ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author ·...

30
Establishing discourse referents 1 Establishing discourse referents: indefinite noun phrases in German pre schoolers’ narratives* *This work was partly supported by the German Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung, Grant 01UG0711. I would like to thank my colleagues at the Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft in Berlin, Dagmar Bittner, Natalia Gagarina and Milena Kühnast for helpful comments. Special thanks go to Ute Bohnacker for insightful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. Insa Gülzow Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft Schützenstr. 18 D10117 Berlin

Transcript of ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author ·...

Page 1: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  1  

   Establishing   discourse   referents:   indefinite   noun   phrases   in   German   pre-­‐schoolers’  narratives*      *This  work  was  partly  supported  by  the  German  Bundesministerium  für  Bildung  und  Forschung,  Grant  01UG0711.  I  would  like  to  thank  my  colleagues  at  the  Zentrum  für  Allgemeine  Sprachwissenschaft  in  Berlin,  Dagmar  Bittner,  Natalia  Gagarina  and  Milena  Kühnast  for  helpful  comments.  Special  thanks  go  to  Ute  Bohnacker  for  insightful  comments  on  an  earlier  draft  of  this  paper.    Insa  Gülzow  Zentrum  für  Allgemeine  Sprachwissenschaft  Schützenstr.  18    D-­‐10117  Berlin      

Page 2: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  2  

   Abstract  The use of indefinite noun phrases by German children aged 1;11-5;05 and adults was investigated with a story-telling task in a situation of mutual knowledge. While adults produced typical antecedent constructions, two-year-olds used indefinite noun phrases mainly as nominal predicates in naming constructions. Children in the older age groups used less indefinite noun phrases and produced higher proportions of definite expressions. Especially the three-year-olds used comparatively high proportions of pronominal determiners for referent introductions. The results suggest that children initially employ a context-bound (naming) strategy and overestimate the role of deictic pronouns and mutual knowledge in a narrative task. The strategy becomes more cohesion-oriented in the older children and displays the basic requirements of referential linking, but still lacks the systematic use of indefinite noun phrases for referent introduction.

Page 3: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  3  

Introduction  

The  use  of  an  indefinite  noun  phrase  (NP)  is  dependent  on  knowledge  both  on  the  level  

of   morphosyntax   and   pragmatics.   In   naming   structures   like   (1)   for   instance,   the  

indefinite  NP  acts  as  a  predicate  identifying  the  class  membership  of  a  referent,  while  in  

(2)  the  indefinite  NP  is  an  argument  introducing  a  discourse-­‐new  referent.  

(1)   That’s  a  fox.  

(2)   And  then  the  bird  notices  a  fish  on  the  table  and  goes  for  it.  

The   current   study  will   explore   the   role   of   these   two   use   types   in   the   early   narrative  

productions  of  German  children.  Previous  studies  in  various  languages  have  shown  that  

indefinite  NPs  as  part  of  naming  structures  appear  early  at  about  two  years  of  age  (e.g.  

Kupisch,  2006,  Rozendaal  &  Baker,  2008  for  spontaneous  production;  Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  

1979   for   experimental   data).   The   appearance   of   indefinite   determiners   to   mark   a  

discourse-­‐new  referent  on   the  other  hand   is  dependent  both  on   the   text   type  and   the  

children’s   ability   of   taking   the   listener’s   perspective   into   account.   Preschool   English  

children   mark   discourse-­‐new   referents   with   indefinite   NPs   in   spontaneous   speech  

(Rozendaal   &   Baker,   2010)   and   in   elicitation   experiments   (Schaeffer   &   Matthewson,  

2005)   before   they   do   so   in   narratives   (Hickmann,   2003)   and   French   nine-­‐year-­‐olds  

master  the  use  of  indefinite  NPs  in  no  mutual  knowledge  (NMK)  contexts  before  eleven-­‐

year-­‐olds   do   so   in  mutual   knowledge   (MK)   contexts   (Kail   &   Hickmann,   1992).   These  

findings  suggest  that  the  predicative  use  of  indefinite  NPs  and  the  use  of  indefinite  NPs  

as  arguments  in  a  narrative  MK  context  lie  relatively  far  apart.  Studies  exploring  German  

children’s  narrative  strategies  show  similar  results.  Bamberg  (1986)  found  that  children  

around  age  three  hardly  use  indefinite  NPs  for  referent  introduction  in  a  MK  context  at  

all   and   it   is   reported   by   Hickmann   (2003)   that   the   use   of   indefinite   NPs   for   first  

mentions   approaches   adults’   distribution   around   age   seven   in   a   NMK   context.   The  

Page 4: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  4  

results  of  a  study  by  Gülzow  and  Gagarina  (2007)  suggest  that  at  phases  in  development  

when   children   are   still   learning   to   differentiate   and   express   perspectives   other   than  

their  own,  indefinite  NPs  are  used  in  functions  other  than  the  head  of  a  referential  chain.  

Similar  proportions  of  indefinite  NPs  are  documented  in  the  narrative  data  of  two-­‐year-­‐

olds  and  adults   tested   in  a  MK  context.  Clearly,   the  use  of   indefinite  NPs  by   two-­‐year-­‐

olds  contrasts  with  the  near  absence  of  this  expression  in  older  children  and  calls  for  an  

explanation.  

  The  present  study  will  focus  on  how  children  who  are  in  the  process  of  acquiring  

their   target   system   of   referential   expressions   use   indefinite   NPs   in   a   narrative   MK  

context.   As   it   seems   that   in   this   context   a   gap   occurs   between   the   children’s   use   of  

indefinite  NPs  as  predicates  and  as  arguments,  the  study  will  also  address  the  children’s  

use   of   other   referential   expressions.   It   is   well   known   that   children   use   the   definite  

determiner  (e.g.  de  Cat,  2009;  Hickmann,  2003;  Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  1979;  Maratsos,  1974,  

1976;   Rozendaal   &   Baker,   2008;   Schafer   &   de   Villiers,   2000;  Wigglesworth,   1990)   in  

places   where   the   indefinite   determiner   would   be   (more)   appropriate.   Less   is   known  

about   children’s   use   of   pronouns   instead   of   indefinite   NPs,   but   Bamberg   (1986)   for  

instance  found  that  German  children  use  pronouns  to  introduce  referents  in  narratives.  

These   children   generally   mark   the   main   protagonist   with   pronouns   irrespective   of  

whether   the   character   is   introduced,   reintroduced,   or   reference   is   maintained.   The  

overuse   of   the   definite   determiner   on   the   other   hand   is   explained   by   the   children’s  

failure   to   take   the   listener’s   perspective   into   account   (e.g.   de   Cat,   2009),   or   an  

overestimation  of  the  role  of  deixis  (e.g.  Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  1979).  Deixis  is  a  characteristic  

element  of  children’s  early  references  and  precedes  referential  linking.  Karmiloff-­‐Smith  

(1979)   noted   in   her   experimental   studies   that   children   use   definite   determiners   to  

signal   the   deictic   function   before   they   use   them   to   signal   an   anaphoric   relation.   In  

Page 5: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  5  

German,   pronominal   reference   is   possible   with   two   forms   that   mark   the   same  

distinction:  while  one  form  displays  a  clear  deictic  component,  the  other  appears  mainly  

as   an   anaphor.   If   at   early   stages   of   development   children   overuse   definite   forms   and  

preferably  encode  deixis,  it  can  be  expected  that  not  only  definite  determiners  are  used  

to  introduce  referents  in  a  MK  context  but  also  pronominal  forms  encoding  deixis.  

