Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

87
Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy

Transcript of Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Page 1: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Why isn’t vision perfect?An exercise in psychoanatomy

Page 2: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Why isn’t vision perfect?An exercise in psychoanatomy

Tracing the flow of information through the nervous system

using functional experiments

Page 3: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

V1

LGN

Parietal (action)

temporal (perception)

If neural representation fails at any stage, perception will fail

Page 4: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

The most basic aspect of vision: spatial resolution

Resolution has a limit:Coarse patterns are seen, fine detail is not

How and where does resolution fail?

We start at the beginning with the photons themselves

Page 5: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 6: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Cause of imperfect resolution:

A spreading of light, or of the effects of light

Page 7: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 8: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 9: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Campbell & Robson (1968)

Spatial frequency (cycles/degree)

Sensitivity

Contrast-Sensitivity Function (CSF)

Resolution limit: 50cpd

Page 10: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors that might limit visual resolution:

Optics of the image (including diffraction)Sampling by the retinal mosaic Light collection by the cone aperturesNeural convergence:

intra-retinal or retino-thalamicthalamo-corticalintra-cortical

Page 11: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors limiting visual resolution:Optics of the image (including diffraction)

Page 12: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

One test for the role of optics:

Do perfect optics make vision perfect?

Two possible approaches:

• Test resolution using interference fringe targets, bypassing the optics; or…

• Use Adaptive Optics to compensate for individual optical aberrations

Page 13: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

DaveWilliams

Page 14: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Laser interferometrybypassses optical losses:target stripes are generated directly on the retina

by intereference of two uniform laser beams

Page 15: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Bypassing optics improves vision, but only from 45 cpd to about 60 cpd.Vision is still not perfect; neural losses are at least as important as optics.

Page 16: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

AdaptiveOptics

Page 17: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Adaptive Optics: supernormal, yet still imperfect vision

Page 18: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors limiting visual resolution:Optics of the image (including diffraction)…important, but not sufficient

Page 19: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Two ways the retinal mosaic might limit resolution:

• Filtering

• Sampling

… Effective size of cones

… Spacing of cones

Page 20: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 21: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Sampling limits on resolution?

• Foveal photoreceptor mosaic frequency: 110 cpd

• Nyquist sampling limit for 1 row of cones: 55cpd

Page 22: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 23: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

30 cpd

Page 24: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

60 cpd

Page 25: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

120 cpd

Page 26: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

120 cpd processed by a single row of cones: aliasing

Page 27: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Roorda and Williams

Page 28: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Sampling limits on resolution?

• Foveal photoreceptor mosaic frequency: 110 cpd

• Nyquist sampling limit for 1 row of cones: 55cpd

• Nyquist limit for 8 rows of cones: 440 cpd ??

Page 29: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 30: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 31: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors limiting visual resolution:Optics of the image (including diffraction)...important, but less important than neural losses Sampling by the photoreceptor mosaic…unimportantLight collection by the cone apertures ?

Page 32: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Unresolvable high-contrast patterns appear desaturated

Sherif Shady and Don MacLeod(Nature Neurosci 2002)

Page 33: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

10 30 50 70 90 110

Page 34: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors limiting visual resolution:Optics of the image and photoreceptor sampling…Light collection by the cone apertures… not severely limiting (>100cpd)Neural Losses:

intra-retinal or retino-thalamicthalamo-corticalintra-cortical

Page 35: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

LGN

Page 36: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Peter Lennie…Matt McMahon,

…Dave Williams, and Martin Lankheet

Page 37: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 38: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

McMahon et al.J.Neurosci.1999

Page 39: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors limiting visual resolution:Optics of the image (diffraction, etc.)Light collection by the cone aperturesnot severely limiting (>100cpd)Neural convergence:

intra-retinal or retino-thalamic still not limiting (>80cpd)thalamo-corticalintra-cortical ?

