Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

22
SOUGHT EXPERIENCES AT (DARK) HERITAGE SITES Avital Biran University of Surrey, UK Yaniv Poria Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel Gila Oren The College of Management Academic Studies, Israel Abstract: Current literature on dark tourism largely follows a supply perspective, almost ignoring the tourist experience. Focusing on Auschwitz-Birkenau death camp (here after Auschwitz), the epitome of dark tourism, the present study sheds light on the nature of this tourism experience by clarifying the relations between the symbolic meanings assigned to the site and core elements of the tourist experience (motivation and sought interpretation ben- efits). The findings suggest that Auschwitz hosts a heritage experience rather than a merely dark tourism one, and that alongside site attributes, tourists’ perceptions of the site should be considered in the conceptualization of the tourist experience. The findings challenge the current understanding of dark tourism as a distinct phenomenon to heritage tourism. Keywords: dark tourism, experience, motivation, interpretation. Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. INTRODUCTION Studies have highlighted the growing fascination of tourists with sites of death, disaster, and atrocities (e.g. Cohen, 2010; Logan & Reeves, 2009; Stone & Sharpley, 2008). Research on such sites largely focuses on the display (Stone & Sharpley, 2008; Wight, 2005), exploring which history is presented and which is hidden or even obliterated. Surpris- ingly, Seaton and Lennon’s (2004) claim that consumer-oriented research of visits to sites of death, disaster and atrocities ‘‘has hardly even begun’’ (p. 81) is still valid. Equally valid is Wight’s (2005) argu- ment that while a conceptual approach is dominant, empirical research of visits to sites presenting death is lacking. These may explain why the understanding of this social phenomenon ‘‘remains limited’’ (Stone & Sharpley, 2008, p. 574) as indeed the tourists’ experiences Avital Biran is based at the University of Surrey (Guildford GU2 7XH, United Kingdom. Email <[email protected]>). Her main research areas are management and interpretation of heritage in tourism.Yaniv Poria is based at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel. His main research interest is heritage tourism.Gila Oren is a graduate of the Holocaust Educator Program (Yad Vashem and Ministry of Education, Israel) and her main research interest is the management of Holocaust sites. Annals of Tourism Research, Vol. 38, No. 3, pp. 820–841, 2011 0160-7383/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Printed in Great Britain doi:10.1016/j.annals.2010.12.001 www.elsevier.com/locate/atoures 820

description

Research paper on dark tourism. Study aims to clarigy the relationship between perception of the site as one's own heritage, motivation for the visit, and sought interpretation benefits.

Transcript of Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

Page 1: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

Annals of Tourism Research, Vol. 38, No. 3, pp. 820–841, 20110160-7383/$ - see front matter � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Printed in Great Britain

doi:10.1016/j.annals.2010.12.001www.elsevier.com/locate/atoures

SOUGHT EXPERIENCES AT (DARK)HERITAGE SITES

Avital BiranUniversity of Surrey, UK

Yaniv PoriaBen-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel

Gila OrenThe College of Management Academic Studies, Israel

Abstract: Current literature on dark tourism largely follows a supply perspective, almostignoring the tourist experience. Focusing on Auschwitz-Birkenau death camp (here afterAuschwitz), the epitome of dark tourism, the present study sheds light on the nature of thistourism experience by clarifying the relations between the symbolic meanings assigned to thesite and core elements of the tourist experience (motivation and sought interpretation ben-efits). The findings suggest that Auschwitz hosts a heritage experience rather than a merelydark tourism one, and that alongside site attributes, tourists’ perceptions of the site should beconsidered in the conceptualization of the tourist experience. The findings challenge thecurrent understanding of dark tourism as a distinct phenomenon to heritage tourism.Keywords: dark tourism, experience, motivation, interpretation. � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. Allrights reserved.

INTRODUCTION

Studies have highlighted the growing fascination of tourists with sitesof death, disaster, and atrocities (e.g. Cohen, 2010; Logan & Reeves,2009; Stone & Sharpley, 2008). Research on such sites largely focuseson the display (Stone & Sharpley, 2008; Wight, 2005), exploring whichhistory is presented and which is hidden or even obliterated. Surpris-ingly, Seaton and Lennon’s (2004) claim that consumer-orientedresearch of visits to sites of death, disaster and atrocities ‘‘has hardlyeven begun’’ (p. 81) is still valid. Equally valid is Wight’s (2005) argu-ment that while a conceptual approach is dominant, empiricalresearch of visits to sites presenting death is lacking. These may explainwhy the understanding of this social phenomenon ‘‘remains limited’’(Stone & Sharpley, 2008, p. 574) as indeed the tourists’ experiences

Avital Biran is based at the University of Surrey (Guildford GU2 7XH, United Kingdom.Email <[email protected]>). Her main research areas are management and interpretationof heritage in tourism.Yaniv Poria is based at Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel. Hismain research interest is heritage tourism.Gila Oren is a graduate of the Holocaust EducatorProgram (Yad Vashem and Ministry of Education, Israel) and her main research interest isthe management of Holocaust sites.

820

Page 2: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 821

in such sites are yet to be explored. Focusing on Auschwitz, perhaps theepitome of dark tourism (Stone, 2006), this study seeks to enhance thecurrent understanding of dark tourism.

The current study highlights aspects of the tourist experience whichare important to the conceptualization of dark tourism, namely moti-vation for the visit and sought benefits of on-site interpretation. Addi-tionally, the meaning attached to sites of death and atrocity by thetourist, though often ignored in dark tourism literature, is at the coreof this study. Following Stone and Sharpley’s (2008) view of the cen-trality of the construction of death in contemporary society, this studyespouses the experiential approach (Apostolakis, 2003). This approachcaptures the experience as an interactive process involving the touristand the resources, and highlights the symbolic meaning of the site.This symbolic meaning, as presented here, is valuable to the under-standing of dark tourism as a social phenomenon, shedding light onthe heterogeneity of sought experiences at sites presenting deathand atrocity.

DARK TOURISM AND VISITS TO DARK SITES

Black spots tourism (Rojek, 1993), thanatourism (Seaton, 1996) andmorbid tourism (Blom, 2000) are only some of the names suggestedfor describing visits to death-related sites. Dark tourism (Lennon &Foley, 2000), is the term most commonly used. Yet, common usagedoes not imply that there is an accepted definition of dark tourism,and the term ‘‘continues to remain poorly conceptualized’’ (Jamal &Lelo, 2009, p. 6) and ‘‘theoretically fragile’’ (Stone & Sharpley, 2008,p. 575). A review of the literature reveals three different (and at timescontradicting) approaches that have been adopted in dark tourismstudies. These are, the supply and demand perspectives which adopt adescriptive understanding, and an integrated supply-demand perspective,which adopts an experiential understanding of dark tourism.

The first approach and the prevailing one is a supply perspective(Seaton & Lennon, 2004; Stone & Sharpley, 2008). Lennon and Foley(2000), for example, perceive dark tourism as visitation to sites associ-ated with death, disaster, and depravity. Similarly, Stone (2006) de-fines it as ‘‘the act of travel to sites associated with death, sufferingand the seemingly macabre’’ (p.146). The supply perspective adoptsa descriptive understanding (Apostolakis, 2003), emphasizing the indi-vidual’s presence in spaces associated with death. This line of thoughthas led to an eclectic collection of studies exploring a diversity ofdeath-related sites, ranging from lightest to darkest. The lightest sitesare ‘‘dark fun factories’’ (Stone, 2006, p. 152)—entertainment andcommercial sites associated with death, such as Dracula tourism inRomania (Light, 2007) or Jack the Ripper walks in London (Stone,2006). The darkest sites include sites of death characterized by higherpolitical and ideological influence, offering an educational experience(Stone, 2006). Among the darkest sites are genocide camps in Cambo-dia and the death camps of the Holocaust period (e.g. Auschwitz).

