Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

44
The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) MSc Zoo Conservation Biology Full time 2014-2015 School of Science and the Environment Name: Michael Godfrey Wilson 14058121 Supervisor: Dr Lesley Lace

Transcript of Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

Page 1: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra)

MSc Zoo Conservation Biology

Full time

2014-2015

School of Science and the Environment

Name: Michael Godfrey Wilson 14058121

Supervisor: Dr Lesley Lace

Page 2: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

2

With the exception of any statements to the contrary, all the data

presented in this report are the result of my own efforts. In addition, no

parts of this report have been copied from other sources. I understand

that any evidence of plagiarism and/or the use of unacknowledged third

party data will be dealt with as a very serious matter.

Signed ........................................................................

Date ........................

Page 3: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

3

Abstract Humans have maintained wild animals in zoological collections for the purposes of education,

conservation, research, and recreation for many years. In captive environments, animals can

develop abnormal behaviours due to a lack of sensory stimulation. A zoo environment has to

provide opportunity for animals to carry out a wide array of their natural behaviours.

However, due to spatial and financial constraints, captive environments do not always

provide for this. Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) are a particularly difficult case due

to their large natural ranges and predatory behaviours. Wild tiger populations have declined

by 97% in the past 100 years making captive breeding programmes a major focus of the zoo

community; as a result, tiger husbandry research is paramount. This study analysed the

effects on behaviour of two Sumatran tigers housed at Chester Zoo, in North West England

with respect to disturbance (visitor numbers) and noise levels.

The data in this study was collected by a focal observational sampling method of the tigers’

behaviour, every minute for 30 minutes as well as a head count of visitors and a recording

based upon the reading of a decibel reader. The behaviour displayed at the time was recorded

and later analysed using R studio. A range of behaviours were recorded and compared to

visitor numbers and noise levels. Preference of enclosure location was also determined. Noise

levels did not significantly affect behaviour whereas visitor numbers did. The animals also

preferred wooden platforms that were elevated above the ground and stayed close to dense

vegetation, where they spent over 70% of the time. The male performed almost twice as

much stereotypic pacing, 22%, than exploratory behaviour, 11%, however this was not

reflected in the female who spent no time displaying stereotypical behaviour.

Page 4: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

4

The primary objectives of this study are to determine the frequency of occurrence of resting,

exploring, and stereotypic behaviours in two captive Sumatran tigers as well as the amount

and quality of enclosure space that was utilized by the study animals. The researcher hopes

to examine the relationship between behaviours and visitor number as well as ambient noise

level.

Key Words: Zoos, Behaviour, Stereotypy, Sumatran, Tiger, Enclosure, Noise, Visitor

Page 5: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

5

Introduction There are estimated to be only 400-500 Sumatran tigers in the wild (Luo et al., 2004, 2010;

Linkie et al., 2008) and their range is much reduced and fragmented. The recent extinction of

the Caspian, Javan, and Bali subspecies and the 97% decrease of total tiger numbers since the

early 1900s (Nowell and Jackson, 1996; Kitchener and Dugmore, 2000; Luo et al., 2004)

emphasises, that tiger extinction is a very real and quickly occurring concern for conservation

and captive breeding. There are 295 registered Sumatran tigers in captivity (Luo et al., 2010),

partly due to the fact they are popular animals with the public (Luo et al., 2010). This suggests

that reintroduction could be a feasibly possible means to securing the future of the species

(Johnsingh and Madhusudan, 2009).

Tigers being large, attractive and an endangered ensures they fall very much into the category

of flagship species, making them one of the most popular zoo exhibits. This has led to the

large captive population that is seen today, which provides a great educational opportunity

regarding their conservation. It has been shown that displaying wild animals to the public can

be an effective education tool. However, it is far more effective when the animals on display

perform healthy species-specific behaviours in naturalistic-looking exhibits (Shettel-Neuber,

1988; Swanagan, 2000; Catibog-Sinha, 2008). It can also break down the conception that

humans have dominion over nature, and encourages people to view them as more than

merely a recreational display for the public.

However, some animals may develop abnormal behaviours when the captive, human-made

environment is not sufficient for the full range of instinctive, natural behaviours to be

performed (Broom, 1991; Tribe, 2003). Unfortunately zoos are often unable to provide the

Page 6: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

6

requirements needed to allow for these behaviours due to spatial and financial constraints

(Tribe, 2003).

Over the past 40 years, a number of zoos have undertaken major changes in the husbandry

of felids to enhance their lives by incorporating environmental enrichment into the care of

the animals (Law et al., 1997; Broad and Weiler, 1998). However, effective enrichment is

difficult to develop and execute for felids due to their complex hunting behaviours and large

natural ranges. Lack of suitable enrichment and high volumes of visitors can lead to

Stereotypy (Broom, 1991; Pitsko, 2003).

Stereotypic behaviours are often considered a response to stress (Broom, 1991; Pitsko, 2003),

however this could also be the symptom of a psychological disorder. Either way, both of these

are indicators of poor welfare (Broom, 1991). Therefore, to increase welfare in captive

animals, stereotypy should be addressed. This would not only benefit the general wellbeing

of the animal, but also would increase the success of captive breeding programmes. In

addition, the delivery of the conservational and educational message and aesthetic value of

the exhibit would be enhanced. Captive breeding of endangered species aims to maintain

genetic diversity within the captive population and this is especially important for Sumatran

tigers due to their massively decreased wild population (Hutchins, 2003; Catibog-Sinha, 2008;

McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008).

Unnatural substrate has been known to cause stereotypic behaviours in captive animals

(Hediger, 1969) and can be more uncomfortable for captive cats (Law et al., 1997). However,

some of these unnatural substrates, such as the wooden platforms are built for the purpose

of environmental enrichment, which has been, suggested to reduce abnormal behaviours.

Concrete floors are considered more hygienic due to the ease of cleaning. However, recently,

Page 7: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

7

research has proved otherwise and so more natural substrates are now preferred by zoo

enclosure designers (Law et al., 1997).

Vegetation can help to create a more natural environment for captive animals; however, it is

difficult to replicate the vegetation of the Sumatran tigers’ natural habitat. Firstly, the habitat

in the UK is not suitable for the propagation of most Sumatran plants and the dense Sumatran

rainforest would not allow for visibility for visitors. However efforts are made regarding

vegetation in order to allow hiding areas away from the public, which also create areas of

shade (Law et al., 1997). Planting allows for more complex environments and attracts other

natural stimuli such as birds and insects (Law et al., 1997).

Tigers are avid swimmers so the availability of a pool is beneficial for their welfare. It also

provides an alternate form of exercise and enrichment as well as a means of cooling down in

hot temperatures (Bush et al., 2002).

