THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented...

59
1 THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING TASK AND RELATIONSHIP by Kylie Rochford WP-13-04 Copyright Department of Organizational Behavior Weatherhead School of Management Case Western Reserve University Cleveland OH 44106-7235 e-mail: [email protected]

Transcript of THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented...

Page 1: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

1

THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING TASK

AND RELATIONSHIP

by

Kylie Rochford

WP-13-04

Copyright

Department of Organizational Behavior

Weatherhead School of Management

Case Western Reserve University

Cleveland OH 44106-7235

e-mail: [email protected]

Page 2: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

2

THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING TASK AND

RELATIONSHIP

Kylie Rochford

ABSTRACT

It is now generally accepted in the neuroscience literature that within our brain

there are two distinct neural networks – the Task Positive Network which is

concerned with analytical reasoning, and the Default Mode Network which is

concerned with social and relational reasoning. Furthermore, a recent fMRI

study shows that these two neural networks are anti-correlated, that is, when

humans are engaged in an analytical task their ability to engage in a social or

relational task is suppressed and vice versa. This phenomenon is referred to as

the ‘opposing domains hypothesis’. In this paper we explore the implications of

the opposing domains hypothesis for leadership and leadership effectiveness. We

provide a review of the pertinent leadership literature to surface the task-

relationship distinction in various conceptualizations and measurements of

leadership. We then explore possible strategies that might minimize the extent of

suppression of the opposing domain, which we argue will increase a leader’s

ability to switch between task demands and relationship demands, thereby

increasing leadership effectiveness.

Page 3: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

3

The Opposing Domains of Leadership: Integrating task and relationship

INTRODUCTION

Over the past two decades relationship oriented approaches to leadership have come to the

forefront of leadership theory (Bryman, 1992; House & Aditya, 1997; Hunt, 1999), while task

oriented approaches have been relegated to a periphery role. The work of early scholars clearly

argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see

for example Blake & Mouton, 1964), however the exact nature of the relationship between task

abilities and relationship abilities of leaders and the impact of this relationship for leadership

effectiveness have received limited attention. Recent findings in cognitive neuroscience, namely

the opposing domains hypothesis (Jack et al., 2013), provide evidence to suggest that analytical

or task reasoning and social or relational reasoning are driven by two distinct anti-correlated

neural networks – the Task Positive Network (TPN) and the Default-Mode Network (DMN). By

‘anti-correlated’ the authors mean that there is a natural antagonism between these two networks

– that is, activation of one network results in the suppression of the other (Greicius et al., 2003,

Fox et al., 2005, Fransson, 2005, Golland et al., 2007, Tian et al., 2007, Uddin et al., 2009). This

has led neuroscientists to conclude that analytical reasoning and social reasoning are reflective of

two distinct cognitive modes (Jack et al., in press).

If analytical reasoning and social reasoning are indeed two distinct anti-correlated cognitive

modes, then one must ask how a leader is able to attend to task requirements while also attending

to the leader follower relationship. In this paper we review the leadership literature with the

Page 4: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

4

opposing domains hypothesis in mind. We consider the implications of the opposing domains

hypothesis for leadership in general and surface the task-relationship distinction underlying

many past and current leadership frameworks. We begin our discussion with a brief review of

the opposing domains hypothesis which further explains the relationship between the TPN and

the DMN. We then explore how the distinction and trade-off between the task and relationship

domains is delineated throughout the leadership literature. Finally, we discuss possible strategies

leaders might use to enhance their ability to switch between the two cognitive domains, which

we believe will result in greater leadership effectiveness. The paper concludes with the

presentation of a conceptual framework and a series of propositions for future research.

OPPOSING DOMAINS HYPOTHESIS

Distinguishing between Default Mode Network and the Task Positive Network

Default Mode Network

The brain has three different neural systems that are each involved with a different type of social

reasoning (Jack, in press). These systems are highlighted in Figure 1 by the yellow/orange areas.

The first system is comprised of the fusiform face area on the ventral surface, the extra-striate

body area on the lateral surface, and regions centered on the posterior portion of the superior

temporal sulcus. This network is involved with any task that involves reasoning about living as

opposed to non-living stimuli (Wiggett, Pritchard, & Downing, 2009). The second system

includes the inferior parietal sulcus and the frontal premotor cortices. This system is more

commonly known as the ‘mirror neuron’ system and is involved with tasks that involve either

executing actions or watching the actions of others regardless of social context. An example of

the use of these two systems in the leadership literature is the phenomenon of emotional

Page 5: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

5

contagion - “the tendency to automatically mimic and synchronize facial expressions,

vocalizations, postures, and movements with those of another person and, consequently, to

converge emotionally” (p. 153-154, as cited in Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994, p.5).

Emotional contagion has been used to explain the convergence of emotion from leader to

follower (Bono & Ilies, 2006; Johnson, 2008; see also Sy, Cote, & Saavedra, 2005), and from

followers to leader (Dasborough, et al., 2009). While the first network should be activated by

virtue of the interaction between two human beings, the second system would be activated as we

mimic the observable behaviors and consequently converge in our (leader and follower)

emotional states.

Finally, and of most interest to this paper, the third system, known as the ‘mentalizing system’,

consists of the dorsal MPFC, the medial parietal/posterior cingulate, and a right lateralized

region near the junction of the temporal and parietal cortices (Jack, in press). This system is

activated by tasks that involve the attribution of mental states regardless of whether the stimulus

is animate (i.e. humans or animals) or inanimate (i.e. robots). Within the mentalizing system,

there is another cluster of regions known as the Default Mode Network (DMN). The DMN is

comprised of regions that include the medial prefrontal, areas in the medial/posterior cingulate,

the lateral inferior parietal, and the superior temporal cortices. The Default Mode Network is

activated when a task requires either attribution of the mental states of others or reflection on our

own mental states or emotions (Jack, 2013) and is anti-correlated with the Task Positive Network

which attends to mechanical or analytical reasoning. When considering the tasks involved with

leadership, the DMN would be involved with any form of relationship building, interactions

between leaders and followers, and the leader’s sense making about their own emotions and the

emotions of others.

Page 6: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

6

Figure 1: Distinguishing between the Task Positive Network and the Default Mode Network. Source: Jack et al., 2013.

Task Positive Network

The Task Positive Network (TPN) is a cluster of brain areas activated by non-social tasks

involving working memory, language, logical reasoning, mathematical reasoning, and

causal/mechanical reasoning (Jack, in press; Van Overwalle, 2011). While the TPN overlaps

some networks in the social regions of the brain (the mirror neuron system, see (Van Overwalle

and Baetens, 2009), it is clearly distinct from the mentalizing network discussed above, both in

its location in the brain (the blue/green areas in Figure 1) and in the types of tasks that activate it

(Jack, in press). The Task Positive Network (TPN) includes the dorsal attention system and the

frontoparietal control network (Vincent et al., 2008, as cited in Jack, in press). Jack (in press, p.

20) characterizes the TPN as being involved in “analytical-empirical-critical reasoning, such as

mechanical reasoning”. This characterization is in sharp contrast to that of the mentalizing

system, and more specifically the DMN, which is involved with social reasoning.

Page 7: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

7

The only exception with regard to the social versus non-social reasoning distinction associated

with the DMN and TPN is when social reasoning is intentional. Intentional actions are

associated with the mirror neuron system (Iacoboni, et al., 2005), which as noted about, overlaps

with DMN. There is also some evidence that intentional actions may be associated with the TPN

even when these actions require social reasoning. However, if this is the case, then it is

hypothesized that the ‘intention’ component of the action involves thinking

critically/strategically/mechanically, which is consistent with the TPN, hence explains the

activation of this network despite there being a social component. The importance of

intentionality in actions and how this links to neural and hormonal functioning has been raised in

the leadership literature. Specifically, in attempting to examine the convergent and discriminant

validity of clusters included in the Emotional Competence Inventory, Boyatzis and Sala (2004)

found that there were two clear factors present – one factor reflected an intentional reasoning

about the emotions of others in order to achieve a task, which is later labeled ‘emotional

intelligence’ (Boyatzis & Goleman, 2007); while the second factor reflected social reasoning that

was non-task focused, which is later labeled ‘social intelligence’ (Boyatzis & Goleman, 2007).

Boyatzis and Sala predicted and later found evidence that social intelligence is linked to the

parasympathetic nervous system (Boyatzis, et. al., 2012), while emotional intelligence is linked

to the sympathetic nervous system. Given our current understanding of the TPN, leadership

tasks associated with this network might include financial planning, anything requiring metrics,

forecasting, and in light of the previous point, strategic networking and reasoning about the

emotions with others primarily for the purposes of task achievement.

Page 8: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

8

The opposing domains hypothesis

As discussed above, it is generally accepted in the cognitive neuroscience domain that the

Default Mode Network (DMN) and the Task Positive Network (TPN), are anti-correlated

(Greicius et al. 2003, Fransson, 2005, Fox et al 2005, Golland et al. 2007, Tian et al. 2007) – that

is, as activity in one network increases, activity in the opposing network decreases (see Figure 2).

A number of explanations for this relationship have been entertained (see Buckner, Andrews-

Hanna, & Schacter, 2008 for a review), the most recent being the opposing domains hypothesis

(Jack et al., 2013). The opposing domains hypothesis postulates that the anti-correlation between

the DMN and the TPN is a result of the type of task a person is engaged in. Specifically, the

opposing domains hypothesis states that holding the required level of attention constant, social

tasks requiring the reasoning about the minds of others will activate the DMN, and therefore

suppress the TPN and non-social tasks requiring reasoning about physical objects will activate

the TPN and consequently suppress the DMN. In other words, when a person is engaged in a

social task, their non-social or analytical abilities are suppressed. Conversely, when a person is

engaged in a non-social task or analytical task, their social abilities and awareness are

suppressed.

Figure 2: Anti-correlation of TPN and DMN

Source: Fox et al., 2005, 9673-9618

Page 9: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

9

Jack et al. (2013) used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to record brain activity

when participants were engaged in social versus mechanical tasks. The social tasks required

participants to answer questions pertaining to the beliefs and attitudes of the characters in either

an emotional and moral laden text passage or video clip. The mechanical tasks required

participants to complete science puzzles or view and answer questions on video clips taken from

the Video Encyclopedia of Physics. A rest condition was also included in which participants

stared at a red cross on the ceiling. The findings showed that the neural activation during the

social tasks, specifically the activation of the temporarparietal junction, medial parietal/posterior

cingulate and the medial prefrontal cortex, was accompanied by the deactivation of the neural

networks responsible for mechanical reasoning, specifically, the superior frontal sulcus, lateral

prefrontal cortex, and the anterior intraparietal sulcus (see Figure 3 and Figures 4-9 in Table 1).

