Posture Health Alexander Technique

download Posture Health Alexander Technique

of 48

Transcript of Posture Health Alexander Technique

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    1/48

    Alexander Technique scientific briefing

    THE ROLE OF THE POSTURAL REFLEXES

    in

    HEALTH AND WELL-BEING

    Gerald Foley

    DRAFT

    11 July 2009

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    2/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    3/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    4/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    5/48

    3

    SECTION 1: BACKGROUNDUnderstanding of the nervous system began to grow rapidly from the middle of the

    19th century. Charles Sherringtons experimental studies during the 1890s resulted in

    some of the key scientific breakthroughs but it was his master-work The integrative

    action of the nervous system published in 1906 which synthesised what had gonebefore and created the still-existing framework of modern neuroscience. Rudolph

    Magnus, taking inspiration from Sherrington, began work on the postural reflexes in

    1908 and produced the definitive study of their functioning in 1924.

    To discuss either reflex and posture without defining them is inviting trouble. Bothwords are so encrusted with popular impressions that hardly any two people would

    agree on what exactly they mean by them. The following discussion is not an attempt

    to lay down the law on what these terms should mean. It is simply to clarify from

    the beginning how they are used in this paper.

    Charles Sherrington and Rudolph Magnus

    These two distinguished scientists share the common fate of many great pioneers in

    their subjects. Their work defined the territory and became so widely and deeply

    taken for granted that people no longer refer to their original contribution. As thispaper is specifically concerned with their findings on the postural reflexes, it is worth

    putting their findings into context by giving a brief account of their careers.

    Charles Sherrington

    Sherrington was born in 1857. A bright student, he qualified as a member of the

    Royal College of Surgeons in 1884 and obtained a medical degree from CambridgeUniversity in 1885.

    In 1891, he became the Physician-Superintendent of an animal research centre, calledthe Brown Institute in the University of London. In the four years he spent there he

    produced a stream of research papers which began to build the foundation on whichmodern neurology is based.1He was elected a fellow of the Royal Society in 1893

    and became Professor of Physiology at Liverpool University in 1895. This was one

    of the most productive periods of Sherringtons career. He became professor of

    Physiology at Oxford in 1913 and remained there until his retirement in 1936 at the

    age of 79.

    He published a total of 320 scientific papers in his career covering nearly every aspectof mammalian nervous functioning. He identified the function of the synapse in the

    nervous system, and coined the name; he found that reflexes must be regarded asintegrated actions of the total organism, not isolated activities of groups of muscles as

    was believed at the time; and carried out a variety of experimental studies on the

    postural functions of the nervous system.

    Additionally he maintained a broad range of cultural interests. His last book was a

    life of the 16thcentury French physician Jean Fernel whom Sherrington saw as a key

    figure in the emergence of the scientific attitude. This was published as The

    Endeavour of Jean Fernel in 1946. The following year The integrative action of the

    nervous system was republished as a tribute to Sherrington on his ninetieth birthday.

    1Cohen (1958)p7

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    6/48

    4

    He contributed a long new introduction which showed how little the intervening forty

    years had dimmed his interest and intellectual capacities.

    He became President of the Royal Society in 1920, received his knighthood in 1922,

    the Order of Merit in 1924, and was awarded the Nobel Prize in 1932. In addition he

    received numerous international honours. He died in 1952.

    Rudolph Magnus

    It was Rudolph Magnus, rather than Sherrington himself, who carried out the defining

    studies on the postural reflexes. Magnus was born in Germany in 1873 and studied in

    the University of Heidelberg where he qualified as a medical doctor with aspecialisation in pharmacology in 1898. He then took up a position in the

    pharmacology department in the University and became an associate professor. At

    that stage in his career, he was primarily interested in the physiological effects of

    drugs and kept in touch with the rapid developments in physiology then taking place.

    He attended the Third International Physiology Congress in Berne in 1895 where he

    witnessed an experiment by Sherrington.2

    Three years later at another internationalphysiology congress, this time in Cambridge, he saw an experiment by Sherrington

    which he described as elegant.3 In the meantime, his own reputation in

    pharmacology was growing and he presented some of the results of his research intothe effects of various drugs on intestinal functioning at an international congress in

    1904.

