The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom...

73
The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current Developments Hugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden (Paper prepared for presentation at the 2013 AERA Conference) Learning Lessons (Mehan 1979) reported one of the earliest qualitative studies of classroom discourse. The study described the basic structures of classroom lessons (Instruction-Reply-Evaluation and Extended Sequences), the turn-taking procedures that teachers deploy to maintain the flow of classroom discourse, and the skills that students’ acquire—often implicitly—in order to contribute as competent members of the classroom community. That study was the product of a unique collaboration between two university researchers: Courtney Cazden, who returned to a primary classroom for one year (1974-5) as the teacher, and Hugh (Bud) Mehan, who observed and analyzed her teaching and wrote the book. In this paper, we describe the origins and purposes of the Cazden-Mehan collaboration, the basic structure of classroom lessons, and recent developments in the study of

Transcript of The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom...

Page 1: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current Developments

Hugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

(Paper prepared for presentation at the 2013 AERA Conference)

Learning Lessons (Mehan 1979) reported one of the earliest qualitative studies of

classroom discourse. The study described the basic structures of classroom lessons

(Instruction-Reply-Evaluation and Extended Sequences), the turn-taking procedures that

teachers deploy to maintain the flow of classroom discourse, and the skills that students’

acquire—often implicitly—in order to contribute as competent members of the classroom

community. That study was the product of a unique collaboration between two university

researchers: Courtney Cazden, who returned to a primary classroom for one year (1974-5)

as the teacher, and Hugh (Bud) Mehan, who observed and analyzed her teaching and

wrote the book.

In this paper, we describe the origins and purposes of the Cazden-Mehan

collaboration, the basic structure of classroom lessons, and recent developments in the

study of classroom discourse that have blossomed since that time. It was originally

presented as an oral conversation at the 2011 conference now converted into a book, and

we will retain some of the first-person informality, mostly in the plural we but

occasionally as a separate I for either Cazden or Mehan as indicated.

How The Project Started

We met first in a summer-long SSRC Conference on “Language, Society, and the

Child” at UC Berkeley in the Summer of 1968, coming there with different intellectual

biographies.

Page 2: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 2AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Cazden: I started out as a public school teacher in a working class neighborhood in

Connecticut (1954-1961). My first and second grade students came from stable families—

white, African American, Puerto Rican—whose fathers had good blue-collar factory jobs.

Our elementary school functioned well, but other teachers and I wondered why so many of

our elementary school graduates ended up in the lowest high school tracks.

Especially in the post-Sputnik urgency about school achievement, public voices

suggested that students’ language, especially Black dialect, might be part of the problem.

Intrigued by such claims, I entered a doctoral program at the Harvard Graduate School of

Education (HGSE) in 1961 and became excited by new developments in cognitive

psychology and linguistics in Cambridge at that time. I participated for several years in

Roger Brown’s pioneering longitudinal study of the language development of three young

children: two from graduate student families and one whose parents had only high school

education (Brown 1973).

By the early 1970’s, now on the HGSE faculty, I wanted to see if my primary

school teaching would be different, now informed by my new understanding of language

development. So I asked Bud Mehan if he could arrange a teaching assignment in a

working-class elementary school, and if he would be interested in documenting my

teaching.

Mehan: At the time of Courtney’s request, I was directing the teacher education

program at UC San Diego and was able to arrange for her to teach in a primary-grade

classroom in the urban core of San Diego for the 1974-95 academic year. Her combined

first, second, and third grade classroom was composed of Latino and African American

students referred to her by other teachers at the school.

Page 3: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 3AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

I approached the prospect of documenting Cazden’s classroom instruction from an

amalgamation of academic orientations. Trained as a sociologist with a specialization in

ethnomethodology (Mehan & Wood 1975), I focused on the social practices that

constituted such educational facts as special education students, ability groupings, and

tracking systems. This constructivist orientation in sociology was reinforced by concurrent

developments in linguistics, anthropology, and psychology. While it dominated the field

of linguistics, Chomsky’s theory of competence was challenged by Shuy et al. (1967),

Gumperz (1971), Labov 1972, Cazden et al. (1972), Erwin-Tripp (1972), Hymes (1974),

and other psycholinguists and sociolinguists for neglecting the social origins and functions

of language. Their studies of “communicative competence” were echoed by

anthropologists such as Frake (1964) and Goodenough (1964) who framed culture in

terms of participation or membership in a society—what one has to know, believe, and

especially do, in order to operate in a manner that is acceptable to the members of a

community or a society. I wanted to extend that logic to the classroom, asking: “what do

students have to do in order to be seen as competent members of the classroom

community?” I hoped to answer that question by examining Courtney’s interactions with

the students in her classroom.

Thus, our interests coalesced in this collaborative project, and we were both

energized by a shared concern for educational equity. Deficit theories were prevalent ways

of explaining the gap in academic achievement among blacks and whites. In the

biologically-based version of deficit thinking (e. g. Jensen 1969), black youth were

portrayed as cognitively inferior to their white counterparts because of racial differences.

In the culturally-based version (e. g., Deutsch et al 1968), the “inferior” speech and

Page 4: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 4AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

thought of low-income black and Latino youth were blamed on their families’ cultural

practices. We hoped that a study of classroom discourse might provide an opportunity to

examine, and perhaps challenge, such deficit thinking.

Videotape played a central role in this study. It has become common for

researchers to extract quotes or strings of utterances from audio- or videotape. But this

study was among the first to use videotape as a tool to study the social organization of

complete events in educational settings, such as classroom lessons, testing sessions,

counseling sessions.1

We videotaped the first hour of school activities everyday for the first week of

school and one hour a day approximately every third week until April. This schedule was

influenced by a combination of practical circumstances and theoretical interests. The one-

hour unit of analysis was selected because videotape for the portable studio equipment we

had available was one hour long. The first hour of the day was chosen because of our

theoretical interests in the skills that students must deploy in order to be competent

members of the classroom community. The day usually began with students being served

snacks and informally interacting at their desks. Then the students were assembled on the

rug to hear the schedule of the day, join in “show and tell.” The first hour was concluded

with the class either instructed as a whole or divided into small groups for instruction in

math and language arts. This procedure produced a corpus of materials with nine instances

of a similar event.

1 For other early studies, see for example, Cicourel et al. 1974; Mishler 1975; Sinclair & Coulthard 1975, McDermott 1976; McDermott et al. 1978, Erickson & Shultz 1978, Shuy & Griffin 1978, Gumperz & Herasimchuk 1972, Cole et al. 1978.

Page 5: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 5AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

The Structure of Classroom Lessons

Teacher-led lessons are the most prevalent form of events that occur regularly in

classrooms. (Others include individual instructional time, sharing time, small collaborative

groups, presentations of learning, laboratory experiments.) Classroom lessons have unique

organizational features that distinguish them from other classroom events and from

ordinary conversations that occur outside of classrooms.

Example #1

Initiation Reply Evaluation

T: And whose is this? Many: Veronica T: Oh, a lot of peopleknew that one

T: Whose name is this? L: Mercedes T: “Mercedes,” all right

This exchange from a lesson Courtney taught early in the school year when

children were still learning their classmates’ names illustrates one of the most salient

features of classroom lessons: they are organized in three-part sequences. A teacher’s

initiation act induces a student’s reply, which in turn invokes a teacher’s evaluation.

Classroom lessons tend to be teacher centered and require students to respond, often

individually, with student behavior evaluated quite publicly.

Evaluations are more likely to occur in classroom interactions than in other

situations in everyday life. Consider these two hypothetical exchanges:

Example #2 Example #3

Speaker #1: What time is it, Denise? Speaker #1: What time is it, Denise?

Speaker #2: 10:30 Speaker #2: 10:30

Speaker #1: Very good, Denise! Speaker #1: Thank you, Denise

Page 6: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 6AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

It would be unusual for example #2 to occur in everyday conversation, because in

everyday conversation, speakers routinely ask questions in order to obtain information

they do not possess (“information seeking questions”), identify the next speaker who is

someone who presumably possesses that information, and acknowledge or thank them for

their trouble (“acknowledgement”). The presence of an evaluation, which comments on a

student’s reply to a question, is one of the features that distinguishes conversations that

take place in classrooms and other educational settings (such as tests), from those that

occur in everyday situations.

