Svenja Gusewski, Raúl Rojas, Lindsey Hiebert The …...Monolingual English-speaking children with...

1
RESULTS & DISCUSSION BILINGUAL PRESCHOOLERS: THE EMERGING, PRODUCTIVE USE OF ENGLISH TENSE MARKING Background Monolingual English-speaking children with language impairment (LI) consistently show diculties with certain grammatical morphemes (e.g., past tense –ed) (Rice, 2003) and irregular past tense (Bishop, 2013). Typically developing (TD) bilingual children learning English as a second language (L2) show similar diculties with regular and irregular past tense use (Paradis, 2005). à The similarity of English grammatical error patterns exhibited by TD bilinguals relative to those of monolingual children with LI places bilinguals at risk for misidentification. Purpose and impact To systematically and longitudinally track the emerging, productive use of English tense marking in bilingual preschool- age children. Aim 1: To analyze changes in finite verb morphology over time. Aim 2: To explore the utility of adapting traditional verb morphology composites in order to index the emergence of the tense-marking system. Addresses the need for longitudinally-based, developmental data (Ho, 2013) on early language growth and the emergence of the English tense marking system in preschool age bilinguals, who are a rapidly growing population of increasing concern. Acknowledgements: We would like to thank the participating children and families and recognize the following members of the UT Dallas Bilingual Language Laboratory: G. Morales, L. Taet, K. Purinton, R. Sanchez, N. Gao, M. Lopez, and K. van der Gracht. BACKGROUND & PURPOSE Results FVMC growth -The FVMC had an intercept of 54%; a positive and significant growth rate of 11%; and ended with 76% accuracy in the third semester. Adapted FVMC and Productivity-Criterion FVMC growth -The Adapted FVMC intercept (53%) was lower, while the Productivity-Criterion FVMC intercept (58%) was higher than that of the FVMC (54%). -The positive and significant growth rates of the Adapted (10%) and the Productivity-Criterion FVMC (9%) were slower than that of the FVMC (11%). -Relative to the FVMC (76% accuracy), the Adapted FVMC ended with 73% accuracy in the third semester, while the Productivity- Criterion FVMC ended with 76% accuracy. Eect of grade -PK4 children’s FVMC intercept was 15% higher (69%) than that of PK3 children, ending with 91% accuracy in the third semester. -PK4 children’s Adapted FVMC intercept was 14% higher (67%), ending with 87% accuracy in the third semester. -PK4 children’s Productivity-Criterion FVMC intercept was 13% higher (71%), ending with 89% accuracy in the third semester. Discussion The final GCMs for the two adaptations, relative to the FVMC, indicate that bilingual preschoolers (a) show diculties with irregular tense marking. (b) that these diculties likely stem from the incorrect application of regular past tense rules (overregularizations or double marking). (c) exhibit slower growth in the acquisition of irregular past tense. Our findings are comparable to published data on bilingual preschoolers/ kindergarteners with and without LI (Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007). On average, our bilingual sample exhibited lower verb morphology accuracy than TD monolingual preschoolers, but higher accuracy than monolingual children with SLI (Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013; Goman & Bedore, 2000; Leonard, Miller, & Gerber, 1999). Svenja Gusewski, Raúl Rojas, Lindsey Hiebert The University of Texas at Dallas Callier Center Communication Disorders Participants and language samples 39 TD bilingual preschoolers tracked longitudinally over 3 semesters: fall’13; spring’14; and fall’14. 