    Like many of the major scientists of his day, he had a broad classical education and

    was particularly interested in Goethe and Kant. Round this time, he gained access tothe collection of Goethes scientific experimental equipment at the Goethe Museum in

    Weimar and persuaded the museum authorities to allow him to repeat the experimentson which Goethes had based his theory of colour. Arising from this experimental

    work and his research into the Goethe archives, Magnus delivered a series of lectures

    on Goethe as a scientist at the University of Heidelberg. These appeared in book form

    in 1906 and were published in an English translation in 1949.4

    Magnus remained interested in philosophical issues all his life and was particularly

    curious about how the nervous system provides us with a priori or innate

    knowledge. This was, in fact, to be the subject of a lecture at Stanford University,

    never delivered because of his death, but which was published some years later.5 In

    1908, he visited Sherrington in Liverpool and spent some time working with him in

    his laboratory on a problem of muscle excitation. This visit changed the course of

    Magnus life.

    His biographer comments:

    he could not have anticipated that this would be the start of a long

    series of investigations on posture for which he would gain lasting

    international recognition.6

    On his return to the University of Utrecht, where he had just been appointed Professor

    of Pharmacology, he set up a programme to investigate the neurophysiology of

    2O. Magnus (2002)p51

    3Ibid. p66

    4

    Ibid. p1455Magnus (1930)p97

    6O. Magnus (2002)p143

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    7/48

    5

    posture. It turned out to be a task which occupied the greater part of his scientific

    talents for the rest of his life. Sherrington, despite his own interest in the subject, wascontent to leave the bulk of the research on posture to Magnus and his colleagues in

    Utrecht.

    In his contacts with Magnus, Sherrington had clearly converted him to the view that

    the question of posture was not only more complex than it looked at first sight, butthat it also opened up fruitful areas of investigation into the overall functioning of the

    neuromuscular system. When presenting the results of his work some eighteen years

    later, Magnus had this to say about why he had chosen posture as his primary research

    subject

    Movement affords many points of attack for research because by

    movement, changes in the condition of the body or its parts occur,

    which attract the attention of the observer and can be recorded and

    measured. This is not the case when posture is studied so that our

    desire for causality is not stimulated, and we therefore do not

    immediately suppose that active processes are at work. Inconsequence of this the physiology of posture is of relatively recent

    date and many facts to be described in these lectures have been

    discovered by still living scientists among whom Sherrington must be

    named in the first place. The chief result of these investigations is that

    posture is an active process, and is the result of the cooperation of a

    great number of reflexes, many of which have a tonic character.7

    The First World War disrupted research activities as well as communication between

    the two men and it was the mid-1920s before the full fruits of Magnus work were

    made public. Not long afterwards, in 1927, he died unexpectedly at the age of 54

    while on holiday in Switzerland. Although it was widely expected he would benominated for the Nobel Prize in that year it is not awarded posthumously.

    What is a reflex?

    Even among scientists, the term reflex is used in a variety of ways. Because of the

    lack of an agreed definition, some authors have even wondered whether the

    distinction between reflex and voluntary has any remaining scientific justification.8

    But if care is taken to be clear about what is meant by it, the term reflex can fill a

    need.

    For the purpose of the present paper, Sherringtons definition is adopted. In his

    introduction to the 1947 re-publication of The integrative action of the nervoussystem he wrote:

    The behaviour of the spider is reported to be entirely reflex; but reflex

    action, judging by what we can sample of it, would go little way

    toward meeting the life of external relation of a horse or cat or dog,

    still less of ourselves. As life develops it would seem that in the field

    of external relation conscious behaviour tends to replace reflex, and

    conscious acts to bulk larger and larger. Along with this change, and

    indeed as part of it, would seem an increased role for habit. Habit

    7Magnus (1926a)p531

    8Prochazka (2000)

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    8/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    9/48

    7

    we do something deliberately, we unconsciously bring into play a huge number of

    reflex responses, varying from subtle balancing adjustments in the tone, or tension, inthe muscles in various parts of the body these are referred to as tonic reflexes

    through to the quick and often effortful movements of limbs that take place, for

    example, when we towel ourselves vigorously after a shower or make a dash for a

    bus. The important point is that whatever deliberate action we perform and no matterhow we concentrate on it, the details of the associated supporting and compensatory

    muscular contractions and releases happen reflexly, independently of any conscious

    input from the brain.

