Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

download Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

of 21

Transcript of Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    1/21

    Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology

    J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol., 11: 243262 (2001)

    DOI: 10.1002/casp.629

    Stereotype Change and Prejudice Reduction:Short- and Long-term Evaluation

    of a Crosscultural Awareness Programme

    MIRIAM E. HILL* and MARTHA AUGOUSTINOS

    Department of Psychology, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia 5005, Australia

    ABSTRACT

    The present study reports an evaluation of an applied prejudice reduction intervention. Previous

    research has indicated that such programmes achieve limited success. The programme evaluated

    was an in-house anti-racist education programme aimed at reducing prejudice towards Aboriginal

    Australians. The target audience were employees of a large public service organization. Knowledge

    of, prejudice towards, and stereotyping of Aboriginal Australians were assessed before commencing

    the programme to establish a baseline. Changes in these variables were assessed immediately after

    completing the programme, and again 3 months after completing the programme. The programme

    had pronounced effects immediately after completion: there was a signicant increase in knowledge

    and signicant decrease in prejudice and negative stereotyping. However, 3 months later, there was

    no signicant difference to baseline levels of prejudice and stereotyping. Knowledge remained sig-

    nicantly higher than at baseline 3 months after completing the programme. High prejudice partici-

    pants alone experienced a signicant, long-term decrease in old-fashioned racism. It is concluded

    that further research must be done to develop more successful strategies of prejudice reduction

    and stereotype change that are also applicable to `real world' contexts. Copyright # 2001 John

    Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

    Key words: stereotype change; applied prejudice reduction; evaluation; Aboriginal Australians

    INTRODUCTION

    Interventions aiming to change or to reduce prejudiced attitudes go by a variety of different

    names andphilosophies.Programmes aimed at changing specic intergroup attitudes tend to

    be cognitive in orientation (Duckitt, 1992). That is, they focus on the beliefs underlying pre-

    judice by attempting to increase knowledge, awareness and understanding through the pro-vision of information. This information is typically proffered in a seminar or workshop style

    group via readings, lms, and discussions on the target outgroup's history, achievements,

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    2/21

    and contributions. Some programmes also incorporate an intergroup contact element, using

    members of the target outgroup to administer the intervention.

    Determining the effectiveness of these interventions is by no means easy. Prejudice

    reduction interventions are widely used in personnel training in government organizations,educational institutions, and in multinational corporations (Day, 1983; Duckitt, 1992;

    Landis et al., 1984). However, researchers appear to have been reluctant to make these

    programmes the object of study. Given the interest of social psychology and the social

    sciences more generally in prejudice, it is surprising to nd formal evaluations of such

    programmes so difcult to locate. One of the reasons for the lack of enthusiasm among

    researchers may be that such programmes tend to employ a mixture of `eclectic methods

    and exercises aimed at producing change' (Duckitt, 1992, p. 259). This combination of

    methods and goals makes designing and implementing empirical evaluations difcult

    (Rossi and Freeman, 1993). In this paper, we report the evaluation of a workplace preju-

    dice reduction intervention. At the same time, we remain aware of the methodological dif-

    culties associated with conducting and reporting such an evaluation.

    The social psychology of prejudice reduction and stereotype change

    Social psychology has made considerable contributions to theories of prejudice reduction

    and stereotype change and prejudice reduction programmes implicitly and explicitly draw

    upon this knowledge. The most inuential and well-known social psychological method of

    prejudice reduction has been the `contact hypothesis', which proposes prejudice reduction

    via intergroup contact (Brewer, 1997; Cook, 1985; Hewstone, 1996; Hewstone and Brown,1986). Recently, however, social psychologists adopting a social cognitive perspective

    have focussed on changing group stereotypes by providing stereotype-disconrming infor-

    mation. These two perspectives, despite looking very different on the surface, offer sur-

    prisingly similar frameworks for prejudice reduction. Indeed, Hewstone (1996) has

    argued that work on stereotype change via stereotype disconrming information is simply

    a cognitive analysis of the contact hypothesis.

    In order for stereotype change to take place, social cognitive research has emphasized the

    necessity for stereotype disconrmers to be otherwise typical of the target group (Hantzi,

    1995; Hewstone etal., 1992; Hewstone etal., 1994; Johnston and Hewstone, 1992; Johnston

    et al., 1994; Johnston and Macrae, 1994; Kunda and Oleson, 1995). The contact hypothesis,however, emphasizes the importance of generalizing from the contact situation to the group

    as a whole, requiring the recognition by participants that the contact is intergroup as well as

    interpersonal (Allport, 1954; Cook, 1985; Duckitt, 1992; Hewstone, 1996; Hewstone and

    Brown, 1986). Research on both intergroup contact and stereotype change suggests that

    when trying to change individual attitudes towards outgroups, highly structured environ-

    ments are necessary to produce the conditions facilitating prejudice reduction.