The presence or absence of visitors may potentially influence the behaviour and location of

the animal; a busy day may result in the animal being more likely to hide away from view or

indoors (Del Thompson, 1989). One might expect that animals born into captivity will be

accustomed to visitors, and therefore not be influenced by number of humans at close

proximity, however further research is required on the behaviour of tigers in captivity, their

preferences to the captive environment and the effects of enrichment on felid wellbeing

(Pitsko, 2003).

Zoos encourage the expression of naturalistic behaviours by the animals on display, in order

to successfully assist in the delivery of their conservation and educational message (Tribe,

2003). Due to the rise in public awareness of the importance of animal welfare, stereotypic

behaviours can be aesthetically displeasing to the visiting public.

Page 8: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

8

The North of England Zoological Society is a charity based in Cheshire; Chester Zoo contains

12000 animals and 400 different species. In 2014, it was voted the best zoo in the UK, second

best zoo in Europe and sixth best zoo globally. It is a pioneer in zoo animal welfare and

husbandry, and has reputation for excelling in this role. The North of England Zoological

society is responding to the demand for more aesthetically natural and ethically minded

enclosures, having invested £40,000 to develop six hectares of land into the 'Islands' exhibit,

which is based on the South East Asian islands of Panay, Papua, Bali, Sumba, Sumatra and

Sulawesi. This is the largest zoo development to have ever occurred in Britain and will have

the largest indoor zoo exhibit in the United Kingdom. It will house 100 different species of

animal and one of the most endangered species on display will be the Sumatran Tiger. Chester

Zoo boasts approximately 1,432,867 visitors annually, making it the most visited, paid-for

attraction in UK outside of London, this is due to rise with the opening of ‘Islands’. Therefore,

animal welfare concerning numbers of visitors and noise level is an important factor to

consider in maintaining their good reputation.

This study provides recommendations for improving the welfare of captive tigers and increase

breeding success. The study of animal welfare is an important component of husbandry. This

paper adds to the literature on Sumatran tiger behaviour, which is underrepresented, despite

them being the second most common subspecies of tiger in captivity. This study also provides

useful information to Chester Zoo for the future management of the study animals, as well as

other zoo exhibit planners and animal keepers. By analysing the relationship between visitor

numbers, noise levels, animal behaviour and animal exhibit preferences, zoos will be able to

attempt to optimise certain behaviours and reduce stereotypic behaviours that are

performed by their animals whilst still allowing for an enjoyable visitor experience. These

recommendations will not only be beneficial for just Sumatran tigers but should be applicable

Page 9: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

9

to other subspecies of tigers not included in this study as well as other large felids, such as

the jaguar and lion, also housed at Chester Zoo.

The primary objectives of this study are to determine the frequency of occurrence of resting,

exploring, and stereotypic behaviours in two captive Sumatran tigers as well as the amount

and quality of enclosure space utilized by the study animals. As well as this, the researcher

aims to examine the relationship between behaviours and visitor number as well as ambient

noise level.

This study will contribute to the species-specific literature on Sumatran tigers. The upcoming

opening of the “Islands” exhibit in Chester Zoo provides an excellent opportunity to gather

data prior to their move. These data may be used by future researchers in a comparative

study to determine the extent to which the move improves the behaviour of the animals. This

project could also provide specific recommendations for the management of captive tigers.

Page 10: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

10

Methods

Study Site and Animals

Chester Zoo, in the North of England, was the data collection site for this study. Fieldwork

took place between January and September of 2015. Two individual purebred Sumatran tigers

(Panthera tigris sumatra) were the subjects of the study; one male, named Fabi, and one

female, named Kirana. Fabi and Kirana are 7 and 8 years of age respectively. They were both

born in captivity and raised by their biological parents. Fabi was born at Prague Zoological

Gardens and Kirana at Paignton Zoo Environmental Park. Fabi moved in 2009 to the enclosure

used in this study and Kirana in 2008. Both tigers have been housed continuously in this

enclosure since their moves. They have had a successful breeding history with Kirana giving

birth to three cubs in January 2015 and 2 cubs in 2013, suggesting that welfare is of a high

enough standard for breeding success.

The enclosure plan was split into various different sections depending upon the various

differing substrate, vegetation and other surface area attributes (see Figure 1) this facilitates

the collection of data and analysis. This method was also used by (Pitsko, 2003). Some

substrates, such as S1 to S7, are natural (not synthetic). Others, S7 to S16, are unnatural

(manmade). The tigers were able to fully submerge within the pond, which was constructed

out of concrete. The wooden platforms S12 to S14 were of varying heights with S12 being the

shortest and S14 being the tallest.

Page 11: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

11

Figure 1 - Sumatran Tiger Enclosure at Chester Zoo

Page 12: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

12

Variables

Data was collected using a focal observational sampling method of the tigers’ behaviour. This

is commonly used when observing the behaviours of an individual animal during a sampling

period (Altmann, 1974). During the pilot study it became evident that scan sampling was an

inappropriate method due to the size of the enclosure, and when the animals were on

opposite sides of the enclosure to one another, it was impossible to record both animals

simultaneously.

At one-minute intervals, focal samples of four variables were recorded for each animal. These

variables were: The Animal’s behaviour, The Animal location, Visitor number and Noise level

in A-weighted decibels (dBa).

Behaviours of individual animals were recorded every minute in phases of 30 minutes;

however, due to the observer collecting data consistently over the course of the day, the

phases were often back-to-back resulting in much longer continuous periods of data being

collected.

Recording took place during opening hours of the zoo and when visitors were present

between 10.30 and 18.00. Additional, unusual events, occurring in or near the exhibit were

also recorded and included keeper activity or the nearby monorail train.

Page 13: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

13

Animal Behaviour and Location

The researcher classified behaviour based on an ethogram of common tiger behaviours

observed during the pilot study (Appendix 1). Much of the literature also contained common

felid behaviours and this was considered in the production of the ethogram (Lindburg, 1988;

Seidensticker and McDougal, 1993; Lyons et al., 1997). Each tiger was observed continuously

for the duration of the phase, unless it went out of sight. The behaviours were recorded on

the minute, along with the location within the enclosure that the behaviour was performed

(Figure 1). This constant observation allowed the observer to distinguish between similar

behaviours, for example walking and pacing. In order to simplify the data analysis, behaviours

were grouped into one of three distinct categories described as resting, exploratory and

stereotypic behaviour.