Page 3: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

822 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

The supply perspective ignores the diversity of the individual’sinner experience and motives, leading, in turn, to a simplified under-standing of dark tourism, one which arbitrarily combines possiblyunrelated experiences. It should be noted that the adaptation of adescriptive approach is a common characteristic of research intonew tourism niches (e.g. Uriely, 2005 on general tourism, Collins-Kreiner, 2010, on pilgrimage tourism, and Apostolakis, 2003, on her-itage tourism).

The second approach employs a demand-oriented perspective, wheredark tourism is defined in terms of the motivation for the visit. Seaton(1996), for example, proposes that dark tourism is ‘‘travel to a locationwholly, or partially, motivated by the desire for actual or symbolicencounters with death’’ (p. 240). Yet, in line with the descriptiveunderstanding of dark tourism, the demand oriented approach followsthe hidden assumption that the presence of tourists at death-relatedsites reflects at least some degree of thanatouristic motives (Slade,2003). As such, all tourists to battle fields or Auschwitz, for example,are seen as dark tourists. This approach overlooks the possibility thatthe reasons for visiting and the sought experiences might be com-pletely devoid of interest in death. However, several studies indicatethat this may be the case. Slade (2003), for example, suggests that Aus-tralians and New Zealanders visiting Gallipoli are engaged in a pro-found heritage experience and are not interested in death itself.Similarly, Teye and Timothy (2004) and Timothy and Teye (2004),did not reveal fascination with death and morbid experiences as partof their study of African and White Americans visiting slavery sites inGhana. Additionally, Hughes (2008) exploring Tuol Sleng Museumof genocide crimes in Cambodia, notes that tourists may visit simply be-cause this is a ‘‘must see’’ site.

The third approach, integrated supply-demand perspective, highlightsthe need to consider the nature of both supply and demand, narrowingthe scope of dark tourism. Sharpley (2005, 2009), for example, calls forclarifying the links between the site’s attributes and the experiencesought, based on a ‘‘continuum of purpose’’ (Sharpley, 2009, p. 19)and recognizes four ‘‘shades’’ of dark tourism (1) Black tourism, repre-senting a ‘‘pure’’ dark tourism experience in which fascination withdeath is satisfied by purposeful supply (intentionally created to satisfythis fascination and profit from it); (2) Pale tourism, describing touristswith minimal interest in death who visit accidental dark sites (sites notoriginally formed as for-profit tourist attractions); (3) Grey tourism de-mand, relating to tourists motivated by fascination with death visitingunintended dark tourism sites, and (4) Grey tourism supply where sitesinitially established to exploit death, attract tourists with little interestin death. Ryan and Kohli (2006) support Sharpley’s framework in theirstudy of the buried village of Te Wairoa in New Zealand. They suggestthat rather than defining this site as merely dark tourism (based on itsattributes) this site represents a Grey tourism supply, so that while it is asite of death and atrocity—and is being promoted as such—tourists’experience is one of peaceful natural scenery and cultural heritage.

Page 4: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 823

Of particular importance to this study is Smith and Croy’s (2005)conceptualization of dark tourism. In line with the experiential ap-proach adopted in the current study, Smith and Croy argue that it isthe perception of the site as dark (rather than site’s attributes per se)which determines whether tourists are motivated to visit by dark mo-tives. As such, the third approach and particularly Smith and Croy’s(2005) conceptualization, draws attention to the possibility that notall tourists to sites presenting death are indeed engaged in a dark expe-rience. Furthermore, studies of dark sites reveal that some tourists arenot familiar with the site’s attributes (Poria, Butler, & Airey, 2004). Asawareness is a precondition to perception (McClellan, 1998), based onthe third approach those tourists are not dark tourists while scholarsadopting the first and second approaches will classify these individualsas engaged in a dark experience. Thus, the third approach suggeststhat tourists to dark sites may engage in other non-dark experiences.For example, several studies highlight educational experience (Austin,2002; Teye & Timothy, 2004) and enjoying the scenery (Poria et al.,2004) as key motives of visits to sites of dark attributes. The experientialapproach is consistent with the post-modernist move in tourism re-search, which emphasizes the subjective over the objective and the indi-vidual’s experiences of tourism (Collins-Kreiner, 2010; Uriely, 2005).The move from a descriptive to an experiential conceptualization isclaimed to represent the development of the tourism body of knowl-edge (Belhassen, Caton, & Stewart, 2008).

Dark Tourism and Heritage Tourism Literature

The descriptive conceptualization of dark tourism encompasses tour-ist attractions that are most often considered and classified as heritagesites. This is particularly true for what Stone (2006) termed darker con-flict sites (e.g. battlefields of the two World Wars) and darkest camps ofgenocide (e.g. Auschwitz, camps in Rwanda and Kosovo), which arethe focus of the current study. Hence, it is suggested here that an alter-native approach to the understanding of the tourist experience at sitesof death should draw on heritage tourism studies. Seaton and Lennon(2004) support this idea when they note that dark tourism sites—likeheritage sites—involve ideological and political issues. The rationalfor drawing on heritage tourism studies also relies on the fact that sitespresenting death and atrocity have been previously studied as dissonantheritage (Tunbridge & Ashworth, 1996), heritage that hurts (Uzzell &Ballantyne, 1998) or difficult heritage (Logan & Reeves, 2009). Litera-ture centering on visits to heritage sites may allow a more meaningfulunderstanding of tourist experiences at dark sites. Specifically, such lit-erature recognizes the multifunctional nature of sites presentingdeath, rising from the various symbolic meanings of the death on dis-play. For example, sites presenting death are captured as a place forremembrance, mourning, a space for a spiritual experience, demon-stration of national identity, educational experiences, or simply a ran-dom stop (Austin, 2002; Logan & Reeves, 2009; Slade, 2003). These

Page 5: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

824 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

functions are largely overlooked by dark tourism literature, as the com-monly adopted descriptive approach assumes tourists to such sites aremerely interested in death.

The experiential approach to heritage tourism, which emphasizesthe tourist’s perception of the heritage presented as a key element inunderstanding the tourist experience (Apostolakis, 2003; Timothy &Boyd, 2003), is relevant to the present study. Specifically, Poria, Butler,and Airey (2003) point to a link between one’s perception of the site aspersonal heritage and a variety of behaviors, such as motivation to visit,satisfaction with the visit, and preferences towards on-site interpreta-tion (also Poria, Reichel, & Biran, 2006). The importance of the per-sonal meaning assigned to the site is also evident in studies focusingon death-related sites. For example, in studies of Fort Siloso in Singa-pore (Muzaini, Teo, & Yeoh, 2007) and of Ground Zero in New York(Lisle, 2004), the researchers highlight the need to distinguish be-tween ‘‘ordinary’’ (Muzaini et al., 2007, p. 29) tourists and those witha personal connection to the site (e.g. families of victims). Beech(2000), focusing on Buchenwald concentration camp, identifies twotypes of tourists seeking different experiences. The first type of touristsare those who have no personal attachment to the site and perceive thevisit as leisure. The second type are tourists for whom the site carriespersonal meaning (e.g. relatives of inmates and those identifying withthe heritage presented). This personal connection is the primary mo-tive for their travel and they do not regard the visit as a leisure activity.Timothy and Teye (2004) further suggest that learning is a key themein the experience of White American tourists in Elmina Castle(Ghana), whereas for African-Americans this is a profound emotionalexperience of ‘‘coming home’’ (Timothy & Teye, 2004, p. 119). Theabove studies emphasize the need to consider the individual’s percep-tion of the site in the conceptualization of dark tourism.

