Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

download Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

of 25

Transcript of Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    1/25

    A Glimpse into the Meditating Brain *

    Bryan Williams

    University of New Mexico

    Abstract: Meditation has served as a traditional Eastern technique to transform consciousness andgain higher insight by focusing attention and introspectively observing ones own mental processes.Some research now suggests that regularly practicing meditation may also benefit health and well-being by helping to calm the mind and body. With encouragement from the Dalai Lama, neuroscientists arecurrently studying the meditating brain in order to learn more about how it works, how it changes, andhow it can promote mind-body health. In this paper, a basic overview of the latest findings relating tothe possible brain correlates of meditation is presented, and the implications of these findings for healthand the psychological quest to better understand subjective conscious experience are discussed.

    * This paper is an extended version of an invited talk given at the Morning Star Center for SpiritualLiving, Norman, OK, June 7, 2009. My appreciation goes to Dylan Oaks for his help and support, and toGrant Lacquement for permission to cite his work and for lending useful resource material.

    1. Introduction

    In seeking to deal with the stress and demands that come with the challenges of everyday life, many people now find solace in the momentary respite often attained through the practice of meditation. According to a recent survey by the National Center for Health Statistics in Maryland (Barnes, Bloom, &Nahin, 2008), meditative practice among adults has significantly increased from 7.6% in 2002 to 9.4%in 2007, making it one of the most commonly used complementary and alternative therapies in theUnited States. It is currently estimated that there are about 10 million American meditators, andhundreds of millions around the world (Walsh & Shapiro, 2006, p. 227).

    Various forms of meditation are known to exist, but most forms can be generally grouped into one of two classes for the simple purpose of distinction (see Table 1 below). The first class is often calledconcentrative meditation, in which attention is focused on a particular object, image, word, sound, or bodily process, such as breathing. The second class is often called mindfulness meditation, whichinvolves expanding attention in a passive way to allow broader awareness of ones own mentalprocesses. In other words, one expands their inner awareness and introspectively observes theirthoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations. This inner focusing can help filter out extraneousdistractions that can potentially interrupt the exploration of higher mental states and the attempt togain insight. The boundaries between these two classes should not be considered fixed, however, assome forms of meditation can and do blend techniques from both classes (Goleman, 1988).

    Although they tend to differ from one another in terms of technique, philosophy, and outcome, recentresearch suggests that the various forms of meditation may share one important commonality: they appear to beneficial for health and well-being. For example, volunteers who practiced a simple mantra(word)-based concentrative meditation technique twice a day showed a significant reduction in stressand negative mood after three months (Lane, Seskevich, & Pieper, 2007). In addition, a practitioner of Transcendental Meditation (TM) once stated: "I often feel an increased calmness in tense situations where I work. Even my co-workers say they dont understand how I can be so calm. Its all due tomeditation" (Ferguson, 1975, p. 17). Other study findings suggest that Yoga and various forms of mindfulness meditation may provide supplemental benefit for the treatment of stress, mood, andanxiety symptoms (Arias et al., 2006; Chiesa & Serretti, 2009; Davidson et al., 2003; Grossman et al.,2004; Ivanovski & Malhi, 2007; Oman et al., 2008; Shapiro et al., 1998; Walsh & Shapiro, 2006). 1

    Table 1. The Two Classes of Meditation and Some of Their Various Sub-Forms

    Class /Form Brief Description

    Concentrative:

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    2/25

    Transcendental Meditation(TM)

    A 20- to 30-minute practice usually done twice daily, in whichthe meditator focuses attention on a specific word, image, or sound (called a mantra ) that is traditionally obtained from theSanskrit language. Originally derived from ancient Vedictradition by Maharishi Mahesh Yogi.

    Yoga Meditation Originally derived from ancient Hindu culture, various types exist(e.g., Tantric, Hatha, Kundalini, Qigong, Sahaja, Nidra,Samatha). Each type utilizes its own techniques in body posture(asana ), breath control ( pranayama) , focused image or ideaattention ( dharana ), and contemplation ( dhyana ) to move towarda goal of achieving the state of Samadhi , a union with the"Universal Self."

    Meditative Prayer A form of religious contemplation seen in Christianity, Judaism,and Islam, wherein a devout practitioner focuses their attentionon a certain phrase or prayer from a given religious text (e.g.,the Bible, the Koran) with the goal of opening themselves to,and attaining oneness with, a certain divine entity (e.g., God,Christ, Allah).

    Mindfulness:

    Zen Meditation (Zazen) Originally derives from the Mahayana school of Buddhism foundin Chinese, Japanese, and Korean culture. Following thephilosophy of Zen Buddhism, the practitioners goal is to enter Satori , a state of enlightenment in which they become fullyattuned to the reality both inside and outside their body, andthey gain the ability to ask the appropriate questions concerningthese realities. To gain the insight needed to understand theanswers, the practitioner practices meditating on a traditionalriddle or puzzle (known as a koan ).

    Vipassana ("Insight")Meditation

    A practice derived from the Theravada Buddhist tradition of Thaiand Burmese culture, wherein the practitioner passively

    observes their present thoughts and bodily sensations with thegoal of increasing equanimity, a state of passive acceptancethat relies on awareness of these thoughts and sensations.

    The potential benefits of meditative practice may stem in part from its ability to help calm the mind and body. Physiological monitoring of novice TM meditators has often revealed notable drops in their breathing rate, oxygen intake, and heartrate during meditation, along with a rise in their skinselectrical resistance. This suggests that, as they are meditating, these individuals gradually becomecalmer and experience a drop in body metabolism (Davidson, 1976; Ferguson, 1975; Travis & Wallace,1999; Wallace, 1970; Wallace & Benson, 1972; Wallace et al., 1971; West, 1979; Woolfolk, 1975). 2Studies of novice Yoga and Zen meditators have found similar drops in breathing rate and/or oxygenintake, while skin resistance either tends to rise or become more stable, again suggesting a calmingstate (Corby et al., 1978; Elson et al., 1977; West, 1979; Woolfolk, 1975). As we shall see in Section 4, thephysiological effects of practice may be more complex for advanced meditators.

    Another potential benefit of meditative practice is that it appears to be effective in improving onesattention. Cognitive studies find that novice mindfulness and TM meditators tend to perform betterthan non-meditators on attentional tasks, and they tend to be less affected by distracting stimuli (Chan& Wollacott, 2007; Moore & Malinowski, 2009; Tang et al., 2007). Meditators also tend to notice fast-moving stimuli that other people may miss (Lutz et al., 2008; Slagter et al., 2007).

    All of these findings naturally lead to a valuable question: What might be going on in the mind, and

    thus the brain, of a meditator to produce these behavioral effects? Recently, this question has intriguedthe minds of neuroscientists, psychologists, Buddhist scholars, and even the Dalai Lama. During aninvited address at the 2005 Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, His Holiness expressed aninterest in the matter and encouraged neuroscientists to further explore the meditating brain in orderto learn more about how it works, how it changes, and how it may lead to better therapies for mind-

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    3/25

    body health (Fields, 2006; Talan, 2006).

    There may be another valuable reason for studying the meditating brain. Some advanced meditatorshave spoken of entering what have been called "deep" or "higher" states of meditation, an experiencethat has also been described in Yogic lore and Buddhist spiritual tradition. During such states, many meditators can often have a profound experience of transcending the physical and mental boundaries of their own individual self. For example, a mindfulness meditator described his transcending experienceduring the deep state in the following manner:

    I am usually aware of the boundary of my body against the skin and you lose that sense in Dhyana...you become a kind of...field of energy, the boundaries of which are not clearly delineated (Gifford-May &Thompson, 1994, p. 124).

    A meditator practicing TM felt as though the center of his being was expanding outward in a physical way. According to him, the experience felt:

    ...literally as though my arms were extended and they extended to the reaches of the universe...whateverthat was...a kind of immeasurable distance...my head would feel incredibly expanded and huge...as if it were capable of being the size that a galaxy could fit into...and so that sense of being enormous and yetnot out of my body...but expanding out from there in all directions, infinitely (p. 125).

    During transcendence, some meditators may describe encountering a different sense of reality. One of them described this as a:

    ...field of awareness that is cosmic...there was no sense of limitation, there was just awareness...endless, boundless, oceanic (p. 126).

    A female meditator described her encounter in terms of a sense of a higher power:

    ...its like a place...its very , very powerful...it has an energy about it...that I dont have in my life...andsuddenly you find this...its like..."Holy schmoly! What have I stumbled on now? What is this energy?"(p. 127, italics in original)

    Some may also describe feeling intense bliss, rapture, and/or deep calmness throughout the deepmeditative state (pp. 128 129).