If more than one behaviour was displayed, for example standing and sniffing, the behaviour

with the most importance was prioritised with reference to the produced hierarchy (Appendix

1). Therefore, although the tiger is standing, which is a resting behaviour, it is also sniffing,

which is exploratory. The exploratory behaviour takes priority, in this case, so this data point

was considered as exploratory. Behaviours considered as stereotypic included pacing,

excessive grooming, tail sucking and paw chewing. However, only pacing behaviour was

witnessed during this study. The only two that could potentially apply to this study were

pacing and excessive grooming. The distinction between pacing and walking was that an

obviously, aimless walking, often back and forth, in circles, figures of eight or any other

repeatable pattern was considered pacing. Whilst walking to a separate area of the enclosure

with a purpose and not repeating was considered walking. Excessive grooming was harder to

distinguish from healthy grooming. However, due to the lack of bald patches or noticeable

Page 14: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

14

hair loss, the study subjects would not be considered to be excessively grooming at the time

of this study.

Page 15: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

15

Visitors and Noise Level

At each minute interval, a head count of the visitors visible from the position of the observer

was recorded. The visitor numbers were divided into 4 categories: Not busy - 0-4 people

looking at the enclosure, somewhat busy – 5-9 people looking at the enclosure, Busy – 10-14

people looking at the enclosure and Very busy – 15+ people looking at the enclosure.

Noise level was also included, seeing as one noisy visitor may affect the behaviour more than

10 quiet visitors. The LEMONBEST™ Mini Digital Sound Level Meter was used to measure

noise level in A-weighted decibels (dBa). Temperature was recorded using an electronic

thermometer and measured in degrees centigrade.

Page 16: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

16

Results

Frequency of behaviours

During analysis, the frequency of the behavioural categories as a proportion of all behaviours

was recorded as a percentage (Table 1).

Table 1 – Time spent performing different categories of behaviour

Resting (%) Exploratory (%) Stereotypic (%)

Male 66.86 11.10 22.04

Female 87.71 12.25 0.04

Total 78.40 11.74 9.86

The male performed twice as much stereotypic behaviour than exploratory behaviour

whereas the stereotypic behaviour performed by the female was negligible. This potentially

could be due to the male having a psychological issue that results in a tendency to perform

stereotypic behaviour or alternatively could simply just be him patrolling his territory and the

observer due to the repeated nature of the behaviour recording it as stereotypic.

A comparison between the proportions seen in Table 1 and those found by Pitsko (2003) was

performed using a Chi Squared comparison of proportions test. Pitsko (2003) found that the

tigers in her sample (n=18) rested for 76% of the time, Explored for 16% of the time and

performed stereotypic behaviour for 8% of the time. The comparison was statistically similar

(Chi squared, df = 2, P = 0.44). Therefore, there is no significant difference between the two

proportions of this study and that of Pitsko (2003), suggesting that although we have a small

sample size it shows similar proportions of different types of behaviour as an experiment with

a much greater sample size.

Page 17: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

17

Breakdown of Exploratory Behaviour The difference between the types of exploratory behaviours displayed by the Male and

Female tigers are shown in Figure 2. The exploratory data was split into the specific

behaviours as recorded in the observer’s ethogram (Appendix 1). Although the total time

performing exploratory behaviours only account for 11-13% of their total behaviour observed

it would be interesting to see what proportion of their exploratory behaviour is spent

performing specific behaviours.

Figure 2 -A Comparison between the Exploratory Behaviours displayed by Male and Female Sumatran Tiger

The Female spends 38% of her total time performing exploratory behaviours positively

socialising with the cubs. The Male walks around more than the Female and also Urinates

more, this would also be expected as he is likely to be patrolling, interacting with objects and

scent marking his territory.

Page 18: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

18

Preference of Location The proportion of time spent at each location was found for each tiger. This was then plotted

as a comparative bar chart (Figure 3) to determine whether there is any difference between

the male and the female preference of location. As Figure 3 shows, both tigers spent a

considerable proportion of time at S13, S14 and S16, which with reference to Figure 1, we can

see are all man-made wooden platforms. These were elevated off the ground there for would

suggest that both tigers have a preference for more 3 dimensional habitats, even if they aren’t

entirely natural. S8 was not always open, and also had poor visibility and the pond was often

not filled, which might explain the low use of these two areas. The male would patrol around

his territory and this in this case the enclosure, this is perfectly natural behaviour and may

explain why the male spends less time in a single area and is generally more spread S14 is also

the highest wooden platform where he would potentially get the best view of his territory

possibly resulting in why he took preference to this. Due to the new born cubs the female was

often somewhere that was accessible to the cubs which may explain the low use of the

highest platform S14. The female has a great preference for the mid height wooden platform,

S13, as she spent over half her time here. It may be due to it being significantly raised however

still accessible to the cubs due to a ramp.

Page 19: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

19

Figure 3 - Location Preference comparison for Male and Female Sumatran Tiger

Page 20: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

20

Noise level and visitor number effects on behaviour?

Visitor number was plotted as a box and whisker graph with noise level, to determine whether

noise level increases with visitors number (Figure 4). In order to determine the statistical

significance of the differences seen in Figure 4 a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test was performed

(Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test, 1461.41, df =3, P < 0.001). This statistically significant result

found that noise level increases with visitor number.

Figure 4 - How ambient noise level increases with visitor number

The behaviour was then compared to noise level to establish whether the behaviour was

affected by the ambient noise (Figure 5).

Page 21: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

21

Figure 5 - How ambient noise level effects different behaviours

No relationship between the ambient noise level and behaviour was found. Noise level

displayed a non-Gaussian distribution following an Anderson-Darling test (Anderson-Darling

test of normality, A = 3.66, p < 0.001), therefore ANOVA could not be used. Due to the

continuous nature of the independent variable and the nominal dependent variable, a

Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test was performed (Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test, 2.51, df = 2, P =

0.29); this suggests that noise level has no statistically significant effect on behaviour.

Behavioural categories were compared to visitor number using a Pearson chi squared test, to

determine whether the abundance of visitors have an effect regardless of their noise

(Pearson’s chi squared test, 29.8234, df = 6, P < 0.001). We can conclude that the behavioural

categories are significantly different from each other with regards to visitor number. This

could however be a result of the tiger being more visible when performing certain behaviours

thus attracting more visitors, rather than large numbers of visitors effecting the behaviour of

the tiger.

Page 22: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

22

How temperature effects behaviour and location Behaviour and location do not seem to be dramatically effected by temperature. Although,

S10 which is indoors was used considerably more when temperature was low (Figure 6 and

Figure 7). Due to the data being collected between January and September 2015 in the UK

the recorded temperatures were only between 8.7 and 23.8 degrees C which is relatively

small range of temperature and it never got above body temperature or below the freezing

point of water. Therefore it wouldn’t be expected to dramatically alter their behaviour in

these relatively mild circumstances.