Experience, Motivation, and Interpretation

This study will focus on the motivation for the visit and the soughtbenefits of on-site interpretation. Understanding the motives for travel-ling contributes to the understanding of tourism and is commonlyused as a means for defining and differentiating tourism subgroups(McCain & Ray, 2003). Similarly, the investigation of motives is pivotalto clarifying the nature of visitations to death-related sites (Lennon &Foley, 2000; Stone & Sharpley, 2008). Sharpley (2005) further arguesthat in order to clarify whether ‘‘dark tourism’’ indeed exists, motiva-tion research is required. The decision to focus on sought benefits ofthe interpretation rise from the key role it plays in constructing thetourist experience (Lennon & Foley, 2000; Moscardo & Ballantyne,2008).

The wide range of sites classified as dark, and the diversity of per-spectives on visits to such sites gave rise to a variety of reasons fordark-site visitations. Dann (1998) suggests a comprehensive list of mo-tives, including the desire to overcome phantom, search for novelty,

Page 6: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 825

nostalgia, celebration of crime and deviance, basic bloodlust and inter-est in challenging one’s sense or mortality. Seaton and Lennon (2004)identify two main motives, schadenfreude (the pleasure in viewing oth-ers’ misfortune) and the contemplation of death (which Seaton(1996) called thanatopsis). Ashworth (2002) emphasizes motives suchas satisfying curiosity about the unusual, being entertained by the hor-rific occurrences and the suffering of others, empathic identification(either with the victims or perpetrators) and, as in heritage tourism,seeking self-identification and self-understanding. In a later study,Ashworth (2004) notes that motives range from pilgrimage, searchfor identity, quest of knowledge, and a sense of social responsibility(i.e. ‘‘Lest we forget,’’ ‘‘Never again’’) to darker motives such as inter-est and indulgence in violence and suffering.

Yet, Stone and Sharpley (2008) state that motives for visiting death-related sites have not yet been fully or systematically investigated, pro-viding only a weak conceptualization of this phenomenon. This paucityof knowledge can be attributed to several issues. First, tourist motiva-tion has been explored in relation to a diverse collection of sites,among them Holocaust sites (Lennon & Foley, 2000), Graceland(Rojek, 1993), prisons (Wilson, 2008), and sites of slavery and Apart-heid (Austin, 2002). Not surprisingly, the range of motives identifiedis as broad as the range of sites explored. Second, current studies lar-gely adopt a descriptive approach, following the assumption that one’spresence in a death-related site necessarily reflects thanatouristicmotives (Slade, 2003). Finally, the motives identified are often notbased on empirical research but are largely drawn from theoretical re-search (Seaton & Lennon, 2004; Wight, 2005). These limitations com-bine with the suggestion brought forth in this study that theunderstanding of the tourist experience at dark sites should also drawon heritage tourism studies.

The interpretation of death-related sites has gained much academicattention (Stone & Sharpley, 2008; Wight, 2005). However, a review ofthe current literature on interpretation suggests that researchers andpractitioners tend to overlook the tourists. Moreover, tourists are oftenregarded as a homogeneous group and as passive receptors that shouldbe educated (Wight & Lennon, 2007). Studies ignore the fact that tour-ists have different levels of knowledge and familiarity as well as diversity ofviews in relation to the display (Goulding, 2000; Waitt, 2000), all of whichmay affect their preferences of on-site interpretation. While some tour-ists may be interested in interpretation that is educational, others maybe seeking an emotional, spiritual, or sentimental experience. It is sug-gested here that investigating the expectations of the interpretationand its link with the perception of the death presented on site, may assistin clarifying tourists’ experiences of dark heritage sites.

Research Objectives

As is evident from the literature, alongside the growing fascinationwith visits to death-related sites, scholars have emphasized the limited

Page 7: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

826 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

understanding of ‘‘dark tourism’’, attributing this to gaps in current re-search. Specifically, previous studies have been criticized for adopting atheoretical approach, whereas empirical investigation of tourist prac-tices and experiences is lacking (e.g. Seaton & Lennon, 2004; Wight,2005). For example, Seaton and Lennon (2004) go as far as suggestingthat ‘‘all of this is speculation’’ (p. 69). Other scholars have stressedthat while studies mainly focus on the supply of dark tourism, limitedattention has been given to the tourist experiences of dark sites (e.g.Sharpley, 2005; Stone & Sharpley, 2008). Accordingly, the first objec-tive of this study is to further clarify the nature of this phenomenonthrough empirical research focusing on the tourist experience. Follow-ing the experiential approach, the second objective of this study is toexplore core components of the experience in relation to the individ-ual perception of the site. Specifically, the study aims to clarify the rela-tionships between perception of the site as one’s own heritage,motivation for the visit, and sought interpretation benefits. Addressingthese research objectives is of potential contribution to a better under-standing of the nature of visitation patterns to death-related sites aswell as the management of such sites.

Study Methods

Auschwitz, considered to be ‘‘the darkest edges of the dark tourismspectrum’’ (Stone, 2006, p. 157), was chosen as the focus of this study.From 1942 to 1945, Auschwitz (located near Oswiecim, a Polish city an-nexed to the Third Rich), was the center for the mass extermination ofEuropean Jewry, mostly in gas chambers. It is estimated that betweenone and one and a half million people were murdered in Auschwitz,about 90 percent of them were Jewish (Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum,2009). The reason for selecting this site is its common ‘‘use’’ as anexample for dark tourism (e.g. Lennon & Foley, 2000; Stone & Sharp-ley, 2008). For example, two books on the subject—Lennon andFoley’s (2000) Dark Tourism and Tunbridge and Ashworth’s (1996) Dis-sonant Heritage—feature the camp’s gate on their cover, echoing thelure of the site. An additional reason is that as this study adopts theexperiential approach, a key requirement of the site studied was thatit would carry different meanings to different people. In addition toit being a World Heritage Site (United Nations Educational, Scientificand Cultural Organization, 2010), studies indicate that Auschwitzholds different meanings to people of different ethnicities and reli-gious beliefs (Ambrosewicz-Jacobs & Bialecka, 2008). This is reflectedin the wide appeal of this site to a variety of people. In 2008, overone million people visited Auschwitz. About a third of these visitorswere Polish (36.3%), but the vast majority (63.7%) came from some30 different countries (Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum, 2008).

The Auschwitz museum website advises visitors to spend about threehours at the site, visiting both Auschwitz I, where the camp first openedand Auschwitz II-Birkenau, the site of mass extermination (Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum, 2010). Admission is free, and for a fee, tourists may

Page 8: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 827

view a film of the camp’s liberation. The exhibition at Auschwitz I hastwo main sections—a permanent exhibition and the national exhibi-tions. The permanent exhibition includes documentary photographs,documents, and items seized from Jewish deportees (shoes, eye glasses,suitcases and toothbrushes, as well as human hair). The national exhi-bitions, are devoted to national memorials of the dead of the variousnations and ethnic groups murdered in Auschwitz (among them Romaand European Jews from Russia, Poland, France and Belgium). Thegrounds of Birkenau include the wrecks of the gas chambers, cremato-ria and the prisoners’ barracks.

This study is composed of three main stages. First, the exploratorystage included semi-structured interviews aiming to reveal motives for vis-iting atrocity sites in general and Auschwitz in particular, and the soughtbenefits from the visit and the interpretation. The interviews (n = 30)were conducted with tourists who intended to visit Auschwitz and otherdeath camps, before the actual visit. The choice of participants relied onpurposive sampling to achieve greater diversity of responses. Potentialparticipants were contacted and invited to participate, through touristguides to Poland (in Poland and Israel). Additionally, tourists to Krakow(a major tourist city located about an hour’s drive from Auschwitz) wereapproached at the city center and hotel lobbies. Interviews took place inPoland (n = 20) and Israel (n = 10). The interviews, which lasted 30–90minutes, were conducted by one of the authors. The main issues risingfrom the interviews were transcribed and then analyzed using the SpiderType Diagram technique which aims at identifying themes in the inter-views and the links between them. Due to the semi-structured natureof the interview, little disagreements were found between the threejudges during the analysis.