    It is thus paradoxical that although we readily take responsibility for our conscious

    acts we do not know exactly how we manage to do them. More interestingly, it is the

    superstructure of consciousness which enables humans unlike horses, dogs, and still

    less spiders to acquire habits that distort and interfere with the working of their

    reflexes and undermine the functioning of their own selves. The way in which human

    learned behaviour interacts, often detrimentally, with the relationship between the

    deliberate and the reflex is a theme which is developed in the later parts of this paper.It is also worth making clear that Sherrington had no sympathy with the reductionist

    view that all activity is reflex, simply the result of automatic neurological responses to

    external or internal stimuli. Although the reflexes provide the essential underpinning

    for all the bodys activities, for Sherrington the volitional decision-making mind

    occupies the primary role in human behaviour.

    The question of posture

    The word posture is also used in widely different ways. Many of its meanings are

    associated with deliberately assumed ways of holding the body. Walking about,

    stiffly balancing a book on the head, used to be a common way of training youngpeople in what was supposed to be good posture. In this paper, the word posture

    refers to the natural, or innate, relationship of the parts of the body to each other in

    sitting, standing or walking; it is perhaps best approximated by the old-fashioned

    word carriage.

    The question of posture, at first sight, seems an unlikely focal point for some of the

    major advances in neuroscience made in the early decades of the 20th century. Yet

    from an early date Sherrington had seen how the maintenance of posture was just as

    complex and demanding of the nervous system as movement.

    As he said:

    much of the reflex reaction expressed by the skeletal musculature ispostural. The bony and other levers of the body are maintained in

    certain attitudes both in regard to the horizon, to the vertical, and to

    one anotherInnervation and co-ordination are as fully demanded for

    the maintenance of a posture as for the execution of a movement. 12

    Far from representing a fixed and rigid configuration of the muscles, posture displays

    them in action in patterns as dynamic, if not so immediately evident as those of

    movement.

    12Sherrington (1906)p339

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    10/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    11/48

    9

    Although the experimental work was carried out on animals, the neurological

    structures and basic functioning of the nervous system is similar in all vertebrates.Magnus makes a variety of references in his published work to ways in which his

    findings shed light on human functioning. The physiologist Berta Bobath, for

    example, relied on his findings in developing her methods of diagnosis and treatment

    of children suffering from cerebral palsy and related postural abnormalities as a resultof brain damage.14

    The vestibular apparatus

    The balance, or equilibrium, of the body is intimately related to posture. The term

    static equilibrium is often used to refer to when the body is retaining its position

    relative to the force of gravity. The body must also be able to retain its balance when

    its parts are moved relative to each other and when the whole body is in motion; this

    is usually referred to as dynamic equilibrium. Both of these aspects of balance arelargely controlled by the vestibular apparatus.

    Since the vestibular apparatus was the subject of many of Magnus experiments, it is

    worth outlining briefly what is involved. The inner ear houses a maze of winding

    passages, collectively called the labyrinth. The labyrinth is divided into three areas,

    the vestibule and, projecting above and backwards from it, the three curved ducts

    known as the semicircular canals; and the cochlea which contains the hearing

    receptors. Within the vestibule are two sacs, the utricle and saccule, known as the

    otolith organs. The utricle and the saccule, together with the semicircular canals, are

    known as the organs for equilibrium and make up the vestibular apparatus.

    The otolith organs provide information on the tilt of the head. The walls of both the

    saccule and the utricle contain a small thickened area called the macula. Each of the

    two maculae, which are set at right angles to each other, supports a set of tiny hair

    cells. The hair cells are bathed in a gelatinous layer called the otolithic membrane inwhich is embedded a layer of dense calcium carbonate crystals called otoliths otolith

    literally means ear-stone.

    When the head is in its normal position with the gaze horizontal, the hair cells in the

    utricle are positioned horizontally and those in the saccule are positioned vertically.

    When the head is then tilted forward, backward or sideways, the otolithic membrane

    lags slightly behind the movement of the head. This causes the hair cells to bend,resulting in the transmission of impulses through the utricular and saccular nerves to

    the vestibular branch of the vestibulocochlear nerve.15 The otolith organs, in this way,act as a three-dimensional system, a complex type of spirit level, for monitoring the

    tilt of the head from moment to moment.The three semicircular canals provide information on movements of the head. They

    are set at right angles to each other in three planes and consist of ducts filled with afluid called endolymph. One end of each canal has a small expanded or dilated area

    called the ampulla. In each ampulla, there is a ridge or swelling upwards from the

    base of the canal called the crista. On top of the crista, a group of hair cells projects

    upwards and is covered by a small mass of gelatinous material called the cupula.

