Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0...

63
Eero Simoncelli HHMI / New York University Probing sensory representations with metameric stimuli Tuesday, August 25, 15 1

Transcript of Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0...

Page 1: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Eero SimoncelliHHMI / New York University

Probing sensory representations with metameric stimuli

Tuesday, August 25, 15 1

Page 2: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[figure: Hubel ‘95]

Where does all that visual information go?

RetinaOpticNerve

LGNOpticTract

VisualCortex

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

2

Page 3: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Sensoryinput

Remember

Act

Destiny of sensory information

Discard

(brain)

Tuesday, August 25, 15 3

Page 4: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Metamers

• Two stimuli that are physically different, but appear the same to a human observer

• Classic example: trichromatic color perception

• Another example: texture perception

Tuesday, August 25, 15 4

Page 5: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Spectral nature of light

[Newton, 1665]

Tuesday, August 25, 15

This image is in the public domain.

Diagram of a prism removed due to copyrightrestrictions. Please see the video.

5

Page 6: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

power

wavelength

0

500

1000

1500

400 450 500

Pow

er

(arb

itra

ry u

nits)

550 600 650 700

Wavelength (nm)

Test LightPrimaryMixture

Basic Color Matching

Quantifying Matches: Color Matching Experiment

Monday, June 21, 2010

From “DB SID Slides”,

CT-21

Arbitrarytest light

Mixture of3 primary lights

Perceptual color matching experimentpower

wavelength

[Young, Helmholtz, Grassman, etc, 1800‘s; slide c/o D. Brainard]

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Courtesy of David Brainard. Used with permission.

6

Page 7: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Perceptual color matching experiment

0

500

1000

1500

400 450 500

Pow

er

(arb

itra

ry u

nits)

550 600 650 700

Wavelength (nm)

Test LightPrimaryMixture

Basic Color Matching

Quantifying Matches: Color Matching Experiment

Monday, June 21, 2010

From “DB SID Slides”,

CT-21

Mixture of3 primary lights

Arbitrarytest light

[Young, Helmholtz, Grassman, etc, 1800‘s; slide c/o D. Brainard]power

wavelength

power

wavelength

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Courtesy of David Brainard. Used with permission.

7

Page 8: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Theory (Grassman, 1853): the visual system performs a linear projection of the wavelength spectrum onto a three-dimensional response space

L N

• Predicts/explains perceptual “metamers” - lights that appear identical, but have physically distinct wavelength spectra (1800’s)

• Codified in CIE standards for color representation (1931)

• Underlying mechanism (cone photoreceptors) verified (1987)

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

8

Page 9: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Suction Microelectrode

Monday, June 21, 2010

RP-14,

CD-5

[Baylor, Nunn & Schnapf, 1987]

Baylor, Nunn, & Schnapf (1987)

Measured Primate L, M, S Spectral Sensitivities (Log Scale)

Monday, June 21, 2010

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Figure removed due to copyright restrictions. Please see the video.

Source: Figure 3 from Baylor, D. A., B. J. Nunn, and J. L. Schnapf.

"Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis."

The Journal of Physiology 390, no. 1 (1987): 145-160.

Courtesy of Denis Baylor. Used with permission.

9

Page 10: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Visual texture

Homogeneous, with repeated structures

Tuesday, August 25, 15 10

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative Commons

license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametric texture model based on joint statistics

of complex wavelet coefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no. 1 (2000): 49-70.

Page 11: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Tuesday, August 25, 15 11

Page 12: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Julesz (1962)

• Hypothesis: Two textures with identical Nth-order pixel statistics will appear the same (for some N).

• Hand-constructed counter-examples (N=3):

Julesz ‘78 Yellott ‘93 Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative Commons

license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametric texture model based on joint statistics

of complex wavelet coefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no. 1 (2000): 49-70.

12

Page 13: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Physiologically-inspired Julesz-style texture model

[Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000]Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

13

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excludedfrom our Creative Commons license. For more information,see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametrictexture model based on joint statistics of complex waveletcoefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no.1(2000): 49-70.

ssi299
Line
Page 14: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Physiologically-inspired Julesz-style texture model

+

3.11.4

12.5...

Joint statistics

[Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000]Tuesday, August 25, 15 14

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excludedfrom our Creative Commons license. For more information,see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametrictexture model based on joint statistics of complex waveletcoefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no.1(2000): 49-70.

Page 15: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Texture synthesis

+

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

Joint statistics

[Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000]

+

Joint statistics

Tuesday, August 25, 15 15

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excludedfrom our Creative Commons license. For more information,see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametrictexture model based on joint statistics of complex waveletcoefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no.1(2000): 49-70.