In the second stage, a structured questionnaire was designed. Thequestionnaire opened with a series of questions addressing tourists’perception of the site (adopted from Poria et al., 2006). This was fol-lowed by a set of questions regarding tourists’ motives for visitingAuschwitz and a set of questions designed to clarify sought benefitsof the interpretation. The questions on motives for visiting Auschwitzwere based on previous research (e.g. Sharpley, 2005; Timothy & Teye,2004) and the semi-structured interviews. Due to the aforementionedlimitations of current motivation research in dark tourism, the motivesidentified in the interviews carried more weight. Similarly, the ques-tions focusing on sought benefits of the interpretation were derivedfrom previous studies (e.g. Austin, 2002; Timothy & Boyd, 2003),and mostly from the exploratory stage. Specifically, during the inter-views the researchers noted that participants tend to speak of two dis-tinct aspects of learning and education at the site. One aspect wasinformational, learning more about the site and the events that tookplace there (i.e. what happened there? who and how many people weremurdered there?). The other was deeper, an attempt to understandpast events (i.e. why did it happen? why did it happen specifically toJews? why did no one stop it?). Participants were asked to provide theiranswers on a 7-point scale (1—disagree, 7—agree). The questionnairehad two versions, one in English and the other in Polish. To establish

Page 9: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

828 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

accuracy the questionnaire was written in English, translated intoPolish, and back-translated to English. A feasibility study (of both ver-sions) aiming to assess the clarity of the questionnaire and the distribu-tion of answers was conducted in March 2008, in Poland, among 25international tourists to Krakow.

The third stage, the main data collection, was conducted in April2008. Data were collected through personal interviews, although someparticipants preferred to complete the questionnaire on their own.The sample comprised both domestic and international tourists toPoland, over the age of 15. In order to reflect diversity of tourists’ per-ceptions and attitudes, it was decided to include in the sample: 1) tour-ists to Krakow who already visited Auschwitz before (n = 14, 7.1%); 2)tourist to Krakow who did not visit Auschwitz before but are familiarwith the site (n = 32, 16.2%); 3) tourists waiting to enter the site whodid not visit Auschwitz before, namely first time visitors (n = 121,61.4%); and 4) tourist waiting to enter the site who already visitedAuschwitz before, i.e. returning visitors (n = 30; 15.2%).

Data were collected in two locations: at the entrance to the site(n = 151, 76.3%) and at the city center of Krakow (n = 47, 23.7%). Ateach location, a Quasi-random Sampling (also known as SystematicSampling), which is based on a random sampling frame was applied.Every nth participant at each of the locations was approached, wheren was determined by the number of visitors and crowdedness (rangingfrom 15 to 20). Among those approached at the entrance to the site,approximately 20% refused to participate (responding that participa-tion would delay their entrance to the site); in the city center the refu-sal rate was lower—10%. Krakow residents were excluded from thestudy by using screening questions. The underlying assumption forthe exclusion of Krakow residents was that the proximity of the cityto Auschwitz may affect residents’ perception of the site and their vis-itation patterns. Additionally, participants were asked whether they arefamiliar with the site of Auschwitz and individuals who stated that theyare unfamiliar with the site were later excluded from the sample(n = 7). This is due to the common understanding that awareness isa precondition to attitudes, perceptions, and behavior (McClellan,1998). Thus, the final sample is composed of 198 participants.

FINDINGS

Of the sample (n = 198), 77.3% of the questionnaires were completedin English and 22.7% in Polish. The gender distribution was 51.3%female and 48.7% male. Among the respondents, 30% completed sec-ondary or primary school, 46.3% had completed an undergraduatecourse, and 23.7% had completed a postgraduate course. In terms ofeducation distribution, this finding is in line with previous studies sug-gesting that heritage tourists tend to be of higher educational level(Timothy & Boyd, 2003). Among those who indicated their age group,the mode was 20–39 years (59.7%). Most of the participants (77.2%)were Christian, 6.5% were Jewish, and 16.3% identified themselves as

Page 10: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 829

members of other religions. The largest number of participants camefrom Poland (24.7%), followed equally by participants from the UnitedStates and United Kingdom (12.6% from each).

Tourist Perceptions

Participants were asked to indicate their level of agreement withthree statements aiming to capture perception of the site as personalheritage. Cronbach’s alpha was relatively high (a = 0.82), suggestingthat the statements measure the same theoretical concept. These state-ments serve as a basis for classifying participants. Previous studies sug-gest two different methods to distinguish between tourists to heritagesetting in terms of their perception of the site, namely classificationinto two or three groups (Poria, Biran, & Reichel, 2009; Poria et al.,2003). These studies, however, suggest a classification which is basedon an arbitrary division of the scale. Therefore, a cluster analysis wascarried out using one clustering variable, the mean score of the threestatements measuring participants’ perception of the site.

The analysis was conducted in two stages: first a hierarchical methodwas implemented followed by a non-hierarchical cluster analysis, in or-der to attain the benefits of each of them (Hair, Black, Babin, Ander-son, & Tatham, 2005). A hierarchical cluster analysis using Ward’smethod was applied to obtain the agglomeration schedule. This meth-od was preferred as it minimizes the increase in the total sum ofsquares across clusters (Hair et al., 2005). The criterion for selectingthe initial cluster solution was the agglomeration coefficient. As thelargest increase (191.47%) occurred in the step between three clustersto two, the three-cluster solution was selected. In the second stage anon-hierarchical method using K-mean algorithm was conducted.The clusters’ cancroids derived from the hierarchical analysis wereused as initial seed points. The results of the K-mean cluster yieldedthree groups (the term groups is used rather than clusters to facilitatedease of reading) of tourists varying in their perception of the site. Thefirst group, (n = 63, 31.8% of the sample) display the lowest mean score(M = 1.65, SD = 0.60) and represents those who do not see Auschwitz aspart of their own heritage, whereas the third group (n = 53, 26.7%) iscomposed of those who do perceive the site as part of their personalheritage (M = 6.30, SD = 0.59). The largest group identified, GroupTwo (n = 82, 41.4%), includes those who are somewhat ambivalentabout this site, displaying no specific sense of owning or disowning to-wards the heritage presented (M = 3.81, SD = 0.69). Significant differ-ences were found between the three groups in their perception of thesite (F = 746.63, p < .01) and the post-hoc Sheffe test revealed signifi-cant differences among all three groups (p < .01 in all cases).

Motivation for the Visit

To identify common dimensions of motives, a factor analysis wasconducted. Due to the exploratory nature of this study, principal

Page 11: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

830 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

component analysis was the method chosen (Field, 2009). Assumingthat factors could be correlated, an oblique rotation was used, and fol-lowing Field (2009), only items loading with an absolute value higherthan 0.4 were included in the factors. No items were excluded fromthe analysis and all items were loaded on one factor only. The results(see Table 1) indicate that the motives for visiting Auschwitz can begrouped into four factors. The first, ‘‘see it to believe it’’ relates to par-ticipants’ interest in seeing the site out of a need to believe that suchatrocities really happened. The second factor, ‘‘learning and under-standing’’ highlights participants’ interest in being educated aboutWorld War II and the atrocities that took place in Auschwitz. This fac-tor indicates that participants aspire to gain an understanding (ratherthan simply being provided with information) as well as bequeath theirknowledge. The third factor, ‘‘famous death tourist attractions,’’ iscomposed of motives relating to the site being a famous site, generalinterest in sites of death, and willingness to see the real site. Interest-ingly, visiting the site in order to feel empathy with the victims is alsoloaded on this factor. The fourth factor, termed ‘‘emotional heritageexperience,’’ is composed of motives linked with one’s desire to con-nect with his/her heritage and have an emotional visiting experience.