    When the head moves, the movement of the cupula, because of its inertia, is delayed

    slightly compared to that of the head. This drags the hair cells on the crista out of

    14Bobath (1985)p

    15Davson (1990)p678

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    12/48

    10

    their resting position, causing them to generate nerve impulses. Smooth movement is

    insufficient to stimulate the semicircular canals; there must be a change in the rate ofmovement, either acceleration or deceleration. These nerve impulses are collected in

    the ampullary nerves and are also fed into the vestibular branch of the

    vestibulocochlear nerve.

    Magnus findings

    Magnus and his colleagues published numerous scientific papers as their research

    proceeded so that the scientific world was kept aware of their progress. Their detailed

    final report covering the findings of the whole research project was published in

    German under the title Korperstellung in 1924. Although this volume was nottranslated into English until 1987, this was not the problem it would be today since

    most scientists of Magnus time were literate in German, which had been regarded as

    the language of science in the 19th century. In 1924, Magnus scientific reputation

    was already high as a result of his published papers and with the publication of the

    final report his full research results would have been accessible to all the leadingneuro-physiologists around the world.

    The first major public presentation of Magnus work in English was in the 1925

    Croonian Lecture at the Royal Society in London, with Sherrington, who was at that

    time President of the Royal Society, in the chair. It was probably the most prestigious

    setting of the time for the public announcement of important scientific work. Magnus

    also presented his findings in 1926 in the two Cameron Prize Lectures in the

    University of Edinburgh, both of which were reprinted in The Lancet in the same

    year.

    As a starting point for his Royal Society lecture, Magnus identified four aspects of

    posture which he felt needed to be examined in detail. He termed them, partialproblems and listed them as reflex standing,normal distribution of tone, attitude,and

    the righting function. This division is, of course, artificial since in the intact animal

    all these aspects of posture are present and interacting all the time. But considering

    them separately provides additional insights into what is involved in the totality of

    posture, both when it is working properly and when it malfunctions.

    Reflex standing

    If an animal is to stand normally, the muscles used in standing must be able to

    maintain the necessary tone. Magnus found that spinal animals, those in which thewhole brain had been extirpated, were capable of complex movements when they

    were suspended in an upright position. They were, for example, able to make runningand walking movements when the pads of their feet were stimulated, showing that

    these actions are controlled in the spinal cord. But these animals collapsed if they

    were placed in a standing position.

    Magnus remarks:

    The centres of the spinal cord can indeed cause and regulate very

    complicated combinations of movements, but they are unable to give

    the muscles that steady and enduring tone which is necessary for

    simple standing.16

    16Magnus (1925)p341

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    13/48

    11

    Ed When more of the brain was left in place by making the cut further up the

    brainstem, somewhere between the medulla oblongata and the foremost part of themidbrain, the animal was able to stand. But it did so in a state of what is called

    decerebrate rigidity. The antigravity muscles, the extensors of the limbs, the

    extensors of the back, the elevators of the neck and tail, and the closing muscles of the

    jaws, had abnormally high tone, whereas their antagonists, the flexors, had virtuallynone. The overall result was that, although the animal could stand if it were placed

    on its feet, the distribution of tone was abnormal and the animals posture was stiff

    and distorted.17

    Magnus makes the additional comment:

    The stimuli inducing the enduring tone of the standing muscles in

    decerebrate rigidity arise from different sources, the proprioceptive

    sense organs in the contracted muscles themselves playing the most

    prominent role.18

    He is pointing out here that excessive muscle tone, once it has developed, has atendency to become self-sustaining. This is because when there is excess tension inthe muscles their own internal sensing organs, their proprioceptors, are stimulated to

    produce signals to the nervous system which result in that state being maintained.

    Normal distribution of tone

    In normal standing, the extensor and flexor muscles have the level of tone required to

    keep them in balance with each other. Magnus found that this occurred when the cut

    in the brain was made at such a level that the thalamus was included, producing whatthe researchers called a mid-brain animal or a thalamus animal. In these creatures, he

    found that both the distribution of muscle tone and the standing posture were more orless normal.