To increase the robustness of this finding and to rule out alternative explanations, controls were

put in place for external versus internal attention and perceptual attention demands.

Figure 3: fMRI images showing activation of TPN (Areas A, B, and C) and DMN

(Areas D, E, and F). A: Superior frontal sulcus; B: Lateral prefrontal cortex; C:

Anterior intraparietal sulcus (Task Positive Network) D: Temporoparietal

junction; E: Medial parietal/posterior cingulate; F: Medial pre-frontal cortex

(Default Mode Network)

Page 10: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

10

Jack et al. (2013) conclude that the anti-correlation between the TPN and DMN “reflects a

powerful human tendency to differentiate between conscious persons and inanimate objects in

both our attitudes and modes of interaction” (p.396). The implications of this statement for

organizational behavior are wide and varied. While this paper focuses on leadership and related

variables, the natural tension between task and relationship; inanimate and animate; and social

and non-social can be found in various streams of literature including, but not limited to

personality frameworks, human needs, group dynamics and socialization, types of conflict, trust,

intelligence, decision making, mindfulness, and moral reasoning.

THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP

Conceptualizations of leadership

Nearly three decades ago Stodgill (1974) famously concluded that “there are almost as many

definitions of leadership as there are persons who have attempted to define the concept” (p.259).

In synthesizing the plethora of definitions on offer, there appears to be two distinct components

of the enactment of “leadership”: task achievement and relationship development. Further, of

the multitude of psychological and behavioural leadership competency models on offer, the

individual competencies in these models can also be classified as relating to either instrumental

task achievement or relationship development. Intertwined in the distinction between leadership

as task achievement versus relationship development are patterns in how power is distributed and

how decisions are made (Bass & Bass, 2008). In what follows, we first provide a brief historical

overview of the emergence of the task and relationship dimensions in the leadership literature

beginning with the early task-oriented conceptualizations of leadership and moving to consider

Page 11: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

11

Table 1: Changes in neural activation in the TPN and DMN by task type

Analytical/mechanical Task Empathetic/social task

Figure 4: Activation of the superior frontal sulcus - TPN (blue line) and deactivation of DMN (red line)

when subjects were given an analytical task.

Figure 7: Activation of the temporoparietal

junction – DMN (red line) and deactivation of TPN

(blue line) when subjects were given a social task.

Figure 5: Activation of the lateral prefrontal cortex-

TPN (blue line) and deactivation of DMN (red line)

when subjects were given an analytical task.

Figure 8: Activation of the medial

parietal/posterior cingulate– DMN (red line) and

deactivation of TPN (blue line) when subjects were given a social task.

Figure 6: Activation of the anterior intraparietal

sulcus - TPN (blue line) and deactivation of DMN

(red line) when subjects were given an analytical

task.

Figure 9: Activation of the medial prefrontal

cortex– DMN (red line) and deactivation of TPN

(blue line) when subjects were given a social task.

Page 12: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

12

two of the more recent ‘new paradigm’ frameworks – transformational leadership versus

transactional leadership and leader-member exchange versus leader-member social exchange.

From scientific management to human relations: the foundations the task-relationship

distinction

The conceptualization of leadership as instrumental task achievement dates back to the work of

Cowley (1928) who defined leadership as “an individual who is moving in a particular direction

and who succeeds in inducing others to follow him…A leader then, is a person who is going

somewhere, who has a motive, who has a program. A headman is an individual who has attained

to the head of a group but who has no outstanding individual motive or program and who is,

therefore, not a leader." (p.145-146). In this passage, Cowley states that the key difference

between a leader and a headman, who he claims is not a leader, is the presence of a ‘motive’ or

‘program’, which may also be thought of as a task. From this perspective, in the absence of a

task, leaders do not exist and without the successful achievement of a task, a person cannot be an

effective leader. Consistent with Cowley’s definition, and in line with much of the popular

management theory in the early 1900s (largely influence by Fredrick Taylor’s scientific

management movement), seminal work in the leadership field emphasised the task-components

of leadership. Followers were considered to be instrumental machines in the leadership process

rather than as active contributors to that process. However, as the human relations school of

thought emerged in the late 1930s, the ‘human’ side of leadership began to receive more

attention, resulting in a host of leadership frameworks that reflected the scientific management

(task) – human relations (relationship) dichotomy. These frameworks include the autocratic

(task)-democratic (relationship) continuum (Tannenbaum & Schmidt, 1958), the Ohio State

Page 13: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

13

Leadership Studies (Shartle, 1950; Fleishman, 1953; Halpin & Winer, 1957) on initiating

structure (task) and consideration (relationship), and the Michigan Leadership Studies (Katz,

Maccoby, & Morse, 1950) which distinguished between a production orientation (task) and an

employee orientation (relationship).

The 1950 to 1970s saw a significant shift away from task-driven approaches to leadership as

theorists began to consider the role of interactions between leader and follower and leading to the

re-conceptualization of leadership effectiveness as requiring both relationship building and task

attainment. Influential in this shift was Bass (1960) who distinguished between successful

leadership and effective leadership, power and coercion, and closely related, coercive leadership

and permissive leadership. Bass (1960) distinguished between ‘successful’ leadership and

‘effective’ leadership as follows: “If A succeeds in changing B, and B attains his goal as a

consequence, the successful leadership is effective. It B fails to gain his objective by following

A’s leadership, the leadership is ineffective…Ineffective leadership occurs when members earn

high scores as successful leaders but their groups are not effective” (p. 119, see also Hersey &

Blanchard, 1969, p. 82-83). This passage clearly reflects the importance of both the relationship

competencies of a leader, which are facilitated by the DMN, and the task competencies, which

are facilitated by the TPN.

1950s – 1970s: Attempting to integrate task and relationship

By the 1960s it seems that there was a general consensus that building relationships with

followers was a fundamental component of leadership and a critical ingredient in task

Page 14: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

14

achievement. Theorists become interested in how we can integrate these two competencies and

in which situations task-oriented leadership might be more effective than relationship-oriented

leadership and vice versa. Bales (1958) was instrumental in this theoretical shift with his claim

that a task oriented group requires two types of leaders to maximize effectiveness; one leader to

attend to the task functions of the group (instrumental leader) and another to attend to the

emotional needs of the group (expressive leader). Bales argued that while the type of leadership

role one assumes may be influenced by the situation and characteristics of followers, by and

large, the type of leader one is likely to become, and therefore the type of role (instrumental or

expressive) that leader will fulfill in a group, can be determined by psychological testing. The

suggestion that two leaders are required in a group stood in stark contrast to the idea that all

leadership roles could be carried out by one individual.

Building on the work of Bales, the early Ohio State Leadership Studies (Fleishman, 1953)

confirmed the conceptual distinction between task-oriented and relation-oriented leadership and

developed a measurement tool for each. Over the next two decades, much of the leadership

literature was focused around further defining the two dimensions. As a result of this work, task

oriented leadership was identified as being concerned with production (Blake & Mouton, 1964),

goal achievement (Cartwright & Zander, 1960; Bowers & Seashore, 1966), labor allocation and

enforcement of sanctions (McGregor,1960), and the initiating structure for followers (Shartle,

1950; Fleishman, 1953; Halpin & Winer, 1957). At the individual level, task-oriented leaders

were said to have a high need for achievement (McClelland, 1961; Wofford, 1970), to be

achievement oriented (Indvik, 1986), and to be cold and aloof to signal their preference for

psychological distance from followers (Blau & Scott, 1962). Conversely, relations-oriented

leaders were described as being focused on follower well-being (Hemphill, 1950), concerned

Page 15: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

15

with developing and maintaining relationship (Hersey & Blanchard, 1982), to be more

democratic as opposed to autocratic (Bass, 1990), and as placing value on friendships, open

communication, and mutual trust. Empirical research has shown that relations-oriented leaders

are associated with higher job satisfaction and lower turnover in organizations (Bass, 1990;

Yukl, 2006).

Yukl (1999) provides the most recent taxonomy of task and relation oriented leader behavior (see

Table 2). Yukl also distinguishes a third dimension in his taxonomy, labeled ‘change

orientation’ which includes a mix of task and relations oriented behaviors. In critiquing his own

other’s (Blake and Mouton, 1964) work, Yukl (1999; 2008) notes the limitations of the task-

relations distinction, specifically, the inconsistent empirical results and the lack of evidence that

it is indeed possible to be high in both task and relations competencies and that being so is

related to leadership effectiveness (Yukl, 1999). Research conducted since adds support to

Yukl’s critique; specifically, Kaiser and Kaplan (2001 as cited in Bass & Bass, 2008) found that

in a sample of managers from a consulting firm, 46% were highly task oriented, 19% were

highly relations oriented, and only 6% showed flexibility between the two dimensions.

The distinction between task-oriented leader behaviors versus relations-oriented leader behaviors

presented in Table 2 appears to closely parallel the distinction made between social and

mechanical tasks in the opposing domains hypothesis. Specifically, the task-oriented behaviors

in Table 2 are, by and large, mechanical and analytical in nature and would therefore activate the

leader’s TPN, while the relations oriented behaviors are generally social in nature and would

therefore activate the leader’s DMN. The anti-correlation between these two networks suggests

Page 16: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

16

Table 2: Yukl (1999) taxonomy of task versus relations oriented leadership behaviors

Task-oriented leader behavior Relations-oriented leader behavior

Plans in detail how to accomplish an

important task or project.

Provides encouragement and support when

you have a difficult or stressful task.

Provides a clear explanation of your

responsibilities with regard to a task or

project.

Backs you up and supports you in a difficult

situation.

Clearly expresses what results are needed to

carry out a task or project.

Gives you credit for helpful ideas and

suggestions.

Determines what resources are needed to

carry out a project.

Consults with you to get your reactions and

suggestions before making a decision that

affects you.

Determines how to organize and co-ordinate

work activities to avoid delays, duplication

of effort, and wasted resources.

Provides opportunities to develop your skills

and show what you can do.

Checks work progress against plans to see if

it is on target.

Expresses confidence in your ability to carry

out a difficult task.

that when a leader is focused on task-oriented behaviors, their ability to attend to the relationship

needs of their followers is diminished. Similarly, when followers are engage in task-related

behaviors, their ability to attend to the relationship needs of the group is also diminished and vice

versa. This is not to say that leaders do not have the ability to be highly analytical and to build

effective relationships; rather that they cannot simultaneously attend equally to each of these

tasks. This trade-off has been noted by a number of scholars, most recently Yukl (2008) notes

that “efforts to improve one performance determinant may have an adverse effect on another

performance determinant…when leaders are preoccupied with responding to external threats

(task), there is less time for people-oriented concerns such as being supportive and developing

member skills (p.711-714). The opposing domains hypothesis provides an alternative

Page 17: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

17

explanation for why this the trade-off Yukl and others have observed. Rather than a lack of time,

the opposing domains hypothesis suggests that it may be the suppression of the opposing neural

network that is responsible for the “adverse effect on another performance determinant”.