Page 16: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Texture synthesis

+

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

Joint statistics

[Portilla & Simoncelli, 2000]

+

Joint statistics

Tuesday, August 25, 15 16

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excludedfrom our Creative Commons license. For more information,see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametrictexture model based on joint statistics of complex waveletcoefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no.1(2000): 49-70.

Page 17: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Images

Model responsesoriginal image

Images with identicalmodel responsesnoise seed

synthesized

Tuesday, August 25, 15 17

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content isexcluded from our Creative Commons license.For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli."A parametric texture model based on jointstatistics of complex wavelet coefficients."International journal of computer vision 40, no.1 (2000): 49-70.

Page 18: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Experimental logic

If model captures the same properties as the visual system, images with identical model responses should appear identical to a human.

Original

Photographic

Image

Random

Seed

Image

Statistical

Image

Generator

Ventral

Model

Responses

RetinaOpticNerve

LGNOpticTract

VisualCortex

ModelResponses

Tuesday, August 25, 15 18

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

Page 19: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Pairs of images with identical model responses:

Top: original, Bottom: synthesized

[Portilla & Simoncelli 2000]Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative Commons

license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametric texture model based on joint statistics

of complex wavelet coefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no. 1 (2000): 49-70.

19

Page 20: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Other explorations:* inpainting* interpolation* seeds

64 Portilla and Simoncelli

3.3. Extensions

As a demonstration of the flexibility of our approach,we can modify the algorithm to handle applicationsof constrained texture synthesis. In particular, con-sider the problem of extending a texture image be-yond its spatial boundaries (spatial extrapolation). Wewant to synthesize an image in which the central pix-els contain a copy of the original image, and the sur-rounding pixels are synthesized based on the statis-tical measurements of the original image. The set ofall images with the same central subset of pixels isconvex, and the projection onto such a convex set iseasily inserted into the iterative loop of the synthe-sis algorithm. Specifically, we need only re-set thecentral pixels to the desired values on each iterationof the synthesis loop. In practice, this substitution isdone by multiplying the desired pixels by a smoothmask (a raised cosine) and adding this to the cur-rent synthesized image multiplied by the complementof this mask. The smooth mask prevents artifacts atthe boundary between original and synthesized pixels,whereas convergence to the desired pixels within themask support region is achieved almost perfectly. Thistechnique is applicable to the restoration of pictureswhich have been destroyed in some subregion (“fill-ing holes”) (e.g., Hirani and Totsuka, 1996), althoughthe estimation of parameters from the defective imageis not straightforward. Figure 19 shows a set of ex-amples that have been spatially extrapolated using thismethod. Observe that the border between real and syn-thetic data is barely noticeable. An additional poten-tial benefit is that the synthetic images are seamlesslyperiodic (due to circular boundary-handling withinour algorithm), and thus may be used to tile a largerimage.Finally,we consider the problemof creating a texture

that lies visually “in between” two other textures. Theparameter space consisting of spatial averages of localfunctions has a type of convexity property in the limit asthe image lattice grows in size.3 Figure 20 shows threeimages synthesized from parameters that are an aver-age of the parameters for two example textures. In allthree cases, the algorithm converges to an interesting-looking image that appears to be a patchwise mix-tures of the two initial textures, rather than a newhomogeneous texture that lies perceptually betweenthem. Thus, in our model, the subset of parameterscorresponding to textures (homogeneous RFs) is notconvex!

Figure 19. Spatial extrapolation of texture images. Upper left cor-ner: example of an initial image to be extrapolated. Center shows anexample texture image, surrounded by a black region indicating thepixels to be synthesized. Remaining images: extrapolated examples(central region of constrained pixels is the same size and shape in allexamples).

Figure 20. Examples of “mixture” textures. Left: text (Fig. 19) tilemosaic (Fig. 3);Middle: lizard skin (Fig. 14) andwoven cane (Fig. 4);Right: plaster (Fig. 15) and brick (Fig. 14).

4. Discussion

We have described a universal parametric model forvisual texture, based on a novel set of pairwise jointstatistical constraints on the coefficients of a multi-scale image representation.We have described a frame-work for testing the perceptual validity of this modelin the context of the Julesz conjecture, and devel-oped a novel algorithm for synthesizing model tex-tures using sequential projections onto the constraintsurfaces. We have demonstrated the necessity of sub-sets of our constraints by showing examples of tex-tures for which synthesis quality is substantially de-creased when that subset is removed from the model.And we have shown the power and flexibility ofthe model by synthesizing a wide variety of artifi-cial and natural textures, and by applying it to the

“outpainting”

Central square of each image is

original texture.Surround is

synthesized.