    He says:

    In the thalamus animal the extensors of the limbs just have sufficient

    tension to balance the weight of the body against gravity, so that every

    force tending to raise or lower the body can easily move it in one or

    the other direction.19

    This was a persuasive experimental demonstration that in the case of these animals

    normal standing, including gentle movement around the equilibrium position, even

    though it involves complex interactions throughout the whole skeletal muscle system,

    was working as a wholly reflex activity and was able to take place in the absence of

    the cerebral cortex.

    Attitude

    Magnus uses the term attitudeto refer to how the parts of the body relate positionally

    to each other. The attitudinal reflexescome into action when the position or the tone

    in one part changes in relation to the rest of the body. An example of this is when a

    part of the body is braced or stiffened. As this happens, the attitudinal reflexes bring

    about compensatory changes in the rest of the body so that the muscular systemremains in an overall state of balanced tone.

    17

    Ibid. p34118Ibid. p341

    19Ibid. p342

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    14/48

    12

    Magnus remarks:

    It is noteworthy that these reflexes are most easily evoked from the

    foremost part of the body, from the head, in which the teleceptive sense

    organs are situated, so that distance stimuli influencing the position of

    the head can in this way also impress different attitudes upon the

    whole body. One can, in fact, in the decerebrate animal, by simply

    changing the position of the head, give to the body a great number of

    attitudes, resembling closely the normal harmonious attitudes of the

    intact animal.20

    By the teleceptive sense organs, Magnus means those sense organs which detectobjects at a distance, as opposed to the proprioceptors which detect changes inside the

    body. The eyes are the most important teleceptors in humans and many otheranimals; but hearing and the sense of smell are equally or more important in others.

    He is pointing out that when these teleceptive organs detect an object it tends to

    evoke changes in the position of the head; we look towards the object the eyes have

    detected or seek the source of the smell. The change in the position of the head,through the medium of the vestibular system and the neck proprioceptors, brings

    about reflex changes in the muscles in the rest of the body.

    As Magnus describes it:

    It is possible, by giving to the head different positions, to change the

    distribution of tone in the whole body musculature The most striking

    reactions appear in the extensors of the limbs and in the neck muscles.

    The effects observed are the result of combined reflexes from the

    labyrinths and from proprioceptive neck receptors, and in this way,

    it is possible to impress upon the whole body different adapted

    attitudes by changing only the position of the head.21

    As an example of the attitudinal reflexes at work in an intact animal, Magnus

    instances a cat standing in the middle of a room. A mouse runs along the foot of one

    wall, attracting the cats attention. The simple act of turning its head to watch themouse evokes a series of attitudinal reflexes which automatically put the cat into a

    posture of readiness, with the weight on three paws, and the other slightly lifted andready to move. The result is that if a signal to pounce were to come from its cortex,

    the cat is perfectly poised for action.

    Magnus description of what is taking place here is a model of meticulous observation

    and analysis:

    The distribution of excitability in the motor centres of the spinal cord

    is rearranged by the turning of the neck, so that, if for some reason

    running movements begin, the limb which has no static function will

    always make the first step. In this way, the moving mouse impresses

    on the cat, through the mediation of tonic neck reflexes an attitude, by

    which the cat is focussed towards the mouse and made ready for

    movement. The only thing the cat has to do is to decide: to jump or not

    to jump; all other things have been prepared beforehand reflexly under

    20Ibid. p342

    21Magnus (1926a)p534

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    15/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    16/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    17/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    18/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    19/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    20/48

    18

    multitudinous inputs from the proprioceptors and the exteroceptors, those organs

    sensing the external world, was an extreme example.

    This is how Magnus put it in the Croonian Lecture in 1925:

    The lower centres for the muscles of the different parts of the body are

    arranged segmentally in the spinal cord; the higher centres in thebrainstem put them into combined action and in this way govern the

    posture of the animal as a whole. We have here a very good example

    of what Sherrington has called the integrative action of the nervous

    system. And integration is particularly necessary in the case of

    posture, because nervous excitations arising from different sense

    organs are flowing towards the postural centres in the brain-stem, and

    must be combined so that a harmonising effect will result. 36

    InBody Posturehe summarises his conclusions:

