Using Ecological Momentary Assessment Methods in Mind-Body Research Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D....

47
Using Ecological Using Ecological Momentary Momentary Assessment Methods Assessment Methods in Mind-Body Research in Mind-Body Research Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D. Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D. University of Pittsburgh University of Pittsburgh Pittsburgh Mind-Body Center Pittsburgh Mind-Body Center July 10, 2006 July 10, 2006

Transcript of Using Ecological Momentary Assessment Methods in Mind-Body Research Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D....

Using Ecological MomentaryUsing Ecological MomentaryAssessment Methods Assessment Methods in Mind-Body Researchin Mind-Body Research

Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D.Thomas W. Kamarck, Ph.D.

University of PittsburghUniversity of Pittsburgh

Pittsburgh Mind-Body CenterPittsburgh Mind-Body Center

July 10, 2006July 10, 2006

Collaborators:

Lori Arnold, M.S., Mary Witzig, George Haff

Saul Shiffman, Ph.D.

Karen Matthews, Ph.D.

Dan Buysse, M.D.

Wesley Thompson, Ph.D.

Vicki Helgeson, Ph.D.

Rebecca Thurston, Ph.D.

FORMAT OF TODAY’S SESSION

1. For Pre-Contemplators

Pro’s and Con’s of EMA

--History

--Why EMA considered as alternative to traditional assessment

--What questions are best asked with these methods

--How information gleaned from these methods may be different from that obtained from alternative approaches

FORMAT OF TODAY’S SESSION

2. For Contemplators

Preparing to take the EMA “plunge”

--Design alternatives

--Options and opportunities

FORMAT OF TODAY’S SESSION

3. For “Action Phase”

Implementation of EMA methods

-- Choosing Equipment

-- Designing Instruments

-- Training Participants

-- Enhancing Compliance

-- Managing Data

FORMAT OF TODAY’S SESSION

1. INTRODUCTION

Kamarck 9:00-10:15

BREAK 10:15-10:30

2. APPLICATIONS

Matthews, Buysee, Thompson 10:30-1:00

Helgeson, Thurston, Kamarck

LUNCH 1:00-1:45

3. IMPLEMENTATION

Witzig & Haff 1:45-3:00

ECOLOGICAL

MOMENTARY

ASSESSMENT

IN THE NATURAL ENVIRONMENT

IN REAL TIME

DATA COLLECTION BY STUDY PARTICIPANTS

EXPERIENCE SAMPLING

Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1977

AMBULATORY ASSESSMENT

Fahrenberg & Myrtek, 2001

Stone & Shiffman (1994). Annals Beh Med, 16, 199-202.

•Time budget analysis

(Thorndike et al., 1937)

•Analysis of behavioral settings

(Barker & Wright, 1951 One boy’s day)

• Self-monitoring and behavior change

(McFall, 1977)

•Experience sampling

Czikszentmihalyi (1990) Flow

EMA PRECURSORS

•Social Interactions

(Reis & Wheeler, 1991)

•Psychopathology

(deVries, 1992)

•Psychophysiological Assessment

(Fahrenberg & Myrtek, 2001)

EMA APPLICATIONS

-100

100

300

500

700

900

1100

1300

1500

1980-1985

1986-1990

1991-1995

1996-2000

2001-2005

THE SCIENTIFIC LITERATURE REFLECTS A GROWING INTEREST IN THESE AREAS IN RECENT YEARS

# O

F M

ED

LIN

E R

EF

ER

EN

CE

S

CITED ARTICLES USING KEYWORDS:

“Ecological Momentary Assessment,” “Experience Sampling,” “Ambulatory Assessment.”

1. Traditional assessment approaches

have significant limitations.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story (perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

3. EMA approaches may provide some advantages for addressing certain types of questions.

4. EMA approaches may not be advantageous in all circumstances.

5. EMA approaches may have some limitations.

PROS AND CONS OF EMA

1.Traditional assessment approaches have significant limitations.

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY

• Not accurate in recalling details of daily lives.

• Errors are systematic, not random.

• Memory as process of reconstruction rather than retrieval.

1.Traditional assessment approaches have significant limitations.

HEURISTICS AFFECTING RECOLLECTION OF RECENT EVENTS (Bradburn, Rips & Shevell, 1987, Science, 236, 157-161).

• Availability– more salient events given greater weight.

• Recency– more recent events have disproportionate impact.

• State biases– effects of present mood.

• Effort after meaning– memory adjusted to fit present circumstances.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

a.RELAPSE PREVENTION MODEL

b.DAILY COPING

c. AMBULATORY VS. CLINIC BP

d. JOB DEMAND

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

a. RELAPSE PREVENTION MODEL

Transgressions (‘slips’) following efforts at smoking cessation are common.

Behavioral and affective response to ‘slips’ predict subsequent prognosis. Coping efforts.

Empirical work: Retrospective reports.