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative Commons

license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametric texture model based on joint statistics

of complex wavelet coefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no. 1 (2000): 49-70.

20

Page 21: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Structural seeding [cf. “adversarial examples” - Szegedy et. al. 2014]

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Source Unknown. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our CreativeCommons license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

21

Page 22: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Can we generalize to inhomogeneous stimuli?

Can we make the model more physiological?

Tuesday, August 25, 15 22

© Springer. All rights reserved. This content is excludedfrom our Creative Commons license. For more information,see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Portilla, Javier, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "A parametrictexture model based on joint statistics of complex waveletcoefficients." International journal of computer vision 40, no.1(2000): 49-70.

Page 23: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

RetinaOpticNerve

LGNOpticTract

VisualCortex

[figure: Hubel ‘95]

(V1)

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

23

Page 24: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

RetinaOpticNerve

LGNOpticTract

VisualCortex

[Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982]

Dorsal pathway: V1->V3->V5position, motion, action

Ventral pathway: V1->V2->V4-> ITspatial form, recognition, memory

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

24

Page 25: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

RetinaOpticNerve

LGNOpticTract

VisualCortex

• Visual neurons responds to content within a small region of the visual input known as the Receptive Field (RF)• In each visual area, we assume RFs cover the entire visual field

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Harvard Medical School. All rights reserved. This contentis excluded from our Creative Commons license. For moreinformation, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

25

Page 26: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Retinal ganglion (midget) cell receptive fields(macaque, magnified x10) [Perry et.al., 1984; Watanabe & Rodiek, 1989]

Inhomogeneity - RF sizes grow with eccentricity

Modified Snellen acuity chart(threshold, x10) [after Anstis, 1973]

WT

P

YC

B

U

D

OJH

A

SF

R

KK

FQ

B

L

PA

M

Z

RH

O

Y

JG

V

loss of resolution

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Vision Research. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our CreativeCommons license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Anstis, Stuart M. "A chart demonstrating variations in acuity with retinalposition." Vision research 14, no. 7 (1974): 589-592.

26

ssi299
Line
Page 27: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[after Geisler et al., 1999]Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Source Unknown. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative

Commons license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

27

Page 28: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Retina

[Freeman & Simoncelli 2011, data from Gattass et. al., 1981; Gattass et. al., 1988; Perry et. al., 1984]

RF sizes grow with eccentricity

Tuesday, August 25, 15 28

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 29: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

V1 V2 V4 IT

V1

V2

V4

IT

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 29

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 30: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

D. H. HUBEL AND T. N. WIESELfield such as that of Text-fig. 2F) are of the same order of magnitude asthe diameters of geniculate receptive-field centres, at least for fields in ornear the area centralis. Hence the fineness of discrimination implied bythe small size of geniculate receptive-field centres is not necessarily lost atthe cortical level, despite the relatively large total size of many corticalfields; rather, it is incorporated into the detailed substructure of thecortical fields.

Text-fig. 19. Possible scheme for explaining the organization of simple receptivefields. A large number of lateral geniculate cells, of which four are illustrated inthe upper right in the figure, have receptive fields with 'on' centres arranged alonga straight line on the retina. All of these project upon a single cortical cell, and thesynapses are supposed to be excitatory. The receptive field of the cortical cell willthen have an elongated 'on' centre indicated by the interrupted lines in thereceptive-field diagram to the left of the figure.

In a similar way, the simple fields of Text-fig. 2D-G may be constructedby supposing that the afferent 'on'- or 'off'-centre geniculate cells havetheir field centres appropriately placed. For example, field-type G couldbe formed by having geniculate afferents with 'off' centres situated in theregion below and to the right of the boundary, and 'on' centres above andto the left. An asymmetry of flanking regions, as in field E, wouldbe produced if the two flanks were unequally reinforced by 'on'-centreafferents.The model of Text-fig. 19 is based on excitatory synapses. Here the

suppression of firing on illuminating an inhibitory part of the receptivefield is presumed to be the result of withdrawal of tonic excitation, i.e. theinhibition takes place at a lower level. That such mechanisms occur in thevisual system is clear from studies of the lateral geniculate body, wherean 'off'-centre cell is suppressed on illuminating its field centre because ofsuppression of firing in its main excitatory afferent (Hubel & Wiesel, 1961).In the proposed scheme one should, however, consider the possibility ofdirect inhibitory connexions. In Text-fig. 19 we may replace any of theexcitatory endings by inhibitory ones, provided we replace the corre-sponding geniculate cells by ones of opposite type ('on '-centre instead of' off'-centre, and conversely). Up to the present the two mechanisms have