    However, although such highly structured environments optimize the possibility of

    stereotype change, there is no evidence that long-term change will take place. No

    follow-up assessments have been conducted to determine whether short-term change is

    maintained in the long term.

    244 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    3/21

    at attacking a different facet of prejudice. However, the greater the variety of methods

    used, and the more widereaching the goals of such a programme, the more difcult it

    becomes to design and execute a methodologically `sound' evaluation. As each individual

    intervention can be made up of quite different components, it can also be quite difcult todetermine how useful the results of one evaluation are in assessing the success of such

    programmes in general.

    Duckitt (1992), for example, only briey reviews prejudice reduction interventions in

    the nal chapter of his book. Commenting on such interventions generally, he notes that

    the most successful prejudice reduction interventions are longer, emphasize intergroup

    similarities rather than differences, and require the active participation of attendees. Even

    so, he criticizes the lack of research on the long-term effects of such programmes.

    Brislin (1993) and Cotton (1993) offer more practically oriented reviews of prejudice

    reduction interventions. Reviewing work place programmes, Brislin argues that prejudice

    reduction interventions must be structured in a particular way in order for the intervention to

    be successful. Outgroup members should have higher status and power than ingroup mem-

    bers, tasks set should require cooperation and interaction with in and out group members,

    and the tasks should be directed towards the attainment of a superordinate goal. Thus, the

    conditions for success are very similar to those required for successful intergroup contact.

    Reviews of `in-school' programmes offer some interesting insights for workplace

    applications. In particular, Cotton (1993) argues that such programmes must be integrated

    into the school curriculum on a long-term basis, at as early an age as possible, and include

    all children. One-off programmes are not successful, and may have negative effects in the

    long term (Sanson et al., 1997). As with adults, co-operative tasks leading to the attain-ment of a common goal are an effective means of encouraging positive intergroup inter-

    action.

    Landis et al. (1984), offer a more ambivalent evaluation of cognitively oriented preju-

    dice reduction interventions. Like Duckitt, Landis et al. note that prejudice reduction inter-

    ventions draw on a variety of approaches in their aim to reduce prejudice. Consequently,

    such interventions often lack clearly dened goals, or clearly dened means of measuring

    whether these goals have been achieved. Under such circumstances, programme evalua-

    tion becomes difcult, and highly problematic (Rossi and Freeman, 1993). Below, three

    published evaluations of interventions are discussed.

    The Cultural Awareness Programme, US. Neville and Furlong (1994) report on the

    evaluation of a university-based Cultural Awareness Programme serving undergraduates,

    the major goal of the programme being to increase awareness about racism on campus and

    in society more generally. The programme consisted of six 2 hour workshops held over a

    semester. The evaluation compared students attending this workshop with those attending

    a personal development workshop and a control group who attended no workshop at all.

    Results indicated that students completing the Cultural Awareness Programme were not

    signicantly more positive in their attitudes towards people of other ethnic backgrounds

    than those students taking a personal development programme or no programme. Nor did

    they report more positive social interactions with people of other ethnic backgrounds. Theauthors speculate that this result was due to the high attrition rates in both the Cultural

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 245

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    4/21

    not complete the programme makes it impossible to draw rm conclusions about its

    effects.

    Shared Learning Programme, UK. Hewstone et al. (1994) evaluated a programmedesigned to increase interprofessional co-operation between doctors and social workers

    (Shared Learning Programme). Adopting a workshop format, the programme included a

    discussion of similarities and differences between social workers' and doctors' approach

    to clients, the roles, duties, and requirements of both groups, and provided the opportunity

    for members of both groups to work with each other in deciding the appropriate response

    to a case study.

    The programme was jointly presented by a doctor and a social worker and emphasized

    the need to develop means of working together more co-operatively to provide better client

    service. Hewstone et al. (1994) argue that such a programme is a real world application of

    the contact hypothesis in that participants act as intergroup representatives. The pro-

    gramme also emphasized the equal status nature of the interaction, and required members

    of each group to work together to achieve a common goala course of action appropriate

    to their case study.

    Hewstone et al. (1994) found a signicant improvement in intergroup attitudes follow-

    ing participation in the programme. Intergroup attitudes became more positive and ingroup

    members were more willing to recognize ingroup and outgroup superiority on different

    aspects of practice. In addition, ingroup knowledge of the outgroup increased between

    pre- and post-course. Ratings of the programme indicated that participants felt they had

    worked together successfully.Hewstone et al. (1994) argue that the similarities between research on stereotype

    change and the outcome of this programme highlight the importance of typicality in

    prejudice reduction interventions incorporating a contact element. They conclude overall

    that the Shared Learning Programme did improve intergroup attitudes and knowledge.

    Hewstone et al. (1994) argue that future programmes should include contact with multiple

    typical outgroup members rather than just one or two.