The three-part Initiation-Reply-Evaluation (I-R-E) structure exists in large part

because of the “language game” (Wittgenstein 1953) organized by the teacher. In the

“recitation language game,” the teacher begins a sequence looking for a simple, preferred

answer, the student responds (with a short answer), and the teacher evaluates the answer as

correct or not. The third part in the IRE sequence is crucial one for controlling the flow of

interaction in the recitation script. This is the place where the correctness or incorrectness

of a student’s reply is established. If confusion or unintelligibility is not cleared up, then

it’s assumed that the student’s reply was acceptable.

In the recitation game, teachers often ask “known information questions” (Sinclair

& Coulthard 1975; Shuy & Griffin 1978; Mehan 1979) in which the teacher typically

knows the answer before hand. Not all teacher-led initiation acts test students’ previously

acquired knowledge, however. In another class of initiation acts, teachers seek new

information, ask students for their opinions, or interpretations. When “information seeking

questions” are introduced into classroom lessons, much longer, or extended sequences of

interaction often transpire. Because the teacher seeks information from students that s/he

Page 7: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 7AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

doesn’t know in advance, lessons can proceed in unexpected directions, as this exchange

between Cazden and her students illustrate. In a lesson designed to elicit information

about students’ birthplaces and locate them on a large map of the US and northern

Mexico, the following exchanges were about the birthplaces of Prenda and then her

parents:

Example #4

Initiation Reply Evaluation

T: Where were you born, Prenda? Prenda: San Diego T: You were born in

San Diego, all right.T: Can you come up andfind San Diego on themap? (P goes to map and points) T: Right there, okay

That is the end of one short IRE sequence. When the topic then shifts to Prenda’s mother,

a more extended sequence develops:

Initiation Reply Evaluation

T: Now, where did your mother come from? P: Oh, Arkansas T: Okay

T: I did point out Arkansason the map yesterday // P: //I know where

Arkansas isT: Can you – do you knowwhere it is, Prenda? (P goes to the map and

points) T: Yeah, good for you

T: How did you come -–how did you know that? P: Cause I //

C: this morning// T: Wait a minute, wait a minute. I couldn’thear what Prenda said

P: Carolyn told me whereArkansas was

T: Carolyn, how did you

Page 8: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 8AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

remember whereArkansas was? C: ‘Cuz all three of the

grandmothers—Miss Colessaid it started with an A, and I said “There” (pointing).

P: Little Rock T: Yes, I thought maybeyou remem-becauseyou mentioned Little Rockyesterday. Okay, we’ll puta green card for your motheror father there

P: My father wasn’t bornthere

At this point in the lesson, a new extended sequence about Prenda’s father is

begun, this time about who traveled farther to San Diego: Prenda’s mother from Arkansas,

or her father from “Baltimore, Maryland.” Both basic and extended sequences are

interactional in being jointly produced by teacher with students. They are sequential in

that Replies routinely follow Initiations and Evaluations routinely follow the Initiation-

Reply pair.

Other studies of classroom discourse in industrialized nations have reported the

prevalence of the IRE sequence. These include North America, Britain, Europe, New

Zealand and Australia (Sinclair & Coulthard 1975; Malcolm 1979). In addition, studies of

societies with colonial legacies also demonstrate evidence of the IRE sequence within

classrooms. Some of these societies include Tibuai (Levin 1978), those portions of

Western Australia that consist of a high percentage of Aboriginal Australians (Malcolm

1979), Puerto Rico (McCollum (1989), and Papua New Guinea (Miller 1981). Native

students attending schools in former colonies for the first time often encounter linguistic

and structural differences between the language used in the classroom and the home—a

discontinuity that seems to interfere with their learning.

Page 9: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 9AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Cross-Cultural Variations and Educational Recommendations

Students of language use in homes and schools have suggested that classroom

lessons may be compatible with the discourse patterns in Anglo families but may be

incompatible with the discourse patterns of certain minority group families. This

discontinuity, in turn, may contribute to the lower achievement in US schools among

students from African American, Hawaiian American, Native American, and Latino

backgrounds.

Variations on the “default condition” of short IRE sequences (Cazden & Mehan

1989) have been recorded in a variety of settings, often with students and teachers from

cultural groupings that differ from the mainstream.2 We illustrate these variations, calling

upon studies that analyze videotape and transcript materials from complete classroom

lessons.

Au & Jordan (1981) show that a Polynesian teacher used discourse techniques that

differ from the basic I-R-E sequence when teaching Native Hawaiian students in the

Kamehameha Early Education Program (KEEP). In the introductory part of a lesson,

designed to elicit students’ knowledge about frogs before students were invited to read

passages from a book about frogs, the following exchanges occurred when the teacher

asked “what would you do with a frog if you captured it:”

Example #53

Initiation Reply Evaluation

2 For example, Erickson & Mohatt (1982), Philips (1983), Vogt et al. (1987), McCarty et al. (1991) report variations on the IRE sequence in classrooms composed of Native American students. Laosa (1982), Losey (1995), Trueba (1983), Delgado-Gaitan (1987), and Gutierrez et al. (1999) document variations in classes composed of Mexican American students. Au (1980), Au & Jordan (1981), and Tharp & Gallimore (1988) reveal variations in classrooms composed of Native Hawaiian students. Michaels (1981), Heath (1983), Foster (1989), and Lee (1995) report variations in classrooms composed of African American students.3 The conventions used by the authors to display transcripts in the original publications have been simplified for ease of reading in examples 5 and 6.

Page 10: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 10AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

T: What would you do with it? V: Feed itS: Put it in a pond T: Oh, you would put it

in a pond. Okay

T: Lets think if we would do anything else with it. What would you do, Cyndy? What would you do if you had a frog? A: I would poke

the legs

C: You could putIt in a bucket A: Yuck

T: Shh. You would put it in a bucket. Okay, that’s something different

T: What would you do with it? A: You could eat the legs T: Okay. Cyndy might

even eat it. Good

T: You can eat frog, can’t you? A: Yeah, the legs

This teacher, like Courtney in the “birthplaces lesson,” is asking information

seeking questions. She does not know before hand what her students propose to do with a

captured frog. Note also that she explicitly asks for a range of answers (“Lets think if we

would do anything else with it”) and more than one student answers the teacher’s

questions before she evaluates their responses. Even when she identified Cyndy to be the

only student to respond, she received a range of different answers—from benign and

helpful (“feed it” “put it in a pond,” “put it in a bucket” to more mischievous (“poke the

legs”). Researchers associated with KEEP point out these discourse features, especially

those that enable students to assemble answers jointly, are consistent with discourse

patterns in Native Hawaiian communities (Au 1980; Au & Jordan 1981; Tharp &

Gallimore (1988).

Page 11: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 11AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Erickson & Mohatt (1982) revealed similar patterns in a classroom of Algonkian

Indian children taught by an Algonkian Indian teacher on the Odawa reserve in Canada.

Example #6 is from the beginning of a lesson that occurs in a first grade classroom:

Example #6

Initiation Reply Evaluation

T: On our calendar, what’s the next number? Many: Ten//

G: Ten

T: And this is . . . C: Ma::rchG: March T: March tenth

T: Somebody’s got these all mixed//E: Nineteen//C: NineteenF: Wednesday//

Class: WednesdayT: How many in the grade ones are putting the date intheir books? I notice some books you forget to put it and I put down March// Class: Ten//ten//ten

T: And this is . . . Class: 1976 T: Now we don’t have this onthe calendar but we put it

in our books. This is the year19764

This teacher is asking “known information questions,” but her manner of allocating

turns for students to talk in the Initiation frame is different than conventional classrooms:

She did not identify students by name. This turn-allocation technique does not obligate

students to stand out as individuals; instead, they can choose to participate in unison as

members of groups when answering questions. Erickson & Mohatt (1982) point out that

her evaluation moves were different also. Here we see that she did not evaluate students’

replies after each turn. In other exchanges during this and other lessons, she dispensed

4 Parallel lines (//) indicate overlapping speech

Page 12: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 12AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

praise in public, criticism in private. These techniques were applauded for being more

compatible with the cultural style of the Native students than the more individualized IRE

sequences of traditional classrooms (Erickson & Mohatt 1982; cf. Philips 1983).