142

[Hubel & Wiesel, 1962]

V1 simple cell

CAT VISUAL CORTEX1not been distinguished, but there is no reason to think that both do notoccur.The properties of complex fields are not easily accounted for by sup-

posing that these cells receive afferents directly from the lateral geniculatebody. Rather, the correspondence between simple and complex fieldsnoted in Part I suggests that cells with complex fields are of higher order,having cells with simple fields as their afferents. These simple fields wouldall have identical axis orientation, but would differ from one another intheir exact retinal positions. An example of such a scheme is given inText-fig. 20. The hypothetical cell illustrated has a complex field like that

Text-fig. 20. Possible scheme for explaining the organization of complex receptivefields. A number of cells with simple fields, ofwhich three are shown schematically,are imagined to project to a single cortical cell of higher order. Each projectingneurone has a receptive field arranged as shown to the left: an excitatory region tothe left and an inhibitory region to the right of a vertical straight-line boundary.The boundaries of the fields are staggered within an area outlined by the inter-rupted lines. Any vertical-edge stimulus falling across this rectangle, regardlessof its position, will excite some simple-field cells, leading to excitation of the higher-order cell.

of Text-figs. 5 and 6. One may imagine that it receives afferents from a setof simple cortical cells with fields of type C, Text-fig. 2, all with verticalaxis orientation, and staggered along a horizontal line. An edge of lightwould activate one or more of these simple cells wherever it fell within thecomplex field, and this would tend to excite the higher-order cell.

Similar schemes may be proposed to explain the behaviour of othercomplex units. One need only use the corresponding simple fields asbuilding blocks, staggering them over an appropriately wide region. Acell with the properties shown in Text-fig. 3 would require two types ofhorizontally oriented simple fields, having 'off' centres above the hori-zontal line, and 'on' centres below it. A slit of the same width as thesecentre regions would strongly activate only those cells whose long narrow

143

V1 complex cell

linearweights

rectifyingnonlinearity

+

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Figure removed due to copyright restrictions. Please see the video.

Source: Hubel, David H., and Torsten N. Wiesel. "Receptive fields,

binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual

cortex." The Journal of physiology 160, no. 1 (1962): 106-154.

30

Page 31: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

250

150

25 40170

V2receptive fields V1 cells

Local texture representationin the ventral stream

Joint statistics

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

Local correlational statistics can be re-expressed as a “subunit” model...

Tuesday, August 25, 15 31

Page 32: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Canonical computationin the ventral stream

Substantial information loss => model predicts metamers

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

V2“complex” cell

+

250

150

25 40170

V2receptive fields V1 cells

Tuesday, August 25, 15 32

Page 33: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Canonical sensory computation

• Linear filter (determines pattern selectivity)

• Rectifying nonlinearity

• Local pooling (e.g., average, max)

• Local gain control

• Noise

[eg. Douglas, 1989; Heeger, Simoncelli & Movshon 1996;

Heeger & Carandini 2014]

Cascaded ...

Tuesday, August 25, 15 33

Page 34: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

© 1999 Nature America Inc. • http://neurosci.nature.com

© 1

999 N

atu

re A

meri

ca In

c. •

htt

p:/

/neu

rosci.n

atu

re.c

om

news and views

In Arthur Conan Doyle’s story SilverBlaze, Inspector Gregory from ScotlandYard asks Sherlock Holmes “Is there anypoint to which you would wish to drawmy attention?”, to which Holmes replies“To the curious incident of the dog in thenight-time.” “The dog did nothing in thenight-time.” “That was the curious inci-dent.” The point of this well knownexchange is, of course, that the dog didnot bark because the criminal was a per-son it knew well and expected. Predictivecoding of sensory stimuli in nervous sys-tems has a similar flavor. It is an encodingstrategy in which predictable features inthe input are suppressed, and only theunexpected is signaled to the next stage ofprocessing.

Almost from the start of the informa-tion age, theorists have argued that sucha coding strategy is very efficient and islikely to be widely used in natural sensorysystems. In particular, predictive codinghas been invoked to explain the detailedshape of the spatio-temporal receptivefields of neurons in the retina and lateralgeniculate nucleus of mammals (forreview, see ref. 1). The visual cortex, how-ever, with its complex and sometimeshighly nonlinear receptive-field proper-ties, has proven resistant to these ideas.No more, though. The study by Rao andBallard2 in this issue of Nature Neuro-science provides a detailed account of howpredictive coding can explain extra-clas-sical receptive-field effects of neurons inprimary visual cortex (V1) and beyond.