Page 40: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

LGNV1

Page 41: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Seeing spatial pattern

Physicalstimuli

Perceptualexperience

Localized neural activity

correlation correlation

What does V1 see?

Page 42: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Orientation-Selective Adaptation

Adapt(30 sec)

Test(200 msec)

Same OrientationHard to see!

Need high contrast

Orthogonal OrientationEasy to see!

Need low contrast

Page 43: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

V1 is the first stage in the visual system where orientation information is extracted:

orientation-selective adaptation is only possible at or after V1.

If we find evidence for orientation-selective adaptation, then it implies that the orientation information has at least reached V1.

Adaptation: Psychophysicists’ microelectrode

Page 44: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

a.

adapt

test

5000ms

fig. 1

250 ms

250 ms

250 ms

? ?

respond/adaptor

250 ms

unresolvable

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0

1.0

1.2

SH DM

unresolvable

orientationdiscrimination

orientation-specificadaptation

orientationdiscrimination

orientation-specificadaptation

testtest

30 40 50 60 7035 45 55 65

(adapting) spatial frequency (cpd)(adapting) spatial frequency (cpd)

30 40 50 60 7035 45 55 65

b.

log sensitivityfor orientationdiscrimination

andlog threshold

elevation fromorientation-

specificadaptation

Page 45: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Invisibleverticalgrating

Adapt(30 sec)

Test(200 msec)

Same OrientationNeed higher contrast

Orthogonal OrientationNeed lower contrast

Result

Page 46: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

a.

adapt

test

5000ms

fig. 1

250 ms

250 ms

250 ms

? ?

respond/adaptor

250 ms

unresolvable

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0

1.0

1.2

SH DM

unresolvable

orientationdiscrimination

orientation-specificadaptation

orientationdiscrimination

orientation-specificadaptation

testtest

30 40 50 60 7035 45 55 65

(adapting) spatial frequency (cpd)(adapting) spatial frequency (cpd)

30 40 50 60 7035 45 55 65

b.

log sensitivityfor orientationdiscrimination

andlog threshold

elevation fromorientation-

specificadaptation

Page 47: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 48: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

adapt test (250 ms)

250 ms

250 ms

perception (subjective horizontal) (5 sec)

a.

b.

36 48 66

-0.5

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

Adapting spatial frequency (cpd)

test

SH

60

SH DM

SH

Tilt Aftereffect in degrees

(mean s.e.m.)

Page 49: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

adapt test (250 ms)

250 ms

250 ms

perception (subjective horizontal) (5 sec)

a.

b.

36 48 66

-0.5

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.5

2.0

2.5

Adapting spatial frequency (cpd)

test

SH

60

SH DM

SH

Tilt Aftereffect in degrees

(mean s.e.m.)

Page 50: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Orientation-selective adaptation from an invisible spatial pattern at 60 to 70 cpd:

invisible to us but visible at V1.

Page 51: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Factors that might limit visual resolution for interference targets:

Optics of the image (including diffraction): not applicableLight collection by the cone apertures: >100 cpdNeural convergence:

intra-retinal or retino-thalamic: 80 cpdthalamo-cortical…lumped with intracortical…intra-cortical: 70 cpd Perception: <= 60 cpd

Page 52: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Conclusions

• Neural losses slightly exceed optical losses at the limit.• Sampling and light collection in the photoreceptor mosaic are not limiting.• Neural losses are distributed through the system; some are intracortical, since cortex can respond to patterns too fine for conscious perception.

Optional Bonus Conclusion about Consciousness

• Primary visual cortex is not directly represented in conscious experience (Crick and Koch).

Page 53: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Paper topics

1) When a computer monitor that flickers too fast for the flicker to be perceived, can the unseen flicker nevertheless activate the visual cortex of your brain? Design an experiment to investigate this.

2) Is it possible to explain He and MacLeod’s results without accepting Crick and Koch’s conclusion that primary visual cortex has no immediate representation in conscious experience?