Erickson & Mohatt (1982) also observed a non-Indian Canadian teacher in the

same school. When he first started teaching, his style was similar to the default condition

in all respects. The longer he taught on the Odawa reserve, the more his teaching practices

shifted in the direction of his students’ culture. In effect, his Indian students informally

socialized him to use more group-based instruction, facilitate more voluntary

contributions, and keep evaluations private as they worked together through time.

The non-Indian Canadian teacher was taught culturally compatible discourse

strategies implicitly by his students. McCullum (1989) makes a similar point about the

cultural congruity of a Puerto Rican teacher’s discourse practices with her Puerto Rican

students. While the Initiation acts of an English-speaking teacher of Puerto Rican students

McCullum studied in Chicago called upon students to respond individually and compete

for the floor, the Initiation acts of a Spanish-speaking teacher of Puerto Rican students in

Puerto Rico permitted students to volunteer answers and respond in groups. McCullum

suggests that the language patterns used in the Puerto Rican classroom signaled a social

relationship between teacher and students that was closer to an “instructional

conversation” (Tharp & Gallimore 1988) than a recitation lesson. And this instructional

conversation was more consistent with the conversational patterns in everyday Puerto

Rican life.

Some researchers have engaged teachers explicitly in culturally compatible

discourse strategies. Heath (1983) reports that the teachers used instructional strategies

Page 13: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 13AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

when instructing low-income students in “Trackton” that were not prevalent in the

students’ homes. Whereas Trackton teachers would use known information questions in

IRE formats, Trackton parents would use statements or imperatives. And when parents did

ask questions of Trackton children, they called for nonspecific comparisons or analogies

as answers. Heath (1983) concluded that the language used in Trackton homes did not

prepare children to cope with the major characteristics of the language used in classrooms:

utterances that were interrogative in form but directive in pragmatic function, known

information questions, and questions that asked for information from books.

In order to increase Trackton students’ verbal skills in naming objects, identifying

their characteristics, providing descriptions out of context, and responding to known

information questions, Heath (1983) worked with the Trackton teachers on ways to

appropriate the community’s ways of interacting with children. After reviewing tapes of

parent-child interactions with researchers, teachers began social studies lessons with

questions that asked for personal experiences and analogic responses, such as “What’s

happening there?” “Have you ever been there?” “What’s this like?” These questions were

similar to the questions that parents asked their children at home. Their use during the

beginning of lessons was productive in generating active responses from previously

passive and “nonverbal” Trackton students. Once the teachers increased the students’

participation in lessons using home-based questioning styles, they were able to use more

school-based questioning styles.

This line of research has led to several educational recommendations. The basic

idea is to use students’ home knowledge and cultural practices as a resource in classroom

instruction. Teachers are encouraged to build upon the strengths of home language and

Page 14: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 14AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

culture. This may involve modifying the participation structures of the classroom to be

more compatible with the participation structures of the home.

The work reviewed above evidences a concern to enable under represented

minority group students to participate more actively in classroom lessons. The work we

review in the next section is concerned with encouraging students to gain control over the

complexities of academic discourse. The work of Teresa McCarty, Carol Lee, and their

colleagues represents a transition from a concern for participation to a concern with

cognitive development within studies of classroom discourse.

For decades Native American students have been portrayed in the literature as quiet,

passive, nonresponsive. They have been said to learn by observing and doing, not through

listening and saying (More, 1989; Tharp, 1989). Often in the name of cultural

compatibility, educators have emphasized nonverbal means of instruction and cue-

response scripted drills as a way to reach passive Indian students. McCarty et al. (1991)

say that these erstwhile attempts have had an unfortunate side effect. Indian students are

neither taught to engage in reasoning with evidence nor to employ inquiry methods.

Working with the Navajo-staffed Native American Materials Development Center,

staff members of the Rough Rock Indian reservation school implemented an inquiry-based

bilingual social studies program. Students were asked to scour their community and return

with lists of things needed in their community. Students were then asked to group like

items and justify their choices. Eventually they reached a consensus, identifying things

needed and things they’d like to have. That consensus led to the lesson generalization:

“Rough Rock is a community because people work together to meet their needs and solve

mutual problems” (McCarty et al., 1991: 57). The lessons in “Navajo Humanities” suggest

Page 15: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 15AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

that Navajo students will indeed respond enthusiastically to inquiry-based questioning that

asks them to provide evidence for their observations.

In a similar manner, Lee (1995) described how six teachers in two urban schools

helped their high school classes composed primarily of African American students

increase their skills of interpreting rich literary texts by introducing and building upon

material drawn from cultural routines and rituals. The teachers first helped their students

analyze the rules for achieving irony in signifying rituals.5 Next they invited students to

read and analyze contemporary literature, such as Tony Morrison’s (1970) The Bluest Eye

and Alice Walker’s (1982) The Color Purple. Teachers regularly asked questions during

instructional units that required the students to make inferences, interpret figurative

language, and draw upon evidence from disparate parts of the text as well as personal

experiences of the social world embodied in the texts. A comparison of students’ scores on

pre- and post-tests of text-interpretation document students’ shift to more sophisticated

interpretations of complex inferential questions.

Recent Developments

Classroom lessons composed primarily of IRE sequences have been criticized for

being overly teacher-centered, for converging on one correct response, and for demanding

factual information or yes-no responses from students (MacBeth 2003; Wells & Arauz

2006; Edelsky 2006). Partially in response to such criticisms, we have witnessed a shift in

focus within the study and practice of classroom discourse. The studies revealing cultural

5 Many signifying episodes are intended to insult and criticize. Examples of signifying, such as the following, whose purpose is to instruct and inform were used in Lee’s study:

Rochelle: Girl, you sure do need to join the Metrecal for lunch bunchGrace: (noncommitedly) Yeah, I guess I am putting on a little weightRochelle: Now look here girl, we both standing here soaking wet and you still trying to tell me it ain’t raining (Lee 1995: 615).

In this conversation between two sisters, one has tried to conceal the fact that she is pregnant from the other.

Page 16: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 16AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

variations on the recitation script we reviewed in the previous section sought to increase

the participation of minority students in classroom lessons in the name of educational

equity. If minority students could be encouraged to take more turns at talk in classroom

lessons, then they could be better prepared to contribute more actively in the full

dimensions of school life. More recent studies that we review in this section are concerned

with prompting students to engage in more sophisticated forms of reasoning.

This cognitive shift does not abandon the equity agenda. It transforms it.

Teachers who work to support academically productive talk in classrooms see this as a

tool to promote knowledge building and powerful learning for students, especially in

science, mathematics, and language arts. Building knowledge and reasoning skills in these

fields, in turn, is seen as a way to better prepare underserved students for college,

participation in the political economy, and democratic institutions.

In other words, we are seeing a shift in the classroom language game from

recitation to reasoning. A prominent goal of the reasoning game is to socialize students

into “academic discourse,” i. e., that genre in which ideas are presented (in written or oral

form) in academic or scholarly contexts that privileges the analytical and the presentation

of evidence to advance an argument (Toulman 1958). Academic discourse is a special

genre. No one is a native speaker. To be sure some students have gained familiarity with

certain of its aspects through family interaction or religious training (for the bar/bas

mitzvah, for example) but it is not a completely natural way of speaking for any students.

Therefore, all students need to learn to use the complete repertoire of academic discourse.

Students who become fluent users of academic discourse learn to explain their

ideas in detail, invoking evidence to support their reasoning. Evidence in support of claims

Page 17: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 17AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

may have been gathered from observations, demonstrations, experiments in science or

math classes, or ideas from texts they have been reading in language arts social studies or

history classes, for example. They are also able to provide evidence-based claims that

compete with those of other students. In order to generate claims supported by evidence

and counter-factual claims, students must learn to listen carefully to each other with

respect, take seriously and evaluate their own and other competing ideas.

Fostering students to reason with evidence is not an easy task (Kovalainena and

Kumpulainen 2007). Teachers require considerable coaching in order to engage students

in productive discussions (Tharp & Gallimore 1988; Michaels , O’Conner, and Resnick

2008). In addition, teachers have to reconcile the tension between a desire for moving

students toward more sophisticated forms of reasoning and maintaining classroom control

(Emanuelson & Sahlström 2008). This tension exists because the initiation acts that foster

reasoning with evidence often invite students to provide interpretations or offer their own

life experiences. While these discourse moves can open up conversations, they can

introduce lines of interaction that pulls lessons in unexpected and unproductive directions.