Figure 6 - Temperature effect on location

Page 23: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

23

Figure 7 - Temperature effect on Behaviour

Page 24: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

24

Summary of results In summary, we found that the tigers spent the vast majority of their time resting, with

between 11-13% performing exploratory behaviours. The Male stereotyped around 22% of

the time, however the female did not stereotype at all. Of the exploratory behaviour both

tigers performed behaviours one would expect that are justifiably natural, female performed

great deal of maternal behaviour toward cubs and male performed territorial behaviour such

as scent marking and patrolling. The tigers did seem to have a preference to the elevated

wooden platforms surrounded by dense vegetation. Visitor number did seem to have a very

small effect on behaviour however it is impossible to tell whether this was visitor number

effecting behaviour or if when performing stereotypic behaviour the male tiger was more

visible thus attracting more viewers, Id Est the behaviour effecting visitor numbers rather than

the other way around. Temperature and ambient noise level had no effect upon the

behaviour and other than S10, a room indoors, being used more in colder temperatures, there

was no effect of temperature on location.

Page 25: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

25

Discussion

In captivity an animal’s environment is dramatically different to the environment they would

experience in the wild, and they depend entirely upon humans for the provision of their daily

needs. This causes a major problem with regards to the stimulation of the individual and can

result in the development of abnormal behaviours, which may lead to a reduction in

reproductive output. As tiger populations in the wild dwindle, the necessity for healthy,

happy, reproductively successful captive populations for the use in captive breeding

programmes is of paramount importance and environmental stimulation will likely increase

captive breeding success. However, in many zoological gardens, space is limited, hunting and

mating opportunities are dramatically reduced, and various environmental components are

dependent on humans. As Chester Zoo opens its ‘Islands’ exhibit in late 2015, a major

opportunity will arise to compare the animals’ behaviour before and after the move. Also, the

data collected prior to the move, can be used to determine enclosure preference for the

tigers, and cater the new enclosure to their needs. This project can be considered a

foundation upon which to develop further research, to determine whether the move had the

expected result, and also contribute to the future management of the animals in question.

This study focused on evaluating the effects of several environmental and animal variables

on the behaviours of two captive Sumatran tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra). With a specific

interest in stereotypic pacing, due to the idea that captive carnivores often display this

behaviour when stressed. This study contributes to the small body of literature for captive

tiger management.

Page 26: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

26

A limitation was identified with the collection of visitor number data in that the observer

would only be able to see at best approximately half of the visitors surrounding the

enclosure at any time, there may well have been many more visitors on the other side of the

enclosure which were not accounted for. The enclosure was located on a major route

through the zoo, as well as being near a restaurant thus in can be assumed that the

recorded number of observers was an under estimation. The noise level may potentially

influence the behaviour and location of the animal, as well as overcoming some of the

limitations of the measurement of visitor number. Another limitation was with temperature,

the range of temperatures recorded were somewhat limited and we would expect a large

warm blooded mammal, such as a tiger, to be able to cope with this range. As a result the

behaviour wasn’t effected by the temperature, however future studies could be performed

in colder temperatures and hotter temperatures, for example the data could be collected

through the middle of both summer and winter, giving a more varied temperature range,

rather than just spring which was when the majority of data for this study was collected.

The study animals spent the majority of their time (average 78%) resting. Therefore, captive

tigers should be provided with various resting sites throughout the enclosure. Captive animals

can often have unnatural sleeping patterns due to lack of mental stimulation. This is a concern

to zoo keepers as it can be an indicator of poor welfare. However due to the nocturnal nature

of tigers, and observations only being carried out during the day time, these finding may not

be representative of typical behaviour during a 24 hour period. Further studies should be

performed overnight, and more data collected on the tiger’s behaviour throughout their

entire day. To aid this cameras could be fitted around the new enclosure which would allow

for vast quantities of behavioural data being collected throughout the day without the

Page 27: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

27

presence of an observer who may, especially over night when it is quiet, influence the

behaviour of the tigers.

The study animals spent around 12% of their time performing exploratory behaviour, this

however could be due to the fact that tigers are naturally nocturnal and a much greater

proportion of the time during the night could be spent exploring, when data was unable to

be collected. When broken down to more specific behaviours being displayed there was

nothing unusual. The female performed positive socialisation with the cubs for around 38%

of the time that was considered exploratory. The male walked around and urinated/scent

marked relatively more than the female. The male being more territorial and the female more

maternal is what would be expected. The male subject was observed to spend 22% of his time

performing stereotypic behaviour, which could be a cause for concern with regards to his

welfare. However, this could be partially due to the fact that a number of enrichment

activities were not being provided in order to ensure safety for the young cubs. Also, although

the Tiger performed repetitive movements as far as the observer was concerned, it could

have just been patrolling its territory which would result in the exploratory and territorial

behaviour we would expect being recorded as stereotypic behaviour. It is impossible to tell

whether this is just natural patrolling or pacing, as a result of a psychological or behavioural

issue.

Having compared the behavioural data to that of Pitsko (2003), our data was significantly

similar to the data collected in a study with a much larger sample size.

According to Margulis et al (2003), felid behaviour isn’t affected by visitor activity however

visitor activity is affected by the behaviour of felids. We found, however, a small effect

between visitor numbers and tiger behaviour, but, with Margulis et al (2003) in mind, this

Page 28: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

28

may be more likely due to the fact that when the male was stereotyping he was more visible,

and a more visible, active animal attracted more viewing visitors. From the point of view of

the visiting and paying public, it is often assumed that stereotypic behaviour is a psychological

or behavioural concern relating to welfare, and it wasn’t unusual for the observer to over hear

comments expressing concern over the welfare of the Tiger whilst it was performing this

behaviour. Therefore it is important to attempt to make the tigers more active and less

stereotypic for the purpose of visitor experience, even if it is not a welfare concern of the

animals.

Judging from the high usage of the wooden man made platforms that were constructed

around the enclosure, it can be assumed the tigers had a preference for these structures.

They spent over 70% of their time on any one of the wooden structures. These structures

were situated either in dense vegetation or in close proximity to it and were also raised off

the ground. This three dimensional element of the enclosure, along with dense vegetation,

has been described as “good quality” of enclosure space and it would be beneficial to the

tigers’ welfare if plenty of these are built around the ‘Islands’ exhibit. An additional benefit

was that they are easily visible from the public viewing areas. Thus, it is evident that an

enclosure which provides high welfare for its animal residents, can be additionally beneficial

in being aesthetically pleasing to the paying visitor.

The results of this study do not necessarily reflect the quality of the zoo or its husbandry

practices. Due to the presence of young cubs, certain enrichment which under normal

circumstances is performed regularly was unavailable at certain occasions to ensure the

safety of the cubs.

