The Spirit of Four

download The Spirit of Four

of 6

Transcript of The Spirit of Four

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    1/6

    The spirit of four: Metaphors and models of number

    construction

    Maria A. Droujkova

    Objectives

    Researchers have noted the need for investigation of relationships between different types of

    reasoning and number construction models (Confrey & Smith, 1995; Olive, 2001; Pepper &

    Hunting, 1998; Steffe, 1994). The goal of this study was to look at the development of number

    construction through the lens of metaphor.

    In particular, the study investigated the interplay between a largely multiplicative environment and

    the development of reasoning within this environment that was significantly different from scenarios

    from other studies.

    Conceptual framework

    Observing young children makes a strong case for viewing mathematical thinking as fundamentally

    metaphoric (R. Davis, 1984). Metaphor is the recursive movement between a source and a target

    that are structurally similar, both changing in the dynamic process of learning (B. Davis, 1996; R.

    Davis, 1984; English, 1997; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Lakoff & Nunez, 1997, 2000; Pimm, 1987;

    Presmeg, 1997; Sfard, 1997).

    For analyzing number construction, I used the counting scheme (Olive, 2001; Steffe, 1994) and the

    splitting conjecture (Confrey & Smith, 1995; Lehrer, Strom, & Confrey, 2002). The metaphor that

    connects sources of sharing, folding or similarity, and the target of multiplicative one-to-many

    actions can be considered the basis of splitting as a cognitive scheme. The metaphor that connects

    - 1 -

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    2/6

    the source of counting and the target of the number sequence is the basis of the counting scheme. In

    the splitting world multiplicative reasoning develops via grounding metaphors with sources such as

    sharing. In the counting world multiplicative reasoning is based on the linking metaphor which

    connects interiorized, reversible counting with iterable units (Olive, 2001; Steffe, 1994).

    Modes of inquiry

    This paper presents a longitudinal case study of reasoning in a child up to the age of five, whose

    home environment was restructured to incorporate more multiplicative activities. Researchers often

    consider metaphor to be private, unformulated and difficult to study (Presmeg, 1997). Additional

    access issues came from the need for a very young subject necessary to trace the beginnings of

    number concept development, and from the longitudinal nature of the study. These considerations

    pointed to the necessity of a close relationship between the subject of the study and the researcher,

    and I invited my daughter Katya to be the subject of the study. As a parent, I was in a privileged

    position of access to the majority of the details of Katyas day-to-day life, as well as to the meaning

    of her utterances and gestures.

    Data sources and evidence

    Data for the study came from fieldnotes of observations as a participant-observer; videotapes and

    audiotapes of unstructured and semi-structured interviews; photographs of activity settings; and a

    collection of artifacts used in activities.

    Results

    The non-sequential order in which conventional number names first appeared in Katyas speech

    corresponded to multiplicative, rather than counting, actions. For example, the utterance two twos

    - 2 -

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    3/6

    appeared about eight months earlier than the word four, and also earlier than the word three.

    Appearance of two threes in games preceded the use of the words four, five and six, and

    appearance of two fours preceded the use of numbers greater than four.

    In constructing numbers from one to four, Katya used individual (Presmeg, 1997) metaphors based

    on instant recognition of the quantity. In these metaphors, the source was an image with a quantity

    intrinsically embedded in it, such as dogs legs for four. Katya mostly used mixed references for

    multiplicative situations, for example, two dogs to signify two times four. This availability of

    two systems of signifiers provided a language necessary to address the asymmetrical nature of the

    multiplication models Katya used. For example, in the case of two dogs the words underlined the

    distinction between sets and set members in the set model of multiplication. Lack of signifiers for

    this asymmetry of multiplication models may be problematic and may hinder development of

    multiplicative reasoning. Confrey and Smith (1995) note that a counting number is typically used

    to name the result or outcome of a split (p.75, italics mine).

    If learners see the splitting and counting worlds as isomorphic (Confrey & Smith, 1995), they can

    understand structures of one world by making parallels with the corresponding structures of the

    other world. Childrens structure transfer attempts become especially visible when they differ from

    accepted standards. For example, researchers often focus on children inappropriately applying

    additive strategies to multiplicative situations (Post, Behr, & Lesh, 1986). Katya frequently tried to

    use multiplicative relationships instead of additive. For example, when asked to continue a pattern

    of arrays made out of circles: 2 by 1, 2 by 2, 2 by 3, ___ she attempted to iterate the previous array

    twice, drawing a 2 by 6 array instead of the expected 2 by 4. Upon my explanation that a pair of

    - 3 -

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    4/6

    circles is addedto the array in each step, Katya said, somewhat angrily, that these pictures are not

    real. Multiplicative relationships were more real to her.