    PoliceSchools Liaison Programme, UK. Hewstone, Hopkins and colleagues report

    the evaluation of the effect of a British PoliceSchools Liaison Programme on students'

    attitudes towards and perceptions of, the police (Hewstone et al., 1992; Hopkins, 1994;Hopkins et al., 1992). In this programme, a police ofcer was placed in participating

    schools on a full time basis. The ofcer's role was to provide positive intergroup contact

    between the students and the police.

    Hewstone and colleagues found that students rated the school police ofcer signi-

    cantly more positively than they did the police in general. Findings indicated that the stu-

    dents did not perceive their school police ofcer as typical of the police as a group, and

    rated the ofcer as closer to the `caring and welfare' profession than to the police. The

    intervention was `failing' because students simply did not perceive the school police of-

    cer as a `typical' police ofcer. Hence, the positive effects of the interpersonal encounter

    did not generalize to the intergroup context.The results of this evaluation make very clear the barriers to successful prejudice reduc-

    246 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    5/21

    problem then is this: for groups in conict, how do you provide positive intergroup contact

    without it appearing so unusual as to effectively not be intergroup?

    Summary: The effects of prejudice reduction interventions

    The limited research that has been published has found the effects of prejudice reduction

    intervention programmes to be mixed. While it is usually found that participants' knowl-

    edge about the target group and related social issues increases, there is scant evidence that

    the programmes produce a change in attitudes or behaviour. Moreover, even when effects

    due to the programme are found, there has been no research as to how enduring these

    effects are. Bearing these ndings in mind, let us turn now to a consideration of the pre-

    judice reduction intervention evaluated here.

    The Cross-Cultural Awareness ProgrammeThe Cross-cultural Awareness Programme is an anti-racist education course used in staff

    training by the Courts Administration Authority (CAA) in South Australia and by several

    Australian government agencies. The programme is marketed by Chip Morgan & Associ-

    ates and was developed based on the work of Chambers and Pettman (1986). The latter,

    working in an Australian context, drew on the work of Judy Katz (White Awareness, 1978),

    but also on adult education techniques to develop their anti-racist approach.

    Chip Morgan & Associates work with the organization involved to adapt the basic

    structure of the programme to their particular needs, as well as train the organization's

    chosen facilitators to administer the course. The broad aims of the programme are to

    increase knowledge of indigenous history and culture and to reduce prejudice towards

    Aboriginal Australians. Whilst Aborigines constitute only 2% of the Australian popula-

    tion, their indigenous status makes them a highly salient social group within Australia.

    Like most indigenous peoples throughout the world, they have experienced considerable

    cultural and social dislocation. Not only are they the most socially and economically dis-

    advantaged social group within Australia (Royal Commission into Aboriginal Deaths in

    Custody, 1991), but they are also the most stereotyped and discriminated against group

    (Larsen, 1978, 1981; Walker, 1994).

    The Cross-cultural Awareness Programme was instituted within the CAA in response to

    Recommendation 96 of the Royal Commission into Aboriginal Deaths in Custody con-ducted from 1987 to 1991. Finding that many of the representatives of the judiciary system

    had little knowledge and understanding of Aboriginal culture, society, and practices, the

    Commission recommended court personnel and judicial ofcers participate in educational

    programmes concerning contemporary Aboriginal societies, cultures, and experience

    (Johnston, 1991).

    Background to the Cross-cultural Awareness Programme1

    The Cross-cultural Awareness Programme is an anti-racist education programme, which

    adopts a workshop format. At the time of this evaluation, the CAA had implemented

    the Cross-cultural Awareness Programme as a 3-day staff training programme. The

    programme had the strong support of the Executive committee of the Authority. The

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 247

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    6/21

    programme was compulsory for all employees of the CAA in regular contact with the public

    as part of their work, and voluntary for all others. Additionally, it was incorporated into the

    induction programmes of Court orderlies and security staff. The programme was offered

    every 6 weeks, to groups of up to 15 staff members from a variety of departments.

    The programme's facilitators. Each course of the programme involved three facilita-

    tors, one woman and two men. These facilitators were Aboriginal employees of the CAA,

    who had volunteered to be trained as facilitators. Thus, the Cross-cultural Awareness Pro-

    gramme utilized employees of the CAA who were also members of the target outgroup to

    act as facilitators. This is done deliberately so that the facilitators are both members of the

    target outgroup, but also members of a salient ingroupthe CAA. It was thus expected

    that the information presented in the programme would be seen as coming from credible,

    peer-group sources, but at the same time from members of the target outgroup. The facil-

    itators themselves were of varying experience in the Australian public service, and occu-

    pied medium-level positions.