The anti-correlation between the TPN and DMN may also help us to understand why the type of

task a leader or group is charged with has been found to moderate the relationship between

leadership style (task versus relationship), task performance and leadership effectiveness

respectively. Two key studies in the early leadership literature present findings at the behavioral

level which appear to parallel those of the opposing domains hypothesis. Specifically, Burke

(1965) found that a group’s performance of a coding task was completed more effectively under

a production-oriented (task) leader but the decision task was carried out more effectively under a

relations-oriented leader. Linking this to the opposing domains hypothesis we could propose that

the coding task presented to the group activated the TPN, and therefore suppressed the DMN.

Followers performed more effectively on this analytical task when the leadership style matched

the activation of the TPN (by focusing on the task). When a relations-oriented leadership style

was used, the leader’s behavior and focus on relationships may have activated the DMN in

followers, which we now know suppresses our analytical ability, hence the lower performance

on the analytical task. Conversely, the decision task, which required interpersonal interaction

and therefore would have activated the DMN, was performed more effectively under a relations

oriented leader. A relations-oriented leader in this task would have allowed for the activation of

the neural networks required to perform effectively. Conversely, a task-oriented leader would

have activated the opposing network, resulting in lower task performance.

Page 18: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

18

Building on the role of task type as a moderator of the relationship between leadership style and

task performance, Weed, Mitchell, and Moffitt (1976) found that the type of task a group was

faced with influenced their preference for a task as opposed to a relations oriented leader. They

found that the only type of task that was a significant moderator between leadership style and

task performance was the difficult-ambiguous task. While this study was focused on the

difficulty and ambiguity of the task rather than the mechanical or social nature of the task, the

authors note that the difficult-ambiguous task was the only task that involved ethical/moral

problems and therefore “the significant findings on this task may be partially attributable to the

types of skills required to deal with these problems” (p.65).

Aside from objective measures of task performance, Hersey and Blanchard (1969) developed the

Tri-Dimensional Leadership Model that sought link task and relations oriented leadership styles

to follower perceptions of leadership effectiveness. The Tri-Dimensional Leadership Model

suggests that leaders who are highly task oriented will be perceived as ‘effective’ because they

know what they want and are able to impose this to accomplish a task without causing

resentment. A highly task oriented leader will be perceived as ineffective when followers’

perceive the leader has no confidence in others, is unpleasant, or only shows interest in short-run

output. In other words, a highly task oriented leader will only be seen as effective if they are

aware of, and attend to, relationships while still accomplishing the task. Similarly, a leader with

a high relations-orientated will be perceived as effective by followers when followers perceive

the leader has implicit trust in people and as being primarily concerned with developing their

talents – behaviors associated with the DMN. They will be seen as ineffective when followers

perceive the leader to be passive and showing little care about the task at hand – behaviors

associated with the TPN. This suggests that regardless of a leader’s dominant style (task-

Page 19: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

19

oriented or relations oriented), they must be able to switch between the opposing neural networks

(the TPN and the DMN) in order to be perceived as effective by their followers. Given this, it

seems that identifying the individual difference variables and situational variables that increase

the ability of a leader to switch between the TPN and the DMN may be a critical step in

maximizing leader effectiveness.

1970s – 2000s: The “New Paradigm” Leadership Frameworks

The late 1970s and early 1980s saw the rise of a “new paradigm” (see also House & Aditya’s

1997 “neo-charismatic” theories) of leadership theories which focused on the relational and

emotional nature of leadership (Bryman, 1992). Included under in this new paradigm banner are

leadership theories involving charisma (Conger, 1989, 1999; Conger & Kanungo, 1998; House,

1977), vision (Bennis & Nanus, 1985; Shashkin, 1988; Tichy & Devanna, 1986); and arguably

the most well-known framework, Bass’ (1985) transformational-transactional framework. The

key theoretical shift which characterizes the new paradigm models appears to be that rather than

the utility of relationship building being task achievement as it was in the earlier task-

relationship models, in the new paradigm models the leader-follower relationship itself became

the variable of interest and the task-related benefits of the relationship appeared to be secondary.

Task achievement and consequently task-oriented leadership moved to the periphery and in

many cases appears to be framed as an inferior approach to leadership. In this section we will

discuss two of the more famous new paradigm models: the transformational-transactional

framework (Bass, 1985) and the leader-member exchange (LMX) framework (Graen, Novak, &

Sommerkamp, 1982).

Page 20: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

20

Transformational- transactional leadership

Transformational leadership was first mentioned by Dowton (1973, as cited in Bass & Bass,

2008) and was closely followed by the rise of charismatic leadership theory1 (House, 1977).

There is still some debate in the literature as to the distinction, or more precisely the usefulness

of making a distinction, between charismatic and transformational leadership. Some theorists

argue that there is no empirical distinction between the two constructs (House, 1999; House &

Shamir, 1993) and hence, much of the later literature refers to ‘Charismatic-transformational

leadership’ (see for example Hunt, 1999; Kirkpatrick & Locke, 1996; Knippenberg & Sitkin,

2013). A second group of theorists continue to argue that there is value in distinguishing

between charismatic leadership and transformational leadership, for example, Bass (1998) argues

that while the same leaders who are charismatic also tend to be transformational, the behaviors

and therefore development of the behaviors are different (see also Conger, 1999; Yukl & Beyor,

1999, as cited in Bass & Bass, 2008). Furthermore, Bass and Bass (2008) notes that it is possible

to be transformational without being charismatic: “a highly intellectually stimulating teacher, for

instance, may transform students without their regarding the teacher as charismatic (p.620). For

the purposes of this paper, it is not necessary to draw conclusions as to the convergent of

discriminant validity of the two constructs other than to note that both constructs draw heavily on

social or relational reasoning as opposed to mechanical reasoning.

The transformational-transactional leadership framework is credited with “shifting the direction

of leadership thinking from a utilitarian focus on influencing others….to an emphasis on the

1 Although House and colleagues (1977; 1999; 1993) are largely credited with charismatic leadership theory, the

concept was raised in the early work of Everett Hagan who framed charismatic leadership as an antidote to

bureaucracy (Hagan, 1962). Even in this early framing we can see the tension between task (bureaucracy) and

relationship.

Page 21: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

21

importance of leadership as having a moral dimension and being concerned with the ‘greater

good’ (Alimo-Metcalfe, 2013, p. 22). The framework contrasts three types of leaders:

transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire. Transformational leaders are characterized as

leaders who are able to motivate followers to view their work from new perspectives, who are

aware of, and enact, the organization’s mission or vision, and who look beyond their own

interests towards those that will benefit the group (Bass, 1985). In contrast, transactional leaders

operate within the existing system, focus on time constraints and efficiency, and use adherence to

process as a means for maintaining control (Bass, 1985). Transactional leaders are reliant on

task structure to specify the “right way” to do things, and thus maintain a dependence on the

leader for guidance on problem solutions (Lowe, Kroeck, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996). Finally,

‘laissez-faire’ leadership is characterized as leaders who “avoid making decisions, abdicates

responsibility, and does not use their authority” (Antonakis, Avolio, & Sivasubramaniam, 2003:

265). Laissez-faire leadership has also been described as the ‘non-leadership’ (Tejeda, Scandura,

& Pillai, 2001) and is widely agreed to be the least effective form of leadership (Bass & Bass,

2008; Antonakis, Avolio, & Sivasubramaniam, 2003; Tejeda, Scandura, & Pillai, 2001).

Transformational leadership and transactional leadership are commonly measured using the

Multi-factor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ) or variations of this instrument (for a review see

Yukl, 1994). Using the MLQ, Bass (1985) found empirical evidence that transformational and

transactional leadership are distinct, yet positively correlated dimensions (see also Lowe et al.,

1996) – that is, that transformational leadership builds on transactional leadership rather than the

alternative view as transformational and transactional leadership being at either end of a

continuum (Burns, 1978). Bass (1985) consequently argued that a leader needs to exhibit both

transactional and transformational competencies and that both are important for leadership

Page 22: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

22

effectiveness. In support of Bass, empirical evidence can be found to support a significant

association between both transformational leadership (Bass, 1998; Lowe et al., 1996; Skakon et

al., 2010; Tims et al., 2011; Yukl, 1999), transactional leadership (Bass, Avolio, & Goodheim,

1987; Waldman, Bass, & Yammarino, 1990; Yammarino & Bass, 1990) and subordinate

satisfaction and performance. This is consistent with the early views of Bales (1958) who argued

that both task and relations oriented leadership are required for effective performance. In the

following section, we describe in more detail the respective dimensions or factors of

transformational and transactional leadership, identify the scale items currently used to measure

each dimension, and discuss how these dimensions may map onto the opposing domains (the

Task Positive Network (TPN) and the Default-Mode Network (DMN)) discussed earlier.

Transformational leadership

Transformational leadership comprises four dimensions: idealized influence (also known as

charisma), inspirational motivation, intellectual stimulation, individualized consideration.

Idealized influence was originally labeled ‘charisma’, however Bass and Avolio (1990) relabeled

the construct due to the popularized connotation of charisma as being “celebrated, flamboyant,

exciting and arousing” (Bass & Bass, 2008: 620), as well as being associated with Adolf Hitler’s

treatment of the German people. Idealized influence is concerned with follower’s perception of

the leader’s ability to instill a sense of pride, respect, and trust and to convey a collective sense of

purpose and mission (Knippenberg & Sitkin, 2013). This dimension includes the leader showing

high levels of integrity, being admired and respected by followers (Alimo-Metcalfe. 2013), being

perceived as confident and powerful, and focusing on high-order ideals and ethics (Antonakis, et

al., 2003). The items in the MLQ pertaining to this dimension include: “I go beyond self-interest

Page 23: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

23

for the good of the group”; I act in ways that build others’ respect for me”; “I talk about my most

important values and beliefs”; and “I consider the moral and ethical consequences of decisions”.