 

The  German  system  of  referring  expressions  

Pronominal   reference   usually   signals   an   anaphoric   relationship   to   a   previously  

mentioned   referent,   but  pronouns   can  also  be  used   for   first  mentions   together  with   a  

deictic  gesture.  In  German  the  personal  pronoun  ermasc/siefem/esneut   ‘he/she/it’  (PRO)  or  

the   PRONOMINAL   DETERMINER   dermasc/diefem/dasneut   (PRO   det)   can   be   used   to   indicate   a  

referent  present  in  the  shared  discourse.  The  term  DEMONSTRATIVE  is  avoided  here,  as  it  

may  cause  confusion  with  the  proximal/distal  pair  of  pronominal  expressions  in  German  

dieserprox/jenerdist   ‘this/that’.   While   deixis   easily   relates   the   pronominal   determiners  

dermasc/diefem/dasneut   to   referents   in   MK   contexts,   deictic   personal   pronouns  

ermasc/siefem/esneut  ‘he/she/it’  are  the  dis-­‐preferred  option.  

 

(3)     DERmasc/ERmasc  (pointing  at  one  student)   kam   zu     spät.  

  PRO  det/PRO         came   too   late  

  ‘HE  was  late’.  

 

In   a   narrative,   deictic   reference   is   not   common   even   if   the   referents   are   present   or  

provided   on   pictures   and   pointing   would   in   principle   be   an   option.   However,   the  

different   deictic   potentials   clearly   separate   the   two   German   pronominal   forms   into   a  

more   exophoric   (pronominal   determiner)   and   a  more   endophoric   (personal   pronoun)  

form.   In   the   present   study   the   distribution   of   different   referential   expressions   is  

Page 6: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  6  

analyzed.  If  the  distributional  analysis  shows  an  effect  of  the  er/der  contrast  in  a  phase  

were  children  overuse  definite  determiners   this  will  add   to  our  understanding  of  how  

children   master   the   shift   from   a   predominantly   context-­‐bound,   deictic   system   of  

reference   to   a   system   displaying   the   potential   of   marking   more   textually   oriented  

reference.  Indefinite  NPs  that  occur  as  arguments  in  anaphoric  chains  clearly  represent  

a  relatively  advanced  stage  in  this  development.  The  predicative  use  of  indefinite  NPs  in  

naming  structures  has  so  far  not  been  related  to  the  development  of  referential  linking  

or  deictic  reference.  If  naming  structures  do  not  represent  an  isolated  use  type,  but  are  

an   integral   part   of   young   children’s   reference   systems,   it   should   be   possible   to   relate  

them  to  the  children’s  early  deictic  phase  as  well.  

 

Naming  structures  and  deixis  

In  his  classical  study,  Brown  (1973:  347f)  argued  that  in  naming  structures  like  That’s  a  

train  or  That’s  a  bear  children  identify  a  referent  by  its  lexical  label  in  a  MK  context.  This  

reflects  the  on-­‐going  process  of  acquiring  an  entity’s  label.  Once  the  child  has  acquired  

the  name  of  the  category,  the  definite  determiner  can  be  used  as  in  Look  at  the  train  and  

Look  at   the  bear.   Karmiloff-­‐Smith   studied   the   acquisition   of   French   determiners   in   an  

extensive  number  of  experiments  and  found  that  the  “indefinite  article   is   initially  used  

as   part   of   a   procedure   for   naming,   i.e.   in   its   appellative   or   nominative   function”  

(Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  1979:  216).  While  Brown  makes  his  observation  on  the  basis  of  whole  

sentences   like  That’s  a   train,   Karmiloff-­‐Smith’s   data   consists   of   indefinite  NPs   only   as  

children  answered  questions   like  What’s  in  the  bag?   for   instance  with  A  doll.  This   is  an  

important  point  to  note  as  Karmiloff-­‐Smith  discusses  the  role  of  deixis  for  early  uses  of  

the   definite   determiner,   but   not   for   the   indefinite   determiner   although   naming  

structures  that  are  realized  by  a  whole  sentence  involve  a  deictic  element  in  preverbal  

Page 7: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  7  

position.   In   the  German   example  below,   the  preverbal   position   is   taken  by   the  deictic  

pronominal   determiner  dasneut   and   the   indefinite  NP   functions   as   a   nominal   predicate  

assigning  a  property  to  the  referent  of  the  subject  NP.  

 

(4)   Das   ist   EIN  BÄR.  

  that   is   a  bear  

  ‘That’s  a  bear’.  

 

The  main  function  of  this  construction  is  to  assign  class  membership  to  an  entity  that  is  

deictically   identified.   The   preverbal   position   can   also   be   taken   by   the   local   deictic  

expression  da.  

 

(5)   Da   ist   EIN  BÄR.  

there   is   a  bear  

  ‘There’s  a  bear’.  

 

German  da   allows  both  a  deictic  and  an  existential   interpretation.   In  a  MK  context,  da  

can  be  deictic:   the   location   indicated  by  da   can  be   associated  with   the   referent   of   the  

indefinite   NP.   Together   with   examples   like   (4),   these   (with   deictic   da)   will   be   called  

NAMING  STRUCTURES  in  the  present  study.  

 

Indefinite  NPs  in  pre-­‐  and  postverbal  position  

Naming  structures  realize  the  NEW-­‐LAST  PRINCIPLE  (cf.  Hickmann,  2003:  61);  the  indefinite  

NP  presents  new  information  in  post-­‐verbal  position.  Küntay  and  Koçbaş  (2009)  show  

that   in   the   narrative   data   of   preschool   English   and   Turkish   children   “a   very   high  

percentage  of  presentational  constructions  attract  the  indefinite  marker”  and  conclude  

that   the   choice   of   the   indefinite   determiner   is   related   to   the   choice   of   construction  

Page 8: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  8  

(Küntay  &  Koçbaş,  2009:  90).   In  the  study  of  Hickmann  (2003:  231)  and  similar  to  the  

Küntay  and  Koçbaş  study,  most  post-­‐verbal  referent  introductions  are  marked  with  the  

indefinite  determiner  by  German  and  English  children   from  seven  years  onwards.   It   is  

therefor   likely  that  the  preschool  children  in  the  present  study  prefer  to  use  indefinite  

NPs   in  post-­‐verbal  position,  both   in  naming  structures  and  as   referring  arguments.  As  

referring   arguments,   indefinite   NPs   can   represent   post-­‐verbal   objects   and   subjects;  

examples  will  be  given  in  the  results  section.    

The  current  study  will  address  German  children’s  early  use  of  indefinite  NPs  in  a  MK  

narrative.   It   will   focus   on   the   changes   that   can   be   observed   in   the   children’s   use   of  

indefinite   NPs   as   predicates   and   as   arguments   and   on   the   co-­‐occurring   distributional  

changes   regarding   the   children’s   use   of   definite   referring   expressions.   In   a   narrative,  

information   about   hearer   knowledge  may   be   in   conflict  with   the   successful   use   of   an  

indefinite  NP  produced  in  a  MK  context  and  the  children  must  be  willing  to  violate  the  

rule   linking   indefinite   NPs   to   NMK   contexts   in   spontaneous   speech.   On   the   basis   of  

previous  results,  the  following  questions  will  be  addressed  in  this  study:    

 

(A) How  is  the  transition  from  using  indefinite  NPs  as  highly  informative  naming  

devices  to  using  indefinite  NPs  as  referring  arguments  reflected  in  the  data?    

(B) How  does  the  distribution  of  definite  NPs  (definite  determiners,  pronominal  

determiners,  personal  pronouns)  correlate  with  this  transition?  

 

Method  

Participants  

The   narrative   data   of   five   age   groups   was   collected   and   analyzed.   A   total   of   60   pre-­‐

schoolers   between   age   two   and   five   and   15   adults   took   part   in   the   study.   Due   to  

Page 9: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  9  

technical  problems  only  13  of  the  transcripts  in  the  age  group  of  five-­‐year-­‐olds  could  be  

used.    

 

Table  1:  Age  and  age  range  of  subjects  

 

 

Materials  

The   narrative   was   elicited   using   a   sequence   of   six   pictures   that   featured   a   bird,   the  

skeleton  of  a  fish  and  a  fox  as  well  as  a  tree  and  a  table  (see  Appendix  1).  The  pictures  

were  designed  to  offer  opportunities  for  story  telling  and  were  mounted  onto  six  pieces  

of   cardboard.   Choosing   protagonists   with   male   gender   in   German   eliminated   gender  

effects   (der   Fuchs   ‘the   fox’,   der   Vogel   ‘the   bird’,   der   Fisch   ‘the   fish’).   Although   little  

reference   to   the   inanimate   entities  was   expected,   these  were   also   controlled   for  male  

gender   (der   Tisch   ‘the   table,   der   Baum   ‘the   tree’).   To   avoid   an   effect   of   marking   the  

protagonist  by  one  kind  of  NP  (cf.  Bamberg,  1986),  active  and  passive  involvement  of  the  

fox  and  the  bird  in  the  action  was  distributed  as  evenly  as  possible.  