    Vipassana meditators sometimes relate experiences similar to those seen in deep states during intensetraining retreats, along with spontaneous body movements and perceived changes in their body image(Kornfield, 1979). Grant Lacquement (2008) states that meditative prayer can uncover a "largerawareness and connection" that is always present, as well as facilitate connection with a higher divinepresence. This seems akin to experiencing a higher sense of reality.

    These experiences that meditators report having during deep states suggest that they may be briefly experiencing an aspect of conscious awareness beyond that of their ordinary, everyday awareness. If that is the case, then what areas and functions of the brain might be associated with the deep meditativestate?

    This is another valuable question that has apparently sparked the interest of the Dalai Lama. Toencourage exploration into the issue, His Holiness has hosted several research conferences in order tofoster a dialogue between neuroscientists and Buddhist scholars, which may allow them to see wheretheir respective disciplines intersect when it comes to exploring the nature of the mind (Barinaga,2003; Knight, 2004).

    This paper provides an overview of what neuroscience has tentatively learned so far about themeditating brain, and briefly discusses what these lessons could mean for mind-body health and the broader psychological quest to better understand conscious experience. But before we can begin toexplore the meditating brain, it is imperative to briefly survey the territory that we will be venturinginto.

    2. The Brain: Mapping the Territory

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    4/25

    My mentor William G. Roll, a retired professor of psychology at the University of West Georgia, oncesuggested that exploring the brain is akin to the trip that Marco Polo took to China around 1275. Likethe world at that time, several parts of the brain are pretty well understood, while other parts stillharbor hidden, undiscovered valleys. A foray into this new territory requires a basic map that otherpeople can follow, so that they do not lose the trail along the way. A map suitable for our purposes isshown in Figure 1.

    Figure 1. A basic map of the human brain, with each of its major lobes and cortices indicated (see text for details).

    The adult brain weighs a little more than three pounds. At its base is the brainstem, a narrow stalk of tissue connected to the spinal cord that contains bundles of nerve cells, or neurons , which are vital forkeeping us alive and conscious. Blossoming out of the back of the brainstem is the cerebellum, aconvoluted mass of neural tissue that helps maintain body balance and muscle coordination. Perchedabove the brainstem and the cerebellum is the largest part of the brain, called the cerebrum. Its upperlayer of tissue, called the cerebral cortex, is comprised of 70 to 100 billion neurons (Schneider &Tarshis, 1995, p. 83). The cortex only 3 to 5 millimeters thick, but it takes up more than a third of the

    brains volume because it is folded like the shell of a walnut into a maze of hills and valleys.

    The cerebrum can be divided into four individual lobes (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital), eachof which is specialized to handle certain behaviors. The frontal lobe plays a prime role in our ability tomake decisions, plan our actions, and coordinate our movements. In connection with this, the rear of

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    5/25

    the frontal lobe contains the primary motor cortex, one of the central brain areas involved inmovement. When the motor cortex is damaged, a person can have difficulty making fine movements with their fingers and limbs (Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, Ch. 19).

    Directly behind the lower part of the frontal lobe is a cortical area that will be relevant to our discussionof certain forms of meditation. This area, which has a shape similar to that of a crescent moon, is knownas the anterior cingulate cortex. In addition to being involved in some forms of attention and complexthought processing, some research suggests the anterior cingulate cortex is also involved in regulating

    some features of the autonomic nervous system, including heart rate, breathing rate, and bloodpressure (Devinsky et al., 1995, pp. 287 288).

    The parietal lobe appears to play a role in both sense and spatial perception. Toward its front is thesomatosensory cortex, which receives and analyzes information relating to pain, pressure, touch, andtemperature from all parts of the body. When it is electrically stimulated, a person may feel a tinglingsensation in their skin, or they may suddenly have the feeling of being lightly touched.

    Toward the back of the parietal lobe is a small sub-region called the superior parietal lobule, which becomes active when people try to visually determine the location, depth, and trajectory of objects inphysical space (Cohen et al., 1996; Haxby et al., 1991; Sack et al., 2002). It can also be activated whenpeople shift their attention toward a particular point in space (Corbetta et al., 1995; Posner & Rothbart,1991). Given its role in spatial perception, the parietal lobe has been considered the "where" pathway in visual signal processing (Linden, 2007, pp. 86 89). This area may also be relevant in our discussion of certain forms of meditation.

    The inner reaches of the temporal lobe are the domain of the hippocampus and the amygdala, the twostructures at the heart of memory and emotion, respectively. The temporal lobe is also involved inhearing, as it contains the auditory cortex, the chief brain area for sound and speech processing.

    In the back of the brain is the occipital lobe, which contains the primary visual cortex, where signalsfrom the eyes are processed. Upon receiving electrical stimulation of their visual cortex, people havereported seeing a bright flash of light or even swirls of color. People with an impaired visual cortex can

    be "mentally blind," meaning that they are often unable to perceive objects in front of them, eventhough their eyes still respond to their presence (Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, pp. 228 238).

    2.1. Imaging the Brain

    Our ability to learn about the specialized abilities of the four lobes has greatly improved over the pastfew decades through advances in brain imaging technology. These advances have been immensely valuable for medicine and neuroscience because they allow us to electronically peer through the skulland glimpse the brain as it partakes of behavior. So far, three kinds of advanced technologies have beenused to study the meditating brain: functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emissiontomography (PET), and single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT).

    Unlike the purely static image of the standard MRI, fMRI provides ongoing impressions of working brain function in relation to behavior. To do this, many fMRI studies commonly use a technique knownas blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) hemodynamic response. As implied by its big name,BOLD measures the amount of oxygen in the brain as a result of cerebral blood flow. Based on the ideathat the brain regions which have the most oxygen-rich blood are the ones that are the most neurally active at the moment, BOLD measurements allow us to estimate the level of neural activity in aparticular brain region during a specific behavior. This in turn allows us to infer which regions may befunctionally associated with that behavior (Buxton, 2001, Ch. 16; Logothetis, 2008).

    In a PET scan, a person is intravenously injected with a liquid (called a tracer) that contains a weak radioactive isotope. As its name implies, the tracer traces a path through the brain as it courses throughthe bloodstream, gradually emitting positrons as the isotope decays. When they interact with othersubatomic particles, these positrons produce X-rays that can be detected, counted, and mapped using adigital scanner. Similar to fMRI, PET follows the idea that the brain areas with the highest amounts of blood are the most neurally active at the time (Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, pp. 123 126; Schneider &Tarshis, 1995, pp. 95 96).

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    6/25

    A SPECT scan proceeds in nearly the same way as a PET scan, except that instead of X-rays, the scannerdetects the individual photons that are gradually emitted by different kind of tracer. Through a reaction with the chemicals found in neural tissue, the tracer is briefly retained by the brain and thereby provides a snapshot of the brains metabolic activity (Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, p. 126; Warwick, 2004).

    2.2. Brain Waves

    In addition to imaging techniques, it is possible to study the meditating brain by monitoring itselectrical activity. The electrochemical activity of the billions of neurons found in the brain produces acontinuous stream of electric waves that are emitted from the surface of the cerebral cortex. Thesebrain waves , as they are called, can be observed and recorded using a device called anelectroencephalograph (EEG).

    To record a persons brain waves by EEG, small metal disks capable of conducting electricity (known aselectrodes) are attached to the scalp at various places around the head to detect the brain wavestraveling up from the underlying cortical surface. Brain waves are quite weak, registering only about athousandth of a volt, so they must be amplified by the EEG before being recorded as a series of jaggedlines on a moving paper chart.

    There are five types of brain waves that are distinguished by their frequency, measured in cycles persecond, or Hertz (Hz): Delta waves (1 3 Hz) have the slowest wave cycles, and commonly appear when we are in a deep sleep. Theta waves (4 7 Hz) can also be present during sleep, usually when we start tofeel drowsy and fall into a light sleep (Carlson, 1992, pp. 242 243). Alpha waves (8 12 Hz) aretypically present during a state of relaxed awareness, when our minds are not actively engaged in deepthought. Beta waves (13 29 Hz) appear when we are actively thinking, alert, and attentive (Schneider& Tarshis, 1995, pp. 412 413). Gamma waves (30 80 Hz) have the fastest wave cycles, and oftenarise when we are mentally integrating and processing complex sensory information (Desmedt &Tomberg, 1994; Joliot et al., 1994).

    3. Studies of the Meditating Brain

    With our survey of its territory complete, we shall now look at what goes on inside the brain during various forms of meditation, based on experimental findings. It should be kept in mind that, unlessnoted, the experiments described in this section were done with novice and intermediate meditators who only have practiced for a relatively short time (anywhere from six months to four years). Advancedmeditators with longer training histories seem to constitute a special case in terms of their physiology and depth of meditation, so we shall look take a closer look at them in Section 4.