The second dimension of transformational leadership is inspirational motivation. This

dimension refers to the way in which a leader energizes their followers by providing meaning,

optimism, and enthusiasm towards a shared vision (Alimo-Metcalfe, 2013), stresses ambitious

goals, and communicates to followers that the vision is achievable (Antonakis et al., 2003). In a

recent critique, Knippenberg & Sitkin (2013) note that inspirational motivation and idealized

influence (discussed above) are usually so highly correlated that they are combined into one

“charisma” factor. However, as discussed earlier, for the purposes of this paper, the discriminant

validity of interest is that between the DMN and the TPN, rather than that of constructs within

each of these cognitive domains. We believe that both idealized influence and inspirational

motivation are associated with social reasoning as opposed to mechanical reasoning, and more

specifically, the DMN. The items in the MLQ used to measure inspirational motivation

include:“I express confidence that goals will be achieved” and “I talk enthusiastically about what

needs to be accomplished”.

Intellectual stimulation refers to a leader’s tendency to encourage followers to arrive at novel

solutions (Alimo-Metcalfe. 2013), to use reasoning before taking action (Lowe, Kroeck,

Sivasubramaniam, 1996), to challenge existing assumptions and beliefs, and to stimulate

problem solving and creative consideration of issues (Antonakis et al., 2003; Knippenberg &

Sitkin, 2013). The items in the Multi-factor Leadership Questionnaire used to measure this

Page 24: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

24

dimension include: “I seek differing perspectives when solving problems” and “I suggest new

ways of looking at how to complete assignments”.

The final dimension of transformational leadership is individualized consideration. This

dimension is concerned with a leader’s recognition of the differing needs of followers

(Knippenberg, & Sitkin, 2013), the creation of opportunities for development, coaching and

mentoring (Alimo-Metcalfe, 2013), and the fulfillment of higher level needs such as self-

actualization (Antonakis et al., 2003). Items in the MLQ pertaining to this dimension include: “I

spend time teaching and coaching” and “I consider an individual as having different needs,

abilities, and aspirations from others”.

Transactional leadership

In contrast to transformational leadership, transactional leadership is characterized by fulfillment

of obligations, monitoring and controlling of outcomes (Antonakis, et al., 2003), and a quid pro

quo relationship between leader and follower (Alimo-Metcalfe, 2013). Knippenberg & Sitkin

(2013: 6) describe transactional leadership as “a more common and traditional form of leadership

based on an exchange between leader and follower that speaks to follower self-interest”. Bass

(1985) proposed three dimensions of transactional leadership: contingent reward, management

by exception (active), and management by exception (passive).

Contingent reward refers to leader behaviors that clarify role and task requirements (Antonakis

et al., 2003) and that punish or reward followers based on their accomplishment or otherwise of

Page 25: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

25

contractual (task) obligations (Alimo-Metcalfe, 2103; Antonakis et al., 2003). Desired follower

actions are rewarded, while the disapproved actions are punished. Items in the MLQ pertaining

to this dimension include: “I discuss in specific terms who is responsible for achieving

performance targets” and “I express satisfaction when others meet expectations”.

Management by exception is concerned with how leaders initiate and carry-out out corrective

behaviors. This dimension is generally considered as two factors: management by exception

active and management by exception passive. Management by exception active has been

referred to as a leader’s “active vigilance” (Antonakis et al., 2003: 265) and monitoring of

performance to ensure that standards/tasks are achieved. In contrast, management by exception

passive refers to a leader intervening only when a mistake has already occurred. Measurement of

this dimension includes items such as: “I concentrate my full attention on dealing with mistakes,

complaints, and failures”; “I direct my attention toward failures to meet standards”; “I wait for

things to go wrong before taking action”; and I show that I am a firm believer in “If it ain’t

broke, don’t fix it.”

Transformational-Transactional Leadership and the Opposing Domains

On the whole, the conceptualization and measurement of transformational leadership appears to

represent a form of social cognition, or more precisely, the type of reasoning that would occur in

the metalizing system (Jack et al., in press) and the Default-Mode Network (DMN), while

transactional leadership appears to be more closely aligned with the Task Positive Network

(TPN). Specifically, recall that the DMN is associated with reasoning about the mental states of

others and reflecting on one’s own mental states. Items such as “I consider an individual as

Page 26: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

26

having different needs, abilities, and aspirations from others” and “I talk about my most

important values and beliefs” appear to be clearly linked to the Default Mode Network.

However other items included in this construct do not map as well. For example, “I express

confidence that goals will be achieved” could be considered as a task-focused item due to the

reference to goal achievement and “I suggest new ways of looking at how to complete

assignments” and “I seek differing perspectives when solving problems” also appear to have a

task focus, particularly if the leader is using these competencies with task achievement in mind.

In light of the work of earlier scholars who argued that both task and relationship are important

components of leadership effectiveness, the inclusion of a few task-focused measures in the

transformational leadership construct may in fact play an important part in the general finding

that transformational leadership is more effective than transactional leadership (Bass & Bass,

2008). This view is consistent with that argued by Bass (1985) who suggested that

transformational leadership augments transactional leadership rather than the two being on

opposite ends of a continuum. Given that we now know that the type of task a leader is faced

with impacts which neural network is activated and therefore which is suppressed, it would be

interesting to further explore the effect sizes of each factor of the transformational scale based on

whether the task is analytical or social in nature. We would expect that when faced with an

analytical task, the task-focused items would explain a greater amount of variance in leadership

effectiveness than they would in a social or relational task.

To further complicate the matter, multiple studies have found that one factor of the transactional

leadership function loads strongly with the transformational factors which has led some scholars

Page 27: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

27

to rightly question the discriminant validity of the MLQ. As noted by Kuvaas and colleagues

(Kuvaas et al., 2012: 758), transactional leadership as measured by the MLQ “seems to represent

two separate factors; one of which loads with transactional subscales, and one of which loads

with transformational subscales (Goodwin, Wofford, & Whittington, 2001). Accordingly, these

scales represent both the social and the economic sides of exchange relationships”. The

opposing domains hypothesis suggests that this cross-loading of items should be greater when

the task is analytical in nature or when followers have a high need for achievement.

Leader-member exchange versus Leader-member social exchange

Alongside the development of the transformational-transactional framework, Graen, Novak, &

Sommerkamp (1982) introduced the Leader Member Exchange (LMX) framework which

focuses on the differential quality of the relationship between leader and follower. The LMX

framework departed from the ‘average leadership style’ argument, which assumes that leaders

adopt an ‘average’ style, in that the relationship they form with each follower is of equal quality.

In contrast, the LMX framework suggests that the quality of a leader’s relationships is different

for each follower and is largely dependent on whether the follower is ‘in-group’ or ‘out-group’.

Low quality relationships are based on the transactional part of the employment contract while

high quality relationships are based on mutual liking, trust, and respect (Graen, Novak, &

Sommerkamp, 1982).

LMX grew from an earlier framework known as the Vertical Dyad Linkage (VDL), which some

have argued was the first relationship-based leadership framework (Schriesheim, Castro, &

Cogliser, 1999). While the LMX framework introduced some new variables and measures of

Page 28: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

28

relationship quality, the underlying assumptions of the VDL framework and the LMX

framework remained similar. Namely, a leader and follower will not engage in a relationship

without (a) mutual respect for the capabilities of the other, (2) the anticipation of deepening

reciprocal trust with the other, and (3) the expectation that interacting obligation will grow over

time as career-oriented social exchanges blossom into a partnership (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995:

237). The literature also presents a second theoretical framing of LMX, which adopts a social

exchange perspective and argues that while competence (mutual respect for capabilities) may be

a component in a social exchange, it is not a prerequisite to entering into a social exchange with

a leader (Bernerth, Armenakis, Field, Giles, & Walker, 2007). It is this distinction between the

VDL theoretical framing of the LMX framework and the social exchange perspective of the

LMX framework that lead to the development of Leader-Member Social Exchange (LMSX),

which sought to reconcile inconsistencies in the operational definition and measurement of

LMX. Bernerth and colleagues (Bernerth, et al., 2007) argued that the current measures of LMX

reflected the VDL theoretical framing of LMX rather than the social exchange perspective:

“researchers who use social exchange as a basis for building models, and subsequently measure a

different construct (i.e. VDL), are not advancing our understanding of the social exchange

between subordinates and supervisors” (Bernerth et al., 2007: 980). The LMSX measure was

designed to specifically capture the social exchange component of the leader follower

relationship.

In summarizing the development of the LMX-LMSX literature, we might conclude that there are

currently two theoretical and empirical conceptualizations of the leader-follower relationship:

LMX and LMSX. The LMX framework is based on the original assumptions of the VDL

framework and thus has a key assumption that follower competence is a pre-requisite for a social

Page 29: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

29

exchange between leader and follower. There are a number of measures in the literature for this

conceptualization of LMX, the most common being the LMX7 and the LMX-MDM. In contrast,

LMSX is based on the social exchange component of the leader follower relationship, therefore

follower competence is not considered a prerequisite of the exchange relationship. Table 3

presents the scale items for two different measures of LMX and for LMSX. In examining the

scale items it appears that the LMX and LMX-MDM scales are more relational in nature while

the LMSX scale is transactional in nature. Indeed the developers of the LMSX measure noted

that the use of the social exchange perspective of LMX raises “an interesting theoretical

question, that is, whether or not a social exchange relationship based on feelings of obligatory

repayment is necessarily a good thing” (Bernerth et al., 2007). Bernerth and colleagues argue

that while the LMSX measure may appear transactional, the lack of intentionality and certainty

in the exchanges moves the relationship beyond the transactional level and allows for feelings of

gratitude, trust, and loyalty. However, when examining the measure used, a person who scored

high on the LMSX scale does not seem to reflect any form of uncertainty or lack of

intentionality, rather a high score on this measure appears to reflect a certain expectation of a

transactional relationship, which according to the VDL conceptualization of LMX, would

suggest a low-quality relationship. The opposing domains hypothesis suggests that the

intentionality of the leader or follower is indeed a critical determinant of the quality of the

relationship that is formed. A leader-follower relationship based on an expectation of exchange

would place both parties in the Task Positive Network, which actually inhibits the ability of each

party to engage in social reasoning – which one would assume is critical in relationship

formation.

Page 30: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

30

We could also argue that framing a relationship as a transactional exchange may result in higher

levels of leadership effectiveness in a task-oriented or highly analytical context due to the

activation of the Task Positive Network (TPN), which is congruent with the network required for

task performance; however in a relational context in which the Default Mode Network (DMN) is

the dominant neural network, a transactional framing of the leader-follower relationship would

suppress the DMN, which would lead to lower task performance (because the task is social or

relational in nature and thus requires the DMN). In other words, we would not expect LMX as

measure by the LMX-7 or the LMX-MBM to have a positive relationship with task performance

when the task is analytical, however when the task is relational in nature we would expect a

positive relationship between LMX and performance. Interestingly, the early literature does

offer some support for this premise: Vecchio (1982) examined the relationship between LMX

and task performance of 48 Air Force enlisted men on a training simulation game (analytical

task) and found no significant relationship. Similarly, Vecchio and Gobdel (1984) examined the

relationship between LMX and bank teller performance (teller difference) and also failed to find

a significant relationship. More recently, Gerstner and Day (1997) conducted a meta-analysis

which included 79 studies and found a small, non-significant correlation between LMX and

objective performance (r = 0.11, further reduces to r = .07 after the removal of one outlier),

however stronger relationships were found between LMX and relational performance indicators

such as organizational commitment (r = 0.42).

