvorstellungen. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkrankheiten 8:734-750,1877 64. Arnaud FL: Sur la theorie de l’obsession. Arch Neurol 13:257-270, 1902 65. Legrain M: De l’obsession et de l’impulsion en gtndral et de l’impulsion a boire en particulier

(dipsomanie). Ann Policlinique Paris 4:201-209,233-250,1894 66. Luys J: Le Cerveau. Paris, Baillibre, 1876 67. Luys J: Trait6 Clinique et Pratique des Maladies Mentales. Paris, Delahaye & Lecrosnier, 188 1 68. Haskovec L: Contribution a la connaissance des id&es obs6dantes. XIII Congrbs International de

Medicine. Section Psychiatric. Comptes Rendus, 1901, pp 121-131 69. Hartenberg P, Valentin P: Le role de l’emotion dans la pathogCnie et la thCrapeutique des

aboulies. Rev Psycho1 Clin Therapeut 1:15-20, 1898 70. Gardiner HM, Metcalf RC, Beebe-Center JG: Feeling and Emotion. A History of Theories. New

York, American Book Co, 1937 71. Zeldin T: France (1848-1945): Intellect, Taste and Anxiety, ~012. Oxford, Clarendon, 1977 72. Ravaisson F: La Philosophie en France au XIX Si&cle (ed 2). Paris, Hachette, 1885 73. Dwelshauvers G: La Psychologie Francaise Contemporaine. Paris, Alcan, 1920 74. Prevost CM: La Psycho-Philosophie de Pierre Janet. Paris, Payot, 1973 75. Moore FC: The Psychology of Maine de Biran. Oxford, Clarendon, 1970 76. Mueller FL: Histoire de la Psychologie de l’Antiquit6 a nos Jours. Paris, Payot, 1960

Page 13: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in ...pierrehenri.castel.free.fr/Annexes/Berrios_OCD in... · Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Its Conceptual History in France

OBSESSIONS IN THE 19TH CENTURY 295

77. Janet P, Seailles G: A History of the Problems of Philosophy, vol 1. London, MacMillan, 1902 78. Serieux P, Capgras J: Les Folies Raisonnantes. Le Dtlire d’Interpretation. Paris, Alcan, 1909 79. Pichot P: The diagnosis and classification of mental disorders in French-speaking countries:

Background, current views and comparison with other nomenclatures. Psycho1 Med 12:475-496,1982 80. Ldpez Pifiero JM, Morales Meseguer JM: Neurosis y Psicoterapia. Un estudio historico. Madrid,

Espasa-Calpe, 1970 81. Deschamps A: Les Maladies de I’Esprit et les Asthdnies. Paris, Alcan, 1919 82. Heckel F: Le N&rose d’Angoisse. Paris, Masson, 1917 83. Boutonier J: 1’Angoisse. Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1945 84. Errera P: Some historical aspects of the concept of phobia. Psychiatr Q 36:325-336, 1962 85. Reuchlin M: Histoire de la Psychologie. Paris, Presses Universitaires de France, 1957 86. Reuchlin M: The historical background for national trends in psychology: France. J Hist Behav

Sci 1:115-123,1965 87. F&t C: The Pathology of the Emotions. London, University Press, 1902 88. Dwelshauvers G: Trait6 de Psychologie. Paris, Payot, 1934 89. Ribot Th: The psychology of the Emotions. London, Walter Scott, 1897 90. Paulhan F: Les PhCnombnes Affectifs (translated by C.K. Odgen as The Laws of Feeling).

London, Kegan Paul, 1887 9 1. Hoffding H: Bosquejo de una psicologia basada en la experiencia (revised by S. Rubiano). Madrid,

Jorro, 1926 92. Pitres A, Regis E: Les Obsessions et les Impulsions. Paris, Octave Doin, 1902 93. Raymond F, Janet P: N&roses et Id&es Fixes, ~012. Paris, Alcan, 1908 94. Raymond F: N&roses et psycho-n&roses, in Marie A (ed): Traitt International de Psychologie

Pathologique. Paris, Alcan, 19 11, pp 1-59 95. Baruk H: La Psychiatric Francaise de Pine1 a nos Jours. Paris, Presses Universitaires de France,

1967 96. Chatel JC, Peel R: The concept of neurasthenia. Int J Psychiatry 9:36-49, 1971 97. Cobb IG: A Manual of Neurasthenia (Nervous Exhaustion). London, Bailliere, 1920 98. Dubois P: Les Psychonevroses. Paris, Masson, 1909 99. Janet P: L’automatisme psychologique. Paris, Alcan, 1889 100. Hesnard A: De Freud a Lacan. Barcelona, Spain, Ediciones Martinez Rota, 1976 101. Sjovall B: Psychology of Tension. An Analysis of Piere Janet’s Concept of ‘Tension Psy-

chologique’ Together With an Historical Aspect. Stockholm, Almquist, 1967 102. Prevost CM: Janet, Freud et la Psychologie Clinique. Paris, Paris Bibliotheque, 1973 103. Schwartz L: Les N&roses et la Psychologie Dynamique de Pierre Janet. Paris, Presses

Universitaires de France, 1955 104. Freud S: Obsessions et phobies. Rev Neurol3:33-38, 1895 105. Freud S: Notes Upon a Case of Obsessional Neurosis. Collected Papers, ~013. London, Hogarth,

1953, pp 296-356 106. Green A: Obsessions et Psycho&rose Obsessionnelle. Encyclopedic Medico-Chirurgicale, ~013.

Paris, Editions Techniques, 1965, pp 37370A”; 37370B’O; 3737OC”; 37370D”