    3.1. Transcendental Meditation

    As we saw in Section 1, several research findings suggest that people who practice TM canphysiologically experience a calming effect in their body while meditating. But what is happening in

    their brain during that time? Various EEG studies indicate that as they sit quietly with their eyes closedand focus on their mantra , many TM meditators show a steady pattern of alpha waves. A small numberof them may show a drop in wave frequency to the lower part of the alpha spectrum (8 to 9 Hz),followed by the brief appearance of a theta wave pattern (Banquet, 1973; Cahn & Polich, 2006;Davidson, 1976; Ferguson, 1975; Jevning et al., 1992; Stigsby et al., 1981; Wallace, 1970; West, 1980; Woolfolk, 1975). These patterns are often recorded from electrode sites located over the frontal lobesand near the brains midline (Wallace et al., 1971). This suggests that, as they meditate, the brain wavesof TM practitioners tend to gradually slow down and approach frequencies that are typically associated with low mental arousal, which would be consistent with a calming effect.

    In addition, some TM practitioners may show patterns of synchronized brain waves, a phenomenonoften known as EEG coherence (Ferguson, 1975, pp. 22 25; Jevning et al., 1992, p. 419; West, 1980,pp. 370 371). In order to better grasp the concept underlying this phenomenon, we might consider thefollowing illustrative example: Lets say that we simultaneously recorded the EEG activity from twodifferent regions of our brain, and then compared them afterward to see if they show any similarity.Most of the time, during our ordinary conscious state, we would find that these two EEGs are a mixed-up bunch of waves scattered across several different frequencies, with little to no similarity at all.

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    7/25

    However, if we repeated the process with a TM practitioner while he or she was in a deep state of meditation, we might find that their two EEGs show a fair degree of similarity, with the two wavepatterns appearing to be in close alignment with each other (Figure 2). As we shall see in Section 4, thisphenomenon tends to be more common among advanced meditators.

    Figure 2. A comparison of the EEGs of a non-meditator (top) and a TM meditator (bottom). While the brain wavesof the non-meditator are largely scattered across different frequencies during the ordinary waking state, those of the

    meditator show a consistent pattern of synchronization around 20 Hz during a deep meditative state (Ferguson,1975).

    At least one imaging study has been done to further explore the areas of the brain that may be activeduring TM (Jevning et al., 1996). Changes in cerebral blood flow were monitored in 34 meditators asthey focused on their mantra . Compared to control volunteers who merely sat and relaxed, themeditators showed increased flow in their frontal lobes, a finding consistent with the EEG patternsrecorded in that same region (Wallace et al., 1971). The meditators also showed increased flow in theiroccipital lobes, which would be in line with the act of visualizing their mantra .

    3.2. Yoga Meditation

    Several types of Yoga meditation are known to exist, and to date, EEG and imaging studies haveexamined the brains activities during five types: Tantric, Kundalini, Sahaja, Nidra, and Iyengar.

    Similar to TM, Tantric Yoga is marked by the attentional focus on a specific mantra , with the goal of attaining unity with it. In two studies (Corby et al., 1978; Elson et al., 1977), brain wave activity wasmonitored in Tantric meditators as they sat in the lotus position and focused on the sound of a two-syllable Sanskrit word mantra . Compared to relaxing control volunteers, meditators with an average of 1.5 to 2 years of training had produced higher amounts of alpha and theta activity along the brainscentral midline. While many of the volunteers became relaxed to the point where they would fall asleep,the steady alpha and theta patterns seen on the meditators EEGs suggests that they were able to enterand maintain a mental state close to the boundary of wakefulness and sleep, yet still remain awake.

    Kundalini Yoga can also involve focusing on a mantra while passively observing ones breathing. In oneimaging study (Lazar et al., 2000), meditators with four years of Kundalini training underwent fMRIscanning while they silently repeated a two-phrase mantra in time with their breaths. During a control

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    8/25

    period, the meditators did not observe their breaths and turned their attention away from the mantra by silently thinking up a list of animal names. Compared to this control period, more neural activity wasseen during the meditation in the anterior cingulate cortex. In the late stages of the meditation, thefrontal, parietal, and temporal lobes became active. This suggests that brain regions associated withattention and control of autonomic nervous system are involved in this Yoga type.

    Rather than mantra focusing, Sahaja Yoga emphasizes focusing ones attention inward on internalprocesses and suppressing all other extraneous thoughts, which is meant to help open the way to the

    experience of an internally "blissful" state. In two studies (Aftanas & Golocheikine, 2001, 2003), EEGs were recorded from both novice (less than six months of training) and advanced (3 to 7 years) Sahajameditators as they attempted inward focus to reach the blissful state. Compared to the novices, theadvanced meditators showed much more theta activity over their frontal lobes and the midline of their brains, and they reported more intense feelings of bliss. In contrast, the novices showed more alpha waves over their occipital lobes and the rear part of their parietal lobes (Figure 3). This suggests that theachievement of slower brain waves may partly be a function of meditation training history, a possibility we shall examine further in Section 4.

    Figure 3. A comparison of the brain wave activity of novice (STM, left column) and advanced (LTM, right column)Sahaja Yoga meditators, looking down from the top of the head. Whereas novices show more alpha in the occipitaland rear parietal lobes (lower left), advanced meditators show more theta in central region of the frontal lobes (top

    and center right) (Aftanas & Golocheikine, 2003, 2005).

    Instead of focusing their attention, Yoga Nidra meditators adopt a more neutral technique, whereinthey "withdraw" from the desire to act and passively observe the bodily sensations and visual imagesthat arise in their mind. In two imaging studies (Kjaer et al., 2002; Lou et al., 1999), meditators whohad more than 5 years of Yoga Nidra training underwent PET scanning as they received a guidedmeditation. As they were guided to imagine the sensation of weight on various parts of their body,several areas including the frontal lobe and the anterior cingulate cortex were activated in the

    meditators brains. Then, when guided to visualize a serene rural landscape in summer, visual regionsin the occipital lobe became active. Finally, as they attempted to generate a mental representation of their self, areas surrounding the superior parietal lobule lit up with activity. Monitoring of themeditators brain waves further revealed increases in theta activity during the meditation, and the PETscans indicated a higher release of dopamine, a neurochemical sometimes associated with feelings of pleasure (Schneider & Tarshis, 1995, p. 154). In line with the ideas of Yoga Nidra, several of the

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    9/25

    meditators reported a reduction in the conscious control of their attention, as well as a "loss of will."This was counteracted by an experience of intense sensory imagery.

    Iyengar Yoga combines meditation with the breathing and body posture exercises that are commonly associated with the term "yoga." In a study using SPECT (Cohen et al., 2009), the brains of four people were examined before and after they received a 12-week Iyengar training program in order to see how they might change. Compared to before their training, the four individuals showed higher blood flow changes in their frontal lobes after the 12 weeks of training. As we shall see in Section 4, a change in the

    brain as a result of meditation training is one of the things that may distinguish advanced meditatorsfrom novices.

    3.3. Meditative Prayer

    Various forms of meditative prayer can be seen across several different religions, although it is theChristian-based form that has been the focus of two recent imaging studies.

    In the first study (Azari et al., 2001), 6 religious school teachers from a German Evangelist community received a PET scan while they attempted to briefly enter a religious meditative state by reciting thefirst verse of Psalm 23 in the Bible. Compared to 6 non-religious control volunteers, the teachersshowed higher activation in two areas of the frontal lobe associated with attention and the reflexiveevaluation of thought (Figure 4).

    Figure 4. Averaged PET scan results from 6 Evangelist teachers engaged in meditative prayer, indicating increasedcerebral blood flow in the forward and central regions of the frontal lobe (Azari et al., 2001).

    In the second study (Newberg et al., 2003), SPECT images were obtained from three Franciscan nuns who had more than 15 years of practice with "centering prayer," in which they continually focus theirattention on a prayer or a phrase from the Bible, which is meant to help them achieve the experience of "opening themselves to being in the presence of God" (p. 626). During such a profound experience, thenuns sometimes describe having "a loss of the usual sense of space" (p. 626). Compared to a restingstate, the nuns brains showed higher amounts of cerebral blood flow in various regions of the frontallobe. In addition, an inverse relationship was observed between the blood flow in the frontal lobe andthe blood flow in the superior parietal lobule, such that as the flow in one increased, the flow in theother decreased (and vice-versa). Given its apparent role to spatial perception (Section 2), the bloodflow changes in the superior parietal lobule may be related to the nuns experience of losing a usualsense of space.

    3.4. Zen Meditation (Zazen)

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    10/25

    To explore the brain physiology of mindfulness meditation, several studies have directly focused on oneof its sub-forms: Zazen, the sitting meditation of Zen Buddhism. During the practice of Zazen, themeditator sits cross-legged on a round cushion with their hands enclosed. Keeping their eyes open, themeditator casts their gaze downward to look about one meter ahead as they centrally ponder a koan .3The aim is not necessarily to produce an answer to the koan , but rather to gain the focus necessary toachieve the enlightened state of Satori (see Table 1). Occasionally, a meditator may go through anintensive training period known as Sesshin , in which they practice Zazen 8 to 10 times a day forapproximately one week.