    A broad overview of the programme's structure. Each of the 3 days of the Cross-cul-

    tural Awareness Programme focused on different issues. On the rst day, participants

    examined Australian history and institutions from an Aboriginal perspective. This section

    aimed to redress the lack of knowledge about the dispossession of Aboriginal Australians

    following colonization in 1788. The rst day also introduced the concept of institutional

    racism, and its place in Australia's history. The second day introduced participants to

    research on attitudes, stereotyping, and prejudice, and encouraged participants to thinkabout and explore how stereotypes are formed and their effects on their targets. Partici-

    pants were asked to examine their own beliefs and how they had developed. On the third

    and nal day, participants used example incidents as a framework for discussion on racism

    and discriminatory behaviour. They were encouraged to role-play how they would react as

    witnesses to racist incidents of varying severity, and to think of anti-racist strategies that

    could be used in their life and work. Throughout the 3 days, combinations of videos, small

    group activities, and discussions encouraged debate and interaction between participants

    and the facilitators.

    Aims of the Cross-cultural Awareness Programme. The programme has four majorobjectives. Participants should: (1) develop a broader understanding of Aboriginal Austra-

    lian cultures; (2) develop an appreciation of Aboriginal Australian cultures in the 1990s;

    (3) develop an understanding of stereotypical attitudes and how to eliminate them; (4) have

    any positive attitudes towards Aboriginal cultures and peoples reinforced. Broadly speak-

    ing it hopes to reduce prejudice and stereotypical responses to Aboriginal Australians. The

    Programme mounts a three-pronged attack on prejudice and stereotyping in particular,

    involving: (1) contact with members of the outgroup, (2) the exploration of stereotype dis-

    conrming information, and (3) activities designed to explore not only the beliefs asso-

    ciated with prejudice, but also associated feelings and emotions. Thus, the importance

    of exploring negative feelings as well as beliefs is emphasized within this course.

    248 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    7/21

    goals are met. In using a social-psychological approach to evaluating the programme as a

    prejudice reduction intervention, we decided to assess its success in meeting three stated

    objectives: changes in knowledge, attitudes, and stereotyping. We evaluated participants'

    knowledge, attitudes and stereotypes before they commenced the programme (baseline-Time 1) and immediately after they had completed the programme (Time 2). Mindful

    of the critiques of Duckitt (1992) and Landis et al. (1984), we also conducted a 3 month

    follow-up (Time 3) of participants to see whether any changes in knowledge, attitudes, and

    stereotypes were maintained.

    Changes in knowledge, prejudice, and stereotyping. The programme aims to increase

    participants' understanding of Aboriginal cultures and Indigenous issues in contemporary

    Australia. We therefore expected a signicant increase in participants' knowledge imme-

    diately following the course at Time 2, and for this increase to be maintained in the long

    term at Time 3.

    The programme also has as its main objective a signicant reduction in prejudiced atti-

    tudes. We included two measures of prejudice by which to evaluate the success of the

    intervention. This included a measure of modern racism and one measure of old-fashioned

    redneck racism. We expected the programme to produce a signicant reduction in both

    modern and old-fashioned racist attitudes at Time 2, and for these reductions to be main-

    tained at Time 3.

    Social psychological theories of reducing prejudice have focussed increasingly on

    changing stereotypes and negative attitudes. Similarly, the Cross-cultural Awareness Pro-

    gramme seeks to reduce negative stereotypical beliefs. Stereotypical beliefs about Abori-ginal Australians have however, been shown to have both positive and negative

    components. Thus we undertook to explore any changes in both negative and positive

    stereotypes (Augoustinos et al., 1994). Success of the Cross-cultural Awareness Pro-

    gramme would be indicated by a decrease in the endorsement of negative stereotypes as

    a result of participating in the course. This decrease would be maintained in the long term.

    Similarly, there would be an increase in endorsement of positive stereotypes, also main-

    tained in the long term.

    Lastly, we explored perceived typicality, a variable that has been demonstrated to

    contribute to prejudice reduction and stereotype change in laboratory-based research.

    Hewstone and his colleagues, for example, have found in a number of studies that forstereotype change to occur, stereotype-disconrming information must be dispersed

    among otherwise `typical' members of the target outgroup. Studies in the eld such as that

    of Hewstone et al. (1992) have also emphasized the importance of typicality in stereotype

    change. Thus we explored perceived typicality of the facilitators and its relationship to

    prejudice and stereotype reduction. Consistent with previous research on typicality and

    stereotyping, it was predicted that higher perceived typicality at Time 2 would be signi-

    cantly associated with lower prejudice and negative stereotyping at both Times 2 and 3.

    Higher Time 3 perceived typicality will be signicantly associated with lower prejudice

    and negative stereotyping at Time 3.

    As no one thus far has considered the relationship of perceived typicality and positiveaspects of a social stereotype, we also took the opportunity to explore this relationship

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 249

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    8/21

    METHOD

    Overview

    Data were collected from six separate courses of the programme in a time period spanningAprilDecember 1994. A total of 62 participants across the six courses participated in the

    evaluation. This included 39 women, and 23 men. The mean age of the total sample was

    41.90 years (SD12.10). Ages ranged from 22 to 70 years. There was no signicant dif-

    ference in age between women and men.