In this section we illustrate classroom discourse featuring teachers who, cognizant of this

challenge, incorporate initiations that elicit students’ observations and interpretations and

encourage them to provide evidence of their reasoning.

The shift from recitation to reasoning also requires a shift in the way we display

transcripts. Teacher-led interaction with students still conforms to a tripartite pattern.

Classroom interaction continues to be organized by teachers, a social fact captured by

placing the teacher in the first position of an instructional sequence. So, too, for the most

part, students continue to occupy the second, reply position in the sequence. The third

Page 18: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 18AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

position in the 3-part sequence changes considerably, however. The work that is done here

is more involved than evaluating the correctness of a student’s reply. Teachers (and

sometimes students) acknowledge replies, reformulate, revoice, or comment upon them.

Therefore, to be consistent with the materials we present—and recommendations of other

researchers (e. g., Wells 1993)--we change the name of the third slot from Evaluation to

Feedback.

Encouraging Students to Display the Grounds of their Reasoning

In a lesson designed to encourage middle school students to practice making

estimates of ratios and proportions, Marilyn Burns (1989) first used cuisenaire rods of

certain lengths and color to measure the length of a student’s desk. Then, she divided the

class into small groups and asked them to measure their desks using rods of different

lengths. After she placed samples of the (magnetized) rods on the chalkboard, the

following exchanges between teacher and students transpired:

Example #7

Initiation Responses FeedbackT: So, what I’m interested in hearing now is how you figured out how many blue, red, and white rodsit took to stretch across the table. And I’m also interested in hearinghow many different ways there areto think about that. So, Craig, howmany blue rods does it take tostretch across the table and how didyou figure that? C: Six and two thirds

T: And how did you figure that out? C: Because, um thereis 3 light greens that fitinto the blue and then you go “3, 3 greens times 6” that would 18and that won’t be a full

Page 19: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 19AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

rod so take off 2. Soyou put down 2 and takeout one green. That would be 2/3, that wouldbe 6/23

T: And Nathan, how many blue rods do you think stretch across the table? N: About 6

T: And explain to me how yougot that answer N: We took the blues and

then took the light greensand compared how many would stretch across andworked our way down andgot up to 20 green. Andwe just stuck these on topto see how many of thesewould fit on 20 green

T: Kim, how many blue rods doyou think it takes to stretchacross the table, and how didyou figure that out? K: 6 3/4. We just laid ‘em

out and then laid abouthalf on the table lengthwiseand if we didn’t have enoughwe just like used some biggerones and like added themtogether

T: So, when I asked this question, I wind up getting 3 different answers. I get 6 2/3 from Craig and I get 6 /34 from Kim and I get 6 from Nathan. It shows me there are still differences of opinion but everyone is quite close with that.

This teacher asks two types of questions, often within the same initiation act. One

asks for students to provide an estimate of measurement. The other asks for a reason for

Page 20: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 20AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

the answer given previously. The teacher doesn’t know exactly what the students’ answers

will be; so, these are information seeking questions that specifically seek interpretations

and rationales. The teacher actively seeks divergent answers to her questions (“And I’m

also interested in hearing how many different ways there are to think about that”). The

students present the teacher with a range of answers; none are exactly the same. Notably

absent is a verbal evaluation after each student’s reply. The teacher neither praises a

correct answer, rejects an incorrect answer, nor verbally encourages further work. Instead,

the teacher provides an overall assessment of this phase of the lesson after a number of

students have replied. This wide range of teacher moves motivates the change of the name

of the third slot in the instructional sequence from Evaluation to Feedback. The lesson

continues with the teacher eliciting more estimates and reasons for estimations from the

students. She concludes the lesson with a lengthy summary of the exchanges and why she

encourages students to explain their reasoning.

Encouraging Students to Wrestle with the “Big Ideas” of Science

The teacher of a combined 1st--2nd grade classroom composed of a diverse group of

students employs “science workshop,” developed by the teacher in collaboration with

Chèche Konnen staff (Warren & Rosebery 2011). In Science Workshop students explore

the meaning potential of a student’s idea or a scientific account such as Newton’s Laws of

Motion or one of naturalist David Attenborough’s figurative descriptions of plant life. The

idea is written on chart paper, contextualized, and then presented to the class for

discussion.

The lesson discussed below is a component of a unit designed to investigate the

“big job of plants,” which was defined as making new plants. In a previous class, the

students suggested various titles for a mural they had composed of the life cycle of a

Page 21: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 21AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

pumpkin plant, and discussed what the suggested titles highlighted about the life of a

pumpkin. During the course of that discussion Hakim made an observation that the teacher

used to start the following conversation:

Example #8

Initiation Reply Feedback

T: And so today we’re gonna’ start with something that happened on Monday. Okay? And uh we’re gonna’think a little bit first about what those words meant, what did they mean by those words, and um how those words helped us to think.

Hakim said, “It’s like a spider, because when the mom dies it lays eggs before it dies.” [reads it again] And I want to start with Hakim telling us what you meant by that and then we’ll all talk together about it. What did you mean by that when you said, “it’s like a spider because when the mom dies it lays eggs before it dies”

H: Because sometimes some pumpkins

T: Speak loud, your loudest voice, okay, honey?

H: Because sometimes some pumpkins open up and they have- and they drop seeds. And sometimes farmers collect them and then they put ‘em in the ground

T: So:: “it’s like a spider” because when the mom dies it lays eggs before it dies”

T: How’s that like a spider what you just said?

H: um ‘cuz they plant more- um because they get planted all over again

T: uh hunh, okay

T: Let’s see what other people think

Page 22: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 22AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Hakim means. 6

A: Well I think

T: Speak nice and loud. And then I want to hear from Keshia, Nick and Nick, so you be thinking

A: I think um that- well so [looking intently at mural of pumpkin patch life cycle on opposite side of the room] the mother um is the big orange pumpkin and then when it gets ready to die before it lays like um eggs, but the eggs are actually not really eggs they’re seeds for the pumpkin, and then the mother dies and the se- um the baby spiders, the seeds start to grow and they get a little bigger, bigger spider, then bigger spider, then bigger spider, and then they get eggs and they die and it just keeps going

T: So you’re saying the parents are the orange ones and then when they die they become rotten and ro- and the eggs that they lay are the seeds into the soil [Annie nods] T: Interesting. Okay,

T: Who was next? Go ahead Ayanna. Then it was Bruno.

A: um like um like if the um- A: Wait what were we talking about?

T: How is this- what does Hakim mean when he says “It’s like a spider because when the mom dies it lays eggs before it dies”

A: Um:: He means like um the mother of the pumpkin, it lays eggs and like the um baby spider is like- the little egg was the baby spider, the baby spider it needs to do all the like- it needs to know like how to first-

6 Two students follow with their comments – skipped because of time – the rest is continuous

Page 23: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 23AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

like it might not know how to like (walk)- like it might- if like it was a baby spider, and it was so small, and no one could see it, it would step on it but it would die, and if it died, it wouldn’t grow up T: Right

T: But the mom dies and in the spider when the mom dies it lays eggs Ay: Yeah

T: The eggs get laid before it dies. How is that like the pumpkin? ‘Cuz that’s what Hakim said, it’s like a pumpkin. The pumpkin is like the spider

Ay: Because the- that- the way as the pumpkin dies is like it gets rotten and the way as the spider dies it’s gonna get old T: Yeah//

Ay: =and it’s kinda like the same because um the um rotten pumpkins- ‘cuz of the rotten pumpkins and the um spiders, they’re getting old and that’s how they die. T: uh huhn

T: And what happens when they die? Ay: They lay eggs

T: Both of em? Ay: Well not the pumpkin [Smiling]

T: Well that’s what Hakim says, it’s like the spider Ay: The pumpkin- the pumpkin doesn’t

Ay: The lay eggs because- um if the pumpkin lays eggs it would- wait- if the pumpkin laid eggs um that would be weird because if the pumpkin laid eggs, and um the spider laid eggs, and the pumpkin didn’t, didn’t have nothing in it, that would be//