    In one of the earliest studies (Kasamatsu & Hirai, 1966), the brain wave activity of 16 Zen priests and 32of their disciples was recorded during a period of Sesshin at a traditional Zen Buddhist training hall. While meditating, novice disciples with 1 to 5 years of Zazen training were found to produce a steady pattern of alpha waves, even with their eyes open. 4 More intermediate disciples with 5 to 20 years of training had a tendency to exhibit a slowing of their brain waves, as indicated by drops in alpha wavefrequency. (We shall look at the priests in Section 4.)

    Three recent EEG studies of Zazen have been geared toward examining Su-soku , a training techniqueusually given to Zen Buddhist initiates to help them adapt to the practice of Zazen (Kubota et al., 2001;Murata et al., 2004; Takahashi et al., 2005). During Su-soku , the initiate focuses all of their attention

    on their breathing, usually by counting each of their breaths as they inhale and exhale at a steady rate. 5To see how this practice of this technique might affect the brain, Japanese researchers briefly instructedthree groups of college student volunteers in Su-soku , and then recorded their brain waves while they performed the technique. The students EEGs indicated that theta and some alpha activity was presentin the mid-region of their frontal lobes while practicing Su-soku (Figure 5), and that this activity wasassociated with changes in their heart rhythms, indicating a possible link to the nervous system changesthat are sometimes seen during meditation (Section 1). An imaging study using fMRI also revealedactivation of the frontal lobes in 11 Zazen meditators who engaged in Su-soku during the scanningsession (Ritskes et al., 2003), further indicating the involvement of the frontal region in this technique.

    Figure 5. EEG activity in student volunteers practicing Su-soku , showing theta activity, mixed in with some alpha,in the mid-region of their frontal lobes (upper right) (Kubota et al., 2001).

    3.5. Vipassana ("Insight") Meditation

    So far, only a few studies have examined another sub-form of mindfulness meditation known asvipassana ("insight"), in which the meditator initially attends to their present thoughts or to an internal bodily process (usually their breathing), and then gradually broadens their attention outward to become passively aware of the range of internal and external stimuli present in their surroundings. In asense, the meditator lets their attention freely wander about and observe various objects or processes, with the goal of gaining equanimity (see Table 1) and clarity in their awareness.

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    11/25

    As a way to compare mindfulness with concentration meditation, psychologist Bruce Dunn and hisassociates at the University of West Florida had taught a group of 10 college students how to meditateusing a concentrative technique (focusing on their breaths, similar to TM) and a mindfulness techniquethat closely resembles vipassana . After a little over a month of practice with each technique, thestudents were asked to meditate using each technique while their EEGs were recorded. Compared toconcentrative meditation, the vipassana -like mindfulness meditation was associated with more brain wave activity in the delta, theta, alpha, and beta frequencies. The theta activity was localized primarily to the frontal lobes, while the delta, alpha, and beta activity was spread out more across the frontal,

    temporal, and parietal lobes. Curiously, many of these wave patterns appeared simultaneously in theirrespective brain regions. Dunn and his associates suggest that this may be consistent with the idea of meditation of being a state of "relaxed awareness": slow brain waves (e.g., theta) appearing in the frontof the brain may contribute to the meditators calming of their mind, while faster brain waves (e.g.,alpha, beta) occurring in the back of the brain may at the same time keep the meditator alert and awareof their surroundings (Dunn et al., 1999).

    To further explore the brain regions active during vipassana , German neuroscientist Dieter Vaitl andhis associates at Justus-Liebig University had recruited 30 meditators with an average of 8 years of daily vipassana training and asked them to meditate on the breathing sensation in their nose whileundergoing an fMRI scan. Compared to a control group of non-meditators, these advanced meditators

    showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex and the upper middle part of their frontal lobes(Hlzel et al., 2007).

    Figure 6. An MRI comparison of the averaged brain activity of advanced meditators and non-meditators.Compared to the latter, the former showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (large yellow area) and the

    upper part of the frontal lobes (small yellow area) (Hlzel et al., 2007).

    4. Studies with Advanced Meditators

    Given their long (five years or more) and often intense history of training, one might think thatadvanced meditators could be particularly revealing about what goes on inside the brain during thepractice of meditation for two reasons. First, close examination of their brain structure and functionmight be able to tell us something about the long-term effects of such practice. Second, personalaccounts of experiencing a "deep" or "higher" meditative state traditionally come from more advanced

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    12/25

    meditators (Section 1), and thus, they might offer some insight into the possible brain correlates of suchstates.

    4.1. Tibetan Buddhists

    One population of advanced meditators that could make valuable contributions to the study of themeditating brain is that of Tibetan Buddhist monks who have devoted a good part of their lives to thepractice of meditation as part of their spiritual lifestyle. Often times this has been difficult, since many of the most accomplished monks have led reclusive lives in isolated Southeast Asian monasteries, butrecently it has been possible to obtain the cooperation of several well-trained monks through thegracious assistance of His Holiness, the Dalai Lama (Barinaga, 2003; Knight, 2004). Some of the mostinteresting research with Buddhist monks so far has been conducted by psychologist Richard Davidsonand radiologist Andrew Newberg.

    Davidson and his colleagues at the University of Wisconsin have performed two studies in which they were able to examine the brain waves of two groups of long-practicing (6 years or more) monks duringtwo separate forms of meditation. In one study (Lutz et al., 2004), eight monks had shown stronggamma wave patterns and signs of EEG coherence (Section 3.1) across their frontal and parietal lobes while engaging in a Tibetan meditative technique meant to produce an inner state of "benevolence andcompassion" toward living beings.

    In the other study (Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2007), 14 monks focused their attention completely on asmall dot on a computer screen during their practice of the "one-pointed concentration" meditation. While doing so, fMRI scans of their brains indicated several regions in their frontal and parietal lobesthat became activated, all of which are thought to be involved in attention. In addition, the findingssuggest that the more years of practice that the monks had, the less activation they showed. This in turnmay suggest that the more practiced the monks are in focusing their attention during meditation, theless mental (and thus, brain) effort they have to exert in order to achieve a focused meditative state.Their continual practice may help them develop the ability to slip into such a state quickly and easily.

    Newberg and his associates at the University of Pennsylvania Medical Center obtained SPECT scans

    from eight Tibetan Buddhists, each with more than 15 years of training, as they quietly focused on amental image with gradually increasing intensity, with the goal of attaining "a sense of absorption intothe visualized image associated with clarity of thought and a loss of the usual sense of space and time"(Newberg et al., 2001, p. 114). During their focus, increased blood flow was seen in their frontal lobesand the anterior cingulate cortex. In addition, an inverse relationship was found between the amount of blood flow in the frontal lobes and the amount in the superior parietal lobule such that as the flow inone region increased, the flow in the other decreased (and vice-versa) (Figure 7). These findings arenotably similar to those that Newberg and his associates obtained in a separate SPECT study of Franciscan nuns practicing meditative prayer (Newberg et al., 2003; see also Section 3.3).

    Figure 7. Averaged SPECT results from eight Tibetan Buddhists engaged in focused meditation. Compared to aresting baseline, increased blood flow was observed in the frontal lobes (left image), while a decrease was seen in the

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    13/25

    parietal lobes (right image) (Newberg et al., 2001).

    Neuroscientist Dietrich Lehmann and his colleagues at the University Hospital of Zurich, Switzerland,had the opportunity to record the EEG of a Buddhist Lama who was able to voluntarily self-induce fivemeditative states, each of which he reported as a separate and profound experience. In the first twostates, the Lama focused on a mental image of the Buddha appearing either in front of, or just above,him. During his visualization, a pattern of gamma waves appeared over areas in his occipital andparietal lobes that are involved in producing mental images. In the third state, the Lama concentratedon a mantra composed of 100 syllables by verbally reciting a list of words containing that many syllables. While he was reciting, gamma waves appeared over areas in his frontal and temporal lobesthat have a role in speech. In the last two states, the Lama imagined his self transcending into a"boundless unity," or "emptiness," and then returning to his body. Once more, a pattern of gamma waves appeared, this time over regions in his frontal and parietal lobes that may be involved in self-perception and identification (Lehmann et al., 2001).