Recent literature has begun to explore the possibility that both economic (also called

instrumental and/or transactional) and social components of a leader-follower relationship can

have positive consequences on task performance. For example, Kuvaas et al., (2012)

differentiate between ‘social leader member exchange; (SLMX) and ‘economic leader member

Page 31: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

31

exchange (ELMX) and argue that social and economic relationships should not be considered at

either end of the quality continuum, rather they reflect different types of relationships. Kuvaas

and colleagues cite Goodwin et al. (2009: 973) who argued that the “ instrumental and social

aspects of the relationship appear to exist simultaneously…although one form of LMX probably

dominates over the other.” (Kuvaas et al., 2012: 761). At this stage, the literature has not

considered the conditions under which an economic exchange relationship (ELMX) is beneficial

to task performance and leadership effectiveness as opposed to a social exchange relationship

(SLMX), however as suggested above, we belief that the type of task and the aspects of

performance being measured (analytical versus relational) might be a useful starting point.

Page 32: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

32

Table 3: Scale items for common measures of LMX and LMSX

LMX-7 LMX-MDM LMSX

I usually know where I stand with my manager.

I like my supervisor very much as a person.

If my manager does something for me, I will return the

favor at some point.

My supervisor understands my problems and needs

well enough.

My supervisor defends my work actions to a superior,

even without complete knowledge of the issue in

question.

My manager and I have a two-way exchange relationship.

My supervisor recognizes some of my potential.

My supervisor would come to my defense if I were

‘attacked’ by others.

I do not have to specify the exact conditions to know my

manager will return a favor.

Regardless of how much power my manager has built

into his or her position, my manager would be

personally inclined to use his/her power to help me

solve problems at work.

My supervisor is the kind of person one would like to

have as a friend.

If I do something for my manager, he or she will

eventually repay me.

I can count on my supervisor to ‘bail me out’ at

his/her expense when I really need it.

My supervisor is a lot of fun to work with.

I give more than I take with my supervisor (R)

I have enough confidence in my supervisor to defend

and justify his/her decisions when he/she is not

present to do so.

My supervisor would defend me to others in the

organization if I made an honest mistake.

My opinion has an influence on my manager, and his or

her opinion has an influence on me.

My working relationship with my supervisor is

effective.

I respect my supervisor’s knowledge of and competence

on the job am willing to apply extra efforts, beyond those

normally required, to further the interests of my work

group.

I have a balance of inputs and outputs with my manager.

I do work for my supervisor that goes beyond what is

specified in my job description.

When my supervisor gives me support, I feel I owe it to

him or her to return the favor.

I am impressed with my supervisor’s knowledge of

his/her job.

My efforts are reciprocated by my manager.

I admire my supervisor’s professional skills. My relationship with my manager is composed of

comparable exchanges of giving and taking.

When I give effort at work, my manager will return it.

Voluntary actions on my part will be returned in some

way by my manager.

Page 33: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

33

DISCUSSION: RESOLVING THE TENSION

The previous section has shown that the distinction between task and relationship competencies

is clearly delineated throughout the leadership literature, , however the exact nature and

consequences of this dichotomy has received little attention. In this section, we present potential

strategies to resolve, or at least minimize the consequences of the tension leaders face between

attending to task requirements and attending to relationships as a result of the antagonism

between the TPN and the DMN. First, we consider the possibility that leaders have a neural

predisposition towards either the TPN or the DMN. We then explore how we might be able to

minimize the extent of the suppression of the opposing network. Specifically, we argue that a

number of individual difference variables and situational variables may moderate the relationship

between the type of task a leader is faced with and the extent to which one network is activated

and the other suppressed. . Finally, we argue that a leader’s ability to switch between the TPN

and the DMN is critical for leadership effectiveness.

Neural disposition: matching the leader to the situation

As with hormonal disposition, there is some evidence to suggest that humans have a natural

disposition towards either analytical-mechanical reasoning and therefore the Task Positive

Network (TPN) or social-relational reasoning and therefore the Default Mode Network (DMN)

(Jack et al., 2013). While neural disposition is a relatively nascent area, hormonal dispositions

are well documented in the literature (see for example, Insel, 1997; Schulkin, J. 1999). Among

the body of work on hormonal disposition, people with higher unconscious power motives have

higher resting levels of epinephrine secretion, and people with higher unconscious Need for

Achievement motives appear to have higher resting levels of vasopressin secretion (Boyatzis et

Page 34: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

34

al., 2012; Boyatzis & Sala, 2004)). At the behavioral level, neural and hormonal dispositions

may underlie certain personality characteristics, learning styles, and perhaps preferred leadership

style.

The idea that leaders have a natural or preferred leadership style is an assumption underlying

many of the trait and contingency leadership models. The early leadership literature offers a vast

array of research geared toward identifying an individual’s disposition for leadership including

physical characteristics (Bernard, 1928 as cited in Bass & Bass, 2008), extroversion

(Goodenough, 1930 as cited in Bass & Bass, 2008), optimism (Drake, 1944 as cited in Bass &

Bass, 2008); task-related personality characteristics (Cummings & Scott, 1965; Medow &

Zander, 1965; Judge, Bono, Ilies, et al., 2002); social personality characteristics (Krumboltz,

Christal, & Ward, 1959; D.S. Brown, 1964). However, as with much trait-based research, for

every study finding support for trait based hypotheses there was another that failed to find

support. In an attempt to reconcile inconsistent findings, scholars began to consider the

interaction between the leader and the situation (Stodgill, 1948). The ‘pure’ situational

approaches to leadership argued that effective leadership is not a result of personality, drive, or

ability, rather effective leadership is a product of the right “time, place, and circumstance” (Bass

& Bass, 2008: 52). However, as with the pure trait theories, situation alone did not provide

consistent evidence of effective leadership, which lead to the emergence of the person-situation

theories which posited that a good ‘fit’ between person and situation was the key to effective

leadership (Boyatzis, 1982).

Page 35: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

35

The person-situation fit approach to leadership was a dominant approach from the 1970s (Bass &

Bass, 2008) and laid the foundations for the introduction of follower characteristics and

relational frameworks that emerged in the new paradigm models (see earlier discussion of

transformational-transactional leadership and leader-member exchange). Considering the

opposing domains hypothesis, one way to relieve the tension between the task and relational

components of leadership could be to match a leader’s predisposition towards the task or

relationship to the type of task they. In many ways, Bales’(1958) suggestion of the need to two

leaders was an attempt to do this. The utility of matching a leader’s competency (task or

relationship) to the situation has found some empirical support in the literature. For example,

Slatter (1955) found that when task demands are high, being liked does not contribute to

leadership effectiveness and social or relational skills are not highly valued, whereas in therapy

groups, sensitivity training groups, and social clubs socio-emotional skills were important.

While matching the leader to the type of task may provide some performance benefits, from a

practical standpoint, this is virtually impossible as task requirements are constantly toggling

between task and relationship. An alternative to approach is to train and develop leaders so that

they possess a high level of competency in both domains. Neuroscience literature suggests that

while a leader may prefer or have a disposition toward either the TPNor the DMN, these

dispositions can be changed through training and experience – a phenomenon known as

plasticity. Therefore, a disposition toward either task or relationship does not preclude a leader

from developing their abilities in the opposing domain. In fact, we believe that this development

is a critical component in allowing a leader to effectively switch between the TPN and the DMN.

Neural resource efficiency

Page 36: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

36

The opposing domains hypothesis could be framed as presenting a form of ‘trade-off’ between

engaging in task-related leadership activities and engaging in relationships building activities. In

this framing, neurological activation is essentially a form of resource which leaders can use to

attain or increase their effectiveness. When the leader expends more neurological resources

attending to relationships, they consequently have fewer resources to invest in the analytical

components of the task and vice versa. In this framing, Bales’ (1958) assertion that in order to

maximize performance we need two leaders – one to deal with the task and the other with

relationships - essentially translates to increasing the neurological resources available to the

group by introducing another leader, while holding the efficiency or rate of each leader’s uses of

these resources constant. An alternative strategy is that we change the rate or efficiency of the

leader’s neurological resources thereby minimizing the extent of activation required to

successfully complete the task. Because the task network (TPN) and the social-relational

network (DMN) are anti-correlated, minimizing the degree of activation in one network also

serves to proportionality decrease the suppression of the opposing network.

When faced with an analytical task, a leader will require a certain degree or level of activity in

the TPN in order to successfully complete that task. The degree of activation required will

depend on a number of individual difference characteristics and a number of situational

contingencies. Any variables which influence the amount of cognitive effort required to

successfully complete the task will moderate the relationship between type of task and the degree

of activation of the corresponding network. For example, when faced with a mathematical

problem, a leader with a high level of mathematical skills will activate their TPN to a lesser

degree than a leader without such skills. Consequently, this leader’s opposing network (the

DMN) will be suppressed less, allowing the leader to invest relatively more neurological

Page 37: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

37

resources in maintaining high quality relationships with followers despite the presence of a

highly analytical task. Other task-related variables that might moderate the relationship between

analytical/mechanical tasks and the degree of activation of the TPN include cognitive

intelligence, emotional intelligence (as opposed to social intelligence, see Boyatzis & Sala,

2004), task competence, task difficulty, and the degree to which the task is ‘routine’.

Conversely, when faced with a social/relational task, the moderating variables would be those

individual difference variables or situational variables that would result in a decrease of activity

in the Default Mode Network, resulting in less suppression of the Task Positive Network. Such

variables might include a leader’s social intelligence, personality (extroversion) and the pre-

existing relationship quality (Figure 10 provides a visual representation of these relationships).

Task Positive Network Competencies

Individual difference variables

Cognitive intelligence

We expect that leaders with a high level of cognitive intelligence (commonly referred to as ‘g’)

will show greater ability in analytical or mechanical type tasks. Cognitive intelligence tests

generally draw heavily on logic problems and reasoning about inanimate objects rather than

moral problems or reasoning about emotions (Lubinski, 2004) which is the type of reasoning

associated with the Task Positive Network (TPN). Thus, when faced with an analytical or

mechanical task, we expect leaders high levels of cognitive intelligence will require less ‘effort’

or neurological resources to successfully complete the analytical or mechanical task than leaders

with lower levels of cognitive intelligence:

Page 38: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

38

P1: A leader’s cognitive intelligence moderates the relationship between

analytical task and the degree to which the TPN is activated. To successfully

complete an analytical task, leaders with higher cognitive intelligence will show

less activity in the TPN than those with low cognitive intelligence.

