Presentation1

20
1 • Speaker Variable: Age Interactions with Gender Sociolinguistic competence in Children Today

Transcript of Presentation1

Page 1: Presentation1

1

• Speaker Variable: Age Interactions with Gender Sociolinguistic competence in Children

Today

Page 2: Presentation1

2

Sex-related variability: Differentiation of speech behavior between males and females related to physiological, neurological and biological factors.

Gender-related variability: Differentiation of speech behavior between males and females related to gender roles.

Key terms

Page 3: Presentation1

3

• Speaker Variable: Gender•Articles:

Eckert, 1998: “Gender and Sociolinguistic VariationEckert, 1988: “Adolescent social structure and the spread of linguistic change”Gal, 1997: “Peasant men can’t get wives”

Today

Page 4: Presentation1

4

Review:Traditional generalization regarding gender differences in

speech: “Women use fewer stigmatized and non-standard variants

than do men of the same social group in the same circumstances. “ Chambers, p. 102

Traditional explanations for the generalization

On what basis do authors relate women to “standard variants”? Data Studies

Page 5: Presentation1

5

The last word?Studies of Language Variation: Men and women in different cultures stand in different relations

to linguistic markets. Exclusion from workplace Obtain jobs where required to be “technicians of language” Given responsibilities for representing an organization

A clear delineation of gender roles in society becomes associated with clear distinctions in male and female use of sociolinguistic markers

Where gender roles also signal differences in social mobility, we may expect other factors to interact with gender: e.g., education, social class

Page 6: Presentation1

6

Eckert:Theoretical Goal:

--explain the mechanisms whereby phonological change spreads outward from urban areas and upward through the socioeconomic hierarchy

Research Goal:

--in a social network study, examine adolescent’s use of innovatory and conservative linguistic forms

Eckert 1988: Belten High Study vowels show flux in this dialect:

(ae) bad [Q] to raised variant [e] (uh) cut ranges from to backed [ç] (ay) right monopthongizes to [a˘]

advances with proximity to Detroitadvances with proximity to Detroitadvances with distance from Detroit

[√]

Page 7: Presentation1

7

Sociolinguistic Competence

A fluent speaker’s knowledge (largely tacit) of admissible variation in language, the types of social meaning that may be embedded in language, and the rules governing alternative structural choices.

•Language forms index social categories (genders, social classes, ages, regional origins, social networks) and stylistic registers

e.g., post-vocalic /r/. Variants: {[®], ø} ”transmitter to receiver”

e.g., (ae)-tensing. Variants: {[iQ], [e´], [Q]} ”cat”

e.g., (-ing)-alternation. Variants: {[IN], [In]} ”singin’”

•A comprehensive theory of language accounts for speakers’ knowledge of:

systemic potential (Is Alternation X part of my system?)

appropriateness (Is Alternation X effective or suitable in this context?)

occurrence (Is Alternation X done, and likely to be understood by another speaker?)

feasibility (Is Alternation X possible, given means of implementation available?)

region

region/class

register

Page 8: Presentation1

8

Milestones in Linguistic Development3 months: Linguistic PrecursorsChild’s physiology gets ready for speech (lowering of larynx,articulatory control) 5 months: BabblingEarly babbling (5-6 mos) “aaaaa”, “bababababa,” “pppppppp”Canonical babbling (7-10 mos) “mada,” “dele”

12 months: One-Word StageProtowords: consistent phonetic form used to refer to something (e.g., “yaya” juice)Holophrastic speech: A single word used to convey an entire utterance, e.g. “allgone”

18-24 months: Two-Word Stage Emergence of syntax, e.g. “Give ball”

>18 months: There’s no Three-Word stage!

Babytalk, Babytalk Child’s language acquisition is “The greatest intellectual feat

any one of us is ever required to perform.”

-- Leonard Bloomfield (1933)

Page 9: Presentation1

9

Categorical vs. Variable Features Linguistic forms may occur categorically, or show fluctuation Fluctuating forms occur with a likelihood or probability value (non-random,

learnable)

(largely) Categorical feature: In right-branching languages, determiners will appear to the left of the nouns they modify

(“the snowstorm”) Tensing of short-a (Northern Midwest dialects) ”cat”

(largely) Variable feature: Postvocalic /r/ deletion in informal settings “Park the car in Harvard yard.” Double-modal constructions “She might could want to come.” Can occur at any level of the grammar

Page 10: Presentation1

10

The issue

Acquisition of variation child learners expect language to contain and

employ socially meaningful variation Adults, not children, have been subject of

sociolinguistic studies of variation But, how does systematic variability develop?

What does it look like?

Page 11: Presentation1

12

Difficulties Obtaining Child Speech

Chevrot, et al. (2000), Roberts (2002) Developmental variation due to differences in physiological and cognitive

maturation Patterns reflecting imitation Distinguishing word-by-word (“lexical”) learning from rule-based learning Testing difficulties: attentional fluctuation (often resulting in insufficient

amounts of data to be representative of the speaker) Low intelligibility of utterances (less so for preschool age) Possibility of DIFFERENT stylistic or social goals (different form-meaning

mapping than in adults)

Page 12: Presentation1

13

Early Perspectives Labov (1964) Categorical features first Vernacular (=dialectal) forms predicted to be acquired LATE in

adolescence (10-12) Standard forms acquired later (around age 14) under contact with other

members of the linguistic community outside of their friends and family.