    4.2. Transcendental Meditators

    A second population that could make valuable contributions is that of advanced TranscendentalMeditators who have closely studied under Maharishi Mahesh Yogi or those teachers that he hadpersonally trained and qualified. Generally, EEG studies of advanced TM meditators have shown atrend towards slower brain waves in the alpha and theta range, similar to that seen in novice andintermediate meditators (Banquet, 1973; Cahn & Polich, 2006; Ferguson, 1975; Jevning et al., 1992; West, 1979, 1980; Woolfolk, 1975; see also Section 3.1), although in some cases this trend seems to have been marked by unique characteristics. For instance, French psychiatrist Jean-Paul Banquet (1973)noticed that as the brain wave patterns of 10 TM meditators slowed from alpha to theta, with the lattersometimes appearing as small "bursts" or "spikes" on the EEG record. These "theta bursts" were alsolater observed in the EEGs of 21 (27%) of the 78 long-term TM meditators studied by Swissneuroscientists R. Hebert and D. Lehmann (1977) (Figure 8). These bursts occurred about every twominutes during the meditation, and, according to the meditators, they seem to be associated withpeaceful and pleasant inner feelings of "drifting" or "sliding" (p. 401).

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    14/25

    Figure 8. EEG activity recorded from an advanced Transcendental Meditator, showing brief "bursts" or "spikes" of theta activity mixed with in with alpha (Hebert & Lehmann, 1977).

    Banquet (1973) further noticed that four TM meditators who reported achieving the deep meditativestate of "transcendence" had suddenly shown a much faster 20 Hz beta wave pattern on their EEG.Similar beta wave patterns have apparently been observed in a few other meditators who have entereddeep meditative states, as well (West, 1980). On a separate note, Canadian neuroscientist MichaelPersinger (1984) found that when a female TM meditator with 10 years of training had reportedexperiencing a meaning state of transcendence in which "she had felt being very close to the cosmic whole" (p. 129), she had shown a brief "spike" pattern of very slow delta waves in her temporal lobe.

    EEG coherence has also been noted in some advanced TM meditators. As briefly mentioned in Section3.1, brain wave synchronization can be exhibited by novice and intermediate meditators, which in the

    early stages of TM can be in the form of synchronized alpha and theta waves occurring across variousregions of the brain (Jevning et al., 1992, p. 419). In deep meditative states, a steady pattern of synchronized beta waves has been observed in early studies of advanced meditators, as well (Ferguson,1975, pp. 22 25; see Figure 2). A more recent analysis of the EEGs of 16 meditators with nearly 10 years of TM training further revealed signs of alpha wave coherence occurring over their frontal lobesduring meditation (Travis & Wallace, 1999).

    The alpha activity occurring over the frontal lobes is consistent with the findings of the only imagingstudy done to date with TM meditators (Jevning et al., 1996). Compared to a group of non-meditatingcontrol volunteers, 18 meditators with 8 to 12 years of daily TM practice showed higher rates of cerebral blood flow over their frontal and occipital lobes.

    4.3. Traditional Yogis

    Traditional Indian Yogis comprise a third population of advanced meditators who may contribute valuable data on the meditating brain. As with Tibetan Buddhist monks, opportunities to locateaccomplished Yogis who would be willing to participate in research have been limited because many of

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    15/25

    them have resided in mountainous retreats and other secluded, out-of-the-way places. One of theearliest came in 1957, when UCLA researchers Basu Bagchi and Marion Wenger had traveled to India inorder to conduct explore the physiological aspects of Yogic meditation and exercise. Their journey took them to three Indian laboratories, two hermitages, and a cave dwelling high up in the Himalayas. Usinga portable transistor polygraph, they were able to measure changes in the autonomic nervous system of five older Yogis as they sat perfectly still in the lotus position and meditated. Compared to a group of yoga students, the Yogis showed faster heart rates, higher blood pressure and skin conductance in theirpalms, and lower finger temperatures while meditating (Wenger & Bagchi, 1961, p. 315). Their breaths

    were also noted to become very slow and shallow, to the point where they would sometimes not becountable. The EEG recordings tended to show a steady alpha wave pattern throughout the meditationperiod, with little sign of any other change (Bagchi & Wenger, 1957). None of the Yogis reportedreaching a deep meditative state during the monitoring sessions, however.

    Indian physiologist B. K. Anand and his colleagues were able to independently follow-up on the work of Bagchi and Wenger a few years later in a study of four Yogis (Anand et al., 1961). Each of the Yogis wasproficient in Raj Yoga, another type of Yoga meditation in which they reportedly become oblivious toany internal and external distractions in their surrounding environment while in the deeper state of mahanand ("ecstasy"). To explore this, Anand and his colleagues monitored the EEGs of the meditating Yogis while trying to externally stimulate them by flashing a bright light, making loud bangs, and lightly

    touching them with a hot glass tube. While in their ordinary waking state, each of the Yogis clearly responded to each of the stimulations. However, while meditating, they showed no response, eitherovertly or on their EEG. Consistent with Bagchi and Wengers (1957) finding, the four Yogis eachshowed a steady pattern of alpha waves, which was not interrupted by any of these stimulations.

    Anand and his colleagues also studied two Yogis who had apparently developed a high tolerance forpain during meditation. To explore this, the researchers placed the Yogis hands in near-freezing (4C) water while they were meditating, and the Yogis were indeed able to keep their hands immersed fornearly an hour without experiencing any signs of discomfort. As with the other four Yogis, steady alphaactivity was observed on their EEGs throughout the immersion period. No EEG signals were seen intheir parietal lobes, which handle sensory information coming from the body, suggesting that they indeed were showing no signs of a response to the ice-cold water (Anand et al., 1961).

    In the 1980s, a field study was made of a group of Buddhist monks who practice an advanced type of Yoga meditation known as g Tum-mo, which ostensibly allows the meditator to produce notable rises intheir body temperature, even in the cold climate of the Himalayan foothills. 6 As a way to informally demonstrate the heating effect of g Tum-mo, the bare bodies of the monks are each wrapped in a sheetthat has been soaked in ice-cold water, and their goal is to fully dry the sheet using only the heat that isgenerated within their body during the meditation. Within 3 to 5 minutes of beginning their meditation,the monks have reportedly been able to heat the sheets enough to make them give off steam, and afterabout 45 minutes, the sheets were completely dried. The monks reportedly do not shiver throughoutthe process despite the exposure to the wet sheets and often cold atmosphere of the mountainousmonastery.

    With help from the Dalai Lama, Harvard psychiatrist Herbert Benson and his associates were able to visit three Buddhist monks in India who had practiced g Tum-mo for more than 6 years to conduct amore formal exploration of the heating phenomenon. As the monks meditated, Benson and hisassociates measured the skin temperature at various points on their bodies. These measurementsindicated that the monks were able to increase the temperature in their fingers and toes by as much as8C while meditating, even while still maintaining a fairly normal heart rate (Benson et al., 1982). EEGmeasurements that Benson and his associates collected during a follow-up study of three other monksshowed increases in beta wave activity during g Tum-mo meditation (Benson et al., 1990).

    Aside from field work, laboratory studies of advanced Yoga meditators have provided useful insight. Inaddition to novice and intermediate meditators, the two studies of Tantric Yoga summarized in Section3.2 also examined the brain wave activity of a small number of advanced Yoga meditators. In the firststudy (Corby et al., 1977), EEG data were collected from a Yogic monk and teacher who had received just over two years of special advanced training. Whereas a majority of the novice meditators spent between 4 and 32% of their time in a meditative state that was marked by the presence of theta waves,

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    16/25

    the monk had spent 86% of his time in this state, suggesting that he was more adept at producing theseslower brain waves. This theta pattern persisted on the monks EEG even after he had stoppedmeditating and had opened his eyes, something not seen in any of the novice meditators.

    Similarly, the advanced meditators participating in the second study had shown higher amounts of theta activity than intermediate meditators (Corby et al., 1978). All of them had visited India to meetthe leader of their spiritual organization, and had received the most advanced set of meditationtechniques. While focusing on a mantra , one of these advanced meditators had reportedly experienced

    a "near- Samadhi " state in which she had the feeling of "having my breathing taken over by the mantra "(p. 574). During that experience, her breathing, heart rate, and electrical skin resistance had sharply increased. While no clear EEG changes occurred during her experience, her meditation period wasmarked by alpha activity and large amounts of theta waves that occasionally came in small "bursts,"similar to that seen in TM meditators (Section 4.2).

    Researchers at San Francisco State University conducted a physiological study of a Japanese Yogi who was highly proficient in the practice of Kundalini Yoga, as indicated by his bestowed title of YogaSamrat from the Indian Yoga Culture Federation. While meditating on a massage table in a positionsimilar to the half-lotus, the Yogi showed a sharp drop in his breathing rate to only 5 breaths perminute. Alpha activity increased on his EEG during his meditation, and was followed afterward by anincrease in theta waves (Arambula et al., 2001).

    Neurologist R. Murali Krishna (2005) recently reported an exploratory study with an Indian Swami andteacher of Nithya Yoga during a visit to his clinic in Oklahoma. PET scans of the Swamis brain duringmeditation revealed increased metabolic activity in the forward parts of his frontal lobe. As the Swamireported the experience of "opening his Third Eye," the lower central region of his frontal lobe lit up with activity. In addition, EEG monitoring indicated that the Swami was able to voluntarily make acontinuous transition between the various types of brain waves during deep meditation, suggesting thathis brain was capable of voluntarily shifting between brain waves and their associated mental states.