 

Procedure  

The children were normal hearing, monolingual speakers of German and recruited at

Kindergartens in Berlin and Potsdam, Germany. The adults were University students from

Berlin, also normal hearing, and monolingual speakers of German with no prior experience in

this kind of task. The parents of the children filled out a questionnaire documenting language

development and the languages spoken at home in order to avoid bilingual or multilingual

speakers and children with language impairment. Additionally, a reduced version of a

standardized test was used to assess typical language development in German (Kauschke &

Page 10: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  10  

Siegmüller, 2009). The children were seen individually by one experimenter in a quiet room

with a table, two chairs and a video camera that recorded the children’s narration and filmed

their hands to capture all instances of pointing. The child and the experimenter sat opposite

each other at a low distance with no barrier between them to emphasize the situation of shared

attention. Once the children seemed comfortable at the table, the experimenter asked them to

take part in a story-telling task: Ich habe Dir ein paar Bilder mitgebracht, Du kannst gleich

eine Geschichte dazu erzählen ‘Here are some pictures for you to tell a story in a moment’.

Before the onset of the actual story-telling the children were presented the sequence of six

pictures in a row. Erstmal zeige ich dir alle Bilder, damit Du weißt, was in der Geschichte

passiert ‘First, I will show you all the pictures so that you know what happens in the story’.

When the children were ready to tell the story they were presented the first picture. Hier ist

das erste Bild. Was passiert denn da? ‘This is the first picture. What is happening’? After the

children had finished with the first, the second picture was positioned to the right side of the

first and served as the new stimulus. The subsequent pictures were presented in a way that the

picture on the left (e.g. the first) was taken away so that the subsequent picture (e.g. the

second) could take its place. Then the new picture (e.g. the third) would be placed on the right

and serve as the stimulus. Apart from the very first picture children always had two pictures

in front of them: the previous picture and the one serving as the current stimulus for the

narrative. The experimenter supported the production of utterances by encouraging the

participants and praising their performance Was passiert denn da? Prima. Und hier? ‘What’s

happening? Very good. And here’? Constant feedback also served to give the children a sense

of common ground. The experimenter avoided sentences that included NPs referring to the

participants so that the children were not influenced by the experimenter’s choice. The adults

were treated to the same procedure and were encouraged in the same way as the children.

This results in an experimental situation that is somewhat unnatural for the adults and

findings cannot be interpreted taking the adults’ performance to represent the norm. However,

Page 11: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  11  

as adults behaved in the way they did, it will be assumed that the children’s productions

approach adult distributions in the course of language development.

It   may   seem   a   daunting   task   to   ask   two-­‐year-­‐olds   to   produce   a   narrative,   but   we  

wanted   to   explore   the   narrative   strategies   of   young   children   before   they   conform   to  

adult   standards.   The   procedure   was   chosen   because   very   young   children   could   be  

overburdened   by   looking   at   too   many   pictures   at   once.   By   reducing   the   number   of  

pictures  we  hoped  to  elicit  more  references  as  the  children  concentrated  on  one  picture  

at  a  time  and  could  only  switch  to  the  next  when  the  experimenter  decided  to  let  them  

do  so.  As   focusing  attention  on  one  picture  may  discourage  children   from  producing  a  

coherent  narrative  and  lead  them  into  describing  individual  pictures  instead  (cf.  de  Cat,  

2011),  we  hoped  to  support  attempts  at  referential  linking  between  pictures  by  making  

the  previous  picture  available.  

 

Results  

Types  of  constructions  with  indefinite  NPs  

The   types  of  utterances   in  which   indefinite  NPs  occur  will  be  analyzed   in   this   section.  

The  examples  below  are  taken  from  the  adult  data  of  the  present  study  (expect  for  the  

first  and  the  second)  and  represent  the  different  categories  used  for  analysis.  The  first  

category  includes  examples  of  utterances  without  verbs:    

 

(6)   Ein  Fisch.   child  130:  2;4,6  [without  verb]  

  a  fish  

  ‘A  fish’.  

 

Among  the  utterances  with  verbs,  predicative  naming  structures  constitute  the  next  

category  [NAM].  

Page 12: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  12  

 

(7)     Das   hier   ist   EIN  RABE.   child  76:  2;08,01  [Nam]  

    that   here   is   a  raven  

    ‘That’s  a  raven  here’.  

 

Naming  structures  observe  the  new-­‐last  principle.  So  do  structures  with  indefinite  NPs  

as   referring   arguments   that   appear   as   objects   in   post-­‐verbal   position   [svO];   a   capital  

bold  letter  indicates  the  indefinite  NP.  

 

(8)     Also,   der  Vogel     klaut   EINE  FISCHGRÄTE.     adult  13,  [svO]  

    well   the  bird     steal   a  fishbone  

    ‘Well,  the  bird  is  stealing  a  fishbone’.  

 

A   subject   indefinite   NP   in   preverbal   position   indicates   newness   not   by   position   but  

morphological  marking;  both  a  post-­‐verbal  object  [Svo]  or  a  post-­‐verbal  adverbial  [Svx]  

is  possible.  

 

(9)     EIN  VOGEL  sieht   das   und...   adult  16,  [Svo]  

    a  bird     see     that   and  

    ‚This  is  seen  by  a  bird  and…’.  

(10)   EIN  VOGEL  fliegt   über  die  Wiese.             adult  12,  [Svx]  

    a  bird     fly     across  the  lawn  

    ‘A  bird  is  flying  across  the  lawn’.  

 

In   German,   subject-­‐verb   inversion   occurs   if   some   other   element   occupies   the   first  

position  [xvS].  

 

Page 13: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  13  

(11)   Auf  dem  Teller   liegt   EIN    TOTER  FISCH.         adult  12,  [xvS]  

    on  the  plate     lie     a  dead  fish  

    ‘A  dead  fish  is  lying  on  the  plate’.  

 

A   first   step   compares   the   number   of   utterances   without   verbs   to   the   number   of  

utterances  with   verbs   (Figure  1).   In   this   first   analysis,   the   categories   [Nam],   [svO],   [Svo],  

[Svx],  and  [xvS]  are  collapsed  into  the  category  ‘with  verbs’.  

 

Figure  1:  Use  of  indefinite  NPs  with  and  without  verbs  

 

Figure   1   shows   that   the   majority   of   indefinite   NPs   used   by   two-­‐year-­‐olds   are  

utterances  that  contain  no  further  constituent  (81%,  N=62).  While  structures  with  verbs  

increase   proportionally   across   age   groups,   raw   numbers   show   that   their   overall   use  

decreases.   In   the  next   step,   the  children’s  utterances  with  verbs  will  be  analyzed  with  

regard  to  the  type  of  construction  that  contains  the  indefinite  NP  (Figure  2).  

 

Figure  2:  Clause  structure  of  utterances  with  indefinite  NPs  

 

Figure  2  shows  that  two-­‐year-­‐olds  exclusively  produce  naming  structures  (12)  when  

using   an   indefinite   NP   together   with   a   verb.   Three-­‐year-­‐olds   and   four-­‐year-­‐olds   use  

naming  structures  in  a  majority  of  cases  while  in  the  data  of  the  five-­‐year-­‐olds  indefinite  

NPs  are  mainly  used  as  referring  arguments.  In  the  data  of  the  adults  there  is  only  one  

example   of   a   naming   structure,   the   remainder   are   indefinite   NPs   used   as   referring  

arguments.    