    Thirty-one of the 62 participants returned the longitudinal 3 month follow-up question-

    naire for a return rate of 50%. Mean age of this sample was 41.26 years old (SD12.19).

    The returner sample consisted of 18 women and 12 men. There was, again, no signicant

    difference in the age of women and men.

    Design. As this was a workplace programme, it was not possible to conduct a rando-mized experiment. The study design was a quasi-experimental prepost design, with a sec-

    ond post test conducted 3 months after completing the programme. While there are

    obvious weaknesses to this prepost design, it remains one of the most popular designs

    for conducting evaluation research (Mohr, 1988).

    The evaluation questionnaire

    The core of the evaluation questionnaire comprised the instruments assessing knowledge,

    modern racism, old-fashioned racism and endorsement of stereotypical descriptors of

    Aboriginal Australians. Although the order in which these questionnaires were presentedto participants was not varied, the items within each questionnaire were presented in a ran-

    dom order for each presentation (pre, post, and 3 months after completing the course) to each

    of the six groups of participants. These core questionnaires were preceded by questions that

    assessed participants' overall perception of the course as an education and training pro-

    gramme, the results of which are not reported here. The questions measuring perceived typi-

    cality of the facilitators were embedded within these programme quality questions.

    Knowledge: the general knowledge questionnaire. We measured the change in parti-

    cipants' level of knowledge of Aboriginal cultures and history by constructing a 14-item

    questionnaire that drew directly on information presented during the programme. Partici-pants received two marks for every correct answer and no marks for every incorrect

    answer. Scores could therefore range from 0 to 24.

    Prejudice. Two measures of prejudice were included in the questionnaire. One mea-

    sure was the Australian version of the Modern Racism Scale (MRSAugoustinos et al.,

    1994), adapted from McConahay et al.'s (1981) original scale. The second was a measure

    of old-fashioned, redneck racism: Walker's (1994) Attitudes towards Aborigines Scale

    (ABS). Whereas the MRS contains items that are not conventionally racist, the ABS con-

    tains items that tap traditionally racist attitudes that focus on social distance, dislike, and

    beliefs about `racial' superiority.Two measures of prejudice were included because we thought it possible that the

    250 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    9/21

    Augoustinos et al. (1994) obtained internal consistency alpha reliability coefcients of

    0.85 (study 1) and 0.82 (study 2) for the Australian version of the MRS. In the present

    study this scale obtained an alpha reliability coefcient of 0.83 at Time 1. Pedersen and

    Walker (1997) have reported good internal consistency reliability alphas for the ABS(Walker, 1994, 0.88; Pedersen and Walker, 1997, 0.89). In the present study, the

    ABS obtained an alpha reliability coefcient of 0.76 at Time 1.

    The scoring procedure for the MRS was as follows: Participants were asked to select the

    number on the scale best reecting their opinion in response to each of the seven items.

    The ve-point rating scales on which participants responded ranged from 1 (agree

    strongly) to 5 (disagree strongly). This scale was scored 2 to 2, with lower scores indi-

    cating lower prejudice. The MRS thus had a potential range of 14 (low prejudice) to

    14 (high prejudice).

    The 11-item ABS was scored similarly to the MRS. Participants were asked to select

    the number on the ve-point rating scale that best reected their opinion. The scales on

    which participants responded ranged from 1 (agree strongly) to 5 (disagree strongly). This

    scale was scored 2 to 2, with lower scores indicating lower prejudice. The ABS thus

    had a potential range of22 (low prejudice) to 22 (high prejudice).

    Stereotyping. We undertook to explore any changes in stereotyping using an abbre-

    viated list of the negative, and positive adjectives developed by Augoustinos et al.

    (1994). These included 12 positive adjectives (artistic, caring, easy-going, independent,

    loyal, musical, proud, self-assured, spiritual, talented, respect the land, wise) and 12 nega-

    tive adjectives (aimless, bludgers,2 demanding, dirty, drunken, hostile, inferior, lazy, trou-

    ble-makers, unemployable, unreliable, wasteful). The reliability coefcient alphas at Time

    1 in the present study were 0.81 and 0.87 for the positive and negative scale respectively.

    Participants were asked to indicate on ve-point rating scales ranging from 1 (agree

    strongly) to 5 (disagree strongly), the extent to which they believed each stereotypic adjec-

    tive accurately described Aboriginal Australians. The individual items were summed and

    divided by the number of items in each scale to form indices of positive and negative

    stereotype endorsement. Scores on each of the stereotyping scales could thus range from

    0 to 4 with higher numbers indicating higher stereotype endorsement.