Task competence

A leader who shows a high level of competence in a given task will require less neurological

resources than leaders with a lower level of competence. Competence may be a result of

experience or other skills and abilities. A high level of task competence will result in the leader

experiencing the task with less intensity resulting in a lower level of activation in the TPN.

Thus,

P2: A leader’s level of task competence moderates the relationship between analytical

task and the degree to which the TPN is activated. To successfully complete an

analytical task, leaders with higher task competence will show less activity in the TPN

than those with low task competence.

Emotional intelligence

Our justification for including emotional intelligence in the TPN as opposed to the DMN is

discussed earlier in the paper, however, in short, when a leader uses sense-making around the

emotions of as a strategic tool to achieve the task, the neuroscience literature suggests that are

operating in the task domain (TPN) rather that the social (DMN). We expect that when faced

Page 39: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

39

with an analytical task, leaders with high emotional intelligence will require less cognitive effort

to engage followers than leaders with low emotional intelligence:

P3: A leader’s emotional intelligence moderates the relationship between

analytical task and the degree to which the TPN is activated. To successfully

complete an analytical task, leaders with higher emotional intelligence will show

less activity in the TPN than those with low emotional intelligence.

Situational variables

Task difficulty

The inclusion of task difficulty as a situational variable rather than an individual difference

variable (in which case task difficulty would be the ‘perception’ rather than an objective

measure) is based on the assumption that there is some objectivity in the difficulty of an

analytical task. For example, there are ‘easy’ mathematical problems which would be classified

as easy by the large majority of the adult population, and there are ‘difficult’ mathematical

problems which would be experienced as such by the majority of the population. Holding

competence, experiences, and intelligence constant, more difficult tasks will require more effort

or neurological resources than easy tasks.

P4: Task difficulty moderates the relationship between analytical task and the degree to

which the TPN is activated. Leaders faced with a difficult analytical task will show more

activity in the TPN than leaders faced with an easy analytical task.

Routinization

Page 40: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

40

We expect that leaders faced with familiar or routine task will show less activity in the TPN than

when faced with novel or new tasks. This proposition is closely linked to the learning cycle –

when a task is new it requires greater cognitive effort than when a task is familiar. For example,

when learning to drive, the require attention on the ‘task’ of driving is high, which leaves less

neurological resources for holding a conversation with passengers. Perhaps this is why learner

drivers are prohibited from carrying passengers in many countries. As driving becomes more

routine or familiar, the ‘task’ component requires less neurological resources to achieve the same

level of performance, which allows drivers to hold conversations and while simultaneously

engaging in the analytical task. We expect this logic also applies to the daily work of leaders in

that when analytical tasks are routine, leaders will require less neurological resources to attain a

given level of performance in that task, and will therefore show less activity in the TPN. Thus,

P5: The extent to which an analytical task is routine in nature moderates the

relationship between analytical task and the degree to which the TPN is activated.

Leaders faced with a routine analytical task will show less activity in the TPN

than when faced with a non-routine task.

Page 41: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

41

Figure 10: Proposed conceptual model

Page 42: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

42

Default Mode Network Competencies

Individual difference variables

Social intelligence

Social intelligence refers to how people handle relationships, their awareness of the needs and

concerns of others, and their ability to induce desirable responses in others (Boyatzis, 2011). We

believe that leaders with a high level of social intelligence are likely to feel more comfortable in

a social or relational task and will likely to perceive that task as less challenging and less intense

than a leader with low social intelligence. Thus, when faced with a social or relational task, we

expect leaders with high social intelligence (as opposed to emotional intelligence - see Boyatzis

& Sala, 2004) to require less neurological resources to successfully complete the task relative to

those leaders with low social intelligence.

P6: Social intelligence moderates the relationship between social tasks and the

degree of activation of the Default Mode Network. When faced with a social task,

leaders with high emotional-social intelligence will show less activity in the DMN

than those with low emotional-social intelligence.

Personality

When faced with a social or relational task, leaders with an extroverted personality are

expected to show less neurological activation in the DMN than leaders with an introverted

personality. Extroverts enjoy and derive energy from engaging in social or relational tasks

(Barrick & Mount, 1991), which we suspect would be reflected in a lower level of

activation in the Default Mode Network. Conversely, introverts find social interactions to

Page 43: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

43

be energy depleting and would therefore find social or relational tasks more demanding,

which we believe will result in higher levels of activation in the Default Mode Network

when faced with such tasks. Thus,

P7: Personality moderates the relationship between social tasks and the degree of

activation of the Default Mode Network. When faced with a social task,

extroverts will show less activity in the DMN than introverts.

Situational variables

When faced with a social or relational task, we expect leaders who have already developed high

quality relationships with their followers to show less activity in the Default Mode Network.

High quality leader follower relationships are characterized by mutual trust, respect, and

obligation and followers are seen as ‘trusted assistants’ while low-quality leader follower

relationships are characterized by low trust, respect and obligation and followers are seen as

‘hired hands’ (Zalesny & Graen, 1987). In order to successfully complete a social or relational

task, we expect that established high-quality leader follower relationships will reduce the

difficulty and intensity of the task, thus reducing the degree of activation of the Default Mode

Network:

P8: Existing relationship quality moderates the relationship between social task

and degree of activation in the Default Mode Network. When faced with a social

task, leaders with high quality relationships with their followers will show less

activity in the Default Mode Network than those with low quality relationships

with their followers

Page 44: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

44

Switching between the TPN and DMN

The idea that a task can be classified as either analytical or social is useful for theoretical

purposes; however as with most dichotomies, the distinction is rarely this clean-cut. In reality,

and particularly in the context of leadership, all tasks have a relational component and an

analytical component. Given this, a leader’s ability to switch between the Task Positive Network

and the Default Mode Network becomes key consideration. We suspect that minimizing the

suppression of the opposing network will make it easier, faster, and less costly for the leader to

switch between the two networks. For example, we have already argued that leaders with certain

social competencies require less cognitive effort to complete a social task than those without

these competencies resulting in less activation of the DMN and less suppression of the TPN.

Hence, the difference or gap between the two networks is reduced which would make switching

between the two networks faster and less costly. Reducing the gap between the two networks

could also be viewed as reducing the intensity at which a leader experiences the task. There is an

optimal level of intensity that allows a leader to switch relatively effortlessly between the TPN

and the DMN allowing them to attend to task requirements and relationships. Additionally, the

ability to sense the optimal timing the switch in relationship to task demands, their own needs,

and the needs of their followers may also be a critical component in understanding leadership

effectiveness. For example, in the context of learning a new task, interruptions can be extremely

costly, not only in terms of task outcomes (errors) but also in the ability to ‘pick up where you

left off’. Once a person has gained a higher level of mastery, interruptions will be less costly and

following the interruption, the person will be able to re-engage with the task faster.

Page 45: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

45

Prior research on the intensity of emotions suggests that in order to move a person from a

negative emotional state (NEA) to a positive emotional state (PEA), the intensity of the emotion

must be reduced to reach a tipping point. (Boyatzis, 2013; Boyatzis et al., 2012). It seems

reasonable to suggest that a similar principle exists for switching between that TPN and the

DMN.. Boyatzis (2013) argues that when people are in the Positive Emotional Attractor they

are “more perceptually open and accurate in perceptions of others” (p. 1978; see also Boyatzis et

al., 2012; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), which is consistent with the work of the DMN in

allowing individuals to engage in reasoning about the emotions of others. In contrast, the

Negative Emotional Attractor is said to be linked to human survival, particularly to defend

against threats. Additionally, the NEA balances “unchecked optimism” which has been linked to

poor investment decisions (Gibson & Sanbonmatsu, 2004). – an analytical task that would

require activation of the TPN rather than the DMN The link between the PEA-NEA and the

TPN-DMN is also reflected in Fiedler’s Cognitive Resources Theory (1986). Fiedler & McQuire

(1987, as cited in Bass & Bass, 2008) found that under non-stressful conditions, leaders with

fluid intelligence (IQ) perform better than leaders with crystallized intelligence (experience);

however under stressful conditions, leaders with crystallized intelligence performed better.

Cognitive resource theory posits that the reason for this distinction is that under stressful

conditions, a leader with fluid intelligence will rely on intellectual solutions to a task even when

such solutions are inappropriate. In other words, under stressful conditions, a leader is ‘stuck’ in

the Task Positive Network (TPN) and also in the Negative Emotional Attractor due to the stress

condition, which limits their ability to switch into the DMN and the Positive Emotional Attractor

to explore non-intellectual solutions. Leaders with crystallized intelligence (intelligence based

on past experience and learning) are likely to experience the same situation with less intensity,

Page 46: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

46

thus these leaders will be 1) closer to the NEA-PEA tipping point and 2) more able to switch

between the TPN and the DMN. Thus,

P9: Reducing the degree of activation in the dominant network and therefore

minimizing the degree of suppression of the opposing neural network will

increase the ability of a leader to switch between the Task Positive Network and

the Default Mode Network and increase the speed at which they are able to do

so.

Given that leadership almost always requires consideration of both task requirements (TPN) and

relationships (DMN), it follows that the greater ability a leader has to switch between these two

modes of thinking the more effective they will be as a leader. The importance of a leader being

able to ‘switch’ between opposing states has been discussed in the literature and has more

recently been referred to as ‘dual tuning models’. George and Zhou (2007) found that both

positive and negative moods have a strong positive correlation with creativity and that

supervisors can create a context that supports both states by offering developmental feedback,

displaying interactional justice, and by being trustworthy (see also George, 2011). Along with

the ability to switch between the two opposing networks, which we argued requires a reduction

in intensity of the dominant network, we also expect the ability to appropriately time the shift

from task to relationship to be important both in terms of minimizing disruption for followers

and for maximizing the effectiveness of the shift. For example, knowing that activation of the

Default Mode Network suppresses our analytical-mechanical reasoning abilities (Jack et al.,

2013), it would be unwise for a leader to engage followers in activities requiring social or

relational reasoning at a time in which distraction from the task could result in serious

consequences. Similarly, in situations in which require social or relational reasoning,, for

example during the group formation period, introducing task-focused activities may inhibit

Page 47: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

47

relationship development in the group. The timing of the switch may also have implications for

the timing of feedback. Based on the opposing domains hypothesis, the closer the time period

between action and feedback, the more congruence there should be between the type of feedback

and the type of task. For example, if feedback is being given while a person is performing an

analytical task, the feedback given should be analytical or technical in nature as this type of

feedback is consistent with the neural network the receiver is engaged in. If the leader wishes to

give feedback that requires the follower to engage in social or relational reasoning, they would

be better to wait until the receiver has disengaged from the analytical task. The same can be said

for giving task-related feedback in an emotionally charged situation:

P10: The ability of a leader to switch between the Task Positive Network and the

Default Mode Network is positively related to leader effectiveness.