 

(12)   Hier   ist   EIN    VOGEL.   child  105:  2;07,17  

    here   is   a  bird  

Page 14: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  14  

    ‚Here’s  a  bird’.  

 

Of  the  few  examples  of  indefinite  NPs  used  as  referring  arguments  that  are  documented  

in   the   data   of   the   children,  most   occur   in   post-­‐verbal   position.   In   a  majority   of   cases,  

these  indefinite  NPs  are  objects  rather  than  subjects.  

 

(13)   Und   da     hat     der  Vogel  EIN  FISCH.       child  79:  4;05,28  

    and   there   has   the  bird   a  fish  

    ‘And  the  bird  has  a  fish  there’.  

 

In   the   adults’   data,   utterances   are   almost   evenly   distributed   between   the   three  

categories  svO,  Svo/Svx  and  xvS.  Thus  in  a  majority  of  cases  indefinite  NPs  are  subjects  

rather   than   objects,   but   only   about   half   of   the   subjects   occur   in   topic   position.   In   the  

children’s   data,   examples   with   subject   indefinite   NPs   in   preverbal   position   are   not  

documented.  

 

NP  types  in  first  mentions  

Six   categories   were   used   for   analysis:   indefinite   NPs   like   ein   Vogel   ‘a   bird’   (14)   and  

definite  NPs  like  der  Vogel  ‘the  bird’  (15);  for  the  children  bare  nominals  like  Fisch  ‘fish’  

were   also   included   (16).   Pronominal   NPs   included   pronominal   determiners   like   der  

‘PRO   det’   (17)   and   personal   pronouns   like   er   ‘he’   (18).   In   German,   it   is   not   easy   to  

distinguish   between   an   utterance   with   the   pronominal   determiner   dermasc   and   the  

personal   pronoun   ermasc   if   the   preceding  word   ends   in   an   alveolar   plosive  dann  rennt  

er/der   ‘the  he  runs’.  The  decision  was  made  for  the  personal  pronoun  er  if  a  pause  was  

detectable   before   the   pronominal   form.   The   rest   category   includes   other   types   of  

Page 15: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  15  

referring   expressions   such   as   the   reflexive   pronoun   sich   ‘x-­‐self’   and   null   subjects,   or  

demonstrative  dieser  ‘this’.  

 

(14)   Und   das   ist   EIN  VOGEL…   child  28:  3;0,22  

    and   that   is   a  bird  

    ‘And  that’s  a  bird’.  

(15)   DER  VOGEL   will   den  Fuchs   auffressen.   child  28:  3;0,22  

    the  bird     wants   the  fox     eat  

    ‘The  bird  wants  to  eat  the  fox’.  

(16)   Da     hat     der     FISCH   gefangt.   child  20:  3;4,2  

    there   has   PRO  det     fish   caught  

    ‘There  he  has  caught  the  fish’.  

(17)   DER     fällt   ins     Wasser   child  48:  4;0,10  

    PRO  det   falls   into     water  

    ‘He  falls  into  the  water.  

(18)   Dann   will   ER   den  Raben   fressen.   child  53:  4;5,17  

    then   want   he   the  raven     eat  

  ‘Then  he  wants  to  eat  the  raven’.  

 

NPs  that  refer   to  a  participant   for   the   first   time  are  regarded  as   first  mentions.  NPs  

that  do  not  refer  to  a  participant  for  the  first  time  are  regarded  as  subsequent  mentions.  

In  the  present  study,  subsequent  mentions  are  not  further  differentiated  into  reference  

maintenances   and   referent   reintroductions.   The   design   of   the   study   is   an   inadequate  

testing   ground   for   the  maintenance/reintroduction   contrast   as   the   three   participants  

closely  interact,  they  all  have  male  gender  and  there  is  no  main  protagonist.  It  has  been  

shown  that  the  need  to  resolve  ambiguity  in  this  study  setting  leads  to  the  use  of  lexical  

expressions  even  in  cases  where  reference  is  maintained  (cf.  Bittner  &  Kühnast,  2012).  

Page 16: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  16  

This   aspect  will   be   returned   to   in   the  discussion  where   results   are  also   related   to   the  

Bamberg  (1986)  study.  

 

Table  2:  NPs  used  for  first  mentions  

 

Table  2  shows  that  references  are  mostly  nominal  (bare  nominals,  NPs  with  definite  

or   indefinite   determiners)   and   that   none   of   the   participants   in   the   study   use   the  

pronouns   ermasc/siefem/esneut   ‘he/she/it’   for   first   mention   of   a   referent.   Two-­‐year-­‐olds  

predominantly  use  bare  nominals  and  indefinites  for  first  mentions.  Across  age  groups,  

the   use   of   bare   nominals   decreases   with   age,   which   is   a   clear   sign   of   the   children’s  

reference   systems   becoming   more   adult-­‐like.   The   proportion   of   indefinite   NPs   also  

decreases,  which  sharply  contrasts  with  the  adults’  data  where  indefinite  NPs  are  used  

for  first  mentions  in  a  majority  of  cases.  Across  age  groups  children  increase  their  use  of  

the  definite  determiner.  While  adults  do  not  use  pronominal  NPs  for  first  mentions  at  all,  

children  use  the  pronominal  determiner  dermasc/diefem/dasneut.  Especially  the  data  of  the  

three-­‐year-­‐olds   displays   a   high   proportion   of   pronominal   determiners   for   referent  

introduction.  

 

Table  3:  NPs  used  for  subsequent  mentions  

 

NP  types  in  subsequent  mentions  

 

In   subsequent   mentions   children   of   all   age   groups   produce   all   five   referential  

expression  types.  Adults  use  no  indefinite  NPs  or  bare  nominals,  but  rely  on  NPs  with  a  

definite   determiner   in   a   majority   of   cases.   Quite   to   the   contrary,   two-­‐year-­‐olds  

Page 17: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  17  

predominantly  use   indefinite  NPs  and  bare  nominals.  This  use  decreases  to  adult   level  

across  age  groups  for  both  the  indefinite  determiner  and  bare  nominals.  The  remaining  

referential   expressions   used   by   adults   are   personal   pronouns   and   pronominal  

determiners  plus  a  rest  category  that  mainly  consist  of  the  reflexive  pronoun  sich  ‘x-­‐self’  

(62   out   of   a   total   of   136).   In   contrast   to   the   adults,   all   age   groups   of   children   and  

especially   the   three-­‐year-­‐olds   use   a   high   amount   of   pronominal   determiners.   They  

increase  their  use  of  personal  pronouns  from  almost  zero  in  the  group  of  two-­‐year-­‐olds  

above  the  level  of  adults  in  the  group  of  five  year-­‐olds.  

When   comparing   first   with   subsequent  mentions  within   age   groups,   no   significant  

differences  were  found  for  the  group  of  two-­‐year-­‐olds  (χ2=6.55,  d.f.=4,  p=.161).  For  all  

other   age   groups   on   the   other   hand,   the   difference   between   first   and   subsequent  

mentions   is   significant   (three-­‐year-­‐olds:   χ2=26.87,   d.f.=4,   p<.001;   four-­‐year-­‐olds:  

χ2=54.13,   d.f.=4,   p<.001;   five-­‐year-­‐olds:   χ2=47.15,   d.f.=4,   p<.001;   adults,   χ2=411.2,  

d.f.=3,  p<.001).  This   finding  suggests   that   there   is  a  categorical   shift  between   the   two-­‐

year-­‐olds  and  the  older  children  in  that  the  youngest  children  do  not  differentiate  their  

marking  of  first  and  subsequent  reference  by  the  use  of  different  types  of  NPs.  Fisher’s  

exact  test  was  performed  across  ages  for  first  and  subsequent  mentions.  Results  reveal  

significant   differences   between   age   groups   both   for   first  mentions   (χ2=110.5,   d.f.=16,  

p<.05)  and  subsequent  mentions  (χ2=772.9,  d.f.=20,  p<.05).  