    Perceived typicality of facilitators. The perceived typicality of the facilitators was

    assessed at Times 2 and 3. Participants were asked to indicate on a ve-point rating scale

    ranging from 1 (not at all typical) to 5 (very typical), how typical they thought their facil-

    itators were of most Aboriginal Australians. This scale was recoded and scored from 0 to

    4, with higher numbers indicating higher perceived typicality.

    Procedure

    Pre-course and post-course. Participants were welcomed by the facilitators as they

    entered the room. Participants were approached individually by the rst author and asked

    if they would be willing to participate in an independent evaluation of the programme.

    They were then given an information sheet, consent form, and the rst questionnaire pack-

    age. It was emphasized both verbally and in writing that their participation was voluntary,

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 251

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    10/21

    were free to withdraw from the study at any time. Participants were encouraged to ask any

    questions that arose either before, during, or after, completing the questionnaires. They

    then lled out the rst questionnaire before the commencement of the course. All com-

    pleted questionnaires were collected by the rst author. At the end of the nal day ofthe programme, the second questionnaire was administered, with a verbal reminder that

    their participation remained voluntary and that they were free to withdraw from the eva-

    luation. Across the six courses from which data were collected only one participant

    declined to participate in the evaluation.

    Longitudinal follow-up. The longitudinal follow-up questionnaire was posted to par-

    ticipants at their work place, 3 months after they had completed the programme. Another

    informed consent form was enclosed, again stressing that their participation was voluntary

    and that they could withdraw at any time. Once again, the condentiality of all information

    supplied was emphasized. Participants were asked if they could return the completed ques-

    tionnaire within 1 month. A stamped, addressed envelope was included to facilitate this

    return. After 1 month, reminders were sent to those participants who had not returned

    the questionnaire, stressing the value of their answers, and asking them to return the com-

    pleted questionnaires by a set date.

    RESULTS

    The immediate effects of the programme, that is change between Time 1 (pre-course) andTime 2 (immediately post-course), are presented rst. These ndings are based on the

    responses of all 62 participants. These are then followed by the results for those 31 parti-

    cipants who participated in the longitudinal follow-up (Returners).

    Time 1 and Time 2

    Before commencing the primary analyses, participants' responses were examined for dif-

    ferences by gender, age group, and any of the prejudice or stereotyping variables. A med-

    ian split was used to divide the 62 participants into two age groups: older (participants 41

    years of age or older) and younger (participants less than 41 years of age). There were no

    signicant differences between these two age groups. There were no signicant gender

    differences. Missing data were excluded from the analysis on a pairwise basis. Mean

    scores, standard deviations and ranges of the variables under consideration are displayed

    in Table 1.

    Knowledge. As predicted, there was a signicant increase in scores on the General

    Knowledge test between Time 1 and Time 2 [t(60)15.51, p1tailed< 0.001].

    Prejudice

    (1) Modern Racism Scale (MRS). The mean modern racism score at Time 1 was 4.18(5.68), which tends towards the low prejudice end of the scale. There was a signicant

    252 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    11/21

    prejudiced sample, even before commencing the programme. There was a signicantdecrease in old-fashioned racism as measured by the ABS between Time 1 and Time 2

    [t(58)2.91, p1-tailed< 0.005].

    Difference in prejudice. Given that we had two measures of prejudice towards Abori-

    ginal Australians, it is of interest to compare the levels of modern racism in the sample

    with those of old-fashioned racism. Participants' displayed signicantly higher levels of

    modern racism than old-fashioned racism (ABS) at both Time 1[t(59)4.85,

    p1 tailed< 0.001], and Time 2 [t(60)2.66, p1tailed< 0.01].

    Stereotyping. As can be seen from the descriptive statistics in Table 1, participants

    commenced the programme with low mean levels of negative stereotype word endorse-

    ment, and moderate levels of positive stereotype endorsement. There was a signicant

    decrease in negative stereotyping between Time 1 and Time 2 [t(59)6.08,

    p1 tailed< 0.001]. There was a signicant increase in positive stereotyping between Time

    1 and Time 2 [t(61)4.36, p1tailed< 0.001].

    Typicality. At Time 2, participants on average, perceived facilitators as moderately

    typical of most Aboriginal Australians (M2.67, SD0.89). Pearson's correlations were

    performed to explore the predicted relationships between prejudice, stereotyping, and per-ceived typicality. As predicted, greater perceived typicality of the facilitators at Time 2

    Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and ranges of the variables in the study (N62)

    Variable N Mean Median Standard deviation Possible range Actual range

    Knowl1 61 12.20 12.00 5.83 224

    028Knowl2 62 21.52 22.00 4.30 1028Typical2 61 2.67 3.00 0.89 04 14MRS1 62 4.18 3.50 5.68 14 to 8

    14 to 14MRS2 62 7.21 7.00 4.94 14 to 4ABS1 60 11.27 12.00 6.06 22 to 1

    22 to 22ABS2 61 12.84 13.00 6.56 22 to 3PST1 62 2.63 2.50 0.56 1.253.67

    04

    PST2 62 2.89 2.83 0.52 1.673.83NST1 60 1.72 1.71 0.65 0.083.0804

    NST2 62 1.17 1.33 0.76 0.002.75

    Key to variable abbreviations: General Knowledge Test (Knowl); how typical the facilitators are perceived asbeing of MOST Aboriginal Australians (Time 2Typical2). Modern Racism Scale (MRS); Attitudes towardsAborigines Scale (ABS); Positive Stereotyping (PST); Negative Stereotyping (NST). The numeral `1', `2', or `3'

    attached to the variable name indicates whether variable is from Time 1, Time 2, or Time 3 assessment.