H: //The pumpkin does have something in it

T: Tell her what you think

H: The pumpkin has seeds insideAy: I know but I’m talking about the egg. The egg doesn’t have- a spider only has an egg ‘cuz if the pumpkin had an egg (3 sec pause)

T: But you’re thinking inside the egg of

Page 24: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 24AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

a spider when it opens it’s a spider// Ay: Yeah//

T: //Inside the egg of a pumpkin when it opens it’s not-you don’t think it’s a pumpkin? Ay: No [shaking her head] T: That’s interesting

T: You have anything to say about that? H: Yeah

T: Go ahead H: The seed is almost like an egg

T: Speak up and talk to Ayanna H: The seed is almost like an egg

T: Can you explain how you think that? H: Because there’s seeds inside a pumpkin because there’s seeds inside a pumpkin and sometimes some of the pumpkins splat open and they get planted by theirselves

T: And so how is a seed like an egg? I heard you [to Bruno] say it, that the egg is the seed. Go ahead, say what you think. B: The seed is kinda like an egg because an

egg ‘cuz like something’s inside a egg

T: Talk to Ayanna, too B: So the um spider when it lays eggs and then it dies, it’s like fir- when it dies and lays eggs something’s in there, a spider, a born spider, and the same thing with the pumpkin, it has- but except it has more eggs, like the seeds, but the seeds are eggs but it looks like- but in- the spider and the pumpkin have something inside the seed and the egg T: So the seed is

the egg

T: So when you- inside the egg of a spider, say, would be a spider, or something that might start a spider, right? We don’t know// B: (// )

T: //Inside the seed, is there a pumpkin? (Nick on T’s left, vigorously shaking his head “no” Suzi nodding her head “yes” vigorously.)

B: Something like uh//

T: //What do you think? Suzi, you’re saying “yes,” go ahead S: ‘Cuz it’s like a sprout of a

pumpkin T: ‘Cuz we saw, right, what happens, it’s like the beginning of a pumpkin//

B:// like the ( )//S: Uh huhn

B: ‘Cuz like the spider has something in the egg and the pumpkin has something in the seed, they both have something in the seed and the egg//H: //and then it grows and grows and grows

Page 25: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 25AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

T: The seed and the egg have something in it that and it grows and grows and grows B: It’s like it’s born T: It’s like it’s born, it’s like

the beginning of it getting born.Cool

The teacher commenced this lesson by tying the topic back to issues raised by

students in a previous lesson—the similarities between pumpkins and spider life cycles.

Similarities—and differences--are key concepts in science (as well as other domains of

academic life and everyday life). Therefore, this teacher is working to ensure her students

understand these important terms and can use them in academic discourse. In doing so, the

teacher encouraged many types of responses from the students: claims, interpretations,

explanations, and justifications.

The teacher encouraged students to offer divergent or competing interpretations.

When the teacher asked: “Let’s see what other people think Hakim means,” she obtained

an interpretation from Annie that diverged from his: “I think . . . the mother um is the big

orange pumpkin and then when it gets ready to die before it lays like um eggs, but the

eggs are actually not really eggs they’re seeds for the pumpkin, and then the mother dies

and the se- um the baby spiders, the seeds start to grow and they get a little bigger, bigger

spider, then bigger spider, then bigger spider, and then they get eggs and they die and it

just keeps going.” During the course of this lengthy response, this student formulated an

identity relation between the big orange pumpkin and the mother and equated the spider

world with the pumpkin world (“spider eggs” become “seeds for the pumpkin”). Annie

also alluded to the development of new life (“the seeds start to grow”) as a process of

incremental growth (“they get a little bigger…”). Thus, Annie expanded Hakim’s analogy

by voicing the idea that life and death are intimately connected—a sophisticated biological

observation.

Page 26: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 26AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

At another point in the lesson, the teacher asked “and so how is a seed like an

egg?” she obtained a claim from Bruno that differed from Hakim’s: “The seed is kinda’

like an egg because an egg- cuz like something’s inside a egg.”

Ayanna offered yet another interpretation of Hakim’s original analogy: the

possibility of difference in the spider egg (which contains a live baby spider) and

the pumpkin seed (which does not contain a live pumpkin). Here Ayanna accepted

the teacher’s invitation to explore alternatives by focusing her attention on the

inside of the spider egg and pumpkin seed.

In addition to encouraging different interpretations, the teacher often expected

students to justify their reasoning. For example, Hakim began his statement with a

“because clause” which served to justify his position: “Because sometimes some

pumpkins open up and they have- and they drop seeds.” On another occasion in the lesson,

the teacher asked Hakim to explain his reasoning: “Can you explain how you think that?”

Hakim obliges: “Because there’s seeds inside a pumpkin, because there’s seeds inside a

pumpkin and sometimes some of the pumpkins splat open and they get planted by

theirselves.”

When she explicitly invited other students to assess Hakim’s assertion that

pumpkins are like spiders, the teacher received a range of explanations, all of which began

with a “because clause:”

Hakim: “Because sometimes some pumpkins open up and they have- and they

drop seeds. And sometimes farmers collect them and then they put ‘em in the ground. “

Hakim: “um ‘cuz they plant more- um because they get planted all over again.”

Page 27: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 27AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Ayanna: “and it’s kinda’ like the same because um the um rotten pumpkins- ‘cuz

of the rotten pumpkins and the um spiders, they’re getting old and that’s how they die.”

On another occasion, when the teacher asked “is there a pumpkin inside a seed,”

one student (Nick) answered “no,” while another (Suzi) answered “yes.” The teacher then

encouraged them to provide evidence of their reasoning. From Susi, she received this

response “‘Cuz it’s like a sprout of a pumpkin.”

Throughout this discussion, the teacher seldom evaluated the quality of students’

answers or sanctioned violations of turn-allocation procedures. She did reformulate

students’ answers often, however. For example, Hakim modified his original position and

now asserted “The seed is almost like an egg.” Bruno recapitulated this position: “The

seed is kinda’ like an egg because an egg ‘cuz like something’s inside a egg.” The teacher

reformulated these observations as “a seed is like an egg.”

When Bruno offered the claim that seeds and eggs are similar in that they are both

born, the teacher expanded Bruno’s insight further, “It’s like it’s born, it’s like the

beginning of it getting born.” Here, the teacher and students are jointly constructing an

understanding of sophisticated biological concepts together (Warren & Rosebery 2011).

When Annie offered her elaborate analogy about mothers/pumpkins and

eggs/seeds (discussed above), the teacher reformulated that response as: “So you’re

saying the parents are the orange ones and then when they die they become rotten and ro-

and the eggs that they lay are the seeds into the soil.” After Annie nodded in the

affirmative, she said “ Interesting. Okay . . .” before asking her next question.

The teacher reformulated Hakim’s observation: “Sometimes some pumpkins open

up and they have- and they drop seeds. And sometimes farmers collect them and then

Page 28: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 28AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

they put ‘em in the ground” as “ So:: ‘it’s like a spider’ because when the mom dies it lays

eggs before it dies.”

Encouraging Teachers to Deploy Discourse Moves to Improve Students’ Reasoning

Some teachers learn to socialize students to use academic discourse on their own;

however, many teachers require explicit tuition in order to do so. Responding to this need,

some teacher-researcher learning communities assist teachers deploy discourse moves that

stimulate students to move toward more sophisticated forms of reasoning and modes of

expression.7 These teacher-researcher collaborations employ weekend or week-long

workshops, semester-long seminars, and in some cases web-based courses, to assist

teachers increase the quantity and quality of academically productive discussions in

classroom lessons.

In this section, we use materials from the “Talk Science” project (Doubler,

McWilliams, and Michaels 2012) to illustrate this new development in the analysis and

use of classroom discourse. Michaels and O’Conner (2012) have identified 9 “talk

moves,” adapted from Chapin, O’Conner & Anderson (2009), that are intended to help

teachers support 4 goals: 1) helping students share, expand, and clarify their own ideas,

going public with their thinking, so that others can think with them; 2) helping students

listen carefully to one another; 3) helping students dig deeper into their reasoning, with

data and evidence; and 4) think with others. After presenting the Talk Science example,

7 For example: The Inquiry Project and Talk Science Professional Learning Pathways (with on-going research with teachers in urban, suburban, and rural schools in Massachusetts and Vermont using the web-based PD tools).  Resources are available to all:  [http://inquiryproject.terc.edu/prof_dev/pathway/pathway4.cfm]2) Professional development in math, taking place in school-based study groups, with Chapin, O'Connor, & Anderson, N., (2009) in conjunction with the facilitators guide: Anderson, Chapin, & O'Connor, (2012). 3) Institute for Learning (on-line facilitated workshops) on Accountable Talk: Goldman, P., [http://ifl.lrdc.pitt.edu/ifl/index.php/professional_development/online_facilitated_workshops] CRMSE-SDSU

Page 29: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 29AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

we chart discourse moves described by O’Conner et al (2009) and Michaels & O’Conner

(2012) on display in this lesson.