Coping behaviors following ‘slip’ are associated with reduced risk for relapse.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

a. RELAPSE PREVENTION MODEL

Shiffman et al. (1996) JCCP, 5, 993-1002.

133 smoking cessation program participants

Electronic diaries in weeks following cessation.

Reports of coping related to subsequent lapse during same day, but not to ultimate relapse status.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

b. DAILY COPING

Stone et al. (1998) JPSP, 74, 1670-1680.

Compared reports of coping using EMA vs. retrospective measures.

100 Ss with marital or job stress

EMA interviews every 40 mins over 2 days.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

b. DAILY COPING

Items endorsed in EMA interviews were not endorsed in retrospective report 28 % of time on average.

Items endorsed in retrospective report not endorsed in EMA interviews 31 % of the time on average.

Retrospective reports invoke different types of cognitive processes than momentary accounts.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

c. AMBULATORY VS. CLINIC BP

ABP measures are better predictors of CVD than are clinic BP measures.

More representative, but also measurement advantages.

In 216 healthy adults, 4 automated BPs over 2.5 hours in laboratory and in field.

Compared these two sets of measures in terms of association with carotid IMT.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

c. AMBULATORY VS. CLINIC BP

Kamarck et al. (2002) J Hypertension, 20, 1535-41.

2. EMA approaches tell a different story

(perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

c. JOB DEMAND

Kamarck et al. (2004) Health Psychology, 23, 4-32.

Perceived demand and control using repeated EMA assessments over 6 day period in 330 adults.

Demand, control also assessed using global retrospective reports: Karasek Job Content questionnaire.

n = 152 Questionnaire

Demand Control

Diary

Task Demand r =.53*

Decisional Control r = .31*

* p < .0001 after adjustment for age, sex and education.

c. JOB DEMAND

2. EMA approaches tell a different story (perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

0.75

0.77

0.79

0.81

0.83

0.85

0.87

0.89

Mean Ratings of Task Demand by Quartile

Me

an

Ca

roti

d IM

T (

mm

)

b=.02, F (1, 328) = 8.44, r2 = .02, p =.004

Kamarck et al. Health Psychology: 2004

2. EMA approaches tell a different story (perhaps more valid?) than traditional approaches.

3. EMA approaches may provide some advantages for addressing certain types of questions.

a. QUESTIONS ABOUT MEAN LEVELS OF CHARACTERISTIC, WHEN CONTINUOUS BUT FLUCTUATING (e.g., mood).

b. QUESTIONS ABOUT FREQUENCY OR DURATION OF DISCRETE EVENTS, WHEN FREQUENT AND NOT SALIENT (e.g., social interactions).

c. QUESTIONS ABOUT VARIABILITY, WITHIN-PERSON ASSOCIATIONS,OR TIME COURSE (e.g., stress and BG).

d. QUESTIONS ABOUT SETTING DIFFERENCES OR EFFECTSOF ENVIRONMENTAL EVENTS ON BEHAVIOR (e.g., triggers of relapse).

4. EMA approaches may not be advantageous

in all circumstances.

a. WHEN CHARACTERISTIC IS NOT EXPECTED TO VARYACROSS TIME OR SETTING (trait adjectives, e.g., “dependable”).

b. WHEN EVENT OCCURS SO RARELY NOT LIKELY TO BE CAPTURED WITH MONITORING (e.g., doctor’s apppointments).

c. WHEN EVENT IS SO SALIENT, NOT LIKELY TO BEMISSED (e.g., AICD event).

d. WHEN PERSON IS NOT LIKELY TO HAVE ACCESS TO THE EVENT OR CHARACTERISTIC (e.g., anger inhibition).

4. EMA approaches may have some limitations.

a. CORRELATION VS. CAUSATION

NEED TO CONSIDER THIRD FACTORS (e.g., BP at work)

b. COMPLIANCE ISSUES

c. REACTIVITY OF ASSESSMENT

d. SAMPLE REPRESENTATIVENESS

e. LIMITATIONS OF SELF-REPORT

1. Time-based vs. event-based.

2. Fixed vs. random sampling.

3. Sampling frequency, sampling coverage.

4. Combination assessment strategies.

5. Reporting platform (electronic vs. paper).

6. Factors affecting subject burden.

7. Factors affecting compliance.

8. Reactivity of assessment.

DESIGN FEATURES

1. Time-based vs. event-based.

Continuous= time-based approaches

Discrete= event-based approaches

Advantages of event-based:

Can capture a high proportion of events

Disadvantages of event-based:

Much is left up to the subject

Disincentive to be thorough

Behavioral samples are not representative;

cannot generalize to person-level characteristics

2. Fixed vs. random sampling.

Fixed:

• Can synchronize with other devices.

• Helpful for data analysis?

• Assessments more predictable.

Random:

• Ensures that samples are representative.