Page 29: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

29

Literature Review

The Sumatran Tiger, Panthera tigris sumatrae

The Sumatran Tiger’s geographic range is across the island of Sumatra in Indonesia (Luo et al.,

2010). There have been captive populations since 1937 and there are currently 295 registered

individuals compared to the 400-500 estimated in the wild (Luo et al., 2004, 2010; Linkie et

al., 2008). Due to their isolation on Sumatra, Sumatran tigers possess multiple unique

characters, including two diagnostic mtDNA nucleotide sites, ten mtDNA haplotypes, and 11

(out of 108) unique microsatellite alleles (Luo et al., 2010). The Sumatran tigers relatively high

genetic variability and the phylogenetic distinctiveness suggest they historically had a large

effective population size and highly restricted gene flow between the island and mainland

population (Luo et al., 2010). Like all tigers the Sumatran Tiger population is thought to be

declining as a result of habitat loss and fragmentation which is around 3.2-5.9% per year

(Achard et al., 2002; Linkie et al., 2008), this is due to expansion of oil palm plantations and

planting of Acacia plantations (Kinnaird et al., 2003; Linkie et al., 2003, 2004, 2006; Gaveau et

al., 2007). There are high levels of illegal trade in tiger parts (Nowell, 1999, 2007) and also

increasing human-tiger conflict (Nyhus and Tilson, 2004), also contributing to their decline.

History and role of Zoological Parks

A zoo is defined as any establishment where wild animals are kept for exhibition to which the

public have access, with or without charge for admission, for seven or more day in any period

of twelve consecutive months (Great Britain, 1981). Many institutions similar to this have

been documented throughout history, previously known as menageries and often used as a

display of the society’s elite.

Page 30: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

30

Within the last century, the role of the zoo has rapidly changed (Tribe and Booth, 2003;

Cuarón, 2005; Catibog-Sinha, 2008; Marino et al., 2010) becoming institutions for not only

research and recreation but also important institutions for education and conservation,

especially within urban societies. With approximately 50% of the world's population of

humans currently living in cities (Mitchell, 1998; Turley, 1999; Hutchins, 2003; Catibog-Sinha,

2008; Gusset and Dick, 2010; Marino et al., 2010), a zoological collection is one of the only

ways to experience direct contact with wildlife's diversity. As a result zoos possess the

potential to stimulate curiosity in nature and encourage its preservation (Mallinson, 2001;

Hutchins, 2003; Miller et al., 2004; Conway, 2011).

A modern zoo is now is considered an interactive, entertaining institution, more responsive

to the demand for naturally aesthetic and ethically driven animal exhibits (Tribe and Booth,

2003; Cuarón, 2005; Catibog-Sinha, 2008). This emerging role is evident in the mission

statements of many modern zoos as well as from the World Zoo Conservation Strategy

(Mallinson, 2001).

Zoos provide four key roles: Conservation, Education, Research and Recreation (Shettel-

Neuber, 1988). They assist in conservation through breeding threatened populations in ex

situ conditions (Bowkett, 2009). The role is analogous to the biblical story of Noah’s ark where

zoos can sustain populations of animals until the political or ecological cause of their decline

is resolved allowing for their eventual reintroduction (Turley, 1999; Mallinson, 2001;

McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008; Bowkett, 2009; Conde et al., 2011). Tiger reintroduction has

proved successful in India, however to ensure its success much preparation is required and

long term management needs to be maintained to ensure that the causes of its initial decline

don’t return (Johnsingh and Madhusudan, 2009).

Page 31: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

31

A major problem with zoos is that they simply aren’t big enough (Rahbek, 1993; Mallinson,

2001; Conway, 2011). Often this results in very small, non-breeding populations being

sustained (Conway, 2011). Other problems with ex situ breeding can be maintaining genetic

integrity (Catibog-Sinha, 2008; McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008) as well as domestication and

loss of natural behaviours (Bowkett, 2009).

Most concerning is the sheer expense, they can cost more than the equivalent in situ breeding

for mammals (Balmford et al., 1995) and often the more cost effective conservation efforts

can be overlooked if captive breeding is prioritised (Snyder et al., 1996; Bowkett, 2009).

Despite its limitations, it can be a useful and necessary conservation method under the right

circumstances and the debate has shifted to when ex situ breeding is warranted considering

it competes with in situ efforts for funding (Hutchins, 2003; McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008;

Bowkett, 2009).

The World Zoo Conservation Strategy puts more emphasis on in situ conservation

programmes (Hutchins, 2003; Reid et al., 2008). The World Association of Zoos and Aquaria

quickly became the third largest contributor to field conservation (Conde et al., 2011; Gusset

and Dick, 2011), with the potential to become the primary non-governmental global field

conservation organisation (Hutchins, 2003; Tribe and Booth, 2003). Although zoos do

financially contribute it is relatively small amounts (Tribe and Booth, 2003; Gusset and Dick,

2010) due to financial constraints.

The combining of in situ and ex situ conservation efforts represents a great opportunity for

zoos (Mallinson, 2001; Hutchins, 2003; Gusset and Dick, 2010; Conde et al., 2011) and may be

the only hope for the survival of some species (Hutchins, 2003; Catibog-Sinha, 2008;

McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008).

Page 32: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

32

As zoos have become more federated they are far more capable of managing their collections

as a single large collection and as a result overcome some of the problems posed by small

population size (Tribe and Booth, 2003; McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008; Bowkett, 2009;

Conde et al., 2011). This will dramatically enhance the potential of future captive breeding

populations (Tribe and Booth, 2003).

There is a great deal of controversy around captive breeding in the conservation literature

(Rahbek, 1993; Rabinowitz, 1995), however as we progress into the Holocene extinction and

reintroduction science develops, it will play a larger role in supplementing in situ conservation

efforts (Mallinson, 2001; Conde et al., 2011; Conway, 2011).

A great deal of people agree that zoos have a great potential when it comes to educating the

general public and often it is portrayed as being the single most important contribution zoos

can make (Hutchins, 2003; Tribe and Booth, 2003; Miller et al., 2004; Patrick et al., 2007;

Catibog-Sinha, 2008; McGregor Reid and Zippel, 2008; Conway, 2011; Gusset and Dick, 2011).

However, It is also complicated to not increase the perception of human domination over

nature, when people believe the animals are confined for the visitors leisure (Kellert, 1997;

Miller et al., 2004). A well-developed educational programme with naturalistic and interactive

exhibits certainly has the potential to educate people and have a conservation impact

(Swanagan, 2000; Tribe and Booth, 2003; Miller et al., 2004; Catibog-Sinha, 2008).

Zoos are also engaging in more scientific research and offer a varied opportunities of study

(Hutchins, 2003; Catibog-Sinha, 2008; Conde et al., 2011). As a result of putting science to the

forefront of management they successfully develop husbandry and breeding methodologies

that are transferable to conservation programmes (Turley, 1999; Conway, 2011).