In Example #9 from a science classroom in a 4th grade classroom in the Boston

area serving predominately African American youth from low-income backgrounds, the

teacher arranged students into small groups. Cubes made of copper and aluminum were

dropped into beakers of water. Students were instructed to record their observations in

their science notebooks. The teacher composed charts on the wall that summarized the

characteristics of the cubes in water. When the students were assembled on the

classroom’s common meeting area, a rug, with their science notebooks in front of them,

the teacher initiated a conversation about what they observed. She framed the discussion

by asking which of the metal cubes would make the water level rise— the weight or the

volume of the objects. She started the discussion by calling on a student:

Example #9

Initiation Response Feedback

T: Mathias, you want to talk to thegroup about the conclusion that youcame up with? And lets make sureeveryone’s looking at Mathias, toshow we are listening to him M: Well, my group—we

came up-w-we found out thatum that I-well, we found outthat the-we thought becau-itwas because of the volume inthe water level, because umum we found out that the s-the-s-the volume in the water level were the same,but the weight was different.And I thought that, if—if theweight, was the um, there was more weight, um, in thealuminum cube then the um

Page 30: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 30AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

aluminum cube, then itshould depend on the volu—on the volume because, theweight, if it was more, thecopper was more, then itwould have more volumeif it w--, it really dependedon the weight

T: Does anyone want to respondto that? Lets make sureeveryone’s looking at Mathias, toshow we are listening to him andkind of respond with their ownideas, or can add another idea toit? Flevor, go ahead. Talk to Mathias about how you feel aboutwhat he said F: I agree with what you said

because this for example likeif you put—if you had a big umcan of water and you putsomething like that in a big cupof water, the water level wouldrise a lot, and if you put in a copper cube, and its not evengonna’—its not going to rise thatmuch, even though that coppercube would weigh more thana eraser T: Ahh

T: And can someone explain orrepeat for us what Flevorthought would happen if I putan object this big (holds uperaser) in water? Javon, whatdo you think would happen ifI put an object this big in a cup of water? J: He said that if you, if you

had like a big br-like a bigbottle of like water and put the eraser in it, then it would probably like rise a lot then//

T: //Why? What would make that Water rise a lot? What is it about this object that would make thewater level rise a lot more thanthe cooper cube or the aluminum

Page 31: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 31AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

cube? J: because that has a differentvolume than the copper cube

T: Alicia, you want to share withthe group some of your thoughts? Al: Well, I kind of disagree

Because like the uh

T: With who? A: With Flevor, because…

T: Talk to him and tell him whyyou disagree with him A: I disagree with you

because like the um, erasercould soak up the water

T: Aisha, you want to addsomething? Ai: I have a question for you

Flevor. Um, what if theobject had like buoyancy,like it was able to float?

T: Oh! I think that’s a good group question, question for the group

T: But go ahead, Flevor T: Then it would be adifferent story, becauseif-if it w-if it hadbuoyancy, then it wouldn’treally be taking up muchspace, so, but I wouldn’tknow if it would—it wouldn’t be sinking likeI was talking about

(lesson continues)

Even though this lesson continues past the point at which the transcription stops,

this excerpt provides sufficient detail to illustrate the ways in which a teacher can invite

students to reason with evidence. The teacher introduced the norms for classroom

discussion throughout the school year such as insisting that students listen carefully to one

another, give each other time to think, and direct a comment at the author of a previous

comment. She reinforced them within this lesson: (“lets make sure everyone’s looking at

Mathias, to show we are listening to him;” “talk to Mathias about how you feel about what

he said;” “talk to him and tell him why you disagree with him”).

Page 32: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 32AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Here are the discourse moves appearing in this transcript followed by quotes from

the teacher:

Discourse goal—Helping students use evidence of reasoning

Specific moves:

“Do you want to talk to the group about the conclusion that you came up with?”

“Javon, what do you think would happen if I put an object this big in a cup of

water?”

“Alicia, you want to share with the group some of your thoughts?”

“Aisha, you want to add something?”

Discourse Goal--Helping students deepen their reasoning by challenging or providing

a counter example

Specific move:

“Does anyone want to respond to that?”

Discourse Goal—Helping students listen carefully to one another

Specific moves:

“Lets make sure everyone’s looking at Mathias, to show we are listening to him”

“And can someone explain or repeat for us what Flavor thought would happen if I

put an object this big (holds up eraser) in water?

Discourse move--Inviting students to listen carefully to one another by rephrasing or

repeating

“Can someone explain or repeat for us what Flevor thought would happen if I put

an object this big (holds up eraser) in water?”

Page 33: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 33AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Now we turn our attention to the students’ responses to these teacher’s initiation

moves. If we look past their false starts and frequent injections (such as “like,” “um,” and

“ah”), then we see evidence of students reasoning with evidence. Mathias, the first

student invited by the teacher to explain his reasoning offered a long soliloquy: “Well, my

group—we came up-w-we found out that um that I-well, we found out that the-we thought

becau-it was because of the volume in the water level, because um um we found out that

the s-the-s-the volume in the water level were the same, but the weight was different.” The

gist of this response is: First we thought weight, but we found out that it was volume that

made the difference, because if the primary factor had been weight, then the cube that was

heavier would have made the water level rise more. But it didn’t. Therefore the causal

factor has to be volume. In short, his reasoning is cogent and sophisticated, including the

use of evidence-based counterfactuals.

When the teacher asked why a blackboard eraser would make the water level rise a

lot more than the copper cube or the aluminum cube, Javon responded succinctly:

“because that [the eraser] has a different volume than the copper cube.”

Consider this exchange between the teacher and Alicia:

Initiation Reply Feedback

T: Alicia, you want to share withthe group some of your thoughts? Al: Well, I kind of disagree

Because like the uhT: With who? A: With Flevor, because…

T:Talk to him and tell him whyyou disagree with him A: I disagree with you

because like the um, erasercould soak up the water

Page 34: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 34AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

In her second turn-at-talk, Alisha’s also stated her reasons for her conclusion concerning

the response of a blackboard eraser compared to a metal object when placed into a beaker

of water succinctly.

The following exchange demonstrates the unpredictability of lessons when

teachers invite a range of divergent thinking. Aisha took the lesson in a direction probably

not anticipated by the teacher. In the process she introduced a new term, “buoyancy,” to

the discussion and challenged Trevor to justify his position.

Initiation Reply Feedback

T: Aisha, you want to addsomething? Ai: I have a question for you

Flevor. Um, what if theobject had like buoyancy,like it was able to float?

T: Oh! I think that’s a good group question, question for the group

T: But go ahead, Flevor T: Then it would be adifferent story, becauseif-if it w-if it hadbuoyancy, then it wouldn’treally be taking up muchspace, so, but I wouldn’tknow if it would—it wouldn’t be sinking likeI was talking about

Summary

In sum, the examples of teacher-student discourse in this section vary from the IRE

sequences described in recitation lessons. Notably, teachers’ initiations moved from a

convergence to divergence, that is away from the pursuit of a single correct answer to

encouraging multiple responses to an initiating move. These initiations also resulted in

longer and more complex replies from individual students, including evidence-based

reasoning and challenges and questions from students directed at their peers. They

Page 35: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 35AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

extended discussion across many students and turns at talk. These teacher initiations

covered a wide range, calling for students to offer interpretations and to provide evidence

of their claims. They also encouraged students to provide varied interpretations. When

conversations became animated, students didn’t wait for the teacher to initiate the next

round. The students continued responding among themselves, which sometimes produced

a cascade of replies and student initiations.