3) How might brain imaging experiments follow up on He and MacLeod’s observations?

Page 54: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Temporal Resolution: When Vision is Grossly

Imperfect

Page 55: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

120 S 3 S 1 S

AdaptPre-Adapt Test

Time

Page 56: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

806040200

4.0

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

806040200

Scaled adapting modulation

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

100806040200

60

50

4035

30

20 Hz10

4.0

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

100806040200

Adapting modulation (%)

45

30

20 Hz

10

40

HF

JL

Te

st m

od

ula

tio

n th

resh

old

a c

b d

Te

st m

od

ula

tio

n th

resh

old

Shady_Fig2

Page 57: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

50403020100

Frequency (Hz)

-1.5

-1.0

-0.5

0.0

6050403020100

Frequency (Hz)

-2.0

-1.5

-1.0

-0.5

0.0

JL HF

Perception Adaptation

Shady_Fig3

Invisible Invisible

Page 58: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

0.0

3020100

Frequency (Hz)

1.5

1.0

0.5

0.0

3020100

Frequency (Hz)

Perception Adaptation

invisible invisible

Shady_Fig4

Page 59: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 60: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 61: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 62: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 63: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 64: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Conclusions

• Oblique adapting and masking gratings are not less powerful than horizontal ones

• We conclude that the Oblique Effect arises after the site of pattern adaptation and masking

• Unexpectedly, oblique adapters are slightly more powerful than horizontal ones

• Additional experiments and modeling will allow us to quantitatively test the possible models we have presented

Page 65: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 66: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 67: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 68: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 69: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 70: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 71: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 72: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 73: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Data for subject TL

-3

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

0 1 2 3 4

Data for subject MW

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0 1 2 3 4

Modulation Frequency (cpd)

RF Width: Results

=

=

Th

resh

old

(lo

g C

)

Page 74: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Modulation Frequency (cpd)

Spatial integration is anisotropic

Data for subject TL

-3

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

0 1 2 3 4

Th

resh

old

(lo

g C

)

Page 75: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Does the extent of spatial integration vary

with carrier contrast?

50% Contrast5% Contrast

Page 76: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

-3

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

0 1 2 3 4Modulation Frequency (cpd)

low contrastlow contrast

high contrast

high contrast

Results: High Contrast

=

=

Th

resh

old

(lo

g C

)

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0 1 2 3 4

Data for subject TL Data for subject MW

Page 77: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

High Contrast Properties

• Loss in sensitivity at lower modulation frequencies

• Spatial integration minimal both along and across contours

Modulation Frequency (cpd)

At high contrast:

-3

-2.5

-2

-1.5

-1

0 1 2 3 4

low contrast

high contrast

Th

resh

old

(lo

g C

)

Data for subject TL

Page 78: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

LOW frequency: HIGH frequency:

Contrast Gain Control

Contrast gain locally compensates for low frequency modulation

Loss in sensitivity at lower modulation frequencies

Page 79: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 80: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 81: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Receptive Field ShrinksLOW contrast: HIGH contrast:

• Receptive field shrinks at high contrast.- Supported by recent physiological evidence

• Leads to similar behavior for integration along and across image contours.

Page 82: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Contrast

Constant

PerceivedBrightness

MeanLuminance

time

flicker

Page 83: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

Orientation-selective adaptation from an invisible spatial pattern:

invisible to us but visible at V1.

There is intra-cortical loss of spatial information.

V1 does not directly support conscious vision (Crick and Koch)

Page 84: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.
Page 85: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

With 8.5 cpd grating: Modulation threshold doubled at 2.5 cpdImplied RF height: 6 min, or 12 cone rows

With 40 cpd grating: Modulation threshold doubled near 4 cpdImplied RF height: 4 min, or 8 cone rows

Would integration over 8 rows prevent aliasing?

Page 86: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.

120 cpd

Page 87: Why isn’t vision perfect? An exercise in psychoanatomy.