Table 2 presents differences among the three groups of participantsbased on their perception of the site and the four motivation factorsidentified above (Table 1). The results reveal several interestingfindings. First, the mean scores of each factor indicate that, overall,educational motives are the foremost reason for visiting the site.Paired-sample t tests reveal that participants’ desire for an educationalexperience is significantly higher than all other motives (Pairs 1, 4, and6). Participants display moderate interest in visiting Auschwitz becauseit is a famous death site or to ‘‘confirm’’ that such atrocities hadhappened (no significant differences were found between these twofactors, Pair 2). Additionally, the results suggest that emotional motivesare the least important reason for the visit. A paired-sample t test re-veals significant differences between emotional motives and all othermotives (Pairs 3, 4, and 5).

Second, significant differences are found between the three groups intheir motives to visit the site in order to ‘‘see it to believe it,’’ have aneducational experience, and an ‘‘emotional heritage experience.’’ Spe-cifically, those perceiving the site as their personal heritage display thehighest motivation to visit due to these three reasons in comparison tothe two other groups. It is noteworthy that in the context of ‘‘see it tobelieve it’’ and educational motives, significant differences were foundonly between the third group and one of the two other groups (eitherGroup One or Group Two). In terms of ‘‘emotional heritage experi-ence,’’ significant differences were found between all three groups sug-gesting that the stronger the perception of the site as personal heritagethe higher participants’ wish for an emotional experience. No signifi-cant differences were found among the three groups in their desireto visit out of interest in famous atrocities sites.

Looking at the differences across motives within each group, it canbe seen that for all three groups ‘‘learning and understanding’’ is

Page 12: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

Table 1. Factor Analysis of Motivation for the Visit

I want to visit Auschwitz. . . See it tobelieve it

Learning andunderstanding

Famousdeath touristattraction

Emotionalheritageexperience

I need to see it to believe thatsuch mass murder happened

0.89

Because I want to ‘‘see it inorder to believe it’’

0.74

To make the Holocaust realfor me

0.66

To learn more of whathappened in Auschwitz

0.88

As I want to betterunderstand whathappened in Auschwitz

0.74

To learn more about WorldWar II

0.69

To be able to pass the story ofthe Holocaust to others

0.52

Because it is a well knowntourist attraction

0.80

Because it is a famous site 0.77As I am interested in seeing

sites linked with atrocityand death

0.52

To help me feel empathywith the victims ofAuschwitz

0.45

This is the site where ‘‘thingsreally happened’’

0.44

To feel closer to my ownheritage

0.90

Because I want to have anemotional experience

0.58

Eigenvalues 3.39 2.22 1.73 1.18% of variance 24.25 15.87 12.36 8.46Cronbach Alpha 0.71 0.73 0.64 0.61

Total variance explained: 60.95%Extraction method: Principal component analysisRotation method: Promax with Kaiser normalizationRotation converged in 6 iterations

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 831

the main motive for visiting Auschwitz. The mean scores of each groupsuggest that those who do not see the site as their own heritage, andthose who are ambivalent display the lowest interest in visiting the sitefor an emotional experience. For Group One, a comparison of ‘‘emo-tional heritage experience’’ with each of the other motives reveals

Page 13: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

Table 2. Perception of the Site and Motivation for the Visit

Group One(n = 63)

Group Two(n = 82)

Group Three(n = 53)

Overallmean

One-wayANOVA

Differences between groups (ANOVA)Learning and

understanding5.05a 5.36ab 5.80b 5.83 (1.15) F = 6.37**

See it to believe it 4.07ab 4.03a 4.84b 4.26 (1.75) F = 4.06*

Famous death touristattraction

3.85 4.32 4.10 4.11 (1.18) NS

Emotional heritageexperience

2.68a 3.59b 5.43c 3.79 (1.84) F = 49.21**

Differences within groups (Paired Sample t Test)Pair 1:

Famous death touristattraction- Learningand understanding

t = 7.43** t = 6.68** t = 9.93** t = 13.23**

Pair 2:Famous death touristattraction- See it tobelieve it

NS NS t = 2.489* NS

Pair 3:Famous death touristattraction- Emotionalheritage experience

t = 6.50** t = 4.29** t = �4.64** t = 2.27*

Pair 4:Emotional heritageexperience- Learningand understanding

t = 10.81** t = 9.20** NS t = 11.35**

Pair 5:Emotional heritageexperience- See it tobelieve it

t = 6.47** t = 1.93 t = �2.94** t = 3.37**

Pair 6:Learning andunderstanding—Seeit to believe it

t = �4.15** t = �6.86** t = �3.43** t = �8.42**

Group One: Do not perceive the site as part of their personal heritageGroup Two: Ambivalent in their perception of the site as part of their personal heritageGroup Three: Perceive the site as part of their personal heritage

Note: Standard deviations are presented in parentheses. Mean scores with different letters aresignificantly different with post-hoc Scheffe analysis at p 6 .05 or better probability level.* p 6 .05.; ** p 6 .01.

832 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

significant differences (Pairs 3, 4, and 5). For Group Two, a paired-sam-ple t test indicates no significant difference between the emotional mo-tive and ‘‘see it to believe it’’ (Pair 5). Yet, for this group significantdifferences are found between the emotional motive and the educa-tional motive (Pair 4) and interest in visiting famous death sites (Pair

Page 14: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 833

3). Both Group One and Group Two display moderate interest in vis-iting because Auschwitz is a famous attraction and to ‘‘see it to believeit,’’ there is no significant difference between these two motives (Pair2). Among those perceiving the site as their personal heritage, emo-tional heritage experience is the second most important motive for vis-iting. Yet, no significant difference is found between their interest tovisit for emotional motives and educational ones (Pair 4). Finally, thoseregarding the site as their own heritage are significantly less motivatedby the site as a ‘‘famous death tourist attraction.’’ Paired-sample t testreveals significant difference between this motive and all other motives(Pairs 1, 2 and 3) for this group.

Sought Interpretation Benefits

To examine underlying common dimensions of sought benefits, afactor analysis was undertaken. As reflected in Table 3, the sought ben-efits are grouped into three factors. The statements in the first factor,‘‘emotional involvement and understanding,’’ indicate participants’

Table 3. Factor Analysis of Sought Benefits of On-site Interpretation

If you were to visit Auschwitz. Youwould like the interpretation to . . .

Emotionalinvolvementand understanding

Experiencingpersonalheritage

Knowledgeenrichment

Help you better understand the killingprocess that took place at this site

0.92

Help you understand how somethinglike the Holocaust could havehappened

0.82

Help you feel empathy for the victimsof Auschwitz

0.61

Make you feel emotionally involved 0.56

Make you feel connected to your ownheritage

0.99

Allow you to learn something aboutyour own heritage

0.97

Enrich your knowledge about theJewish Holocaust

0.87

Enrich your knowledge about WorldWar II

0.87

Eigenvalues 3.03 1.63 1.21% of variance 37.95 20.49 15.19Cronbach Alpha 0.75 0.95 0.67

Total variance explained: 73.63%Extraction method: Principal component analysisRotation method: Promax with Kaiser normalizationRotation converged in 5 iterations

Page 15: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

834 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

expectation that the interpretation will explain the Auschwitz killingmechanism and the events that led to these killings, in addition to elic-iting emotional involvement and empathy towards the victims. The sec-ond factor, ‘‘experiencing personal heritage,’’ reflects participants’interest that on-site interpretation will enrich their knowledge of one’sown heritage and allow them to feel connected to it. The third factoridentified is ‘‘knowledge enrichment,’’ indicating participants’ wishthat the interpretation will extend their knowledge of the Holocaustand World War II. Similar to the pattern noted during the exploratoryinterviews, participants appear to differentiate between mere provisionof knowledge (the third factor) and gaining understanding of pastevent, which is reflected in the first factor.