The discourse moves in the third position in the instructional triad also vary widely

from the IRE sequences described above. In addition to judging the correctness of

students’ answers or monitoring students’ conformance to turn-allocation rules, teachers

revoice students’ replies (e. g., from Example #8: “Teacher: The seed and the egg have

something in it that and it grows and grows and grows. Bruno: It’s like it’s born. Teacher:

It’s like it’s born, it’s like the beginning of it getting born. Cool”); comment on the course

of the lesson (e.g., from Example #7 “Teacher: So, when I asked this question, I wind up

getting 3 different answers. I get 6 2/3 from Craig and I get 6 /34 from Kim and I get 6

from Nathan. It shows me there are still differences of opinion but everyone is quite close

with that); compliment students for fresh ideas (e. g., from Example #9: “Teacher: Oh! I

think that’s a good group question, question for the group”).

The teacher-student discourse discussed in this section displays many of the

features of Toulmin’s (1958) formulation of productive arguments in the law, the natural

and social sciences. Toulmin proposed that a productive argument must ground a claim by

providing evidence or data that supports the claim. In the snippets discussed in this

section, teachers encouraged students to provide interpretations that are supported by

evidence that grounds their argument. When students offered alternative lines of

Page 36: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 36AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

reasoning, teachers encouraged them to invoke counterfactual evidence to support their

opposing claims.

Toward Further Research

In concluding, we call attention to three questions that merit further attention. First,

all the research discussed so far is limited to teacher-led lessons. Learning Lessons

(1979), where we started, was deliberately limited to those segments from the San Diego

classroom. Because such lessons remain significant, in purpose and frequency, across K-

12 classrooms, this review has not attempted to report beyond that boundary. But other

participant structures are possible and valuable for other educational objectives.

In her new book, Classroom Discourse and Democracy: Making Meanings

Together (2012), Susan Mayer’s objective is for all students to acquire “interpretive

authority,” a specific kind of ”intellectual authority” as O’Connor suggests in her

Foreword. With tape recordings from discussions of literature in six secondary

classrooms, Mayer analyses the discourse in segments she identified as teacher-led (found

in all six), student-led (in two classrooms), and co-led (in 3 classrooms).

To do justice to these differences, Mayer has retained the tri-partite structure—

now renamed Framing, Developing, and Evaluating— and created subcodes for all three

(see Mayer 2009, Appendix 1). Finally, all these moves can be coded for either teacher or

students. Mayer’s work on “more distributed forms of knowledge construction” (Mayer

2012: 99) should stimulate interest in further analyses of such an expanded set of

participant structures, in which “the traditional IRF sequence becomes one possibility

among many” (Mayer 2012: 51).

Page 37: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 37AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Second, the conversations between teachers and students discussed in this paper

also display the limitations of category schemes (including the IRE scheme) that try to

place utterances into discrete, mutually exclusive categories--Initiation or Evaluation, for

instance. Often an utterance performs more than one function at the same time. For

example, in the discussion about spiders and pumpkins (in Example #8), the teacher said:

“So you’re saying the parents are the orange ones and then when they die they become

rotten and ro- and the eggs that they lay are the seeds into the soil. [Annie nods]

Interesting. Okay. Who was next? Go ahead Ayanna. Then it was Bruno.”

This revoicing of the student’s utterance can be seen as an assessment or as an

acknowledgement of what the student has said and as an invitation to say more. We chose

to divide the utterance after “okay” because the revoicing seemed to serve more as an

assessment than an invitation and “who was next . . .” seemed to serve explicitly as an

invitation to reply.

Co-occurrence relationships are said to operate in everyday discourse such that the

appearance of one form of speech (such as a greeting) will be followed by a related term

(another greeting) (Ervin Tripp 1972; Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson 1974). The

conversational forms that are reflexively related to each other by these co-occurrence rules

often appear one after the other. So, for example, a greeting offered by one person will be

followed by a greeting from another, an inquiry about one’s health (“how are you?”) is

followed by “state of being information” (“I’m fine; how about you?”).

Co-occurrence relationships seem to govern interactional sequences in classrooms

as well, at least in teacher-led lessons. Particular kinds of replies follow particular kinds of

initiation acts with great regularity. A call for factual information, such as names and

Page 38: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 38AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

dates, induces that type of information. A teacher’s request for interpretations about an

analogy between spiders and pumpkins generates a wide range of them. So, too,

students’ displays of their reasoning about how they estimated ratios and propositions co-

occurred with the teacher’s request for students to explain their reasoning. It is also

interesting to note that certain kinds of teacher follow-up responses to student responses

(which would be located in the 2nd position in Mayer’s scheme) seem to co-occur with

more complex utterances and reasoning on the part of students. The co-occurrence

relationship between what Michaels and O’Connor (this volume) call “Productive Talk

Moves” – is another kind of relationship worth exploring. All of these moves defer

evaluation, and open up a slot for more student explication and reasoning.

The relation between types of initiations and types of replies is not isomorphic, of

course. But our tabulation of the distribution of replies related to initiations showed co-

occurrence relations occurred 88% of the time in 480 sequences across nine lessons

(Mehan 1979). It would be interesting to see, for example, whether the same

predominance of such relations holds true in other of the newly-expanded set of

participant structures, and whether exceptions are considered by the participants as

violations to be repaired, or ruptures to conventional co-occurrence expectations that lead

in productive directions as in Aisha’s question addressed to another student placed in the

2nd/Reply column in Example (#9) above.

Finally, when these lessons (and the larger curriculum unit they enact) are over, we

do not know from the information in the teacher-student exchanges alone what the

students have learned toward particular curricular objectives. We also do not know if there

is any correlation between the extent of individual students’ verbal participation and their

Page 39: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 39AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

learning. Carol Lee’s work, cited above, is a notable exception assessing learning;

O’Connor & Michaels (this volume) is one example of an attempt to test the talk/learning

correlation. Perhaps the time has come when analyzing interaction should no longer be

considered enough.

References

Au, Kathryn Hu-Pei. 1980. Participation Structures in a Reading Lesson with Hawaiian

Children: Analysis of a Culturally Appropriate Instructional Event. Anthropology

& Education Quarterly 11 (2): 91–115.

Au, Kathryn Hu-Pei and Cathie Jordan. 1981. Teaching Reading to Hawaiian Children:

Finding a Culturally Appropriate Solution. Pp. 139-52. In: Henry Trueba, Grace

Pung Guthrie, and Kathryn Hu-Pei Au (Eds). Culture and the Bilingual Classroom:

Studies in Classroom Ethnography. Rowley: Newbury House.

Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language: the Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard

University Press.

Burns, Marilyn. 1989. Mathematics for Middle School. New York: Cuisenaire Company

of America.

Cazden, Courtney B. 1965. The Acquisition of Noun and Verb Inflections. Unpublished

Doctoral Dissertation. Cambridge MA: Harvard Graduate School of Education.

Cazden, Courtney B. 1976. How Knowledge about Language Helps the Classroom

Teacher—or Does It? A Personal Account. Urban Review 9: 74-90.

Cazden, Courtney B. 2001. Classroom Discourse: The Language of Teaching and

Learning. 2nd Edition. Portsmouth NH: Heinemann.

Page 40: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 40AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Cazden, Courtney B., Vera P. John and Dell H. Hymes (Eds.). 1972. Functions of

Language in the Classroom. New York: Teachers College Press.

Cazden, Courtney B. and Hugh Mehan. 1989. Principles from Sociology and

Anthropology: Content, Code, Classroom, and Culture. Pp.47-59. In: Maynard C.

Reynolds (Ed.) Knowledge Base for the Beginning Teacher. New York: Pergamon

Press.

Delgado-Gaitan, Concha. 1987. Traditions and Transitions in the Learning Process of

Mexican Children: An Ethnographic View. Pp. 333-359. In: George Spindler &

Louise Spindler (Eds.) Interpretive Ethnography of Education at Home and

Abroad. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Deutsch, Martin, Irwin Katz, and Arthur Robert Jensen. 1968. Social Class, Race, and

Psychological Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Doubler, Susan J., Harold McWilliams, and Sarah Michaels. 2012. The Talk Science

Project. Cambridge MA: TERC

Erickson, Frederick and Jeffrey Shultz. 1978. The Counselor as Gatekeeper: Social

Interaction in Interviews. New York: Academic Press.