“By the age of six a child exposed to English will have constructed the grammar of his language. This does not mean that no further development of his knowledge of language is possible. ...We also learn certain less usual constructions of the language. These exceptional or marked patterns of the language are not taken to be part of the core grammar of the language, they belong to the marked periphery of the grammar and may be acquired later. The native speaker will also have to learn all of the social or cultural conventions associated with his language, for instance, that certain words belong to a very high style whereas others are informal. These conventions are not part of the grammar, they belong to the more general domain of human behavior.” (Haegeman 2005, p. 17)

Page 13: Presentation1

14

Crosslinguistic Evidence: studies of acquisition of variability in children

For what ages has systematic variation been found? Fischer (1958) British English (t,d), (-ing): social variation ages 3-10 Roberts (1994, 1996, 1997) American English (Philadelphia variety), (t,d): both, ages

3-4 Díaz-Campos (2005) Venezuelan Spanish, intervocalic-d deletion: both, ages 3-5 Sankoff and Blondeau (2006) Montreal French trilled /r/ vs. uvular /R/: both, ages 3;6-

4;11 Purcell (1984) Hawaiian Creole English, various variables: both, 5-12 Chevrot, Beaud & Varga (2000) Southeastern French /R/: both, ages 6-7 Romaine (1978) Scottish English trilled /r/: social and stylistic, ages 6-8 Fischer (1958) British English (t,d): stylistic variation ages 10-11 Reid (1978) Scottish English glottal stop, (-ing) alternation: stylistic, age 11

Page 14: Presentation1

15

Roberts (1994,6,7) 16 children ages 3;2-4;11 obtained large amounts of data; a range of

styles (6-13 sessions/child) deletion of final (-t,d) in consonant clusters adult’s patterns: (most deletion to least

deletion):Grammatical constraint:

monomorphemes, e.g., next [nEks] semi-weak verbs, e.g., lost, slept [las]

[slEp] regular past tense forms missed [mIs]

Phonological constraint: delete more for a following

consonant>vowel>pause “past tense”>”past us”> “ran

past.”

American English (Philadelphia area)

•Findings: •Preschoolers similar to adults:

–phonological constraints mastered

•Rule-based not word-based pattern:

–semiweak verbs treated differently from adult semiweak forms

•Social constraints less-well mastered

Page 15: Presentation1

16

Díaz-Campos (2005) 36 children in 2 cohorts: ages 42-53 mos

(3;6 - 4;5) and 54-71 mos (4;6 - 5;11) Working (WC) and middle (MC) social

classes Targeted 2 speech styles Do children's productions fluctuate in a

manner showing sensitivity to formality? Examined interaction between

socioeconomic class and age (to tease apart developmental effects)

intervocalic /d/-spirantization e.g., boda [boDa]

Findings: Again, preschoolers similar to adults Both social classes deleted more in

informal than formal styles

Venezuelan Spanish

•Deletion levels in younger cohort suggest this is not a maturational, but a sociolinguistic effect:

WC=28% MC=10% •Concludes that preschoolers are showing adult-like command of a variable linguistic feature.

Page 16: Presentation1

17

Chevrot, Beaud & Varga (2000) 60 children ages 6-7, 10-12 Exp. 1: Studied deletion of post-

consonantal, word-final /R/ (e.g., sucre, vinaigre, coffre)

Exp. 2: Pseudoword experiment tests rule- vs. lexical-based learning

“bydeincre,” “maullopre” Factors tested: age, formality of situation,

phonological environment Findings (Exp. 1): 6-7 yr olds deleted more than 10-12 yr olds more deletions in informal than in formal

style Following phonological environment is

most crucial predictor of deletion Older children show a stronger stylistic

“adaptation”

Southeastern French

Findings (Exp. 2): written prompts lead to conservation

of /R/, whereas oral learning associated with style-based deletions commensurate with Exp. 1

Conclusions: phonological constraint emerges prior to age 6;

sociolinguistic constraint emerges around age 6-7

lexical learning ruled out

Page 17: Presentation1

18

Limitations

•Descriptive coverage (Romance and Germanic)

•Phonological variation only

•Lexical learning not completely ruled out

Page 18: Presentation1

19

Insights from Rohan home environment:

English/Sinhalese/Jamaican Creole first words 10-12 mos (“mama,”

“dada,” “amma”) 68 words, 14 mos earliest signs of phonological

variation, 17 mos

(17 mos.)

Page 19: Presentation1

20

Insights from Rohan(1) postvocalic-rheart, harbor, mirror*, shark “Amma says /ha˘t/; you [Mommy] say /ha®t/”

(23 mos.) “Trevor is at the ‘[ha˘ b´]’ Is it ‘/ha®˘b´®/’,

Mommy?” (23 mos.) “mirror” [mi®´] “shark” [Sa˘k] (“babytalk style”, 26 mos. to

present)

(2) tensing of short-(i) (Eastern US) “locomotive” [ti˘v] generalized to “detective”

*deletion in syllable-initial contexts prohibited (mi-ROR)

(36 mos.)

Page 20: Presentation1

21

Current Perspective Children do acquire socially-influenced variable patterns prior to

adolescence Children become socially competent language users early--as they acquire

language Simultaneity of acquisition of variable and categorical features makes it

difficult to defend a view that sociolinguistic competence vis a vis acquisition of variation is layered on top of or follows “basic acquisition.”

Adult-modeled variation may be instructive for learning styles