Table 4 presents differences between the three groups of partici-pants based on their perception of the site and their sought benefits

Table 4. Perception of the Site and Sought-interpretation Benefits

Group One(n = 63)

Group Two(n = 82)

Group Three(n = 52)

Overallmean

One-wayANOVA

Differences between groups (ANOVA)Knowledge enrichment 5.12 5.41 5.33 5.29 (1.24) NSEmotional involvement

and understanding5.12a 5.07ab 5.81c 5.28 (1.26) F = 6.61**

Experiencing personalheritage

1.92a 3.24b 5.72c 3.48 (2.20) F = 78.22**

Differences within groups (Paired Sample t Test)Pair 1:

Emotionalinvolvementand understanding-Knowledgeenrichment

NS t = �2.21* t = 2.35* NS

Pair 2:Experiencingpersonal heritage-Emotionalinvolvement andunderstanding

t = 13.99** t = 8.70** NS t = 11.63**

Pair 3:Experiencingpersonal heritage—Knowledgeenrichment

t = �13.45** t = �9.74** NS t = �10.40**

Group One: Do not perceive the site as part of their personal heritageGroup Two: Ambivalent in their perception of the site as part of their personal heritageGroup Three: Perceive the site as part of their personal heritage

Note: Standard deviations are presented in parentheses. Mean scores with different letters aresignificantly different with post-hoc Scheffe analysis at p 6 .05 or better probability level.* p 6 .05.; ** p 6 .01.

Page 16: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 835

of the interpretation, according to the factors identified (Table 3).Some findings emerging from Table 4 are noteworthy. First, the meanscores of the three factors indicate that overall, participants value on-site interpretation as a tool to enrich their emotional involvementand knowledge of World War II and the Holocaust. Furthermore, interms of the overall scores, paired sample t test reveals no significantdifference between these two sought benefits factors (Pair 1). Finally,though participants rank experiencing personal heritage as the leastsought benefit, the higher standard deviation (2.20) suggests that par-ticipants’ expectations vary.

Second, no differences are found between participants with respect totheir preference for the interpretation to enrich one’s knowledge.However, significant differences are found between the three groupsin their desire that the interpretation will yield emotional involvementand understanding. Participants perceiving the site as their own heri-tage, show greater preference for the interpretation to help themunderstand the occurrences that took place in Auschwitz, and facilitatean emotional involvement. Significant differences are also found be-tween the three groups in relation to ‘‘experiencing personal heri-tage.’’ Specifically, the post-hoc analysis suggests that the moreparticipants tend to see the site as their own heritage the more theywish to learn about their personal heritage and feel connected to it.

Finally, referring to the differences within each group, the findingsreveal that those who see the site as their personal heritage show highinterest in all the three sought benefits identified (all mean scores arehigher than 5). The mean scores suggest that this group displays thehighest interest for the interpretation to facilitate an emotional expe-rience. Particularly, this group displays significantly higher preferencefor an emotional experience than for a knowledge-enriching experi-ence (Pair 1). However, there is no significant difference between theirinterest in emotional experience and personal heritage experience(Pair 2). No significant differences are found between this group’s de-sire that the interpretation provide a personal heritage experience andtheir desire that it will enrich their knowledge (Pair 3). Furthermore,the results suggest that those who are ambivalent as well as thosewho do not see the site as personal heritage share a similar pattern.Both display high preference for gaining emotional benefits andknowledge enrichment (mean scores higher than 5) and the lowestpreference for personal heritage experience. A comparison betweenthis sought benefit and interest in emotional involvement or knowl-edge enrichment reveal significant differences for both groups (Pairs2 and 3).

CONCLUSION

This study attempts to enhance the current understanding of tour-ism to sites presenting death and atrocity, employing the experientialapproach to examine elements which are at the core of the touristexperience. Auschwitz, considered ‘‘the pinnacle of European dark

Page 17: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

836 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

tourism’’ (Tarlow, 2005, p. 58), was chosen as the focus of this study.The findings challenge the current understanding of visits to Ausch-witz as dark tourism. In terms of motivation, dark-tourism literatureemphasizes fascination with death as the main (and even sole) motivefor visiting sites in which death is presented (Stone & Sharpley, 2008).The findings suggest that tourists’ motives are varied, and include a de-sire to learn and understand the history presented, a sense of ‘‘see it tobelieve it,’’ and interest in having an emotional heritage experience.Furthermore, the relative importance attributed to the motives re-vealed, indicates that interest in death is the least important reasonfor the visit. The findings indicate that tourists are mainly motivatedby a desire for an educational or emotional experience. Thus, support-ing Slade’s (2003) argument that people’s presence at places associ-ated with death does not necessarily mean that they arethanatourists. Following Sharpley’s (2005) framework of shades of darktourism, and the minimal interest in death noted in the context ofAuschwitz (an accidental dark site), it can be argued that ‘‘pale tour-ism’’ may actually be heritage tourism.

The findings demonstrate that the motives for visiting Auschwitz aresimilar to those for visiting a ‘‘regular,’’ not dark, heritage site. Educa-tional motives are often emphasized as a major reason for engaging inheritage tourism (Timothy & Boyd, 2003). Desire for emotionalinvolvement has also been identified as a key factor for visiting heritagesites (Poria, Reichel, & Biran, 2006), as are identity formation and con-struction (Buzinde & Santos, 2008; Prentice & Anderson, 2007). Simi-larly, in line with previous studies of heritage tourism motives, thecurrent study points to tourists’ desire to maintain their identity byconnecting to their own heritage and ‘‘seeing themselves’’ (Golden,1996). Finally, seeking an authentic experience or the ‘‘real thing’’ isoften mentioned as an important motive for heritage tourism (Apos-tolakis, 2003). These findings are in line with motives identified inthe context of heritage sites of dark attributes, such as Anne FrankHouse (Poria et al., 2006), slavery sites (Austin, 2002; Timothy & Teye,2004) and sites of natural disaster (Ryan & Kohli, 2006). Correspond-ing with Sharpley’s (2005) assertion of the need for motivation re-search to clarify whether ‘‘dark tourism’’ indeed exists, thesefindings suggest that a variety of experiences exist in dark sites.

With regard to participants’ preferences of interpretation, the find-ings further support the idea that participants perceive the visit toAuschwitz as a heritage experience. The sought benefits revealed hereindicate that rather than a tool for satisfying curiosity over death anddying, participants value on-site interpretation as a tool for enhancingtheir educational experience and knowledge, and as a source for emo-tional experience and connection to one’s personal heritage. Thesetypes of benefits are in accord with studies which highlight the impor-tance of the quest for emotional and educational experiences at heri-tage sites (Austin, 2002; Poria et al., 2009). These findings alsocorrespond with studies highlighting the importance of on-site inter-pretation in yielding meaning and emotional experience as well as pro-viding knowledge (Guter & Feldman, 2006).