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 253

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    12/21

    Summary. The ndings above indicate that the Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    has considerable effect in the short term. Between commencing and completing the pro-

    gramme there was a signicant increase in knowledge of Aboriginal Australians and Aus-

    tralian history, there were signicant decreases in both modern and old-fashioned racismand in negative stereotyping, and there were signicant increases in positive stereotyping.

    In the short term then, the Cross-cultural Awareness Programme achieves its aims of

    decreasing negative attitudes and increasing and/or reinforcing existing positive attitudes.

    Three month follow-up: Time 2 to Time 3

    Of the 62 participants who lled out questionnaires at Time 1 and Time 2, only 31 returned

    the longitudinal follow-up questionnaire at Time 3 for a return rate of 50%. This attrition is

    not unusual in a longitudinal study, and is a particular weakness of the pre-post test design

    used here (Mohr, 1988). Variables were examined to establish if there were any signicantdifferences between those 31 participants who returned the 3 month follow-up question-

    naire (Returners) and those 31 who did not (non-Returners). Returners were signicantly

    higher in Time 2 knowledge (M22.58) than were non-Returners (M20.45)

    [t(60)2.00, p1tailed0.05]. Returners also showed signicantly greater change in

    ABS (old-fashioned racism) (M3.27) between Time 1 and Time 2 than did non-Retur-

    ners (M0.48) [t(57)2.20, p1tailed< 0.05]. There were no other signicant differ-

    ences between Returners and non-Returners of the 3 month follow-up questionnaire.

    Analysis. Mean scores, standard deviations and ranges of all measures are presented in

    Table 2. There were no signicant differences among these variables due to gender or agegroup. Data were analysed using t-tests to determine whether there were signicant differ-

    ences between Times 1, 2, and 3. Unless otherwise indicated all t-tests are one-tailed. To

    Table 2. Returners (N31) descriptives

    Variable N Mean Median Standard deviation Possible range Actual range

    Knowl1 31 12.77 12.00 5.16 222Knowl2 31 22.58 22.00 3.91 228 1428Knowl3 31 20.13 20.00 4.79 828

    Typical2 31 2.84 3.00 0.73 04 24Typical3 30 1.97 2.00 1.03 04MRS1 31 3.29 3.00 4.59 14 to 5MRS2 31 7.10 7.00 4.09 14 to 14 14 to 1MRS3 31 3.84 3.00 4.56 14 to 4ABS1 30 9.93 10.00 5.45 22 to 0ABS2 31 13.19 14.00 5.52 22 to 22 21 to 1ABS3 30 11.90 14.00 6.14 22 to 1PST1 31 2.68 2.50 0.56 1.583.67PST2 31 2.96 2.92 0.51 04 1.923.83PST3 29 2.84 2.75 0.51 2.003.75NST1 30 1.73 1.83 0.60 0.333.00

    NST2 31 1.19 1.33 0.73 04 0.082.25NST3 30 1.57 1.58 0.70 0.173.17

    254 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    13/21

    avoid the chances of obtaining a signicant result due to chance alone, the signicance

    level used for each group of t-tests was corrected using the Bonferroni method (Darling-

    ton, 1991). As three comparisons were conducted within each variable (Time 1 vs. Time 2;

    Time 2 vs. Time 3; Time 1 vs. Time 3), using an alpha level of p1tailed< 0.05, the cor-rected signicance level was p1tailed< 0.017. Unless otherwise indicated, all signicant t-

    tests were signicant at the corrected signicance level of p1tailed< 0.017.

    It was expected that any effects of the course between Time 1 and Time 2 would decline

    somewhat between Time 2 and Time 3, as the immediate impact of the programme waned.

    However, for the programme to be successful in the long term, the predicted effects would

    be expected to remain signicantly different between Times 1 and 3. As such, all of our

    predictions are one-tailed.

    Knowledge. There was a signicant increase in scores on the General Knowledge test

    between Time 1 and Time 2 [t(30)13.28]. Between Time 2 and Time 3 there was a

    signicant decrease in scores on the General Knowledge test [t(30)4.55]. However,

    despite this decrease, scores on the General Knowledge test at Time 3 remained signi-

    cantly higher than those at Time 1 [t(30)8.04].