    4.4. Zen Meditators

    As with Indian Yogis, opportunities to obtain valuable data on the meditating brain from Zen Buddhistmonks can be traced to an early history, largely due to the efforts of local researchers. In the mid-1960s,Japanese researchers Akira Kasamatsu and Tomio Hirai (1966) were able to record the meditatingEEGs of 16 Zen priests and 32 of their disciples during a period of Sesshin . Whereas the disciples had atendency to show steady alpha waves even with their eyes open (Section 3.2), the priests tended toexhibit further progression of their EEG activity. A few minutes after beginning Zazen, the priests would show the alpha patterns over the frontal lobes typically seen in the disciples. However, afterabout 30 minutes, this alpha pattern would gradually slow down into a rhythmic pattern of theta waves. About 20 seconds later, this theta pattern would take the form of short "bursts" or "spikes," similar tothat seen in the EEGs of some TM and Yoga meditators (Sections 4.2 & 4.3). At the end of the Zazenperiod, a return to the alpha pattern was seen, which persisted for several minutes afterward.

    In line with this observation, the data gathered by Kasamatsu and Hirai (1966) suggest that theappearance of the theta pattern is related to the amount of Zazen training that the meditator has had, with the pattern appearing largely in meditators with more than 20 years of training. Findings fromlater EEG studies of Zazen further supported this relation (Chiesa, 2009, p. 587, Ivanovski & Malhi,2007, pp. 84 87; Woolfolk, 1975, p. 1330), suggesting that these progressive EEG changes may be thefruits of regular long-term practice.

    A group of Danish psychologists was recently able to examine the activity in the brain at the beginningof Zen meditation in an fMRI study with five meditators who had between 7 and 23 years of training(Brentsen et al., 2001). The fMRI scans revealed that, at the onset of their meditation, the meditators

    brains were increasingly active in the left side of the frontal lobe, the right parietal and temporal lobes,and the anterior cingulate cortex. In contrast, a decrease in activity was seen in the visual regions of theoccipital lobe.

    4.5. Vipassana Meditators

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    17/25

    Recent studies of advanced vipassana meditators have made a preliminary effort in exploring anotheraspect of the meditating brain: the possible effects that regular practice in meditation may have on thestructure and function of the brain, which we might call "training effects."

    The possibility that even a short period of regular practice in meditation may produce functionalchanges in the brain was initially explored in a study by psychologist Richard Davidson and hisassociates at the University of Wisconsin, in which they recorded the EEGs of volunteers at a corporatefirm both before and after they received an 8-week training program in a simple form of mindfulness

    meditation designed to help reduce stress. Compared to before the program, the volunteers showedmore alpha activity in the left central region of their brain after the relatively short program. A group of control volunteers who did not take the training program showed little to no such signs of alphaincrease. The program volunteers also reported having less anxiety and fewer negative feelings than thecontrol volunteers (Davidson et al., 2004).

    More long-term effects of meditation practice were explored in an imaging study conducted by a teamof researchers from Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Yale University, andMIT, led by psychiatrist Sara Lazar. They recruited 20 meditators who had an average of 9 years of training in vipassana and measured the thickness of the neural tissue in their cerebral cortex using anadvanced form of MRI. Compared to a group of non-meditating control volunteers, the meditatorsshowed indications of thicker tissue in cortical regions that have been found to be active duringmeditation, including central regions of the frontal lobe, as well as an area between the frontal andtemporal lobes known as the insula, which is involved in the perception of internal bodily responses(Lazar et al., 2005). In most cases, the thickness of our brains cerebral cortex tends to get thinner withage. These findings suggest that meditation may help slow this process in cortical areas around thefrontal lobe, a potential health benefit.

    Neuroscientist Dieter Vaitl and his associates at Justus-Liebig University in Germany extended thefindings of Lazars team by using MRI to measure the masses of neural cell tissue that comprise thecerebral cortex, known as gray matter. 7 Compared to non-meditating control volunteers, 20 meditators who had an average of 8.6 years of vipassana training showed greater concentrations of gray matternear the insula, the hippocampus, and the left temporal lobe (Hlzel et al., 2008).

    Using the same MRI technique, a team of neurologists at the UCLA School of Medicine recently made afurther attempt to extend these findings to a wider variety of meditators. Scanning the brains of 22meditators who had an average of 24 years of training in vipassana , Zazen, Yoga, and other forms of meditation, the team found larger volumes of gray matter in the hippocampus, the left temporal lobe,and the lower part of the frontal lobe (Luders et al., 2009).

    These results suggest that certain areas regularly active in the brains of long-term meditators may structurally alter themselves over time, such that the masses of tissue around these regions slightly thickens, or begins to concentrate more in these areas, perhaps due to greater neural cell proliferationin those areas.

    5. Discussion

    The experimental findings reviewed here tentatively suggest that the meditative state may induce short-term changes in brain activity that are in line with the dual views of meditation as a technique of relaxation, and as a technique of mental cultivation. Although their individual techniques andphilosophies somewhat differ, the various forms of meditation that we have looked at here seem toshare the basic commonality of being able to gradually induce a calmer state of mind, based on EEGmonitoring. As one enters a meditative state, brain wave activity begins to slow and ease intofrequencies in the alpha and theta range, frequencies commonly associated with relaxation and low arousal. In some cases, these alpha and theta patterns may briefly linger even after one has slipped out

    of the meditative state and returned to the normal waking state, a unique ability not often seen in theEEGs of non-meditators.

    The mental cultivation aspect is perhaps best reflected by advanced meditators in their ability tofrequently produce and maintain steady alpha and theta rhythms, to achieve EEG coherence, and, in

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    18/25

    the case of Buddhist monks, to enter the meditative state with relatively little mental effort. These featsseem to suggest that, through continual training of their attention and awareness, these individualshave been able to gradually develop and refine their mental and brain processes to a point where they are capable of operating more coherently, and are under greater voluntary control. Such a capacity would be consistent with the traditional perspectives on meditation expressed in Buddhism andTaoism 8 , which emphasize mental development in order to encourage beneficial thought processes(calmness and concentration) and positive emotions (love and joy), and reduce negative ones, such asfear and anger (Goleman, 1988; Walsh & Shapiro, 2006).

    The possibility that ones attention is harnessed and developed during meditation may be supported by the finding that several forms of meditation engage cortical regions that may be part of a network of brain areas involved in attention, such as the frontal lobes (Frith et al., 1991; Ingvar, 1994; Pardo et al.,1991) and the anterior cingulate cortex (Devinsky et al., 1995; Posner & Dehaene, 1994; Posner &Petersen, 1990). Some studies suggest that the anterior cingulate cortex is also involved in theregulation of some functions of the autonomic nervous system (Devinsky et al., 1995), which may tie into the changes in breathing, heartrate, and electrical skin resistance that are sometimes observed inmeditators. The regular engagement of the frontal lobe may promote neural activity and proliferation inthat region, and thereby work against age-related thinning of its cortical tissue, as suggested by thetraining effects observed in vipassana meditators (Section 4.5). Though gradual over time, such effects

    would likely serve as a health benefit of regular meditative practice.Regular practice may also bring about training-related changes in the grey matter of the cerebral cortex,suggesting that there is some degree of flexibility in the way our brains are structured. It is interestingto note that similar training effects on grey matter have been observed in the brains of people who are just learning a new skill, such as how to juggle (Draganski et al., 2004). If these findings are valid, thenthey may indicate that it is possible to, in a sense, "re-structure" the brain through persistent practice sothat it adapts to function more effectively in performing a complex skill, adding new meaning to the oldadage that "practice makes perfect."

    The role of the theta rhythm may also be relevant to the issue of which brain areas are activated inmeditation. Although it is most often associated with low arousal and light sleep, there is now someevidence to suggest that theta activity occurring around the frontal and midline regions of the brain cansometimes be associated with attention and mental concentration (Inanaga, 1998; Klimesch, 1999).One study has produced findings to indicate that this kind of theta rhythm may be reflective of alternating activity between the frontal lobes and the anterior cingulate cortex (Asada et al., 1999),providing a possible key link between meditation, theta waves, and these two brain regions. This may be only one part of the equation, however; it seems that meditation may be a complex mental processinvolving several other components of the autonomic nervous system and the brain, including thehippocampus, the amygdala, and various parts of the brainstem (Newberg & Iversen, 2003).

    On a separate note, the experimental findings with advanced meditators offer us a bit of insight into thepossible brain correlates of deep or higher meditative states, insight that may be valuable in the quest todevelop an answer to the problem of how neural processes in our brain give rise to consciousexperience, the so-called "hard problem" of consciousness (Chalmers, 1995). One might argue that inorder for us to gain a better understanding of consciousness in general, we must be able to consider allforms of it, including deep meditative states.