The basic idea behind hierarchical pre-dictive coding is simple. It starts with thequestion of how images of natural scenesshould be analyzed. An obvious answer is

expected, or predicted, image features. Dueto the convergence of spatially adjacentmodules onto the next higher level, thereceptive fields become progressively larg-er as one ascends the hierarchy. Learningof the synaptic weights of the units in eachstage—which can be thought of as theirreceptive fields—is unsupervised, that is,it does not require an external ‘teacher’ thatrewards or punishes individual choices.

Using a two-stage prediction network,Rao and Ballard demonstrate how unitsin their first stage come to show ‘end-stopping’ (a defining feature of the‘hypercomplex’ cells of Hubel andWiesel). Such a cell responds strongly toan appropriately oriented bar of a certainlength. As the length of the bar isincreased—extending into an area sur-rounding the cell’s classical receptivefield—the response decreases until thecell ceases to fire. End-stopping in thesepredictive-coding units results from thecharacter of the natural images used intraining. As Rao and Ballard show direct-ly, their training images (natural scenesinhabited by animals, trees, rocks and soon) contain edges at different orienta-tions but at a preferred scale; short barsare much less likely to occur than longerones. During training, units in the sec-ond stage of the system come to expectsuch elongated edges and signal this tothe lower stage. The firing rate of first-

in terms of frequently occurring features,such as blobs of opposing polarity, orient-ed edges, curved line segments and so on.A significant fraction of all images can bedecomposed in terms of these more ele-mentary features. In other words, a clevervisual system that grew up in the naturalworld would not be surprised to find thesefeatures in real images. What is informa-tive, however, are deviations from thenorm, and in predictive coding, only theseunexpected features are signaled to the nextstage. Applied iteratively, this strategy leadsto the following hierarchical network (seeFig. 1 in Rao and Ballard): at each stage ofprocessing, the input from the previousstage is analyzed in terms of certain learnedfeatures. Each stage signals to the next thedifference between these expected imagefeatures and the actual image, and eachstage sends back to the one below it the

Christof Koch is at the Computation and NeuralSystems Program, California Institute ofTechnology, Pasadena, California 91125, USA.Tomaso Poggio is at CBCL in the Brain SciencesDepartment and in the AI Lab at MIT,Cambridge, Massachusetts 02142, USA.email: [email protected] [email protected]

Fig. 1. A purely feedfor-ward model10 canaccount for the availablequantitative data onview-tuned inferotempo-ral cells. The model is anhierarchical extension ofthe classical Hubel andWiesel approach ofbuilding complex cellsfrom simple cells. It is thefirst demonstration thathierarchical schemes ofbiological neurons canaccount quantitativelyfor both the physiologyand the psychophysics ofhigh-level object recogni-tion. The model consistsof sequences of layerswith linear (blue arrows)and nonlinear operations(green arrows), similarto logical AND and ORgates. These two types of operations respectively provide pattern specificity and transforma-tion invariance. The nonlinear MAX operation, similar to a winner-take-all over all inputs ofthe cell, is key to the model’s properties and is quite different from the basically linear sum-mation of inputs usually assumed for complex cells. (Riesenhuber and Poggio, unpublished).

Input image

Simple cells

Complex cells

Composite features

View-tuned cells

Predicting the visual world:silence is goldenChristof Koch and Tomaso Poggio

In predictive coding, only unexpected input features aresignaled to the next stage of processing. Rao and Ballard usethis approach to model extra-classical receptive field effects.

nature neuroscience • volume 2 no 1 • january 1999 9

[Koch & Poggio, 1999; cf. Fukishima, 1980;

Serre, Oliva, Poggio 2007; etc]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 34

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Koch, Christof, and Tomaso Poggio. "Predicting the visual world:Silence is golden." Nature neuroscience 2, no. 1 (1999): 9-10. © 1999.

Page 35: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Original image

Synthesizing Ventral Stream Metamers

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Source Unknown. All rights reserved. This content is

excluded from our Creative Commons license. For more

information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

35

Page 36: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Original imageModel

responses

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

Synthesized image

Synthesizing Ventral Stream Metamers

model

250

150

25

170

40

model

250

150

25

170

40

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Source Unknown. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative

Commons license. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

36

Page 37: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Original imageModel

responses

3.11.412.5

.