    The result of the present study is that in the brain stem, from the upper

    cervical cord to the midbrain, lies a complicated central nervousapparatus that governs the entire body posture in a coordinated

    manner. It unites the musculature of the whole body in a common

    performance37

    But although he was happy that he had identified this area of the lower brain as the

    location of the key nerve centres necessary for the normal functioning of the postural

    reflexes, he saw this conclusion as the starting point for further investigation. As hesaid:

    at least a beginning has been made with the anatomic-physiologic

    disentangling of the central apparatus for the body posture. Apart

    from establishing the general arrangements of centers and pathways invarious parts of the brain stem, it has been possible to ascertain the

    function (or a part of the function) of at least one anatomically known

    nucleus, and to determine the anatomic position of the centers for a

    few physiologic functions.38

    The recognised that the amount of work required to identify what was happening at a

    detailed level was huge. He outlined the task in the following words:

    For the majority of reflexes it is not yet known what anatomically

    known structures (nuclei), localized physiologically in specific regions

    are involved, in which anatomically known pathways the afferent and

    efferent excitation runs in the central nervous system, and by whichneurones these pathways are formed. For many reflexes it is still not

    known whether the pathways run on one or both sides, whether and

    where they cross, etc. There is, therefore, much work to be done

    before the structure of the central apparatus for body posture will be

    known in all details39

    36Magnus (1925)p340

    37

    Magnus (1924)p65338Ibid. p676

    39Ibid. p655

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    21/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    22/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    23/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    24/48

    22

    But the activity of the postural reflexes is not restricted to the aftermath of a clearly

    defined phasic action. Human activities shade from one into the other, sometimesbringing large swathes of muscles into vigorous activity, sometimes involving no

    more than minor movements of body parts or simply a local change in muscular tone.

    The postural reflexes play a continuing background role, maintaining a tendency to

    bring the musculature back into its natural balanced state. If the neuromusculaturehad no such reference, or default, state to which it automatically had a tendency to

    return, there would be nothing to prevent neuromuscular patterns of tension remaining

    as residues of phasic activities, and even accumulating to a level at which the overall

    functioning of the organism becomes impaired.

    Continually recalibrating the senses

    Magnus pointed out that the postural reflexes have another critically important role,that of constantly recalibrating the senses. This is necessary because, in the course of

    any particular phasic action, not only is the normal resting relationship between the

    body parts changed, but the bodys relationship to the external world is also altered.Magnus says that the postural reflexes restore the normal or baseline conditions to

    which the exteroceptive and proprioceptive sense organs refer.

    In his conclusion to the second Cameron Lecture he puts it this way:

    By the action of the subcortical mechanisms described in these lectures

    the different sense organs are always brought into the normal relation

    with the external world. For the nerve endings in the skin this is

    accomplished by the above described attitudinal and righting reflexes.

    In the case of the eyes a very complicated reflex mechanism has been

    developed differing in various species of animals, which regulates the

    position of the eyes in relation to the environment. Here alsolabyrinthine and neck reflexes come into play.45

    He then adds some further explanatory words, re-emphasising the importance of this

    function of the postural reflexes in continually recalibrating the sensory organs as the

    body performs its activities, whether voluntary or reflex:

    The result of all these arrangements is that the sense organs are

    righted in relation to the external world, so that every sensory

    impression, before being transmitted to the cortex cerebri, has already

    acquired a certain special condition (local sign) depending on the

    previous righting function acting on the whole body or parts of it. In

    this way the action of involuntary brain-stem centres plays a veryimportant part in conscious activities, especially as regards spatial

    sensations.46

    No one knows how Magnus would have carried forward his work on the postural

    reflexes. But some tantalising hints of how he might have developed some of his

    ideas beyond those set down inBody Posture are contained in the draft of a lecture he

    was due to give in Stanford University in 1928. His death prevented the finalisationand delivery of the lecture but Stanford University published it in a book in 1930.

    45Magnus (1926b)p588

    46Ibid. p588

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    25/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    26/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    27/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    28/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    29/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    30/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    31/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    32/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    33/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    34/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    35/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    36/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    37/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    38/48

    36

    Alexander never felt that there was anything esoteric about his approach; he strongly

    believed that it should be integrated into normal medical practice and he had a varietyof prominent medical friends who publicly supported him. He was particularly

    opposed to quackery and fringe medical cults and was extremely pleased that Dewey

    emphasised the scientific nature of his Technique.