Erickson, Frederick and Gerry Mohatt. 1982.The Cultural organization of Participation

Structures. Pp. 136-173. In: George Spindler (Ed). Doing the Ethnography of

Schooling. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and Jovanovich.

Edelsky, Carol. 2006. With Literacy and Justice for All: Rethinking the Social in

Language and Education. New York: The Falmer Press.

Page 41: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 41AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Emanuelson Jonas and Fritjof Sahlstrom 2008. The Price of Participation: Teacher

Control versus Student Participation in Classroom Interaction. Scandinavian

Journal of Educational Research 52 (2): 205-223.

Erwin-Tripp, Susan. 1972. Language Acquisition and Communicative Choice. Stanford:

Stanford University Press.

Foster, Michele. 1989. ‘It’s cookin' now’: A Performance Analysis of the Speech Events

of a Black Teacher in an Urban Community College. Language in Society 18 (1):

1–29.

Frake, Charles. 1964. A Structural Description of Subanun Religious Behavior. In: Ward

Goodenough (Ed.). Explorations in Cultural Anthropology. New York. McGraw

Hill.

Goodenough, Ward. 1964. Cultural Anthropology and Linguistics. In: Dell H. Hymes

(Ed.). Language in Culture and Society. New York: Harper and Row.

Gumperz, John J. 1971. Language in Social Groups. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Gumperz, John J. and Herasimchuk, Eleanor. 1972. The Conversational Analysis of

Meaning: A Study of Classroom Interaction. Pp. 99-143. In: Roger Shuy (Ed.)

Current Trends and Perspectives, 23rd Annual Roundtable. Washington DC:

Georgetown University Press.

Gutiérrez, Kris D., P. O. Baquedano-López, & C. Tejada, C. 1999. Rethinking Diversity:

Hybridity and Hybrid Language Practices in the Third Space. Mind, Culture and

Activity 6 (4): 286-303.

Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with Words: Language. Life, and Work in Communities

and Classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Page 42: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 42AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Hymes, Dell H. 1974. Foundations in Sociolinguistics. Philadelphia: University of

Pennsylvania Press.

Jensen, Arthur R. 1969. How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement.

Harvard Educational Review 39: 1-123.

Kovalainena, Minna and Kristiina Kumpulainen. 2007. The social Construction of

Participation in an Elementary Classroom Community. International Journal of

Educational Research 46: 141-158.

Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania

Press.

Laosa, L. M. 1982. School, occupation, culture and family: The impact of parental

schooling on the parent-child relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology. 74:

791-827.

Lee, Carol D. 1995. A Culturally Based Cognitive Apprenticeship: Teaching African

American High School Students Skills in Literary Interpretation. Reading

Research Quarterly 30 (4): 608-630.

Levin, Paula. 1978. Students and Teachers: A Cultural Analysis of Polynesian Classroom

Interaction. Unpublished PhD Dissertation. La Jolla: University of California San

Diego.

Losey, K. M. 1995. Mexican American Students and Classroom Interaction: An Overview

and Critique. Review of Educational Research 65 (3): 283-318.

MacBeth, Douglas. 2003. Hugh Mehan’s Learning Lessons Reconsidered: On the

Difference Between the Naturalistic and Critical Analysis of Classroom Discourse.

AERJ 40 (1): 239-280.

Page 43: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 43AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Malcolm, Ian. 1979. The West Australian Aboriginal Child and Classroom Interaction: A

Sociolinguistic Approach. Journal of Pragmatics 3 (3-4): 305-320.

McCarty, T. L., Stephen Wallace, Regina Hadley Lynch, and AnCita Benally. 1991.

Classroom Inquiry and Navajo Learning Styles: A Call for Reassessment.

Anthropology & Education Quarterly 22 (1): 42-59.

McCollum, Pamela. 1989. Turn-Allocation in Lessons with North American and Puerto

Rican Students: A Comparative Study. Anthropology & Education Quarterly 20

(2): 133-156.

McDermott, Raymond P. 1976. Kids Make Sense. PhD Dissertation. Stanford University.

McDermott, Raymond P., Kenneth Gospodinoff, and Jeffrey Aron. 1978. Criteria for an

Ethnographically Adequate Description of Concerted Activities and Their

Contexts. Semiotica 24: 00-00

Mayer, Susan J. 2009. Conceptualizing interpretive authority in practical terms. Language

and Education, xx: 1-18.

Mayer, Susan J. 2012. Classroom Discourse and Democracy: Making Meaning Together.

New York: Peter Lang.

Mehan, Hugh. 1979. Learning Lessons: Social Organization in the Classroom. Cambridge

MA: Harvard University Press.

Mehan, Hugh and Houston Wood. 1975. The Reality of Ethnomethodology. New York:

Wiley Interscience.

Michaels, Sarah. 1981. ‘Sharing time’: Children’s Narrative Styles and Differential

Access to Literacy. Language in Society 10 (3): 423-442.

Page 44: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 44AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Michaels, Sarah and Cathy O’Conner. 2012. Tals Science Primer. 2012. Cambridge MA:

TERC.

Michaels, Sarah, Catherine O’Connor, and Lauren B. Resnick. 2007. Deliberative

Discourse Idealized and Realized: Accountable Talk in the Classroom and in Civic

Life. Studies in Philosophy and Education 27(4): 283-297.

Miller, Barbara. 1981. Teaching Community School Mathematics – A Comparison of Five

Sites. Working Paper. United Nations Indigenous Mathematics Project

Mishler, Elliot G. 1975. Studies in Dialogue and Discourse; an Exponential Law of

Successive Questioning. Language in Society 4 (1): 31-51.

More, A. J. (1989). Native Indian Learning Styles: A Review for Researchers and

Teachers. Journal of American Indian Education 3: 15–28.

Philips, Susan. 1983. The Invisible Culture: Communication in Classroom and

Community on the Warm Springs Indian Reservation. New York: Longman.

Sacks, Harvey, Emmanuel Schegloff, and Gail Jefferson, 1974. A Simplest Systematics

for the Organization of Turn Taking in Conversation. Language 50: 696-735.

Shuy, Roger W., Walter Wolfram, and W. K. Riley. 1967. Field Techniques for an Urban

Language Study. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.

Shuy, Roger and Peg Griffin. 1978. The Study of Children’s Functional Language and

Education in the Early Years: Final report to the Carnegie Corporation of New

York. Arlington VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.

Sinclair, John M. and Malcolm Coulthard. 1975. Towards an Analysis of Discourse: The

English Used by Teachers and Pupils. London: Oxford University Press.

Page 45: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 45AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23

Tharp, Roland G. 1989. Culturally Compatible Education: A Formula for Designing

Effective Classrooms. (Pp. 51–66). In: Henry T. Trueba, George Spindler, and

Louise Spindler (Eds.), What Do Anthropologists Have to Say about Dropouts?

New York: Falmer Press.

Tharp, Roland and Ronald Gallimore. 1988. Rousing Minds to Life: Teaching, Learning,

and Schooling in Social Context. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Toulmin, Stephen. 1958. The Uses of Argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Trueba, Henry T. 1983. Adjustment Problems of Mexican and Mexican-American

Students: An Anthropological Study. Learning Disability Quarterly 6 (4): 395-415.

Vogt, Lynn A., Cathie Jordan, and Roland G. Tharp. 1987. Explaining School Failure,

Producing School Success: Two Cases. Anthropology & Education Quarterly 18

(4): 276-286.

Warren, Beth and Ann S. Rosebery. 2011. Possible Worlds, Possible Meanings: Working

Language in the Science Classroom. Paper presented at the Jean Piaget Society

Conference. Berkeley, CA. June 2.

Wells, Gordon. 1993. Reevaluating the IRF Sequence: A Proposal for the Articulation of

theories of Activity and Discourse for the analysis of Teaching and Learning in the

Classroom. Linguistics and Education. Linguistics and Education 5:1-38.

Wells, Gordon and Rebecca Mejia Arauz. 2006. Dialogue in the Classroom. Journal of the

Learning Sciences 15 (3): 379-428.

Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1953. The Philosophical Investigations. London: Blackwell.

Page 46: The Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History …€¦ · Web viewThe Study of Classroom Discourse: Early History and Current DevelopmentsHugh (Bud) Mehan and Courtney B. Cazden

Mehan & Cazden 46AERA ’13 DRAFT 5/20/23