 

Discussion  

The  aim  of  the  present  study  is  to  determine  the  role  and  ontogenesis  of  indefinite  NPs  

in   young   German   children’s   narratives   produced   in   a   MK   context.   The   results   of   the  

study   suggest   that  preschool  German   children  who  are   faced  with   a  narrative   task  do  

not  use  indefinite  NPs  in  the  same  way  as  adults  do,  but  start  with  a  nominative  naming  

Page 18: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  18  

strategy.   The   oldest   children   in   the   study   have   not   yet   developed   full   control   of   the  

adults’   use   of   indefinite   NPs   as   antecedents   in   MK   contexts,   but   clearly,   they   have  

abandoned  the  naming  strategy.  In  the  following,  the  results  of  the  study  will  be  related  

to  the  two  questions  that  were  raised  in  the  introduction.  It  will  be  shown  that  despite  

the   near   absence   of   indefinite   NPs   in   the   older   children’s   data,   they   have   achieved  

important  steps  on  their  way  to  referential  linking.  

 

The  naming  function  in  German  narratives  

The  first  research  question  concerns  the  nature  of   the  children’s   transition   from  using  

indefinite  NPs  as  highly  informative  naming  devices  to  using  indefinite  NPs  as  referring  

arguments.   I   will   first   comment   on   the   distribution   of   the   two   use   types   across   age  

groups  and   then  briefly  discuss   their   relation   in   terms  of   the   syntactic  position  of   the  

indefinite  NP.  

Similar  to  findings  in  spontaneous  production  (e.g.  Brown,  1970;  Kupisch,  2006)  and  

experimental  data  (cf.  Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  1979)  the  children  in  the  study  initially  use  the  

indefinite   determiner   to   signal   the  naming   function.  As   long   as   children   fail   to   realize  

that  the  task  in  the  present  study  demands  the  production  of  a  cohesive  narrative,  or  the  

task   verges   on   their   linguistic   and   cognitive   abilities,   it   is   likely   that   they   rely   on   the  

conventions  of  spontaneous  speech.  The  two-­‐year-­‐olds  in  the  study  exclusively  produce  

naming   structures,   three-­‐   and   four-­‐year-­‐olds   use   indefinite   NPs   both   in   naming  

structures  and  as  referring  arguments,  five-­‐year-­‐olds  use  indefinite  NPs  predominantly  

as   referring   arguments,   and   adults   use   indefinite   NPs   only   as   referring   arguments.  

Clearly,   the   change   in   the   distribution   of   the   two   use   types   marks   a   developmental  

change  towards  a  more  adult  like  usage  of  indefinite  NPs.  

Page 19: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  19  

At   the  onset,   the   children’s  use  of   indefinite  NPs  mirrors   labeling   routines   that   can  

frequently  be  found  in  spontaneous  speech  (19).    

 

(19)   adult:  What’s  that?  

child:  A  fox.    

adult:  Yes,  and  what’s  that?    

child:  A  fox.  

 

The  example  shows  that  the  multiple  appearances  of  indefinite  NPs  in  places  other  than  

first  mentions   are   licensed   in   labeling.   Unlike   adults  who   use   indefinite  NPs   no  more  

than  once  to  introduce  a  new  referent  in  a  MK  narrative  context,  children  up  to  the  age  

of  four  produce  indefinite  determiners  both  in  first  and  subsequent  mentions.  

The   children’s  willingness   to   use   indefinite   NPs   in   subsequent  mentions   (with   and  

without   verbs)   correlates   with   a   general   predominance   of   naming   structures   over  

indefinite   NPs   used   as   referring   arguments.   That   is,   children   who   give   up   naming  

structures  in  MK  narratives  more  clearly  mark  subsequent  mentions  by  the  use  of  forms  

other  than  indefinite  NPs.  This  relation  is  especially  evident  in  the  two-­‐year-­‐olds:  these  

children  exclusively  produce  naming  structures  when  using  indefinite  NPs  together  with  

verbs   and   do   not   differentiate   between   first   and   subsequent  mentions   in   their   use   of  

referring  expressions  at  all.  All  other  age  groups  use   indefinite  NPs   (also)  as   referring  

arguments   and   show   a   distributional   difference   between   the   referential   expressions  

used  for  first  and  subsequent  mentions.  It  can  therefore  be  concluded  that  not  only  the  

first   appearance  of   the  use   type   itself   (indefinite  NP   as   referring   argument)  marks   an  

important  step  in  referential  linking,  but  its  appearance  in  the  group  of  three-­‐year-­‐olds  

Page 20: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  20  

coincidences   with   the   realization   of   a   basic   requirement   of   referential   linking:   the  

general  differentiation  of  first  and  subsequent  mentions.    

Parallel   to   the   increase   in   the   proportion   of   indefinite   NPs   used   as   referring  

arguments,  the  use  of  indefinite  NPs  generally  decreases  to  10%  (N=4)  in  the  oldest  age  

group  (cf.  Bamberg,  1986;  Hickmann,  2003).  The  somewhat  contradictory   finding   that  

children   increase   their   skills   regarding   the   formation   of   anaphoric   chains   and   expand  

the   variety   of   structures  with   indefinite  NPs   (see   below)  while   at   the   same   time   they  

seem   to   abandon   their   use   will   be   returned   to   when   discussing   the   second   research  

question.  

 

The  new-­‐last  principle  

Although  naming  structures  can  in  principle  serve  as  antecedents  in  referential   linking  

(That’s   a   fox.   The   fox   is   chasing   the  bird.),   the   adults   in   this   study   introduce   referents  

with   indefinite  NPs  used  as  referring  arguments.   In  these  structures,  subject   indefinite  

NPs  are  most  typical  with  30%  occurring  in  preverbal  (Svo/Svx)  and  32%  occurring  in  

post-­‐verbal  position  (xvS).  These  results  align  with  those  of  Hickmann  (2003:  202)  who  

found   that   the   German   adults   in   her   narrative   study   use   a   majority   of   post-­‐verbal  

subjects   in   subject-­‐verb   inversions   (about   50%)   and   a   high   number   of   preverbal  

subjects   (about   30%)   when   introducing   referents   with   an   indefinite   NP   (Hickmann,  

2003:   203f).   Regarding   position,   post-­‐verbal   indefinite   NPs   realize   the   NEW-­‐LAST  

PRINCIPLE  (Hickmann,  2003:  231)  and  are  typical  in  the  adult  data  of  the  present  study.  In  

addition  to  32%  post-­‐verbal  subjects  (xvS,  see  above)  adults  produce  another  24%  post-­‐

verbal  objects  (svO).  When  using  indefinite  NPs  as  referring  arguments,  children  do  not  

produce  examples  of  pre-­‐verbal  Svo  and  Svx  structures,  but  only  produce  examples  of  

Page 21: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  21  

xvS   (20),   (21)  and   svO   (22).  These   share   the  post-­‐verbal  position  of   the   indefinite  NP  

with  naming  structures  (23).  

 

(20)   Da   taucht     ein  Fuchs     auf.   adult  2,  [xvS]  

  there   appears   a  fox   verb.part  

  ‘A  fox  appears  there’.  

(21)   Auf  dem  Teller   liegt   ein  toter  Fisch.   adult  12,  [xvS]  

  On  the  plate   lies   a  dead  fish.  

  ‘A  dead  fish  lies  on  the  plate’.  

(22)   Der  Vogel   hat     einen  Fisch   auf  dem  Tisch   gefunden.                        child  189:  5;5,13,  [svO]  

  the  bird     has   a  fish     on  the  table   found  

  ‘The  bird  found  a  fish  on  the  table’.  

(23)   Da   ist   ein  Fuchs.   child  20:  3;4,2,  [Nam]  

  there   is   a  fox  

  ‘There  is  a  fox’.  

 

Besides   the   post-­‐verbal   position   of   the   indefinite   NP,   examples   of   xvS   often   exhibit   a  

sentence-­‐initial   locative   expression   that   they   share   with   naming   structures.   The  

sentence-­‐initial  position  can  either  be  filled  with  the  locative  da   ‘there’  (21)  typical  for  

naming   structures,   or   some   other   locative   expression   such   as   auf   dem   Teller   ‘on   the  

plate’   (22).   In   the   present   study,   children   older   than   two   produce   utterances  

representing   both   the   xvS   and   the   svO   category,   but   no   examples   of   Svo/Svx.   It   can  

therefore  be  argued  that  the  children  are  sensitive  to   input   frequencies  with  regard  to  

the  post-­‐verbal  position  of  indefinite  NPs  and  that  they  observe  the  new-­‐last  principle.  