18 children in PK3 classrooms: Mean age (mos.) = 45.3 (4.6) 21 children in PK4 classrooms: Mean age (mos.) = 56.1 (5.4) Home language: 38.5% primarily Spanish; 38.5% primarily English; and 23% relatively equal Spanish and English use. Language sampling analysis (LSA) procedure Four frog stories were counterbalanced: Frog Where Are You?; Frog Goes to Dinner; Frog On His Own; One Frog Too Many (Mayer, 1969, 1974, 1975a, 1975b). This study focuses on narrative retells in English, although the children also retold narratives in Spanish each semester. The narrative retell language samples were orthographically transcribed and coded in SALT 2012 (Miller & Iglesias, 2012). Exclusionary criteria and final sample 17 language samples were excluded. -9 samples were missing. -8 samples with scarce tense marking opportunities: > 90% utterances without main verb (3 samples) < 8 complete and intelligible utterances (2 samples) < 2 min. transcript length (3 samples) Final sample N = 100 language samples Measures of finite verb morphology Traditional finite verb morphology composites (FVMC, Bedore & Leonard, 1998) focus on regular verbs, disregarding irregular verb- marking error patterns, which may signal the emergence of the English tense-marking system (Jacobson & Schwartz, 2005). à Two adaptations of the FVMC were developed to index the emergence of the tense-marking system of bilingual preschoolers learning English. Morphemes included The FVMC included regular past tense (-ed); regular third person singular present (-s); copula; and auxiliary be forms. The Adapted FVMC added auxiliary do forms; irregular past tense; and irregular present tense to the FVMC morphemes. The Productivity-Criterion FVMC included the same morphemes as the Adapted FVMC, yet it counted (a) incorrect use of the target morpheme as partially correct, productive use; and (b) omissions as incorrect, non-productive use. Accuracy calculations For all composites, the total number of correctly used morphemes was divided by the total number of obligatory contexts (correct + incorrect [partially correct] + omissions). Similarly, for all composites, incorrect use included disagreement between subject and auxiliary or copula (they was running), and inflection errors on auxiliaries or copulas (they be running). However, incorrect use for the two adaptations also included productive errors such as overregularizations (runned) and double- marking (ranned), which were disregarded in the FVMC. METHOD Analytic approach: Growth curve modeling (GCM) Conditional GCMs estimated (a) the growth trajectories of each finite verb morphology composite and (b) the eect of grade at study onset. -The intercept (onset of growth) estimated the accuracy in finite verb morphology for PK3 children during the first semester. -The linear slope estimated the growth rate in finite verb morphology accuracy. -Grade was the eect of being a child in PK4 at study onset. Dependent variables (verb morphology composites): -FVMC -Adapted FVMC -Productivity-criterion FVMC Independent variables (covariates): -Academic semester (metric of time) -Grade at onset (PK3 = 0; PK4 = 1) 0.63 0.74 0.85 0.22 0.44 0.69 0.8 0.56 0.68 0.39 0.79 0.97 0.96 0.97 0.8 0.9 0.84 0.62 0.44 0.57 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 Verb Morphology Accuracy TD Bilingual - Gusewski, Rojas & Hiebert, 2016 SLI Monolingual - Goffman & Bedore, 2000 SLI Monolingual - Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013 SLI Monolingual - Leonard, Miller & Gerber, 1999 SLI Monolingual - Guo & Eisenberg, 2014 TD Monolingual - Guo & Eisenberg, 2014 TD Monolingual - Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013 TD Monolingual - Leonard, Miller & Gerber, 1999 TD EnglishL1 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007 TD Bilingual -Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007 TD EnglishL2 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007 SLI EnglishL1 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007 SLI Bilingual - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007 0.69 0.8 0.91 0.67 0.77 0.87 0.71 0.8 0.89 0.54 0.65 0.76 0.53 0.63 0.73 0.58 0.67 0.76 0.5 0.55 0.6 0.65 0.7 0.75 0.8 0.85 0.9 0.95 Y1 Fall Y1 Spring Y2 Fall Verb Morphology Accuracy PK4 FVMC PK4 Adapted FVMC PK4 Productivity-Criterion FVMC PK3 FVMC PK3 Adapted FVMC PK3 Productivity-Criterion FVMC ~ 42 months ~ 51 months ~ 55 months ~ 63 months Figure 1. FVMC and FVMC adaptations. Figure 2. Comparison to published verb morphology accuracy of monolingual and bilingual children (LI and TD).