    EEG observations of TM meditators and Buddhist monks seem to indicate that as one enters a deepmeditative state, brain wave activity tends to speed up rather than slow down, approaching frequenciesin the beta and gamma range. These frequencies are usually associated with heightened alertness andcomplex cognitive thought processes, suggesting that a form of "focused arousal" might characterizethese states.

    In contrast, observations of Yoga and Zen meditators seem to indicate that deep states occurring inthese practices are more often marked by constant alpha and theta patterns, which are suggestive of astate maintained at the boundary between wakefulness and the early stages of sleep. This boundary state, often called a hypnagogic state, can be characterized by intense dream-like images that occur asone is falling asleep. The steady presence of alpha and theta could mean that Yoga and Zen meditators

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    19/25

    are capable of inducing and maintaining a hypnagogic state while still remaining awake. If that is so,then perhaps the images produced during such a state comprise part of a deep Yogic or Zen meditativestate. Alternatively, constant theta activity focused over the frontal and midline regions may reflect astate of deep attentional focus or concentration, as noted above. For now, these ideas remainspeculative, and it is hoped that further EEG research with meditators capable of entering deep states will shed more light on this interesting and important aspect of consciousness.

    6. Conclusion

    Based on our review of the experimental findings, both past and present, what lessons might we be ableto take away about the meditating brain? The first lesson may be a practical one for our health: Asidefrom calming our minds, the findings tentatively suggest that the practice of meditation may help slow the thinning of cortical tissue in our frontal regions that naturally occurs with age. If this is a genuineeffect, then it seems to be associated with regular, long-term practice, so it may be useful to have a daily period of meditation as part of ones health regimen. Thus, if you practice meditation on a regular basis,it may be beneficial in the long run to keep it up!

    Another lesson we might be able to take away is one about deep meditative states. The limited research with advanced meditators suggests that these states, which tend to be subjectively different from theordinary waking state of consciousness, may have a partial basis in activity at both ends of the brain wave frequency spectrum. Some deep states appear to be linked with slow frequencies associated withdeep relaxation, low arousal, and the waking-sleep threshold, while others are associated with fastfrequencies associated with complex cognitive thought processes. Due to the paucity of the research,there is little we can conclude at the moment, but with further work, what we learn about them can bepotentially valuable for gaining a better understanding of the boundaries of consciousness that lie at theedge of the hard problem.

    Lastly, we might be able to take away a lesson about cultural approaches to the human mind. For about2,500 years, Buddhism has offered a spiritual means of personally exploring the inner self andcontemplating the nature of the mind using techniques of deep introspection. At its heart, Westernpsychology shares this same focus of exploration and contemplation using empirical techniques. Eventhough they may take different perspectives, psychologists Roger Walsh and Shauna Shapiro (2006)have argued that there is much that the two disciplines can learn from each other. For example, thefindings from psychology and neuroscience can aid Buddhists in more deeply exploring their first-person insights and mental states, while Buddhists can offer psychology and neuroscience a broaderperspective on introspection and subjective experience, two things that are vital in linking the workingsof the brain to mental behavior (Barinaga, 2003). The study of meditation is one way that this path of knowledge can be facilitated.

    Upon accepting the Nobel Peace Prize in 1989, His Holiness the Dalai Lama once stated: "Both scienceand the teachings of the Buddha tell us of the fundamental unity of all things" (Knight, 2004, p. 670). If a mutual consideration of these two disciplines can lead to a unity of knowledge and perspective, itmight just lead to a greater advancement in our quest to better understand the human mind.

    Notes

    1. It should be noted that although many studies point toward a beneficial effect, the consistency of their findings islimited by their experimental design, methods, and variety of meditative techniques explored (Ospina et al., 2008).

    While these have been improving with time, it is wise to view these findings as "tentative" at the present, withfurther direction to be offered by future research.

    2. One issue that has been raised is whether the calming effects of meditation are any different from the calmnessthat can be achieved through simple rest. To explore this issue, University of Kansas psychologist David Holmes(1984) reviewed the research comparing rest with meditation (the latter being TM in most cases). Based on a simple"vote-counting" comparison, he found little difference between them with regards to physiological processes such asheartrate, skin resistance, and blood plasma changes. (Because of its calming effects, one would expect meditation toshow sharper decreases in these processes than rest.) However, a closer statistical evaluation of the research by TMresearchers Michael Dillbeck and David Orme-Johnson (1987) found significantly more decreases in these processesin meditators than in resting control volunteers. Thus, there may be some indication that meditation is differentfrom simple rest, although this finding should be considered tentative, as well (see Note 1).

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    20/25

    3. A classic example of a koan that is likely to be familiar to many is the falling tree riddle: "If a tree falls in a forestand no one is there to witness it, does it make a sound?"

    4. This is notable because alpha activity is usually much more prevalent when the eyes are closed rather than open(Carlson, 1992, p. 242; Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, p. 53).

    5. Practicing Su-soku is meant to help the initiate gain proficiency in the ability to keep attention focused on aspecific object or thought, as doing so will be necessary in order for them to continually focus on a koan duringZazen (Woolfolk, 1975, p. 1330). This is one example of how techniques from the two classes of meditation(concentration and mindfulness) can be blended, as mentioned in Section 1.

    6. According to the traditional philosophy underlying g Tum-mo, prna (literally meaning "wind" or "air") iscollectively extracted from the scattered, random fluctuations of normal conscious experience and channeled intothe bodys "central channel." As prna dissolves within this central channel, it builds up the "internal heat" thatcreates the rises in body temperature. From this perspective, the heating effect follows from a spiritual energy practice (Benson et al., 1982, p. 234).

    7. As one might surmise, the term "gray matter" refers to the appearance of the neural cell tissue, based on thegrayish brown color of the individual cell bodies that make up the tissue. This is in contrast to the other type of neural tissue mass found in the brain, known as "white matter" due to the pale color of the fatty substance thatsurrounds the outer branches of nerve cells in order to insulate them (Kolb & Whishaw, 1990, p. 6).

    8. For instance, the term bhavana (mental cultivation) is used in the Buddhist tradition, while the term lien-hsin

    (refining the mind) is used in the Taoist tradition (Walsh & Shapiro, 2006, p. 228).

    References

    Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alphareflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High resolution EEG investigation of meditation. Neuroscience Letters , 310 , 57 60.

    Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2003). Changes in cortical activity in altered states of consciousness: The study of meditation by high-resolution EEG. Human Physiology , 29 , 143 151.

    Anand, B. K., Chhina, G. S., & Singh, B. (1961). Some aspects of electroencephalographic studies in yogis. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology , 13, 452 456.

    Arambula, P., Peper, E., Kawakami, M., & Gibney, K. H. (2001). The physiological correlates of Kundalini Yoga meditation: A study of a Yoga master. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback , 26 ,147 153.

    Arias, A. J., Steinberg, K., Banga, A., & Trestman, R. L. (2006). Systematic review of the efficacy of meditation techniques as treatments for medical illness. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 12, 817 832.

    Asada, H., Fukuda, Y., Tsunoda, S., Yamaguchi, M., & Tonoike, M. (1999). Frontal midline theta

    rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters , 274 , 29 32.

    Azari, N. P., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellmann, L., Herzog, H., Stoerig, P.,Birnbacher, D., & Seitz, R. J. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal of Neuroscience , 13, 1649 1652.

    Brentsen, K. B., Hartvig, N. V., Stdkilde-Jrgensen, H., & Mammen, J. (2001). Onset of meditationexplored with fMRI. NeuroImage , 13, S297.

    Bagchi, B. K., & Wenger, M. A. (1957). Electro-physiological correlates of some Yogi exercises. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology , Supplement 7 , 132 149.

    Banquet, J. P. (1973). Spectral analysis of the EEG in meditation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology , 35 , 143 151.

    Barinaga, M. (2003). Buddhism and neuroscience: Studying the well-trained mind. Science , 302 , 44

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    21/25

    46.

    Barnes, P. M., Bloom, B., & Nahin, R. L. (2008). Complementary and alternative medicine use amongadults and children: United States, 2007. National Health Statistics Reports No. 12 . Hyattsville, MD:National Center for Health Statistics.

    Benson, H., Lehmann, J. W., Malhotra, M. S., Goldman, R. F., Hopkins, J., & Epstein, M. D. (1982).Body temperature changes during the practice of g Tum-mo yoga. Nature , 295 , 234 236.

    Benson, H., Malhotra, M. S., Goldman, R. F., Jacobs, G. D., & Hopkins, P. J. (1990). Three case reportsof the metabolic and electroencephalographic changes during advanced Buddhist meditationtechniques. Behavioral Medicine , 16, 90 95.

    Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neuralcorrelates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA , 104 , 11483 11488.

    Buxton, R. B. (2001). Introduction to Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Principles and Techniques . New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin , 132, 180 211.

    Carlson, N. R. (1992). Foundations of Physiological Psychology (2nd Ed.). New York: Allyn and Bacon.

    Chalmers, D. J. (1995). The puzzle of conscious experience. Scientific American , 273(6) , 80 86.

    Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is theefficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 13, 651 657.

    Chiesa, A. (2009). Zen meditation: An integration of current evidence. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 15 , 585 592.

    Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 15 , 593 600.

    Cohen, D. L., Wintering, N., Tolles, V., Townsend, R. R., Farrar, J. T., Galantino, M. L., & Newberg, A.B. (2009). Cerebral blood flow effects of Yoga training: Preliminary evaluation of 4 cases. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 15 , 9 14.

    Cohen, M. S., Kosslyn, S. M., Breiter, H. C., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., Anderson, A. K.,

    Brookheimer, S. Y., Rosen, B. R., & Belliveau, J. W. (1996). Changes in cortical activity during mentalrotation: A mapping study using functional MRI. Brain , 119, 89 100.

    Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L., Meizin, F. M., & Petersen, S. E. (1995). Superior parietal cortex activationduring spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science , 270 , 802 805.

    Corby, J. C., Roth, W. T., Zarcone, V. P., Jr., & Kopell, B. S. (1978). Psychophysiological correlates of thepractice of Tantric Yoga meditation. Archives of General Psychiatry , 35 , 571 577.

    Davidson, J. M. (1976). The physiology of meditation and mystical states of consciousness. Perspectivesin Biology and Medicine , 19, 345 379.

    Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F.,Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A., Bonus, K., & Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immunefunction produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine , 65 , 564 570.

    Desmedt, J. E., & Tomberg, C. (1994). Transient phase-locking of 40 Hz electrical oscillations in

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    22/25

    prefrontal and parietal human cortex reflects the process of conscious somatic perception. Neuroscience Letters , 168 , 126 129.

    Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain , 118, 279 306.

    Dillbeck, M. C., & Orme-Johnson, D. W. (1987). Physiological differences between TranscendentalMeditation and rest. American Psychologist , 42 , 879 891.

    Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature , 427 , 311 312.

    Dunn, B. R., Hartigan, J. A., & Mikulas, W. L. (1999). Concentration and mindfulness meditations:Unique forms of consciousness? Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback , 24 , 147 165.

    Elson, B. D., Hauri, P., & Cunis, D. (1977). Physiological changes in Yoga meditation. Psychophysiology , 14, 52 57.

    Ferguson, P. C. (1975). The psychobiology of Transcendental Meditation: A review. Journal of Altered States of Consciousness , 2, 15 36.

    Fields, R. D. (2006). Meditations on the brain. Scientific American Mind , 17(1), 42 43.

    Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). Willed action and the prefrontalcortex in man: A study with PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B , 244 , 241 246.

    Gifford-May, D., & Thompson, N. L. (1994). "Deep states" of meditation: Phenomenological reports of experience. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology , 26 , 117 138.

    Goleman, D. (1988). The Meditative Mind: The Varieties of Meditative Experience . New York: Jeremy P. Tarcher/Perigee Books.

    Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction andhealth benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research , 57 , 35 43.

    Haxby, J. V., Grady, C. L., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P.,Shapiro, M. B., & Rapoport, S. I. (1991). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways inhuman extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA , 88 , 1621 1625.

    Hebert, R., & Lehmann, D. (1977). Theta bursts: An EEG pattern in normal subjects practising theTranscendental Meditation technique. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology , 42 , 397 405.

    Holmes, D. S. (1984). Meditation and somatic arousal reduction: A review of the experimental evidence. American Psychologist , 39 , 1 10.

    Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Gard, T., Hempel, H., Weygandt, M., Morgen, K., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Investigationof mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience , 3, 55 61.

    Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Hempel, H., Hackl, A., Wolf, K., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2007). Differentialengagement of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal cortex in adept meditators and non-meditators. Neuroscience Letters , 421, 16 21.

    Inanaga, K. (1998). Frontal midline theta rhythm and mental activity. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 52, 555 566.

    Ingvar, D. H. (1994). The will of the brain: Cerebral correlates of willful acts. Journal of Theoretical Biology , 171, 7 12.

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    23/25

    Ivanovski, B., & Malhi, G. S. (2007). The psychological and neurophysiological concomitants of mindfulness forms of meditation. Acta Neuropsychiatrica , 19, 76 91.

    Jevning, R., Anand, R., Biedebach, M., & Fernando, G. (1996). Effects of regional cerebral blood flow of Transcendental Meditation. Physiology and Behavior , 59 , 399 402.

    Jevning, R., Wallace, R. K., & Biedebach, M. (1992). The physiology of meditation: A review. A wakefulhypometabolic integrated response. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 16, 415 424.

    Joliot, M., Ribary, U., & Llins, R. (1994). Human oscillatory brain activity near 40 Hz coexists withcognitive temporal binding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA , 91, 11748 11751.

    Kasamatsu, A., & Hirai, T. (1966). An electroencephalographic study on the Zen meditation (Zazen). Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japonica , 20 , 315 336.

    Kjaer, T. W., Bertelsen, C., Piccini, P., Brooks, D., Alving, J., & Lou, H. C. (2002). Increased dopaminetone during meditation-induced change of consciousness. Cognitive Brain Research , 13, 255 259.

    Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews , 29 , 169 195.

    Knight, J. (2004). Buddhism on the brain. Nature , 432 , 670.

    Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1990). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (3rd Ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman & Company.

    Kornfield, J. (1979). Intensive insight meditation: A phenomenological study. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology , 11, 41 58.

    Krishna, R. M. (2005). Mind of a mystic. In P. S. Nithyananda, Open the DoorLet the Breeze In! Tools for Joyful Living (pp. 147 153). Fremont, CA: Nithyananda Publishers.

    Kubota, Y., Sato, W., Toichi, M., Murai, T., Okada, T., Hayashi, A., & Sengoku, A. (2001). Frontalmidline theta rhythm is correlated with cardiac autonomic activities during the performance of anattention demanding meditation procedure. Cognitive Brain Research , 11, 281 287.

    Lacquement, G. (2008). An Introduction to Meditation . Norman, OK: Author.

    Lane, J. D., Seskevich, J. E., & Pieper, C. F. (2007). Brief meditation training can improve perceivedstress and negative mood. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine , 13, 38 44.

    Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. NeuroReport , 11, 1581 1585.

    Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., McGarvey, M.,Quinn, B. T., Dusek, J. A., Benson, H., Rauch, S. L., Moore, C. I., & Fischl, B. (2005). Meditationexperience is associated with increased cortical thickness. NeuroReport , 16, 1893 1897.

    Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., & Pizzagalli, D. (2001).Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging , 108 , 111 121.

    Linden, D. J. (2007). The Accidental Mind: How Brain Evolution Has Given Us Love, Memory, Dreams, and God . Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.

    Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature , 453 , 869 878.

    Lou, H. C., Kjaer, T. W., Friberg, L., Wildschiodtz, G., Holm, S., & Nowak, M. (1999). A 15O-H 2O PETstudy of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness. Human Brain Mapping , 7 , 98 105.

  • 8/9/2019 Williams, Bryan - A Glimpse Into the Meditating Brain

    24/25

    Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., & Gaser, C. (2009). The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. NeuroImage , 45 , 672 678.

    Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditatorsself-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA , 101, 16369 16373.

    Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring inmeditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 12, 163 169.

    Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousnessand Cognition , 18, 176 186.

    Murata, T., Takahashi, T., Hamada, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Yoshida, H., & Wada, Y. (2004).Individual trait anxiety levels characterizing the properties of Zen meditation. Neuropsychobiology , 50 ,189 194.

    Oman, D., Shapiro, S. L., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Flinders, T. (2008). Meditation lowers stressand supports forgiveness among college students: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of AmericanCollege Health , 56 , 569 578.

    Ospina, M. B., Bond, K., Karkhaneh, M., Buscemi, N., Dryden, D. M., Barnes, V., Carlson, L. E., Dusek,J. A., & Shannahoff-Khalsa, D. (2008). Clinical trials of meditation practices in health care:Characteristics and quality. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine , 14, 1199 1213.

    Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., & dAquili, E. (2001). Themeasurement of cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging , 106 , 113 122.

    Newberg, A., dAquili, E., & Rause, V. (2001). Why God Wont Go Away: Brain Science and the Biologyof Belief . New York: Ballantine Books.

    Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A., & dAquili, E. G. (2003). Cerebral blood flow during meditativeprayer: Preliminary findings and methodological issues. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 97 , 625 630.

    Newberg, A. B., & Iverse