The   children   gradually   expand   the   naming   structure   by   using   verbs   other   than   the  

copula   and   a  wider   variety   of   locative   expressions   in   xvS   structures   and  preserve   the  

post-­‐verbal  position  both  in  their  use  of  xvS  and  svO.    

Page 22: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  22  

 

Definite  referring  expressions  

The  second  research  question  concerns  the  use  of  definite  referring  expressions  and  the  

relation  of  their  distribution  to  the  children’s  transition  regarding  the  use  of   indefinite  

NPs  as  described  above.  I  will  discuss  the  general  distribution  of  referring  expressions  in  

each  age  group  and  comment  on  the  main  changes  between  adjacent  age  groups.  

 

Two-­‐year-­‐olds:  indefinite  NPs  and  naming  structures  

The  exclusive  use  of  indefinite  NPs  in  naming  structures  is  typical  for  the  two-­‐year-­‐olds  

whose   referential   systems   consist  mainly   of   bare   NPs,   NPs  with   definite   determiners  

and  especially  NPs  with  indefinite  determiners  (cf.  Table  2  and  Table  3).  These  children  

do  not  differentiate  between  first  and  subsequent  mentions  in  their  choice  of  referential  

expressions  and  anchor  reference  in  the  situational  context.  The  referential  systems  of  

two-­‐year-­‐olds  are  mainly  nominal  and  incorporate  deixis  when  using  naming  structures  

for  the  identification  of  referents.  

 

Three-­‐year-­‐olds:  pronominal  determiners  and  deictic  reference  

As  already  mentioned,   the  appearance  of   indefinite  NPs  as   referring  arguments   in   the  

data  of  the  three-­‐year-­‐olds  contributes  to  the  shift  towards  referential   linking  which  is  

also  documented   in   the   finding   that   different   distributions   of   forms  occur   in   first   and  

subsequent  mentions.  In  comparison  to  the  two-­‐year-­‐olds,  the  referential  systems  of  the  

three-­‐year  olds  become  more  definite  and  less  nominal;  uses  of  bare  nominals  and  the  

indefinite  determiner  decrease  and  uses  of  the  definite  determiner  and  the  pronominal  

determiner   increase   both   in   first   and   subsequent   mentions.   Children   are   gradually  

abandoning  the  naming  strategy  and  it  is  likely  that  they  begin  to  relate  referents  across  

Page 23: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  23  

pictures  in  order  to  construct  a  story  line.  The  definite  determiner  is  the  most  frequent  

expression   in   first   mentions   and   the   pronominal   determiner   is   the   most   frequent  

expression  in  subsequent  mentions  (cf.  Table  2  and  Table  3).  Clearly,  this  distribution  of  

forms   displays   some   basic   knowledge   of   adequately   ranking   expressions   to   encode  

different  degrees  of  hearer  knowledge  (cf.  Gundel,  Hedberg  &  Zacharski,  1993)  and  thus  

represents  another  step  towards  referential  linking.  In  comparison  to  the  adults,  the  use  

of   the   pronominal   determiner   for   first   mentions   in   narratives   is   atypical   (for  

spontaneous  production  see  Rozendaal  &  Baker,  2010)  and  will  be  interpreted  here  as  a  

symptom  of  a  referential  system  that  is  still  firmly  anchored  in  the  situational  context.  

Bamberg  (1986)  also  found  that  preschool  German  children  introduce  referents  with  

pronominal  forms  in  a  narrative  and  that  they  use  proportionally  more  pronouns  than  

adults   performing   the   same   task.   He   argues   that   the   children   in   his   study   use  

pronominal   forms   for   the   main   character   irrespective   of   whether   the   character   is  

introduced   or   reintroduced   or   reference   is  maintained.   Bamberg   collapsed   the  der/er  

forms  into  one  category,  which  makes  it   impossible  to  assign  different  functions  to  the  

forms  and  show  developmental  tendencies.  In  the  present  study  however  there  is  a  clear  

contrast:  pronominal  determiners  are  much  more  frequent  than  personal  pronouns  and  

it  is  only  the  pronominal  determiners  that  are  used  for  referent  introductions.  

 

The  deixis-­‐first  principle  

Contrary  to  the  Bamberg  study,  the  setting  of  the  present  study  does  not  feature  a  main  

protagonist,   but   active   and  passive   involvement  of   the   characters   ranking  high  on   the  

animacy  scale  (the  fox  and  the  bird)  is  balanced  (see  appendix  1).  This  study  setting  is  

therefore   an   inadequate   testing   ground   for   Bamberg’s   finding   as   children  may   find   it  

difficult   to   identify   a   main   character.   The   fact   that   the   results   resemble   Bamberg’s  

Page 24: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  24  

findings  nonetheless  in  addition  to  the  fact  that  only  pronominal  determiners  are  used  

in   first   mentions   suggests   a   strong   influence   of   factors   in   addition   to   or   other   than  

differences   between   the  marking   of  main   and   secondary   characters.   It   is   argued   here  

that   one   influence   on   the   referential   system   of   preschool   children   (among   others)   is  

what  will  be  called  the  DEIXIS-­‐FIRST  PRINCIPLE.  The  deixis-­‐first  principle  is  not  restricted  to  

deitic   expressions   such   as   the   pronominal   determiner,   but   can   affect   different  

expressions   at   different   stages   in   development.   In   the   case   of   naming   structures   it  

applies  to  a  whole  structure  and  not  to  an  individual  expression,  but  the  mechanism  is  

the  same:  indefinite  NPs  occur  in  structures  that  can  be  used  for  deictic  reference  before  

they  occur  in  structures  that  are  typical  for  referential  linking.  

Lyons  (1975)  argued  that  deixis  is  the  source  of  reference  and  that  the  anaphoric  use  

of   pronouns   is   derivative   of   their   function   as   deictics.   If   children’s   understanding   of  

referring   expressions   is   based   on   their   prior   understanding   of   deixis,   it   comes   to   no  

surprise   that   an   overuse   of   pronominal   determiners  with   a   strong   deictic   potential   is  

documented  in  the  data  of  the  younger  children  in  the  study.  It  can  be  argued  that  the  

children  use  pronominal  determiners  to  mark  a  salient  referent  in  the  common  ground  

available   to   both   speaker   and   hearer,   but   (still)   fail   to   realize   that   in   a   story-­‐telling  

narrative  the  use  of  pronominal  NPs  for  referent  introduction  is  non-­‐standard.  

As  the  progressive  decline  of  naming  structures  that  can  be  observed  in  the  group  of  

three-­‐year-­‐olds   reduces   the   children’s   opportunities   to   deictically   realize   reference   in  

the  situational  context,  the  use  of  pronominal  determiners  represents  an  alternative  for  

this   kind   of   use.   In   comparison   to   the   two-­‐year-­‐olds,   the   referential   system   of   three-­‐

year-­‐olds  is  reorganized  with  respect  to  the  use  of  nominal  and  pronominal  expressions  

and   the  marking  of   first   and   subsequent  mentions,   but   reference   is   in   large  parts   still  

anchored  in  the  situational  context.  

Page 25: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  25  

 

Four-­‐year-­‐olds:  definite  determiners  in  first  and  subsequent  mentions  

In  the  group  of  four-­‐year-­‐olds  the  use  of  the  pronominal  determiner  generally  decreases  

and  the  willingness  of  children  to   introduce  referents  with  the  pronominal  determiner  

drops  down  to  5  percent  (N=2).  This  finding  is  a  clear  indicator  that  the  children  refrain  

from   following   the   conventions   of   spontaneous   speech   in   their   use   of   pronouns   for  

referent  introduction  (cf.  Rozendaal  &  Baker,  2010).  With  regard  to  nominal  expressions  

on   the   other   hand,   children   behave   similar   to   adults   in   spontaneous   speech   when  

introducing  a  new  referent  to  a  MK  context  and  prefer  to  use  the  definite  determiner  (cf.  