    Prejudice. Table 3 summarizes the results for this section.

    (1) Modern Racism Scale. There was a signicant decrease in modern racism between

    Time 1 and 2. Modern racism signicantly increased between Time 2 and Time 3, and

    there was no signicant decrease in modern racism between Time 1 and Time 3.

    Overall, there was no signicant long-term decrease in modern racism.(2) Attitudes towards Aborigines Scale. There was a signicant decrease in old-fashioned

    racism as measured by the ABS between Time 1 and Time 2. There was no signicant

    change in ABS between Times 2 and 3. There was no signicant decrease in ABS

    between Times 1 and 3. Overall, therefore, there was no long-term decrease in old-

    fashioned prejudice as measured by the ABS.

    Difference in prejudice. Consistent with the ndings for the whole sample, Returners

    scored higher in prejudice on the MRS than on the ABS at Time 1 [t(29)4.35], Time 2

    [t(30)2.46], and Time 3 [t(29)4.87]. All t-tests were signicant at p1tailed< 0.05.

    Stereotyping. As can be seen in Table 3, there was a signicant decrease in negative

    stereotype endorsement between Times 1 and 2. There was a signicant increase in nega-

    tive stereotype endorsement between Times 2 and 3. However, there was no signicant

    decrease in negative stereotype endorsement between Times 1 and 3. Overall, there was

    no signicant long-term decrease in negative stereotype endorsement.

    Table 3. Summaries of t-tests for prejudice and stereotype measures

    Scale Time 1Time 2 Time 2Time 3 Time 1Time 3

    MRS t(30)5.90* t(30)6.41* t(30)0.85ABS t(29)3.87* t(29)1.98 t(28)1.55

    Evaluation of a prejudice reduction programme 255

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    14/21

    There was a signicant increase in positive stereotype endorsement between Times 1

    and 2. However, there was no signicant increase in positive stereotype endorsement

    between Times 2 and 3, or between Times 1 and 3. Overall, there was no signicant

    long-term increase in positive stereotype endorsement.

    Typicality. There was a signicant decrease in the perceived typicality of the facilita-

    tors between Time 2 (M2.87) and Time 3 (M1.97) [t(29)3.35, p1tailed< 0.05].

    Descriptives for this variable are presented in Table 4. Pearson's correlations were per-

    formed to assess the relationships between prejudice, stereotyping and typicality. Consis-

    tent with predictions, perceived typicality at Time 2 was signicantly associated with

    lower Time 3 old-fashioned racism (0.37) and negative stereotyping (0.41). However,

    contrary to prediction, it was not signicantly associated with modern racism or positive

    stereotyping. At Time 3, contrary to prediction, perceived typicality was not signicantly

    associated with lower prejudice or negative stereotyping. It was however, weakly asso-

    ciated with higher Time 3 positive stereotyping (0.32). Correlations are signicant at

    p1tailed< 0.05.

    Time 1 to Time 3: low and high prejudice groups

    While the results for the Returners group as a whole were not very revealing, we suspected

    that changes might be more visible if high and low prejudice Returners were considered

    separately. The following section briey considers the results of this analysis.

    Returners were divided into high and low prejudice groups on the basis of a mediansplit of their Time 1 modern racism scores. The MRS was used as the basis for the division

    because, as reported above, ndings point to it as less reactive than old-fashioned prejudice

    scales. The median Time 1 modern racism score for Returners was 3.00. Participants

    were placed in the low prejudice group (N17) if they had a modern racism score of less

    than or equal to 3.00, and into the high prejudice group (N14) if they had a score of

    greater than 3.00. As in the previous section, all t-tests are one-tailed unless otherwise

    indicated.

    High prejudice group (N14). Results for the t-tests are summarized in Table 4. The

    Bonferroni correction was employed to guard against ination of alpha due to multiplecomparisons. The corrected alpha level was again p1tailed< 0.017.

    As can be seen from Table 4, there were few signicant long-term changes for high

    prejudice Returners. Three months after completing the Cross-cultural Awareness

    Programme, high prejudice Returners showed a signicant increase in knowledge from

    Time 1 levels, and a signicant decrease in ABS (old-fashioned racism) from Time 1

    levels.

    Table 4. Summary of results of t-tests for high prejudice Returners (N14)

    Scale Time 1Time 2 Time 2Time 3 Time 1Time 3

    Knowl t(13)8.65* t(13)3.80* t(13)4.98*MRS t(13)5.75* t(13)4.48* t(13)2.28

    256 M. E. Hill and M. Augoustinos

  • 8/2/2019 Stereotype Change & Prejudice Reduction Cross-cultural Awareness Programme

    15/21

    Low prejudice group (N17). Results for the t-tests are summarized in Table 5. The

    Bonferroni was employed to guard against ination of alpha due to multiple comparisons.

    The corrected alpha level was again p1tailed