Page 33: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

33

If zoos want to remain economically viable they cannot forget their recreational role

(Mallinson, 2001). Most people visit a zoo for its recreational value (Mallinson, 2001) and

people who want an enjoyable day out will often be uninterested if the conservation message

is overemphasised (Turley, 1999).

What is welfare and how do zoos provide it?

Animals possess many functional systems that allow for their survival, these often require a

wide range of needs (Broom, 1991). Some of these are complex mechanisms that have

evolved in the species, such as providing environmental enrichment for sensory input (Broom,

1991).

When an animal has a need, its motivational state changes allowing for a behavioural and

physiological response that should rectify the desire (Broom, 1991). An animal employs

various coping responses that allow it to control and maintain mental and physical stability

(Broom, 1991). These are often energy intensive and therefore are rarely used unless the

normal regulatory actions fail (Broom, 1991).

Occasionally, an animal may cope with its environment and thus has adapted, sometimes this

can only be achieved with difficulty (Broom, 1991). If its natural control systems are

overloaded and it fails to cope causing a reduction in its biological fitness, it is deemed as

stressed (Broom, 1991).

Welfare comprises of the state of the individual as it attempts to cope, the extent of what has

to be done in order to cope and the success of such efforts (Broom, 1986).

Welfare can be measured using various indicators and the extent of the stresses for the

animal identified and an ethical decision can then be made to determine the welfare (Broom,

Page 34: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

34

1991). However, ethics should remain separated from a welfare assessment to ensure there

is no emotional influence and guarantee scientific integrity.

Suffering is the occurrence of an acute or prolonged unpleasant subjective feeling due to the

fact that an animal is not capable of carrying out actions required to reduce risk to its

wellbeing (Dawkins, 1990). Suffering is often associated with welfare, but welfare is a wider

term. In the absence of suffering, poor welfare can still exist. For example, stress induced

immunosuppression without the onset of disease (Kelley, 1980). If the animal is having

difficulties coping its behaviour will reflect this allowing us to somewhat quantify the suffering

and therefore realise the extent to which the welfare is poor. This highlights the importance

of observational behaviour studies in the determination of welfare. We are unable to know

the emotional feelings of an animal, however we can recognise the behavioural and

physiological responses that relate to them (Broom, 1991).

Suffering is the emotional feelings, which are unknown and also subjective. Welfare has to

incorporate other factors, as it is not scientifically sound to define welfare just in context of

the level of assumed suffering (Broom, 1991).

Frustration is when an animal knows how to interact with the environment however is

prevented from doing so, this can result in abnormalities in physiology and results in

behavioural changes, which can be indicators of welfare (Broom, 1991). It can be very difficult

for an animal to cope if it has evolved to explore and respond to various stimuli once kept in

an enclosure that doesn’t have a sufficient space or varied enrichment (Broom, 1991).

Sensory deprivation is well known for leading to various behavioural abnormalities (Broom,

1991; Tribe, 2003).

Page 35: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

35

The biological fitness of an animal can indicate welfare (Broom, 1991). Biological fitness is

often defined as the animal’s lifetime reproductive success. However, in zoos, breeding

programme coordinators dictate which individuals will breed in order to maintain genetic

diversity therefore actual biological fitness will be impossible to truly determine.

An abnormal behaviour is a behaviour that differs in pattern, frequency, or context from that

shown by the majority of the species in conditions that allow a full range of behaviour (Broom,

1991). The behaviour of the animal will change to deal with the environmental difficulty,

therefore should only be unusual or short lived experiences. Sometimes, the abnormal

behaviour can develop into a behavioural pathology which no longer processes a beneficial

effect and can potentially harm the perpetrator.

An example of abnormal behaviour that is common in zoos is stereotypy. This is a repeated

sequence of movements that has no obvious purpose (Dantzer, 1986; Mason, 1991; Pitsko,

2003; Tribe, 2003). Stereotypies are commonly seen in situations which are frustrating,

threatening or unstimulating (Broom, 1991; Pitsko, 2003). It seems likely that they are

pointless, energetically expensive signs of psychological pathology (Dantzer, 1986). The more

often the stereotypy is displayed and the length of time the stereotypic behaviour lasts, the

worse the welfare.

There are three major factors that has encouraged change throughout the zoo industry:

The development of zoo legislation and codes of practice,

The development of more suitable captive animal exhibits

Improvements in husbandry and veterinary care.

Page 36: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

36

Most countries around the world have some form of legislation for zoological collections

(Cooper, 2003), that ensure basic provisions are provided for. Zoo enclosure development has

gone through 3 distinct phases over the past 150 years (Shettel-Neuber, 1988):

First generation exhibits – which consist of barred cages which were common in the

pre 20th century zoos. Often held animals in isolation and had limited facilities.

Second generation exhibits – generally larger and constructed out of concrete or

cement, containing animals within a moat or chain link fence. Facilities were more

suitable and allowed renovation. Common around the early to mid-20th century.

Third generation exhibits – larger still and often attempting to replicate the animal’s

natural habitat. Often more immersive to visitors, whilst avoiding welfare issues.

Common from mid-20th century to present (Tribe, 2003).

The various zoo associations that have formed encourage zoos to become members and thus

allow industry self-regulation through the development of codes of conduct and risk of

expulsion from the association.

Research into animal husbandry has massively increased welfare in zoos, resulting in many

cases animals that have higher life expectancy in captivity than in the wild (Kirkwood, 2003).

Regardless the scrutiny of zoos is greater than ever, resulting in the zoo industry attempting

to scrupulously excel in animal welfare, struggling with limited space and still maintaining a

financially viable business. Thorough the understanding of the effect of captivity upon the

behaviour of animals, manipulation of enclosure space and husbandry practices can improve

welfare (Tribe, 2003).

Page 37: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

37

Zoos in the United Kingdom generally consider the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty

to Animals Five Freedoms (Brambell, 1965) as the minimum to its provision of welfare which

state:

Freedom from hunger or thirst

Freedom from discomfort

Freedom from pain, injury or disease

Freedom to express normal behaviour

Freedom from fear and distress

It is in the interest of zoos to ensure high standards of welfare, as an animal displaying

naturalistic behaviours in an environment that mimics its natural one will aid in the delivery

of its conservational and educational messages as well as being more aesthetically pleasing

for the recreation.

Zoos attempt to deliver high welfare through various forms of environmental enrichment.

Environmental enrichment is a practice that enhances the quality of the captive environment

in order to promote optimal psychological and physiological well-being (Mellen and

Shepherdson, 1997). In practice there are four forms of environmental enrichment (Tribe,

2003):

Physical enrichment - providing toys, smells, objects, water features and develop the

enclosure to increase the accessible surface area.