P10a: The timing of the shift between the Task Positive Network and the Default

Mode Network Moderates the relationship between the ability to switch between

networks and leadership effectiveness.

Given that the decision to switch from one cognitive domain to another requires reasoning about

the emotional state of self and others, leaders with greater Default-Mode Network abilities are

likely to be more able to successfully time the shifts than leaders who lack such abilities. Thus,

P10b: Leaders who have greater Default-Mode Network abilities will be more

able to gauge when a shift is necessary and/or appropriate.

Page 48: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

48

CONCLUSION

The distinction between task and relationship in the leadership literature can be traced back to

the early 1900s with the shift from scientific management to the human relations movement.

This distinction still exists in modern day leadership frameworks yet little consideration has been

given to the exact nature of the relationship between task-oriented reasoning and social and

relational reasoning. Recent findings in the neuroscience domain have shed light on this

relationship; specifically, analytical (task) reasoning and relational (social) reasoning are in fact

to two distinct anti-correlated cognitive modes (Jack et al., 2013). This paper has explored the

possible implications of this finding (known as the ‘opposing domains hypothesis’) for

leadership. We began our discussion with a brief review the early task-oriented approaches to

leadership and examined the early frameworks that made clear distinctions between task and

relationship along with various attempts at understanding the implications of each for leadership

effectiveness. We then moved to review the ‘new paradigm’ leadership frameworks, which

place a greater emphasis on the relational components of leadership. We focused specifically on

Bass’ transformational-transactional framework and the LMX-LMSX distinction. From this

review we could clearly see not only that the distinction (and in the later models blurring) of

task-oriented and relationship-oriented leadership behaviors is delineated throughout the

leadership literature, but also that both appear to be critical components of leadership

effectiveness. With this in mind, the final section of the paper presented a series of propositions

regarding how leaders may be able to maintain a degree of balance between the two cognitive

domains by reducing the degree of activation in the dominant network and consequently

minimizing the suppression of the opposing network, which we argued would allow them to

switch efficiently and effectively between task and relationship demands.

Page 49: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

49

Theoretical contribution and practical implications

This paper has identified a key pattern in the leadership literature and linked this pattern to

cutting edge research in the neuroscience domain. In doing so, we have raised a number of

questions regarding our treatment of the task and relationship distinction in the literature to date,

particularly the assumption that leaders are able to attend to both domains simultaneously.

Additionally, we have been able to add further explanation to some historical findings attempting

to understand the interaction between task, leadership style and leadership effectiveness. Finally,

we suggested an array of propositions that might extend our current conceptualization and

operationalization of leadership effectiveness.

From a practical standpoint, this paper suggests that developing a leader’s analytical and

relational abilities may be an important way to minimize the anti-correlation between the Task

Positive Network and the Default Mode Network, and in doing so, will allow leaders to switch

between the networks more efficiently. We believe that the ability to switch between these

networks may be a key component of leadership effectiveness and that this ability is one that can

be acquired through leadership training and development programs. Additionally, knowing that

engagement in analytical tasks inhibits our ability to engage in social or relational reasoning and

vice versa may have important implications for organizations in terms of how they structure and

order tasks that have analytical and relational components. For example, when giving feedback,

managers should consider if the feedback is analytical or task related in nature or interpersonal in

nature and time the delivery of that feedback accordingly. The same may be said for the

ordering of meeting agendas and performance review meetings.

Page 50: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

50

Future research

While this paper has focused specifically on the implications of the opposing domains hypothesis

for leadership, we believe that the distinction and anti-correlation between analytical-mechanical

reasoning and social reasoning exists in many other areas in the organizational behavior domain.

These areas include, but are not limited to, conflict management, trust, and moral reasoning. For

example, distinctions in the literature have been made between cognitive and relational trust

(Lewis & Weigert, 1985); cognitive conflict and affective conflict (Jehn, 1997; De Dreu &

Weigert, 2003); empathy and dehumanization (Haslam, 2006).

Relevant to the leadership domain specifically, further testing is needed in order to understand

which individual difference variables are most important in facilitating the shift between the two

networks. Once we have more information on this we will be able to target these variables in

leadership development training programs. Additionally, manipulation of situational

characteristics within each type of task, for example task difficulty and routineness for analytical

tasks, and prior relationship quality for social tasks will allow us to isolate the key situational

variables at play. Finally, further research on the link between hormonal systems and

neurological systems would allow us to understand how tipping points in hormonal systems

influence neurological tipping points.

Page 51: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

51

REFERENCES

Alimo-Metcalfe, B. 2013. A Critical Review of Leadership Theory. In H. S. Leonard, R. Lewis,

A. M. Freedman, & J. Passmore (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of the Psychology

of Leadership, Change, and Organizational Development (pp. 13–47). John Wiley &

Sons.

Antonakis, J., Avolio, B. J., & Sivasubramaniam, N. 2003. Context and leadership: An

examination of the nine-factor full-range leadership theory using the Multifactor

Leadership Questionnaire. The Leadership Quarterly, 14(3), 261–295.

Bales, R. F. 1958. Task roles and social roles in problem-solving groups. In E. Macoby, T.

Newcomb, & E. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology. New York, NY: Holt.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five Personality Dimensions and Job

Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44(1), 1–26.

Bass, B. M. 1960. Leadership, psychology, and organizational behavior. New York, NY:

Harper & Row.

Bass, B. M. 1985. Leadership and Performance Beyond Expectations. New York, NY: Free

Press.

Bass, B. M. 1990. From transactional to transformational leadership: Learning to share the

vision. Organizational Dynamics, 18(3), 19–31.

Bass, B. M. 1998. Transformational leadership: Industrial, military, and educational impact.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates.

Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. 1990. Developing Transformational Leadership: 1992 and Beyond.

Journal of European Industrial Training, 14(5), 21-37.

Bass, B. M., Avolio, B. J., & Goodheim, L. 1987. Biography and the Assessment of

Transformational Leadership at the World-Class Level. Journal of Management, 13(1), 7–

19.

Bass, B. M., & Bass, R. 2008. The Bass Handbook of Leadership: Theory, Research, and

Managerial Applications. Simon and Schuster.

Bennis, W., & Nanus, B. 1985. Leaders: The strategies for taking charge. New York: Harper

& Row.

Page 52: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

52

Bernerth, J.., Armenakis, A., Field, H., Giles, W., & Walker, H. 2007. Leader–member social

exchange (LMSX): development and validation of a scale. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 28(8), 979–1003.

Blake, R., & Mouton, J. 1964. The managerial grid. Houston, TX: Gulf.

Blau, P., & Scott, W. 1962. Formal organizations. San Francisco, CA: Chandler.

Bono, J. E., & Ilies, R. 2006. Charisma, positive emotions and mood contagion. The Leadership

Quarterly, 17(4), 317–334.

Bowers, D. G., & Seashore, S. E. 1966. Predicting Organizational Effectiveness With a Four-

Factor Theory of Leadership. Administrative Science Quarterly, 11(2), 238–263.

Boyatzis, R. E. 1982. The competent manager: A model for effective performance. New York,

NY: John Wiley.

Boyatzis, R. E. 2011. Learning life schools of emotional and social intelligence competencies.

In M. J. London (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Lifelong Learning (pp. 91–102). New

York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Boyatzis, R.E. 2013. When Pulling to the Negative Emotional Attractor is Too Much or Not

Enough to Inspire and Sustain Outstanding Leadership. In R. J. Burke & C. L. Cooper

(Eds.), The Fulfilling Workplace: The Organization’s Role in Achieving Individual and

Organizational Health. Burlington, TV: Ashgate Publishing, Ltd.

Boyatzis, R. E., & Goleman, D. 2007. Emotional and social competency inventory. Boston:

Hay Group.

Boyatzis, R. E., Passarelli, A. M., Koenig, K., Lowe, M., Mathew, B., Stoller, J. K., & Phillips,

M. 2012. Examination of the neural substrates activated in memories of experiences with

resonant and dissonant leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(2), 259–272.

Boyatzis, R. E., & Sala, F. 2004. The Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI). In G. Geher

(Ed.), Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy (pp. 147–

180). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

Brown, D. S. 1964. Subordinates’ Views of Ineffective Executive Behavior. Academy of

Management Journal, 7(4), 288–299.

Bryman, A. 1992. Charisma and leadership in organizations. London: Sage.

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. 2008. The brain’s default network.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 1–38.

Page 53: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

53

Burke, W. W. 1965. Leadership behavior as a function of the leader, the follower, and the

situation. Journal of Personality, 33(1), 60–81.

Burns, J. 1978. Leadership. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Cartwright, D., & Zander, A. 1960. Group dynamics: Research and theory. Evanston, IL: Row

Peterson.

Conger, J. A. 1989. The Charismatic Leader. Behind the mystique of exceptional leadership.

San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Inc.

Conger, J. A. 1999. Charismatic and transformational leadership in organizations: An insider’s

perspective on these developing streams of research. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(2),

145–179.

Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. 1998. Charismatic leadership in organizations. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Cowley, W. 1928. Three distinctions in the study of leaders. The Journal of Abnormal and

Social Psychology, 23(2), 144–157.

Cummings, L., & Scott, W. 1965. Academic and leadership performance of graduate business

student. Business Perspectives, 1, 11–20.

Dasborough, M., Ashkanasy, N, Tee, E., & Tse, H. 2009. What goes around comes around:

How meso-level negative emotional contagion can ultimately determine organizational

attitudes toward leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(4), 571–585.

De Dreu, C., & Weingart, L. 2003. Task versus relationship conflict, team performance and

team member satisfaction: a meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(4), 741–

749.

Fiedler, F. E. 1967. A theory of leadership effectiveness. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Fiedler, F. E. 1986. The Contribution of Cognitive Resources and Leader Behavior to

Organizational Performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16(6), 532–548.

Fleishman, E. A. 1953. Leadership Climate, Human Relations Training, and Supervisory

Behavior. Personnel Psychology, 6(2), 205–222.

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Essen, D. C. V., & Raichle, M. E. 2005.

The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional

networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of

America, 102(27), 9673–9678.

Page 54: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

54

Fransson, P. 2005. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: An fMRI

investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Human Brain

Mapping, 26(1), 15–29.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. 2005. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and

thought‐action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313–332.