Rozendaal  &  Baker,  2008:  789).  The  indefinite  determiner  is  also  used  for  first  mentions  

but   still   as   part   of   a   naming   structure   in   the   majority   of   cases.   It   can   therefor   be  

concluded  that  the  children’s  choice  of  a  nominal  expression  for  referent  introduction  is  

strongly  influenced  by  the  deictic  or  ostensive  potential  of  an  expression.  While  naming  

structures  feature  a  deictic  expression  in  sentence-­‐initial  position,  definite  determiners  

used  in  a  MK  context  are  not  deictic  expressions  per  se  but  can  be  regarded  as  ostensive  

in   that   they   refer   to   entities  present   in   the   situational   context   (cf.   Lyons,  1999:  160f).  

Karmiloff-­‐Smith   (1979)   has   argued   that   the   definite   article   functions   as   a   deictic  

(pointing)  device  in  early  phases  of   language  acquisition.  She  separates  the  function  of  

articles   into   DESCRIPTORS,   which   link   the   visual   and   social   context   to   language,   and  

DETERMINORS,   which   link   linguistic   contexts.   According   to   Karmiloff-­‐Smith,   children  

acquire  the  descriptor  function  of  articles  prior  to  their  determinor  function.  Following  

this   line   of   reasoning,   the   deixis-­‐first   principle   can   be   interpreted   as   an   extension   of  

Karmiloff-­‐Smith’s  (1979:  215)  model.  The  deixis-­‐first  principle  affects  the  whole  system  

of  referring  expressions  in  preschool  children  which  develop  from  deictic  systems  that  

Page 26: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  26  

are  anchored  in  the  situational  context  to  more  textually  oriented  systems  that  allow  the  

expression  of  referential  linking.  

In   the   subsequent   mentions,   the   four-­‐year-­‐olds   reach   an   adult   level   of   definite  

determiners   (cf.   Table   2   and  Table   3).   In   both   the   adult’s   and   the   children’s   data,   the  

high  amount  of  definite  determiners   in   subsequent  mentions  can  be   interpreted  as  an  

effect  of  frequent  topic  change  since  the  story  was  controlled  for  an  even  distribution  of  

agency   among   the   characters.   Children   are   still   unable   to   exploit   the  potentials   of   the  

er/der   contrast   (see  below)   for  marking  different  participants   (cf.  Bittner  &  Kuehnast,  

2012;   Bosch   Rozario   &   Zhao,   2003),   but   a   definite   NP   can   be   used   to   identify   the  

intended  referent  with  a  lexical  label.  The  increasing  amount  of  definite  determiners  in  

subsequent  mentions  can   therefore  be   interpreted  as  a   consequence  of  understanding  

the   need   to   unambiguously   identify   a   referent.   In   German   adult   L2-­‐data   for   instance,  

over-­‐explication   with   nouns   is   attributed   to   a   concern   for   communicative   success  

(Hendriks,  2003).  In  the  present  study,  equal  gender  among  the  characters  adds  to  the  

same   effect   and   may   also   explain   the   adults’   use   of   a   comparatively   high   amount   of  

unambiguous  reflexive  pronouns.  

As   already  mentioned,   the   proportion   of   the   pronominal   determiner   in   subsequent  

mentions   is   much   lower   than   in   the   three-­‐year-­‐olds   but   it   is   still   well   above   the  

proportion  that  adults  produce.  When  using  pronominal  forms  in  subsequent  mentions  

adults  rely  on  personal  pronouns,  which  clearly  signal  referential  linking  and  the  need  to  

find   an   antecedent.   Four-­‐year-­‐olds   on   the   other   hand   rely   on   the   potentially   deictic  

pronominal  determiners  when  using  pronominal   forms  in  subsequent  mentions.   It  can  

be   concluded   that   the   four-­‐year-­‐olds   systems   of   reference   are   similar   to   those   of   the  

three-­‐year-­‐olds  in  that  the  anchoring  of  reference  in  the  situational  context  is  still  visible  

in  their  use  of  the  indefinite  determiner  in  naming  structures,  the  predominance  of  the  

Page 27: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  27  

pronominal  determiner  (in  subsequent  mentions)  and  the  children’s  use  of  the  definite  

determiner   for   referent   introduction.   The   four-­‐year-­‐olds   systems   of   reference   are  

similar  to  the  adults  on  the  other  hand  in  that  lexical  expressions  are  used  for  referent  

introduction  and   the  most   frequent  expressions   in   subsequent  mentions  are  NPs  with  

the  definite  determiner.  

 

Five-­‐year-­‐olds:  personal  pronouns  versus  pronominal  determiners,  the  er/der  contrast  

The   indefinite  determiner  represents  referring  arguments   in  a  majority  of  cases   in  the  

group   of   five-­‐year-­‐olds.   In   subsequent   mentions,   the   ratio   of   personal   pronouns  

increases   and   proportionally   equals   the   children’s   use   of   pronominal   determiners   (cf.  

Table   2   and   Table   3).   Clearly,   both   findings   must   be   interpreted   as   a   further   step  

towards   adult   realizations   of   referential   linking.   Adults   show   a   clear   preference   for  

personal  pronouns  and  thus  demonstrate  a  textually  oriented  strategy  of  reference.  It  is  

likely  that  the  adults’  use  of  the  pronominal  determiner  does  not  exploit  the  expression’s  

deictic   potential,   but   is   motivated   by   the   er/der   contrast   in   German   that   allows   a  

differentiation  of  topic  maintenance  and  topic  shift.  According  to  Bosch  et  al.  (2003)  the  

German   personal   pronouns   are   more   restrictive   than   the   pronominal   determiners   in  

that   the   former   signal   topic   maintenance   and   the   latter   prefer   less   salient   referents.  

Along   similar   lines   it   is   argued   in   CENTERING   THEORY   that   the   process   of   anaphor  

resolution  connects   the  shortest  expression  (ermasc/siefem/esneut   ‘he/she/it’)   to   the  most  

salient  discourse  referent  (Grosz,  Joshi  &  Weinstein,  1995).  

 

(24)   Peri   möchte  mit   Emilj   ins   Kino   gehen.   Eri  /  derj   darf   aber   nicht.  

  Per   wants   with   Emil   to.the   cinema   go   PRO  /  PRO  det   allow   but   not  

  ‘Per  wants  to  go  to  the  cinema  with  Emil.  But  he  isn’t  allowed  to’.  

 

Page 28: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  28  

In  the  example  given,  the  personal  pronoun  ermasc  ’he’  more  easily  relates  to  the  topic  of  

the  preceding  sentence  Per  while  the  pronominal  determiner  dermasc  more  easily  relates  

to   Emil   and   thus   signals   a   topic   change.   This   distinction   is   unavailable   to   preschool  

children  (cf.  Bittner  &  Kuehnast,  2012),  but  may  motivate  the  adult’s  choices.  

Across   age   groups   personal   pronouns   are   not   used   for   first   mentions   even   in   the  

youngest   age   group.   This   can   be   interpreted   as   a   sign   of   an   early   awareness   of   the  

expression’s   different   conditions   of   use   in   comparison   to   the   pronominal   determiner.  

The  comparatively  late  increase  of  personal  pronouns  can  be  interpreted  as  an  effect  of  

the   gradually   unfolding   competence   of   the   children  with   regard   to   referential   linking.  

Expressions  like  personal  pronouns  that  are  closely  associated  with  the  need  to  find  an  

antecedent  seem  to  attract   the  children’s  attention  relatively   late.  The  children  realize  

that  referent  introduction  on  the  basis  of  deixis  or  ostension  is  inadequate  in  a  narrative  

before  they  realize  that  German  not  only  provides  a  pronoun  that  can  index  a  referent  in  

the  situational  context  but  also  a  pronoun  that  can  index  a  referent  in  the  preceding  text.  