Transcript of Svenja Gusewski, Raúl Rojas, Lindsey Hiebert The …...Monolingual English-speaking children with...

Page 1: Svenja Gusewski, Raúl Rojas, Lindsey Hiebert The …...Monolingual English-speaking children with language impairment (LI) consistently show difficulties with certain grammatical

RESULTS & DISCUSSION

BILINGUAL PRESCHOOLERS: THE EMERGING, PRODUCTIVE USE OF ENGLISH TENSE MARKING Background Monolingual English-speaking children with language impairment (LI) consistently show difficulties with certain grammatical morphemes (e.g., past tense –ed) (Rice, 2003) and irregular past tense (Bishop, 2013). Typically developing (TD) bilingual children learning English as a second language (L2) show similar difficulties with regular and irregular past tense use (Paradis, 2005). à The similarity of English grammatical error patterns exhibited by TD bilinguals relative to those of monolingual children with LI places bilinguals at risk for misidentification.

Purpose and impact To systematically and longitudinally track the emerging, productive use of English tense marking in bilingual preschool-age children. Aim 1: To analyze changes in finite verb morphology over time. Aim 2: To explore the utility of adapting traditional verb morphology composites in order to index the emergence of the tense-marking system. Addresses the need for longitudinally-based, developmental data (Hoff, 2013) on early language growth and the emergence of the English tense marking system in preschool age bilinguals, who are a rapidly growing population of increasing concern.

Acknowledgements: We would like to thank the participating children and families and recognize the following members of the UT Dallas Bilingual Language Laboratory: G. Morales, L. Taffet, K. Purinton, R. Sanchez, N. Gao, M. Lopez, and K. van der Gracht.

BACKGROUND & PURPOSE

Results FVMC growth -The FVMC had an intercept of 54%; a positive and significant growth rate of 11%; and ended with 76% accuracy in the third semester. Adapted FVMC and Productivity-Criterion FVMC growth -The Adapted FVMC intercept (53%) was lower, while the Productivity-Criterion FVMC intercept (58%) was higher than that of the FVMC (54%). -The positive and significant growth rates of the Adapted (10%) and the Productivity-Criterion FVMC (9%) were slower than that of the FVMC (11%). -Relative to the FVMC (76% accuracy), the Adapted FVMC ended with 73% accuracy in the third semester, while the Productivity- Criterion FVMC ended with 76% accuracy. Effect of grade -PK4 children’s FVMC intercept was 15% higher (69%) than that of PK3 children, ending with 91% accuracy in the third semester. -PK4 children’s Adapted FVMC intercept was 14% higher (67%), ending with 87% accuracy in the third semester. -PK4 children’s Productivity-Criterion FVMC intercept was 13% higher (71%), ending with 89% accuracy in the third semester. Discussion The final GCMs for the two adaptations, relative to the FVMC, indicate that bilingual preschoolers (a) show difficulties with irregular tense marking. (b) that these difficulties likely stem from the incorrect application of regular past tense rules (overregularizations or double marking). (c) exhibit slower growth in the acquisition of irregular past tense. Our findings are comparable to published data on bilingual preschoolers/ kindergarteners with and without LI (Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007). On average, our bilingual sample exhibited lower verb morphology accuracy than TD monolingual preschoolers, but higher accuracy than monolingual children with SLI (Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013; Goffman & Bedore, 2000; Leonard, Miller, & Gerber, 1999).

Svenja Gusewski, Raúl Rojas, Lindsey Hiebert The University of Texas at Dallas Callier Center Communication Disorders

Participants and language samples 39 TD bilingual preschoolers tracked longitudinally over 3 semesters: fall’13; spring’14; and fall’14. 18 children in PK3 classrooms: Mean age (mos.) = 45.3 (4.6) 21 children in PK4 classrooms: Mean age (mos.) = 56.1 (5.4) Home language: 38.5% primarily Spanish; 38.5% primarily English; and 23% relatively equal Spanish and English use. Language sampling analysis (LSA) procedure Four frog stories were counterbalanced: Frog Where Are You?; Frog Goes to Dinner; Frog On His Own; One Frog Too Many (Mayer, 1969, 1974, 1975a, 1975b).