Social enrichment - more suitable sex ratios, increased group size or mixed species enclosures.

Feeding enrichment - incorporates a challenge to the acquisition of food.

Page 38: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

38

Conditioning enrichment - bonding between the animal and its keeper, provides cognitive

enrichment and allows for the keeper to easily achieve certain husbandry tasks (Shepherdson,

2003).

The emphasis is that the quality of space provided is considerably more important than the

quantity. Due to the fact that the determining factor of space used in the wild is that which is

required to acquire the resources necessary to maximise biological fitness. Seeing as these

are provided for in zoos the desire to disperse over large distances will likely be reduced

(Tribe, 2003).

Page 39: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

39

Acknowledgements

I would like to take this opportunity to thank all of those who have provided support and

direction throughout the entire process of developing this thesis. In particular, I would like to

express my sincere gratitude Dr Lisa Holmes and Chester Zoo for allowing me to conduct my

research at their institution as well as providing me with useful materials and assistance. I

would also like to thank Dr Lesley Lace of Manchester Metropolitan University, for all her

constructive criticism and support as my academic mentor. I would like to express gratitude

to Dr Martin Jones and Dr Edwin Harris of Manchester Metropolitan University for all of their

support throughout the production of this thesis. I would like to thank Mrs Maire Wilson,

who, whilst not in the best of health, still found the time and energy to proof read the project

for me. Finally my deepest gratitude extends to Ms Alexa Vargas who has assisted not only

financially but also with moral support throughout the year.

Page 40: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

40

References

Achard, F., Eva, H. D., Stibig, H.-J., Mayaux, P., Gallego, J., Richards, T. and Malingreau, J.-P. (2002) ‘Determination of deforestation rates of the world’s humid tropical forests.’ Science (New York, N.Y.), 297(5583) pp. 999–1002.

Altmann, J. (1974) ‘Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods.’ Behaviour, 49(3) pp. 227–266.

Balmford, A., Leader-Williams, N. and Green, M. J. B. (1995) ‘Parks or arks: where to conserve threatened mammals?’ Review of Industrial Organization, 4 pp. 595–607.

Bowkett, A. E. (2009) ‘Recent captive-breeding proposals and the return of the ark concept to global species conservation.’ Conservation Biology, 23(3) pp. 773–776.

Brambell, R. (1965) ‘The Five Freedoms.’

Broad, S. and Weiler, B. (1998) ‘Captive Animals and Interpretation - A tale of two tigers exhibits.’ The Journal of Tourism Studies, 9(1) pp. 14–27.

Broom, D. M. (1986) ‘Indicators of poor welfare.’ The British veterinary journal, 142(6) pp. 524–526.

Broom, D. M. (1991) ‘Animal welfare : concepts and measurement.’ Journal of Animal Science, 69 pp. 4167–4175.

Catibog-Sinha, C. (2008) ‘Zoo Tourism: Biodiversity Conservation Through Tourism.’ Journal of Ecotourism, 7(2&3) pp. 155–19.

Conde, D. a, Flesness, N., Colchero, F., Jones, O. R. and Scheuerlein, A. (2011) ‘An emerging role of zoos to conserve biodiversity.’ Science pp. 1390–1391.

Conway, W. G. (2011) ‘Buying time for wild animals with zoos.’ Zoo Biology pp. 1–8.

Cooper, M. (2003) ‘Zoo legislation.’ International Zoo Yearbook, 38(July 1987) pp. 81–93.

Cuarón, A. D. (2005) ‘Further role of zoos in conservation: Monitoring wildlife use and the dilemma of receiving donated and confiscated animals.’ Zoo Biology, 24 pp. 115–124.

Dantzer, R. (1986) ‘Behavioral, Physiological and Functional-Aspects of Stereotyped Behavior - a Review and a Reinterpretation.’ Journal of Animal Science, 62(6) pp. 1776–1786.

Dawkins, M. S. (1990) ‘From an animal’s point of view: Motivation, fitness, and animal welfare.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences pp. 1–9.

Gaveau, D. L. a, Wandono, H. and Setiabudi, F. (2007) ‘Three decades of deforestation in southwest Sumatra: Have protected areas halted forest loss and logging, and promoted re-growth?’ Biological Conservation, 134(4) pp. 495–504.

Great Britain (1981) ‘Zoo Licensing Act 1981.’

Gusset, M. and Dick, G. (2010) ‘“Building a Future for Wildlife”? Evaluating the contribution of the world zoo and aquarium community to in situ conservation.’ International Zoo Yearbook pp. 183–191.

Page 41: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

41

Gusset, M. and Dick, G. (2011) The global reach of zoos and aquariums in visitor numbers and conservation expenditures. Zoo Biology.

Hutchins, M. (2003) ‘Zoo and aquarium animal management and conservation: current trends and future challenges.’ International Zoo Yearbook pp. 14–28.

Johnsingh, a. J. T. and Madhusudan, M. D. (2009) ‘Tiger Reintroduction in India: Conservation Tool or Costly Dream?’ Reintroduction of Top-Order Predators, (September 2015) pp. 146–163.

Kellert, S. R. (1997) The Value of Life: Biological Diversity and Human Society. Washington D.C: Island Press.

Kelley, K. W. (1980) ‘Stress and immune function: a bibliographic review.’ Annales de recherches veterinaires. Annals of veterinary research, 11(4) pp. 445–478.

Kinnaird, M. F., Sanderson, E. W., O’Brien, T. G., Wibisono, H. T. and Woolmer, G. (2003) ‘Deforestation trends in a tropical landscape and implications for endangered large mammals.’ Conservation Biology, 17(1) pp. 245–257.

Kirkwood, J. K. (2003) ‘Welfare , husbandry and veterinary care of wild animals in captivity : changes in attitudes , progress in knowledge and techniques.’ International Zoo Yearbook, 38 pp. 124–130.

Kitchener, A. C. and Dugmore, A. J. (2000) ‘Biogeographical change in the tiger,Panthera tigris.’ Animal Conservation, 3 pp. 113–124.

Law, G., Macdonald, a. and Reid, a. (1997) ‘Dispelling some common misconceptions about the keeping of felids in captivity.’ International Zoo Yearbook, 35(1980) pp. 197–207.

Lindburg, D. G. (1988) ‘Improving the feeding of captive felines through application of field data.’ Zoo Biology, 218 pp. 211–218.

Linkie, M., Chapron, G., Martyr, D. J., Holden, J. and Leader-Williams, N. (2006) ‘Assessing the viability of tiger subpopulations in a fragmented landscape.’ Journal of Applied Ecology, 43(3) pp. 576–586.