Page 18: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 837

Finally, the findings reveal significant differences in the motivationfor the visit and sought interpretation benefits between those who per-ceive the site as their personal heritage and the two other groups (i.e.those who do not perceive the site as personal heritage and those whoare ambivalent). Whereas those who perceive the site as personal her-itage display greater interest in an emotional experience and feelingconnected to their own heritage, the two other groups mainly seekan educational experience. Furthermore, the three groups identifiedhere are consistent with these identified in studies of heritage attrac-tions. Those perceiving the site as personal heritage can be capturedas identity reinforcers (Prentice & Anderson, 2007). They are character-ized by a high desire for emotional experience and connection to theirheritage. Corresponding with the understanding of heritage spaces ascentral to identity construction (Ashworth, 2002; Buzinde & Santos,2008), this group can be understood as interested in maintaining theirpre-existing identity, as they are interested in feeling connected andexperiencing their own heritage. In line with Slade (2003), this groupcan be said to engage in a profound heritage experience. Those whodo not see the site as personal heritage and those who are ambivalentare knowledge seekers, who are more interested in a knowledge-enrichingexperience (Prentice & Anderson, 2007) than an emotional one. It isimportant to note that none of the groups shows interest in an experi-ence in which death is its core.

To conclude, the finding that tourist’s experiences at Auschwitz, thesymbol of dark tourism, are similar to those at heritage sites providesseveral insights regarding the theoretical framework currently usedto conceptualize visits to dark sites. The findings underline that thetourist experience should be understood in light of the experiential ap-proach as a ‘‘dialogic meaning making’’ (Buzinde & Santos, 2009, p.440) between the individual and the death on display. This raises theneed to reconsider the understanding of dark tourism as a distinct sub-group of tourism based on the site’s attributes only. Moreover, thestudy challenges the current research practice which almost automati-cally adopts a descriptive supply perspective to the understanding ofdark tourism, suggesting that it is not death or the dead that shouldbe considered, but living peoples’ perception of them.

Sharpley (2005) argues that dark tourism ‘‘is a relatively rare phenom-enon’’ (p. 226). This observation, combined with the fact that many her-itage sites include elements of death and atrocities (Ashworth &Hartmann, 2005; Dann & Seaton, 2001), and with the current findingsraise the need to re-conceptualize dark tourism. Such conceptualizationshould be able to differentiate it from other types of tourism experiences,particularly heritage tourism. Based on Stone and Sharpley’s thanatolog-ical framework (2008; also Stone, 2009) it is suggested that dark tourismshould be conceptualized along the place of death in society as well as themotivation for the visit. Specifically, it is suggested that dark tourism ispeoples’ purposeful movement to spaces displaying acts and sights thatare commonly absent from the social realm, which involves a sense of un-ease in seeing or participating in them. This framework further suggeststhat such sights and acts may not necessarily involve death, and that death

Page 19: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

838 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

may not necessary be dark. This theoretical supposition, like any othersupposition, requires an empirical investigation.

In terms of managerial recommendations, the results suggest that ifthe aim of the site is to be made attractive to diverse audiences, theinterpretation should be customized in accordance with tourists’ per-ception of the site. For example, for those not perceiving Auschwitzas their personal heritage, the interpretation should facilitate emo-tional involvement, yet emphasize the educational experience. Forthose who attach a personal meaning to the site, along with knowledgeenrichment the interpretation should reflect their special connectionto the site and allow them to peruse an intense emotional experience.Similarly, the heterogeneous nature of tourists’ motives can be incor-porated into the marketing efforts of Auschwitz. Also of interest isthe finding that tourists differentiate between knowledge enrichmentand gaining a deeper understanding. Thus, the current finding indi-cates the need to consider both these aspects in the conceptualizationand management of the tourist experience.

Limitations and Future Research

Auschwitz is an iconic site of paramount symbolic meaning and maybe seen as a ‘‘must see’’ tourist attraction. For example, the Lonely Pla-net (2010) lists Auschwitz among its ‘‘top picks for Poland.’’ Clearly,there is a need for additional research in other less iconic or promi-nent sites, or which display different types and forms of death to fur-ther clarify the tourist experiences at dark sites. Additionally, whilethis study included both tourists who visited the site as well as partici-pants who had not yet visited, a study which will focus solely on eitherone of these groups, may provide further understanding of the soughtexperiences at dark sites as well as the constraints for the visit. Finally,as in any motivational research, it could be claimed that fascinationwith death was not revealed because people are reluctant to reveal oradmit to ‘‘less socially acceptable’’ (Ashworth, 2004, p. 96) emotionsand motivations. A study fully based on qualitative epistemology mightbe able to explore these aspects further.

REFERENCES

Ambrosewicz-Jacobs, J., & Bialecka, A. (2008). The dynamics of Holocaustremembrance in Poland. In N. Keren (Ed.), Islands of memory: Holocaustremembrance in museums at the beginning of the 21st century (pp. 77–97). TelItzhak: Massuah International Institute for Holocaust Studies, [inHebrew].

Apostolakis, A. (2003). The convergence process in heritage tourism. Annals ofTourism Research, 30(4), 795–812.

Ashworth, G. (2002). Holocaust tourism: The experience of Krakow-Kazimierz.International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 11(4),363–367.

Ashworth, G. (2004). Tourism and the heritage of atrocity: Managing the heritageof South Africa for entertainment. In T. V. Singh (Ed.), New horizons in tourism:Strange experiences and stranger practices (pp. 95–108). Wallingford: Cabi.

Page 20: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 839

Ashworth, G., & Hartmann, R. (2005). Introduction: Managing atrocity andtourism. In G. Ashworth & R. Hartmann (Eds.), Horror and human tragedyrevisited: The management of sites of atrocities for tourism (pp. 1–14). New York:Cognizant Communication Corporation.

Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum (2008), Annual Report 2008. Retrieved September 22,2009 from Auschwitz-Birkenau Memorial and Museum website: http://en.auschwitz.org.pl/m/index.php?option=com_content&task=view&id=620&Itemid=49.

Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum (2009). Auschwitz and Shoah- The number of victims.Retrieved September 19, 2009 from Auschwitz-Birkenau Memorial andMuseum website: http://en.auschwitz.org.pl/m.

Auschwitz-Birkenau Museum (2010). Plan a visit. Retrieved August 2, 2010 fromAuschwitz-Birkenau Memorial and Museum website: http://en.ausch-witz.org.pl/m. Retrieved August 2, 2010.

Austin, N. K. (2002). Managing heritage attractions: Marketing challenges atsensitive historical sites. The International Journal of Tourism Research, 4(6),447–457.

Beech, J. (2000). The enigma of Holocaust sites as tourist attractions: The case ofBuchenwald. Managing Leisure, 5, 29–41.

Belhassen, Y., Caton, K., & Stewart, W. P. (2008). The search for authenticity in thepilgrim experience. Annals of Tourism Research, 35(3), 668–689.

Blom, T. (2000). Morbid tourism: A postmodern market niche with an examplefrom Althorp. Norwegian Journal of Geography, 54, 29–36.

Buzinde, C., & Santos, C. (2008). Representation of slavery. Annals of TourismResearch, 35(2), 469–488.

Buzinde, C., & Santos, C. (2009). Interpreting slavery tourism. Annals of TourismResearch, 36(3), 439–458.

Cohen, E. H. (2010). Educational dark tourism at an in populo site: the Holocaustmuseum in Jerusalem. Annals of Tourism Research, 38(1), 193–209.

Collins-Kreiner, N. (2010). Researching pilgrimage: Continuity and transforma-tions. Annals of Tourism Research, 37(2), 440–456.

Dann, G. (1998). The dark side of tourism. Etudes et rapports, serie L. Aix-en-Provence:Centre International de Recherches et d’Etudes Touristiques.

Dann, G., & Seaton, A. (2001). Slavery, contested heritage and thanatourism.International Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Administration, 2(3), 1–29.

Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications.Jamal, T., & Lelo, L. (2009, April). Exploring the conceptual and analytical

framing: From darkness to intentionality. Paper presented at Tourist Expe-riences: Meanings, Motivations, Behaviors, University of Central Lancashire,United Kingdom.

Golden, D. (1996). The museum of Jewish Diaspora tells a story. In T. Selwyn(Ed.), The tourist image, myth and making in tourism (pp. 223–250). New York:John Wiley.

Goulding, C. (2000). The museum environment and the visitor experience.European Journal of Marketing, 34(3/4), 261–278.

Guter, Y., & Feldman, J. (2006). Holy land pilgrimage as a site of inter-religiousencounter. Studia Hebraica, 6, 87–93.

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2005).Multivariate data analysis. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Hughes, R. (2008). Dutiful tourism: Encountering the Cambodian genocide. AsiaPacific Viewpoint, 49(3), 318–330.

Lennon, J., & Foley, M. (2000). Dark tourism: The attractions of death and disaster.London: Thomson.

Lisle, D. (2004). Gazing at ground zero: Tourism, voyeurism and spectacle. Journalfor Cultural Research, 8(1), 3–21.

Light, D. (2007). Dracula tourism in Romania: Cultural identity and the state.Annals of Tourism Research, 34(3), 746–765.

Logan, W., & Reeves, K. (2009). Introduction: Remembering places of pain andshame. In W. Logan & K. Reeves (Eds.), Places of pain and shame: Dealing with‘difficult past’ (pp. 1–14). London: Routledge.

Page 21: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

840 A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841

Lonely Planet (2010). Poland travel information and travel guide. Retrieved December27, 2010, from Lonely Planet website: http://www.lonelyplanet.com/poland.

McCain, G., & Ray, N. M. (2003). Legacy tourism: The search for personal meaningin heritage travel. Tourism Management, 24(6), 713–717.

McClellan, T. (1998). Tourism marketing—A question of perception. Journal ofVacation Marketing, 4(4), 408–414.

Moscardo, G., & Ballantyne, R. (2008). Interpretation and attractions. In A. Fyall,B. Garrod, A. Leask, & S. Wanhill (Eds.), Managing visitor attractions: Newdirections (pp. 237–252). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Muzaini, H., Teo, P., & Yeoh, B. S. A. (2007). Intimations of postmodernity in darktourism: The fate of history at Fort Siloso, Singapore. Journal of Tourism andCultural Change, 5(1), 28–44.

Poria, Y., Biran, A., & Reichel, A. (2009). Visitor preferences for interpretation atheritage sites. Journal of Travel Research, 48(1), 92–105.

Poria, Y., Butler, R., & Airey, D. (2003). The core of heritage tourism:Distinguishing heritage tourists from tourists in heritage places. Annals ofTourism Research, 30(1), 238–254.

Poria, Y., Butler, R., & Airey, D. (2004). The meaning of heritage sites for tourists:The case of Massada. Tourism Analysis, 9(1/2), 15–22.

Poria, Y., Reichel, A., & Biran, A. (2006). Heritage site management: Motivationsand expectations. Annals of Tourism Research, 33(1), 1172–1188.

Rojek, C. (1993). Ways of escape. Basingstoke: Macmillan.Ryan, C., & Kohli, R. (2006). The buried village, New Zealand—An example of

dark tourism?. Asia Pacific Journal of Tourism Research, 11(3), 211–226.Prentice, R., & Anderson, V. (2007). Interpreting heritage essentialism: Familiarity

and felt history. Tourism Management, 28(3), 661–676.Seaton, A. (1996). Guided by the dark: From thanatopsis to thanatourism.

International Journal of Heritage Studies, 2, 234–244.Seaton, A. V., & Lennon, J. J. (2004). Thanatourism in the early 21st century: Moral

panics, ulterior motives and alterior desires. In T. V. Singh (Ed.), New horizonsin tourism: Strange experiences and stranger practices (pp. 63–82). Wallingford:Cabi.

Sharpley, R. (2005). Travels to the edge of darkness: Towards a typology of ‘‘darktourism’’. In C. Ryan, S. Page, & M. Aicken (Eds.), Taking tourism to the limits:Issues, concepts and managerial perspectives (pp. 187–198). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Sharpley, R. (2009). Shedding light on dark tourism: An introduction. In R.Sharpley & P. Stone (Eds.), The darker side of travel: The theory and practice of darktourism (pp. 3–22). Bristol: Channel View Publications.

Slade, P. (2003). Gallipoli thanatourism: The meaning of ANZAC. Annals ofTourism Research, 30(4), 779–794.

Smith, N., & Croy, W. G. (2005). Presentation of dark tourism: Te Wairoa, theBuried Village. In C. Ryan, S. Page, & M. Aicken (Eds.), Taking tourism to thelimits: Issues, concepts and managerial perspectives (pp. 199–213). Amsterdam:Elsevier.

Stone, P. (2006). A dark tourism spectrum: Towards a typology of death andmacabre related tourist sites, attractions and exhibitions. Tourism, 54(2),145–160.

Stone, P. (2009). Dark tourism: Morality and new moral spaces. In R. Sharpley & P.Stone (Eds.), The darker side of travel: The theory of dark tourism (pp. 56–74).Bristol: Channel View Publications.

Stone, P., & Sharpley, R. (2008). Consuming dark tourism: A thanatologicalperspective. Annals of Tourism Research, 35(2), 574–595.

Tarlow, P. E. (2005). Dark tourism: The appealing ‘‘dark’’ side of tourism andmore. In M. Novelli (Ed.), Niche tourism: Contemporary issues, trends and cases(pp. 47–58). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Teye, V. B., & Timothy, D. J. (2004). The varied colors of slave heritage in WestAfrica. Space and Culture, 7(2), 145–155.

Timothy, D. J., & Boyd, S. W. (2003). Heritage tourism. Harlow: Prentice Hall.Timothy, D. J., & Teye, V. B. (2004). American children of the African diaspora. In

T. Coles & D. J. Timothy (Eds.), Tourism, diasporas and space (pp. 111–123).London: Routledge.

Page 22: Sought Experiences at (Dark) Heritage Sites

A. Biran et al. / Annals of Tourism Research 38 (2011) 820–841 841

Tunbridge, J., & Ashworth, G. (1996). Dissonant heritage: The management of the pastas a resource in conflict. Chichester: Wiley.

United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (2010). Ausch-witz Birkenau German Nazi Concentration and Extermination Camp (1940-1945).Retrieved February 14, 2010, from United Nations Educational, Scientific andCultural Organization website: http://whc.unesco.org/en/list/31.

Uriely, N. (2005). The tourist experience. Conceptual developments. Annals ofTourism Research, 32(1), 199–216.

Uzzell, D., & Ballantyne, R. (1998). Heritage that hurts: Interpretation in apostmodern world. In D. Uzzell & R. Balantyne (Eds.), Contemporary issues inheritage and environmental interpretation (pp. 152–171). London: The StationaryOffice.

Waitt, G. (2000). Consuming heritage: Perceived historical authenticity. Annals ofTourism Research, 27(4), 835–862.

Wight, C. (2005). Philosophical and methodological praxes in dark tourism:Controversy, contention and the evolving paradigm. Journal of VacationMarketing, 12(2), 119–129.

Wight, A. C., & Lennon, J. J. (2007). Selective interpretation and eclectic humanheritage in Lithuania. Tourism Management, 28(2), 519–529.

Wilson, J. Z. (2008). Prison: Cultural memory of dark tourism. New York: Peter Lang.

Submitted 31 December 2009. Resubmitted 27 August 2010. Resubmitted 1 September 2010.Final version 9 November 2010. Accepted 3 December 2010. Refereed anonymously.Coordinating Editor: Gyan Nyaupane

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com