This study focuses on narrative retells in English, although the children also retold narratives in Spanish each semester. The narrative retell language samples were orthographically transcribed and coded in SALT 2012 (Miller & Iglesias, 2012). Exclusionary criteria and final sample 17 language samples were excluded. -9 samples were missing. -8 samples with scarce tense marking opportunities:

> 90% utterances without main verb (3 samples) < 8 complete and intelligible utterances (2 samples) < 2 min. transcript length (3 samples)

Final sample N = 100 language samples

Measures of finite verb morphology Traditional finite verb morphology composites (FVMC, Bedore & Leonard, 1998) focus on regular verbs, disregarding irregular verb-marking error patterns, which may signal the emergence of the English tense-marking system (Jacobson & Schwartz, 2005).   à Two adaptations of the FVMC were developed to index the

emergence of the tense-marking system of bilingual preschoolers learning English.

Morphemes included The FVMC included regular past tense (-ed); regular third person singular present (-s); copula; and auxiliary be forms. The Adapted FVMC added auxiliary do forms; irregular past tense; and irregular present tense to the FVMC morphemes. The Productivity-Criterion FVMC included the same morphemes as the Adapted FVMC, yet it counted (a) incorrect use of the target morpheme as partially correct, productive use; and (b) omissions as incorrect, non-productive use. Accuracy calculations For all composites, the total number of correctly used morphemes was divided by the total number of obligatory contexts (correct + incorrect [partially correct] + omissions). Similarly, for all composites, incorrect use included disagreement between subject and auxiliary or copula (they was running), and inflection errors on auxiliaries or copulas (they be running). However, incorrect use for the two adaptations also included productive errors such as overregularizations (runned) and double-marking (ranned), which were disregarded in the FVMC.

METHOD

Analytic approach: Growth curve modeling (GCM) Conditional GCMs estimated (a) the growth trajectories of each finite verb morphology composite and (b) the effect of grade at study onset. -The intercept (onset of growth) estimated the accuracy in finite verb morphology for PK3 children during the first semester. -The linear slope estimated the growth rate in finite verb morphology accuracy. -Grade was the effect of being a child in PK4 at study onset. Dependent variables (verb morphology composites): -FVMC -Adapted FVMC -Productivity-criterion FVMC Independent variables (covariates): -Academic semester (metric of time) -Grade at onset (PK3 = 0; PK4 = 1)

0.63

0.74

0.85

0.22

0.44

0.69

0.8

0.56

0.68

0.39

0.79

0.97

0.96

0.97

0.8

0.9

0.84

0.62

0.44

0.57

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

0 1 2 3 4 5

Verb

Mor

phol

ogy

Accu

racy

TD Bilingual - Gusewski, Rojas & Hiebert, 2016

SLI Monolingual - Goffman & Bedore, 2000

SLI Monolingual - Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013

SLI Monolingual - Leonard, Miller & Gerber, 1999

SLI Monolingual - Guo & Eisenberg, 2014

TD Monolingual - Guo & Eisenberg, 2014

TD Monolingual - Gladfelter & Leonard, 2013

TD Monolingual - Leonard, Miller & Gerber, 1999

TD EnglishL1 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007

TD Bilingual -Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007

TD EnglishL2 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007

SLI EnglishL1 - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007

SLI Bilingual - Gutiérrez-Clellen, Simon-Cereijido & Wagner, 2007

0.69

0.8

0.91

0.67

0.77

0.87

0.71

0.8

0.89

0.54

0.65

0.76

0.53

0.63

0.73

0.58

0.67

0.76

0.5

0.55

0.6

0.65

0.7

0.75

0.8

0.85

0.9

0.95

Y1 Fall Y1 Spring Y2 Fall

Verb

Mor

phol

ogy

Accu

racy

PK4 FVMC

PK4 Adapted FVMC

PK4 Productivity-Criterion FVMC

PK3 FVMC

PK3 Adapted FVMC

PK3 Productivity-Criterion FVMC

~ 42 months ~ 51 months ~ 55 months ~ 63 months

Figure 1. FVMC and FVMC adaptations.

Figure 2. Comparison to published verb morphology accuracy of monolingual and bilingual children (LI and TD).