Linkie, M., Martyr, D. J., Holden, J., Yanuar, a., Hartana, a. T., Sugardjito, J. and Leader-Williams, N. (2003) ‘Habitat destruction and poaching threaten the Sumatran tiger in Kerinci Seblat National Park, Sumatra,'.

Linkie, M., Smith, R. J. and Leader-Williams, N. (2004) ‘Mapping and predicting deforestation patterns in the lowlands of Sumatra.’ Biodiversity and Conservation, 13(10) pp. 1809–1818.

Linkie, M., Wibisono, H. T., Martyr, D. J. and Sunarto, S. (2008) 2008. Panthera tigris ssp. sumatrae. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. [Online] http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/full/15966/0.

Luo, S. J., Johnson, W. E., Smith L. David, J. L. D. and O’Brien, S. J. (2010) What Is a Tiger? Genetics and Phylogeography. Tigers of the World. Second Edi, Elsevier Inc.

Luo, S. J., Kim, J. H., Johnson, W. E., Van Der Walt, J., Martenson, J., Yuhki, N., Miquelle, D. G., Uphyrkina, O., Goodrich, J. M., Quigley, H. B., Tilson, R., Brady, G., Martelli, P., Subramaniam, V., McDougal, C., Hean, S., Huang, S. Q., Pan, W., Karanth, U. K., Sunquist, M., Smith, J. L. D. and O’Brien, S. J. (2004) ‘Phylogeography and genetic ancestry of tigers (Panthera tigris).’ PLoS Biology, 2(12).

Lyons, J., Young, R. J. and M., D. J. (1997) ‘The Effects of Physical Characteristics of the Environment and Feeding Regime on the Behavior of Captive Felids.’ Zoo biology, 16 pp. 71–83.

Page 42: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

42

Mallinson, J. J. C. (2001) ‘A Sustainable Future for Zoos and Their Role in Wildlife Conservation.’ Human Dimensions of Wildlife pp. 1–13.

Margulis, S. W., Hoyos, C. and Anderson, M. (2003) ‘Effect of felid activity on zoo visitor interest.’ Zoo Biology, 22(6) pp. 587–599.

Marino, L., Lilienfeld, S. O., Malamud, R., Nobis, N. and Broglio, R. (2010) ‘Do Zoos and Aquariums Promote Attitude Change in Visitors? A Critical Evaluation of the American Zoo and Aquarium Study.’ Society & Animals pp. 126–138.

Mason, G. J. (1991) ‘Stereotypies : a critical review.’ Animal Behaviour, 41(March 1990) pp. 1015–1037.

McGregor Reid, G. and Zippel, K. C. (2008) ‘Can zoos and aquariums ensure the survival of amphibians in the 21st century?’ International Zoo Yearbook pp. 1–6.

Mellen, J. D. and Shepherdson, D. J. (1997) ‘Environmental enrichment for felids: an integrated approach.’ International Zoo Yearbook pp. 191–197.

Miller, B., Conway, W., Reading, R. P., Wemmer, C., Wildt, D., Kleiman, D., Monfort, S., Rabinowitz, A., Armstrong, B. and Hutchins, M. (2004) ‘Evaluating the Conservation Mission of Zoos, Aquariums, Botanical Gardens, and Natural History Museums.’ Conservation Biology, 18(1) pp. 86–93.

Mitchell, J. D. (1998) ‘Urban Areas Swell.’ Vital signs of 1998: the environmental trends that are shaping our future pp. 108 –109.

Nowell, K. (1999) ‘Far From a Cure: The Tiger Trade Revisited.’ Current.

Nowell, K. (2007) ‘Asian big cat conservation and trade control in selected range States: evaluating implementation and effectiveness of CITES Recommendations The Asian big cats.’ Traffic.

Nowell, K. and Jackson, P. (1996) ‘Wild cats: Status survey and conservation action plan.’ IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. pp. 842–844.

Nyhus, P. J. and Tilson, R. (2004) ‘Characterizing human-tiger conflict in Sumatra, Indonesia: implications for conservation.’ Oryx, 38(01).

Patrick, P. G., Matthews, C., Ayres, D. and Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2007) ‘Conservation and Education: Prominent Themes in Zoo Mission Statements.’ The Journal of Environmental Education, 38(3) pp. 53–60.

Pitsko, L. E. (2003) Wild Tigers in Captivity: A Study of the Effects of the Captive Environment on Tiger Behavior. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University.

Rabinowitz, A. (1995) ‘Helping a species go extinct: The Sumatran rhino in Borneo.’ Conservation Biology pp. 482–488.

Rahbek, C. (1993) ‘Captive breeding- a useful tool in the preservation of biodiversity ?’ Biodiversity and COnservation, 2 pp. 426–437.

Reid, G. M., Macdonald, A. a, Fidgett, A. L. and Hiddinga, B. (2008) Developing the research potential of zoos and aquaria Editors. The European Association of Zoos and Aquaria.

Seidensticker, J. and McDougal, C. (1993) ‘Tiger predatory behaviour, ecology and conservation.’ Symposium of the Zoological Society of London, (65) pp. 105–125.

Page 43: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

43

Shepherdson, D. J. (2003) ‘Environmental enrichment: past, present and future.’ International Zoo Yearbook, 38 pp. 118–124.

Shettel-Neuber, J. (1988) ‘Second and Third Generation Zoo Exhibits: A Comparison of Visitor, Staff and Animal Responses.’ Environment and Behaviour, 20(4) pp. 452–473.

Snyder, N. F. R., Derrickson, S. R., Beissinger, S. R., Wiley, J. W., Smith, T. B., Toone, W. D. and Miller, B. (1996) ‘Limitations of captive breeding in endangered species recovery.’ Conservation Biology, 10(2) pp. 338–348.

Swanagan, J. S. (2000) ‘Factors Influencing Zoo Visitors’ Conservation Attitudes and Behavior.’ The Journal of Environmental Education, 31(4) pp. 26–31.

Tribe, A. (2003) ‘Zoos and Animal Welfare.’ Gatton: University of Queensland pp. 30–40.

Tribe, A. and Booth, R. (2003) ‘Assessing the Role of Zoos in Wildlife Conservation.’ Human Dimensions of Wildlife: An International Journal, 8(1) pp. 65–74.

Turley, S. K. (1999) ‘Conservation and Tourism in the Traditional UK Zoo.’ The Journal of Tourism Studies, 10(2) pp. 2–13.

Page 44: Thesis absolute final checked M Wilson

The Behaviour of Captive Sumatran Tigers (Panthera tigris sumatra) | Michael Godfrey Wilson

44

Appendix Appendix 1 - Recorded behaviours, their descriptions, categories and priorities