.

.

Synthesized image

Synthesizing Ventral Stream Metamers

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15

© Source Unknown. All rights reserved. This content is

excluded from our Creative Commons license. For more

information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.

37

Page 38: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Tuesday, August 25, 15 38

Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.Source: Cohen, Michael A., Daniel C. Dennett, and Nancy Kanwisher. "What is the bandwidthof perceptual experience?" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20, no. 5 (2016): 324-335.

Page 39: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Tuesday, August 25, 15 39

Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.Source: Cohen, Michael A., Daniel C. Dennett, and Nancy Kanwisher. "What is the bandwidthof perceptual experience?" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20, no. 5 (2016): 324-335.

Page 40: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

chance

Scaling (radius / eccentricity)

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 1.1

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

Model RF size (diam / eccentricity)

1.40.6 1.0 1.82.2

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 40

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 41: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

chance

Scaling (radius / eccentricity)

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 1.1

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

Model RF size (diam / eccentricity)

1.40.6 1.0 1.82.2

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 41

Page 42: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

chance

Scaling (radius / eccentricity)

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 1.1

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

Model RF size (diam / eccentricity)

1.40.6 1.0 1.82.2

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 42

Page 43: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

chance

Scaling (radius / eccentricity)

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1.0 1.1

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

Model RF size (diam / eccentricity)

1.40.6 1.0 1.82.2

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 43

Page 44: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 44

Page 45: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Retina

[Freeman & Simoncelli 2011, from Gattass et. al., 1981; Gattass et. al., 1988; Perry et. al., 1984]

RF sizes grow with eccentricity

Tuesday, August 25, 15 45

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 46: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[Allman & Kaas, 1971; Allman & Kaas, 1974; Gattass et.al., 1981; van Essen et.al., 1984; Maguire & Baizer, 1984; Burkhalter & van Essen, 1986; Gattass et.al., 1987; Desimone & Schein, 1987; Gattass et.al., 1988; Cavanaugh et. al., 2002]

MacaquePhysiology

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 46

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 47: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Extended presentation

Directed attention

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

chance

chance

Undirected attention Directed attention

r 2 = 0.91, 0.89 r 2 = 0.94, 0.91 r 2 = 0.95, 0.85 r 2 = 0.97, 0.95

r 2 = 0.91, 0.94 r 2 = 0.94, 0.98 r 2 = 0.95, 0.97 r 2 = 0.97, 0.87

a

b

region with the largest change (see Methods). Shaded region: same as panel a.

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 47

200 ms 400 ms

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Scaling (diameter / eccentricity) of receptive fields in synthesis model

Page 48: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

chance

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

1.0 1.0

0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50

0.3

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

Scaling (diameter / eccentricity) of receptive fields in synthesis model

Mid-ventralmodel

V1 model

r 2 = 0.91, 0.94 r 2 = 0.94, 0.95

r 2 = 0.95, 0.85 r 2 = 0.97, 0.90

Figure 3. Metamer experiment. Each panel shows, for an individual observer, the proportion of correct responses in the ABX task, as a function of the scaling parameter (ratio of receptive field diameter to eccentricity) of the model used to generate the stimuli. Dark gray points: mid-ventral model (see Fig. 2). Light gray points: V1 model (see Supplementary Fig. 2). Shaded region, 68% confidence interval obtained using bootstrapping. Gray horizontal line: Chance performance. Black lines: Performance of observer model with best fitting critical scaling and gain parameters (see Methods and Results). r2°]HS\LZ�MVY�[OL�MP[Z�PUKPJH[LK�H[�[OL�IV[[VT�VM�LHJO�WSV[�

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 48

= 0.91, 0.94 r 2 = 0.94, 0.95

r 2 = 0.95, 0.85 r 2 = 0.97, 0.90

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

r 2

Page 49: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

[Allman & Kaas, 1971; Allman & Kaas, 1974; Gattass et.al., 1981; van Essen et.al., 1984; Maguire & Baizer, 1984; Burkhalter & van Essen, 1986; Gattass et.al., 1987; Desimone & Schein, 1987; Gattass et.al., 1988; Cavanaugh et. al., 2002]

MacaquePhysiology

V4

V2

V1

S1

0.9

0.6

0.3

0.0Scal

ing

(dia

met

er /

ecce

ntric

ity)

S2 S3 S4 Average

“V2 model” “V1 model”Main experiment

Directed attentionExtended presentation

Tuesday, August 25, 15 49

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventralstream." Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195 -1201. © 2011.