• Interviews less likely to be entrained to regular events, e.g., hourly

appointments.

3. Sampling frequency, sampling coverage.

What frequency is optimal?

Is end-of-day sampling sufficient?

Parkinson et al., 1995, PSPB, 21, 331-339.

30 Sx, momentary (every 2 hrs) vs. End of Day

Within-S corrs over 2 week period

Negative Affect r =.68

Positive Affect r =.66

Evidence for independent contribution of peak, recent, and concurrent mood on end of day ratings.

4. Combination Assessment Strategies.

Environmental, cognitive, or emotional correlates of behavioral events (e.g., relationship between stress and relapse)

Event-based sampling (“record stress level prior to smoking”) not sufficient; what is the base rate of stress?

Combine with time-based random sampling (what is average stress rating throughout the day).

CASE-CROSSOVER DESIGN

Shiffman & Waters (2004). JCCP, 72, 192-201.

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Options:

Paper-pencil diaries

Watches, pagers

PDAs

Interactive Voice Response (IVRs)

Cell Phones

PC and web-based response devices

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Options:

Paper-pencil diaries

Watches, pagers

PDAs

Interactive Voice Response (IVRs)

Cell Phones

PC and web-based response devices

Most widely used

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Advantages of PDAs

1. Time stamped responses

--Permits synchronization with physiological data.

2. Auditory prompts

--Enhances timely responding.

3. Electronic data reduces data entry errors.

4. Programmed code permits greater flexibility in item administration.

-- Filter questions, protocol changes.

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Advantages of PDAs

5. Subject preference

Hufford (in press)

4 studies, diverse samples

Exposed to EDs vs. paper

Average of 75 % preferred EDs

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Advantages of PDAs

Stone et al. (2002) BMJ, 324, 1193-1194.

80 chronic pain patients

Instrumented diary vs. Compliance-enhanced ED

Pain records 3 X/day, 10, 4, 8 +/- 15 mins.

5. Reporting Platform (Electronic vs. Paper).

Advantages of PDAs

According to paper diary records: 90 % of assessments were compliant.

According to photosensors: 11 % were compliant.

6. Factors Affecting Subject Burden.

Hufford (in press):

a. Density of sampling.

b. Length of assessments.

c. User-interface.

d. Complexity of assessments.

e. Duration of monitoring period.

f. Stability of reporting platform.

7. Factors Affecting Compliance.

Hufford & Shields (2002). Applied Clinical Trials, 11, 46-56.

76 peer reviewed publications using electronic diaries.

44 % report compliance rates.

Of these, most report > 80 % compliance.

7. Factors Affecting Compliance.

Hufford & Shiffman (2003) Disease Mngment and Health Outcomes, 11, 77-86.

a. Build compliance into protocol.

b. Subject training.

c. User-interface.

d. ‘Drive’ the protocol.

e. Guide subjects through assessments.

f. ‘Livability’ functions.

g. Create sense of accountability.

7. Factors Affecting Compliance.

h. Participant support

(continuity of “care,” accessibility, flexible scheduling, reminder cards, ongoing telephone contacts, etc.)

8. Reactivity of Assessment

Stone et al. (2003).Pain, 104, 343-351.

91 chronic pain pts. Monitored 2 weeks

4 conditions:

3 x day, 6 x day, 12 x day, no momentary

18 questions about pain and activities

Stone et al. (2003).

Pain, 104, 343-351.

8. Reactivity of Assessment

Within-subject comparison

N = 118, 16 prompts (45 min) + ABP vs.

5 prompts (135 min)

Adjacent weekend days. Scales= 1-4

16 5 p value

Negative Affect 1.4 1.4 .13

Arousal 3.2 3.1 .002

Demand 1.5 1.5 .99

Control 2.9 3.0 .16

Social Conflict 1.2 1.2 .92

8. Reactivity of Assessment

Kamarck et al (unpublished)

Within-subject comparison

N = 118, 16 prompts (45 min) + ABP vs. 5 prompts (135 min)

Adjacent weekend days. Scales= 1-4

16 5 p value

Negative Affect 1.4 1.4 .13

Arousal 3.2 3.1 .002

Demand 1.5 1.5 .99

Control 2.9 3.0 .16

Social Conflict 1.2 1.2 .92

REPORTING GUIDELINES

Stone & Shiffman (2002). Annals Beh Med, 24, 236-243.

1. Sampling

2. Data Collection Procedures

3. Data Acquisition Interface

4. Compliance

5. Participant Training

6. Data Management

7. Data Analysis

RECOMMENDED ASSESSMENTS

1. Comparable end-of-day and global reports

2. Compliance data

e.g., proportion of responses completed, missed prompts, abandoned interviews, delayed interviews

3. Subjective burdenHow much of a burden was it to participate in the study?

How willing would you be to participate in a study like this again?

How much did participating in this study interfere with your usual activities?

4. Previous computer use