Definite  determiners  remain  on  a  similar  level  both  in  first  and  subsequent  mentions  in  

the  group  of  five-­‐year-­‐olds;  children  still  have  to  realize  that  indefinite  determiners  are  

not  unnecessarily  unspecific,  but  can  be  used  for  referent  introduction  in  MK  contexts.  

 

Conclusion  

The  children’s  use  of  the  indefinite  determiner  in  narratives  follows  a  similar  direction  

as   their   use   of   other   referring   expressions   in   development.   Their   early   systems   of  

reference  develop   from  context-­‐bound   to  more   textually   oriented   in   that   (potentially)  

deictic   realizations   decrease   and   (potentially)   anaphoric   realizations   increase.   It   has  

been   argued   here   that   children   initially   follow   a   deixis-­‐first   principle   that   affects   all  

expressions;   it   could  e.g.  be  shown  to  manifest   in   the  children’s  early  use  of   indefinite  

Page 29: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  29  

determiners   in  naming  structures  and  in  the  distribution  of  the  two  German  pronouns  

er/der.  When  the  use  of  expressions  with  a  deictic  potential  becomes  less  frequent,  the  

decrease  does  not  parallel  a  random  increase  of  other  expressions,  but  it  could  be  shown  

that  the  distribution  of  forms  changes  systematically  towards  more  textual  orientation.  

The   somewhat   paradoxical   findings   that   the   use   of   indefinite   NPs   marks   both   the  

onset  and  the  end  of  the  development  of  narratives  in  a  MK  context  could  be  resolved  as  

it   is   different  use   types   that  dominate   in   the   two  phases.  The  use  of   indefinite  NPs   in  

naming  structures  and  as  referring  arguments  is  nevertheless  not  unrelated  and  it  could  

be  shown  that  naming  structures  are  structurally  related   to   the  children’s  early  use  of  

indefinite  NPs  as  referring  arguments.  

 

References  

Bamberg,  M.  (1986).  A  functional  approach  to  the  acquisition  of  anaphoric  relationships.  Linguistics  24,  227-­‐284.  

Bittner,  D.  &  Kuehnast,  M.   (2012).  Comprehension  of   intersentential  pronouns   in  child  German  and  child  Bulgarian.  First  Language  32/1-­‐2,  176-­‐204.  

Bosch,   P.,   Rozario,   T.,   &   Zhao,   Y.   (2003).   Demonstrative   pronouns   and   personal  pronouns.   German   der   vs.   er.   Proceedings   of   the   EACL   2003:   Workshop   on   The  Computational  Treatment  of  Anaphora.  Budapest.  

Brown,  R.  (1973).  A  first  language:  the  early  stages.  Cambridge,  MA:  Harvard  University  Press.  

De  Cat,  C.  (2009).  Experimental  evidence  for  preschoolers'  mastery  of  topics.  Language  Acquisition  16,  224-­‐239.  

De  Cat,  C.  (2011).  Information  tracking  and  encoding  in  early  L1:  linguistic  competence  vs.  cognitive  limitations.  Journal  of  Child  Language  38,  828-­‐860.  

Grosz,  B.   J.,   Joshi,  A.  &  Weinstein,   S.   (1995).   Centering:  A   framework   for  modeling   the  local  coherence  of  discourse.  Computational  Linguistics  21,  203-­‐225.  

Gülzow,   I.   &  Gagarina,  N.  (2007).  Noun   phrases,   pronouns   and   anaphoric   reference   in  young  children  narratives.  ZAS  Papers  in  Linguistics  48,  203-­‐223.  

Gundel,  J.,  Hedberg,  N.  &  Zacharski,  R.  (1993).  Cognitive  status  and  the  form  of  referring  expressions  in  discourse.  Language  69,  274-­‐307.  

Hendriks,   H.   (2003).   The   use   of   nouns   in   reference   maintenance:   the   seeming  contradiction   in   adult   second   language   acquisition.   In   A.   Giacolone   Ramat   (ed.),  Typology   and   Second   Language   Acquisition,   Berlin,   New   York:  Mouton   de   Gruyter,  291-­‐326.  

Hickmann,   M.   (2003).   Children’s   discourse:   Person,   space   and   time   across   languages.  Cambridge,  MA:  Cambridge  University  Press.  

Page 30: ThreeRevision with abstract and references plus author · Establishing+discourse+referents:+indefinite+nounphrases+inGermanpre8 schoolers’+narratives*+ + + *This+work+was+partlysupportedbytheGermanBundesministerium+für+

Establishing  discourse  referents  

  30  

Kail,   M.   &   Hickmann,   M.   (1992).   French   children's   ability   to   introduce   referents   in  narratives  as  a  function  of  mutual  knowledge.  First  Language  12,  73‑94.  

Karmiloff-­‐Smith,   A.   (1979).  A  Functional  Approach   to   Child   Language.   Cambridge,  MA:  Cambridge  University  Press.  

Kauschke,  C.  &  Siegmüller,  J.  (2009).  PDSS  -­‐  Patholinguistische  Diagnostik  bei  Sprachentwicklungsstörungen  (2.  standardisierte,  vollständig  überarbeitete  Auflage).  München:  Elsevier.  

Küntay,  A.  C.,  &  Koçbaş,  D.  (2009).  Effects  of  lexical  items  and  construction  types  in  English  and  Turkish  character  introductions  in  elicited  narrative.  In  J.  Guo,  E.  Lieven,  N.  Budwig,  S.  Ervin-­‐Tripp,  K.  Nakamura  &  Ş.  Özçalışkan  (eds.),  Crosslinguistic  approaches  to  the  psychology  of  language:  Research  in  the  tradition  of  Dan  Isaac  Slobin,  New  York,  London:  Psychology  Press,  Taylor  &  Francis  Group,  81-­‐92.  

Kupisch,  T.  (2006).   The  acquisition  of  determiners  in  bilingual  German-­‐Italian  and  German-­‐French  children.  München:  Lincom  Europa.  

Lyons,  J.  (1975).  Deixis  as  the  source  of  reference.  In  E.  L.  Keenan  (ed.),  Formal  Semantics  of  Natural  Language,  61-­‐83.  Cambridge:  Cambridge  University  Press.  

Lyons,  C.  (1999).  Definiteness.  Cambridge,  MA:  Cambridge  University  Press.  MacWhinney,   B.   (2000).   The   CHILDES   Project:   Tools   for   analyzing   talk.   3rd   Edition.  Mahwah,  NJ:  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates  

Maratsos,  M.   (1974).   Preschool   children's   use   of   definite   and   indefinite   articles.  Child  Development  45,  446-­‐455.  

Maratsos,  M.  (1976).  The  Use  of  Definite  and  Indefinite  Reference  in  Young  Children.  Cambridge,  MA:  Cambridge  University  Press.  

Rozendaal,  M.  I.  &  Baker,  A.  E.  (2008).  A  cross-­‐linguistic  investigation  of  the  acquisition  of  the  pragmatics  of  indefinite  and  definite  reference  in  two-­‐year-­‐olds.  Journal  of  Child  Language  35,  773–807.  

Rozendaal,  M.I.  &  Baker,  A.E.  (2010).  The  acquisition  of  reference:  Pragmatic  aspects  and  the  influence  of  language  input.  Journal  of  Pragmatics    42,  1866-­‐1879.  

Schaeffer,  J.  &  Matthewson,  L.  (2005).  Grammar  and  Pragmatics  in  the  Acquisition  of  Article  Systems.  Natural  Language  and  Linguistic  Theory  23:  53-­‐101.  

Schafer,  R.  &  de  Villiers,  J.  (2000).  Imagining  Articles:  What  a  and  the  can  tell  us  about  the  emergence  of  DP.  In  Proceedings  of  the  Boston  University  Language  Development  conference.  Somerville:  Cascadilla  Press.  

Wigglesworth,   G.   (1990).   Children’s   narrative   acquisition:   a   study   of   some   aspects   of  reference  and  anaphora.  First  Language  10:  105-­‐125.