Page 50: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Reading

b

c

a Figure 7. Effects of crowding on reading and searching. �H�°Two metamers, matched to the model responses of a page of text from the first paragraph of Herman Melville’s “Moby Dick”. Each metamer was synthesized using a different foveal location (the letter above each red dot). These locations are separated by the distance readers typically traverse between fixations49. In each metamer, the central word is largely preserved; farther in the periphery the text is letter-like but scrambled, as if printed with non-latin characters. Note that the boundary of readability in the first image roughly coincides with the location of the fixation in the second image. We emphasize that these are samples drawn from the set of images that are perceptually metameric; although they illustrate the kinds of distortions that result from the model, no single example represents “what an observer sees” in the periphery. (b) The notoriously hard-to-find “Waldo” (character with the red and white striped shirt) blends into the distracting background, and is only recognizable when we (or the model) look right at him. Cross-hairs surrounding each image indicate the location of the model fovea. (c) A soldier in Afghanistan wears sandy-stone patterned clothing to match the stoney texture of the street, and similarly blends into the background.

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]

Tuesday, August 25, 15 50

Page 51: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Camouflage

b

c

a Figure 7. Effects of crowding on reading and searching. �H�°Two metamers, matched to the model responses of a page of text from the first paragraph of Herman Melville’s “Moby Dick”. Each metamer was synthesized using a different foveal location (the letter above each red dot). These locations are separated by the distance readers typically traverse between fixations49. In each metamer, the central word is largely preserved; farther in the periphery the text is letter-like but scrambled, as if printed with non-latin characters. Note that the boundary of readability in the first image roughly coincides with the location of the fixation in the second image. We emphasize that these are samples drawn from the set of images that are perceptually metameric; although they illustrate the kinds of distortions that result from the model, no single example represents “what an observer sees” in the periphery. (b) The notoriously hard-to-find “Waldo” (character with the red and white striped shirt) blends into the distracting background, and is only recognizable when we (or the model) look right at him. Cross-hairs surrounding each image indicate the location of the model fovea. (c) A soldier in Afghanistan wears sandy-stone patterned clothing to match the stoney texture of the street, and similarly blends into the background.

[Freeman & Simoncelli, 2011]Tuesday, August 25, 15 51

© Nature. All rights reserved. This content is excluded from our Creative Commonslicense. For more information, see https://ocw.mit.edu/help/faq-fair-use/.Source: Freeman, Jeremy, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Metamers of the ventral stream."Nature neuroscience 14, no. 9 (2011): 1195-1201. © 2011.

Page 52: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Can we drive individual V2 neurons using local texture stimuli?

Top: synthetic textures, full modelBottom: “spectral noise” (matched only for “V1” statistics)

[Freeman, et. al. 2013]Tuesday, August 25, 15 52

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.

Source: Freeman, Jeremy, Corey M. Ziemba, David J. Heeger, Eero P. Simoncelli, and J. Anthony Movshon. "A functional andperceptual signature of the second visual area in primates." Nature neuroscience 16, no. 7 (2013): 974-981. © 2013.

Page 53: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

V1

Time from stimulus onset (ms)0 100 200

0

25

Firin

g R

ate

(ips)

0

15

0

100

25

0

25

0

35

V2

[Freeman, et. al. 2013]Tuesday, August 25, 15 53

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.

Source: Freeman, Jeremy, Corey M. Ziemba, David J. Heeger, Eero P. Simoncelli, and J. Anthony Movshon. "A functional andperceptual signature of the second visual area in primates." Nature neuroscience 16, no. 7 (2013): 974-981. © 2013.

Page 54: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

15% of V1 neuronssignificantly positivelymodulated

63% of V2 neuronssignificantly positivelymodulated

�� � ����

���

���

���

�� � ����

���

���

���

V1Q� ���

Prop

ortio

n of

cel

lsPr

opor

tion

of c

ells V2

Q� ����

Modulation index

[Freeman, et. al. 2013]Tuesday, August 25, 15 54

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.

Source: Freeman, Jeremy, Corey M. Ziemba, David J. Heeger, Eero P. Simoncelli, and J. Anthony Movshon. "A functional andperceptual signature of the second visual area in primates." Nature neuroscience 16, no. 7 (2013): 974-981. © 2013.

Page 55: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Texture category1 15

Firin

g R

ate

(ips)

0

40

0

25

0

200

30

0

25

0

30

0.23 0.33 0.23

-0.03 -0.05 0.05

[Freeman, et. al. 2013]Tuesday, August 25, 15 55

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.