George, J. M. 2011. Dual tuning A minimum condition for understanding affect in

organizations? Organizational Psychology Review, 1(2), 147–164.

George, J. M., & Zhou, J. 2007. Dual tuning in a supportive context: Joint contributions of

positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behaviors to employee creativity. Academy

of Management Journal, 50(3), 605–622.

Gerstner, C. R., & Day, D. V. 1997. Meta-Analytic review of leader–member exchange theory:

Correlates and construct issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(6), 827–844.

Gibson, B., & Sanbonmatsu, D. M. 2004. Optimism, Pessimism, and Gambling: The Downside

of Optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 149–160.

Golland, Y., Bentin, S., Gelbard, H., Benjamini, Y., Heller, R., Nir, Y., … Malach, R. 2007.

Extrinsic and Intrinsic Systems in the Posterior Cortex of the Human Brain Revealed

during Natural Sensory Stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 766–777.

Goodwin, V. L., Wofford, J. C., & Whittington, J. L. 2001. A theoretical and empirical

extension to the transformational leadership construct. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 22(7), 759–774.

Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. 1995. Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of

leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level

multi-domain perspective. The Leadership Quarterly, 6(2), 219–247.

Graen, G., Novak, M., & Sommerkamp, P. 1982. The effects of leader-member exchange and

job design on productivity and satisfaction: Testing a dual attachment model.

Organizational Behavior and Performance, 30, 109–201.

Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. 2003. Functional connectivity in the

resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253–258.

Hagen, E. E. 1962. On the theory of social change: How economic growth begins. Homewood,

IL: Dorsey Press.

Page 55: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

55

Halpin, A., & Winer, B. 1957. A factorial study of the leader behavior descriptions. In R.

Stodgill & A. Coons (Eds.), Leader behavior: Its description and measurement.

Columbus, OH: Ohio State University, Bureau of Business Research.

Haslam, N. 2006. Dehumanization: An Integrative Review. Personality and Social Psychology

Review, 10(3), 252–264.

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. 1994. Emotional contagion. New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Hemphill, J. K. (1950). Relations Between the Size of the Group and the Behavior of “Superior”

Leaders. The Journal of Social Psychology, 32(1), 11–22.

Hersey, P., & Blanchard, K. (1969). Management of organizational behavior: Utilizing human

resources. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Hersey, Paul, & Blanchard, K. H. 1982. Leadership Style: Attitudes and Behaviors. Training

and Development Journal, 36(5), 50–52.

House, R. 1977. A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J.G. Hunt & L. L. Larson (Eds.),

Leadership: The cutting edge. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

House, R. 1999. Weber and the neo-charismatic paradigm: A response to Beyer. Leadership

Quarterly, 10, 563–574.

House, R. J., & Aditya, R. N. 1997. The social scientific study of leadership: Quo vadis?

Journal of Management, 23(3), 409–473.

House, R., & Shamir, B. 1993. Towards the integration of transformational, charismatic, and

visionary theories. In M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research:

Perspective and directions. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Hunt, James G. 1999. Transformational/charismatic leadership’s transformation of the field: An

historical essay. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(2), 129–144.

Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G.

2005. Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol,

3(3), e79.

Indvik, J. 1986. Path-Goal Theory of Leadership: A Meta-Analysis. Academy of Management

Proceedings, 1986(1), 189–192.

Insel, T. R. 1997. A neurobiological basis of social attachment. The American Journal of

Psychiatry, 154(6), 726–735.

Page 56: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

56

Jack, A. I. in press. A scientific case for conceptual dualism: The problem of consciousness and

the opposing domains hypothesis. In J. Knobe & S. Nichols (Eds.), Studies in

Experimental Philosophy (Vol. 1). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Jack, A. I., Dawson, A. J., Begany, K. L., Leckie, R. L., Barry, K. P., Ciccia, A. H., & Snyder,

A. Z. 2013. fMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive

domains. NeuroImage, 66, 385–401.

Jehn, K. 1997. Affective and cognitive conflict in work groups: Increasing performance through

value-based intragroup conflict. In C. De Dreu & E. Van de Vliert (Eds.), Using conflict in

organizations. London: Sage.

Johnson, S. K. 2008. I second that emotion: Effects of emotional contagion and affect at work

on leader and follower outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly, 19(1), 1–19.

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. 2002. Personality and leadership: A

qualitative and quantitative review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 765–780.

Katz, D., Maccoby, N., & Morse, N. C. 1950. Productivity, supervision, and morale in an

office situation. Oxford, England: Institute for Social Research, Univ.

Kirkpatrick, S. A., & Locke, E. A. 1996. Direct and indirect effects of three core charismatic

leadership components on performance and attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology,

81(1), 36–51.

Krumboltz, J. D., Christal, R. E., & Ward Jr., J. H. 1959. Predicting leadership ratings from high

school activities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 50(3), 105–110.

Kuvaas, B., Buch, R., Dysvik, A., & Haerem, T. (2012). Economic and social leader–member

exchange relationships and follower performance. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(5), 756–

765.

Lewis, J. D., & Weigert, A. 1985. Trust as a social reality. Social Forces, 63(4), 967–985.

Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K.., & Sivasubramaniam, N. 1996. Effectiveness correlates of

transformation and transactional leadership: A meta-analytic review of the MLQ literature.

The Leadership Quarterly, 7(3), 385–425.

Lubinski, D. 2004. Introduction to the special section on cognitive abilities: 100 years after

Spearman’s (1904) “’General Intelligence’, objectively determined and measured”.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 96–111.

McClelland, D. 1961. The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

McGregor, D. 1960. The human side of enterprise. New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Page 57: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

57

Medow, H., & Zander, A. (1965). Aspirations for the group chosen by central and peripheral

members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 224–228.

Popper, M. (2004). Leadership as relationship. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior,

34(2), 107–125.

Schriesheim, C. A., Castro, S. L., & Cogliser, C. C. 1999. Leader-member exchange (LMX)

research: A comprehensive review of theory, measurement, and data-analytic practices.

The Leadership Quarterly, 10(1), 63–113.

Schulkin, J. 1999. The neuroendocrine regulation of behavior. Cambridge University Press.

Shartle, C. 1650. Studies of leadership by interdisciplinary methods. In A. G. Grace (Ed.),

Leadership in American education. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Shashkin, M. 1988. The visionary leader. In J. A. Conger & R. N. Kanungo (Eds.), Charismatic

Leadership: The elusive factor in organizational effectiveness. San Francisco, CA:

Jossey-Bass Inc.

Skakon, J., Nielsen, K., Borg, V., & Guzman, J. 2010. Are leaders’ well-being, behaviors and

style associated with the affective well-being of their employees? A systematic review of

three decades of research. Work & Stress, 24(2), 107–139.

Slatter, W. L. 1955. Why Not A Leadership Training Program? Journal of Dairy Science,

38(12), 1408–1409.

Stodgill, R. 1974. Handbook of Leadership: A survey of theory and research (1st ed.). New

York, NY: Free Press.

Stodgill, R. M. 1948. Personal Factors Associated with Leadership: A Survey of the Literature.

The Journal of Psychology, 25(1), 35–71.

Sy, T., Côté, S., & Saavedra, R. 2005. The Contagious Leader: Impact of the Leader’s Mood on

the Mood of Group Members, Group Affective Tone, and Group Processes. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 90(2), 295–305.

Tannenbaum, R., & Schmidt, W. 1958. How to choose a leadership pattern. Harvard Business

Review, 36(2), 95–101.

Tejeda, M. J., Scandura, T. A., & Pillai, R. 2001. The MLQ revisited: psychometric properties

and recommendations. The Leadership Quarterly, 12(1), 31–52.

Tian, L., Jiang, T., Liu, Y., Yu, C., Wang, K., Zhou, Y., … Li, K. 2007. The relationship within

and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems indicated by resting state correlational

patterns of sensory cortices. NeuroImage, 36(3), 684–690.

Page 58: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

58

Tichy, N. M., & Devanna, M. A. 1986. The transformational leader. Training & Development

Journal, 40(7), 27–32.

Tims, M., Bakker, A. B., & Xanthopoulou, D. 2011. Do transformational leaders enhance their

followers’ daily work engagement? The Leadership Quarterly, 22(1), 121–131.

Uddin, L. Q., Clare Kelly, A., Biswal, B. B., Xavier Castellanos, F., & Milham, M. P. 2009.

Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation,

and causality. Human Brain Mapping, 30(2), 625–637.

Van Knippenberg, D., & Sitkin, S. B. 2013. A critical assessment of charismatic—

transformational leadership research: Back to the drawing board? The Academy of

Management Annals, 7(1), 1–60.

Van Overwalle, F. 2011. A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning.

NeuroImage, 54(2), 1589–1599.

Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. 2009. Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and

mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 48(3), 564–584.

Vecchio, R. P. 1982. A further test of leadership effects due to between-group variation and

within-group variation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67(2), 200–208.

Vecchio, R. P., & Gobdel, B. C. 1984. The vertical dyad linkage model of leadership: Problems

and prospects. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 34(1), 5–20.

Waldman, D. A., Bass, B. M., & Yammarino, F. J. 1990. Adding to contingent-reward behavior:

The augmenting effect of charismatic leadership. Group & Organization Management,

15(4), 381–394.

Weed, S. E., Mitchell, T. R., & Moffitt, W. 1976. Leadership style, subordinate personality, and

task type as predictors of performance and satisfaction with supervision. The Journal of

Applied Psychology, 61(7), 58–66.

Wiggett, A. J., Pritchard, I. C., & Downing, P. E. 2009. Animate and inanimate objects in

human visual cortex: Evidence for task-independent category effects. Neuropsychologia,

47(14), 3111–3117.

Wofford, J. C. (1970). Factor analysis of managerial behavior variables. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 54(2), 169–173.

Yammarino, F. J., & Bass, B. M. 1990. Transformational leadership and multiple levels of

analysis. Human Relations, 43(10), 975–995.

Yukl, G. 1994. Leadership in organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Page 59: THE OPPOSING DOMAINS OF LEADERSHIP: INTEGRATING …...argues that both task and relations oriented leadership is necessary for effective leadership (see for example Blake & Mouton,

59

Yukl, G. 2006. Leadership in organizations (6th Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Yukl, Gary. 1999. An evaluative essay on current conceptions of effective leadership. European

Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 8(1), 33–48.

Yukl, Gary. 2008. How leaders influence organizational effectiveness. The Leadership

Quarterly, 19(6), 708–722.

Zalesny, M.D., & Graen, G.B. 1987. Exchange theory in leadership research. In: A. Kieser, G.

Reber, & R. Wanderer (Eds.), Handbook of Leadership (pp. 714-727). Stuttgart, Germany: C.E.

Paeschel, Verlag.