    In another unexpected example, a square was split into four equal squares, and then each of the

    small squares was split into four tiny squares. Katya used words signifying size gradients, such as

    large, small, and tiny, and babies and adults, consistently across different multiplicative worlds.

    This metaphor of growth united different multiplicative worlds and allowed Katya to compare

    their structures, working on what a mathematician would call powers or base systems. Katya

    used the word spirit to denote the action in each world, for example, talking about the spirit of

    four in the split square above. She claimed that if we cut the 4-square piece in four, the result

    would be zero. Upon cutting, she was surprised that the result was one square. However, in repeated

    activities with the same picture, or with pictures based on other powers from other split worlds,

    Katya consistently said that the result of splitting the power base picture would be zero, or

    nothing, even after observing again and again that it turned out to be one.

    I hypothesized that these names were expressions of metaphors for the origin, and I told Katya that

    researchers call the entity in question the origin. We compared the origins of additive and power-

    based structures, and Katya felt validated to discover a real zero at least at some origin. This

    instance of isomorphism between additive and multiplicative worlds helped Katya to build her idea

    of the origin as a superordinate construct (Confrey & Smith, 1995), whereas the idea was

    problematic while she stayed within the multiplicative world.

    - 4 -

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    5/6

    Deeper understanding of connections between additive and multiplicative reasoning can benefit

    further theory construction in areas such as number construction, ratio and proportion, or

    exponential functions. Practitioners can draw on possible uses of metaphors for working with deep

    mathematical ideas throughout the mathematical curriculum. Since the majority of studies of young

    children are done in additive environments, research of cases developed in a predominantly

    multiplicative environment can provide a valuable vantage point for theory development.

    Reference

    Confrey, J., & Smith, E. (1995). Splitting, covariation, and their role in the development of

    exponential functions.Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(1), 66-86.

    Davis, B. (1996). Teaching mathematics: Toward a sound alternative. New York: Garland.Davis, R. (1984).Learning mathematics : The cognitive science approach to mathematics

    education. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    English, L. D. (1997). Analogies, metaphors and images: Vehicles for mathematical reasoning. In L.D. English (Ed.), Mathematical reasoning: Analogies, metaphors and images (pp. 3-20).

    Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. E. (1997). The metaphorical structure of mathematics: Sketching out

    cognitive foundations for a mind-based mathematics. In L. D. English (Ed.), Mathematicalreasoning: Analogies, metaphors and images (pp. 21-92). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings

    mathematics into being(1st ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Lehrer, R., Strom, D., & Confrey, J. (2002). Grounding metaphors and inscriptional resonance:

    Children's Emerging Understanding of mathematical similarity. Cognition and Instruction,

    20(3), 359-398.

    Olive, J. (2001). Children's number sequences: An explanation of Steffe's constructs and an

    extrapolation to rational numbers of arithmetic. The Mathematics Educator, 11(1), 4-9.Pepper, K. L., & Hunting, R. P. (1998). Preschoolers' counting and sharing.Journal for Research in

    Mathematics Education, 29(2), 164-184.

    Pimm, D. (1987). Speaking mathematically: Communication in mathematics classrooms. London,England; New York, NY: Routledge & K. Paul.Post, T., Behr, M., & Lesh, R. (1986). Research-based observations about children's learning of

    rational number concepts.Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 8(1), 39-48.

    Presmeg, N. C. (1997). Reasoning with metaphors and metonymies in mathematical learning. In L.D. English (Ed.), Mathematical reasoning: Analogies, metaphors and images (pp. 267-280).

    Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    - 5 -

  • 8/9/2019 The Spirit of Four

    6/6

    Sfard, A. (1997). Commentary: On metaphorical roots of conceptual growth. In L. D. English (Ed.),

    Mathematical reasoning: Analogies, metaphors and images (pp. 339-372). Mahwah, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Steffe, L. P. (1994). Children's multiplying schemes. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The

    development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 3-40). Albany,

    NY: State University of New York Press.

    - 6 -