Source: Freeman, Jeremy, Corey M. Ziemba, David J. Heeger, Eero P. Simoncelli, and J. Anthony Movshon. "A functional andperceptual signature of the second visual area in primates." Nature neuroscience 16, no. 7 (2013): 974-981. © 2013.

Page 56: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

1

0.5

Coherence

V1

V2d

V2vV3v

V3d

V4

V1

V2d

V2vV3v

V3d

V4

Subject 2,Right hemisphere

Subject 1,Right hemisphere

Texture (9 sec) Spectral Noise (9 sec)

. . .. . .

Time

[Freeman, et. al. 2013]Tuesday, August 25, 15 56

Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature.

Source: Freeman, Jeremy, Corey M. Ziemba, David J. Heeger, Eero P. Simoncelli, and J. Anthony Movshon. "A functional andperceptual signature of the second visual area in primates." Nature neuroscience 16, no. 7 (2013): 974-981. © 2013.

Page 57: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Diff

eren

t exe

mpl

ars

(sam

e sta

tistic

s)

Different families

Predicting discriminability

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Courtesy of Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Used with permission.Source: Ziemba, Corey M., Jeremy Freeman, J. Anthony Movshon, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Selectivity and tolerancefor visual texture in macaque V2." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 22 (2016): E3140-E3149.

57

[Ziemba, Freeman, Movshon, Simoncelli - unpublishd]

Page 58: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

0 3000

150

Time from stimulus onset (ms)

Firin

g ra

te (i

ps)

Example V1 neuron

[Ziemba, Freeman, Movshon, Simoncelli - unpublishd]

Anesthetized macaque• V1: 102 neurons • V2: 103 neurons

Stimuli presented for 100ms within a 4° aperture

20 repetitions each

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Courtesy of Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Used with permission.Source: Ziemba, Corey M., Jeremy Freeman, J. Anthony Movshon, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Selectivity and tolerance for visual texturein macaque V2." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 22 (2016): E3140-E3149.

58

Page 59: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

0 300Time from stimulus onset (ms)

0

150Fi

ring

rate

(ips

)

[Ziemba, Freeman, Movshon, Simoncelli - unpublishd]

Example V2 neuronAnesthetized macaque• V1: 102 neurons • V2: 103 neurons

Stimuli presented for 100ms within a 4° aperture

20 repetitions each

Tuesday, August 25, 15 59

Courtesy of Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Used with permission.Source: Ziemba, Corey M., Jeremy Freeman, J. Anthony Movshon, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Selectivity and tolerance for visual texturein macaque V2." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 22 (2016): E3140-E3149.

Page 60: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

V1n = 102

0

80

Variance across samples (%)

n = 103V2

0 80

0

80

Varia

nce

acro

ss fa

milie

s (%

)

Variance across exemplars (%)

V1n = 102

0

80

Variance across samples (%)

n = 103V2

0 80

0

80

Varia

nce

acro

ss fa

milie

s (%

)

Vari

ance a

cro

ss fa

mili

es (

%)

0 80

Tuesday, August 25, 15 60

Courtesy of Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Used with permission.Source: Ziemba, Corey M., Jeremy Freeman, J. Anthony Movshon, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Selectivity and tolerance for visualtexture in macaque V2." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 22 (2016): E3140-E3149.

Page 61: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

n = 1, 3, 10, 30, 100Family classification

Exemplar identification

Chance

0 1

0

1

V1 performance

V2 p

erfo

rman

ce

Decoding

Tuesday, August 25, 15 61

Courtesy of Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Used with permission.Source: Ziemba, Corey M., Jeremy Freeman, J. Anthony Movshon, and Eero P. Simoncelli. "Selectivity and tolerance for visual texturein macaque V2." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 22 (2016): E3140-E3149.

Page 62: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

Tuesday, August 25, 15

Portraits of Javier Portilla, Jeremy Freeman, Josh McDermott, Corey Ziemba

and Tony Movshon removed due to copyright restrictions. Please see the video.

62

Page 63: Probing Sensory Representations with Metameric Stimuli · Perceptual color matching experiment. 0 500 1000 1500 400 450 500. Power (arbitrary units) 550 600 650 700. Wavelength (nm)

MIT OpenCourseWarehttps://ocw.mit.edu

Resource: Brains, Minds and Machines Summer CourseTomaso Poggio and Gabriel Kreiman

The following may not correspond to a p articular course on MIT OpenCourseWare, but has beenprovided by the author as an individual learning resource.

For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: https://ocw.mit.edu/terms.