    In his introduction to one of Alexanders books, Dewey wrote

    whilst any theory or principle must ultimately be judged by its

    consequences in operation, whilst it must be verified experimentally by

    observation of how it works, yet in order to justify a claim to be

    scientific, it must provide a method for making evident and observable

    what the consequences are; and this method must be such as to afford

    a guarantee that the observed consequences actually flow from the

    principle. And I unhesitatingly assert that, judged by this standard

    Mr Alexanders teaching is scientific in the strictest sense of the

    word.73

    Alexander continued teaching and training teachers in his methods until his death in1955 just short of his eighty-seventh birthday. His Technique continues to be widely

    taught and is particularly strongly supported in the performing arts.

    What did Alexander discover?

    By the early 1920s, Alexander had become a well-known and successful teacher of

    his Technique in the USA and Britain. When he learned, in 1925, that Magnus had

    identified a central apparatusin the brainstem which controlled posture, it seems to

    have crystallised his thinking about his own methods. He began to refer to the head-

    neck relationship as the primary control of the proper use of body. His first

    employment of the term appears to have been in a lecture he gave to the Child-StudySociety in February 1925. He also uses the term central control in this lecture butsubsequently primary control was his preferred usage.

    In the course of this lecture, Alexander says:

    Regarding the central control: in the technique I am using, it will

    interest you to know that during the past fifteen years, Magnus has

    worked to explain the scientific significance as has been brought to

    our notice recently by Sir Charles Sherrington in connection with

    that very control which I have been using for twenty five years. The

    direction of the head and neck being of primary importance, he found,

    as I found, that if we get the right direction from this primary control,the control of the rest of the organism is a simple matter.74

    Alexander reiterated his belief that he and Magnus were talking about the same thingon various other occasions. In a letter dated 9 July 1932, published in the British

    Medical Journal, he challenged medical men to submit his procedures to whatever

    tests as are consistent with their knowledge of physiology, anatomy and

    psychology.

    He goes on to say:

    73Alexander (1923) pxxvii

    74Alexander (1995)p148

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    39/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    40/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    41/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    42/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    43/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    44/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    45/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    46/48

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    47/48

    45

    REFERENCESALEXANDER, F. M.(1923) Constructive Conscious Control of the Individual- Mouritz, London2004 edition--- (1932) The use of the self- Gollancz, London,(1985 edition)

    --- (1946) The universal constant in living- Mouritz, London(2000 edition)

    --- (1995) Articles and lectures- Mouritz, London

    --- (1995) Articles and lectures - edited by J.M.O.Fischer- Mouritz, London

    BARLOW, W. (1978) More talk of Alexander: aspects of the Alexander Technique - Mouritz,London2005BLAND, J., BOUSHET, D. The cervical spine, from anatomy and physiology to clinical care

    BERTHOZ, A., GRAF, W.VIDAL, P.(1992)

    BLOCH, M.(2004) F.M. The life of Frederick Matthias Alexander- Little, Brown, London

    BOBATH, B.(1985) Abnormal postural reflex activity caused by brain lesions(3rd ed)- Heinemann

    Physiotherapy, London

    BRODAL, P.(1998) The Central Nervous System: structure and function- Oxford University Press,OxfordBROWN, J. J. Cervical contribution to balance: cervical vertigo in BERTHOZ, A., VIDAL, P.

    GRAF, W.(1992)

    CARRINGTON, W. (1994) Foundations of human well-being. The work of Professor Magnus andthe Alexander Technique- STAT Books, London

    COGHILL, G. E. (1929) Anatomy and the problem of behaviour - Cambridge University Press,Cambridge(1963 edition)COHEN, Birkenhead(1958) Sherrington: physiologist, philosopher and poet- Liverpool University

    Press, Liverpool

    DART, R. A.(1996) Skill and poise- STAT Books, London

    DAVSON, H.(1990) Physiology of the eye- Macmillan, London, 5th ed.

    HERRICK, C. J.(1949) George Ellett Coghill: naturalist and philosopher- University of Chicago

    Press, ChicagoHOMOLA, S.(1999) Inside chiropractic: a patient's guide- Prometheus Books, New York

    LEE, G. S.(1920) The Ghost in the White House- E.P.Dutton, New York

    MAGNUS, O. (2002) Rudolph Magnus: physiologist and pharmacologist 1873-1927 - Royal

    Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam

    MAGNUS, R.(1924) Body Posture (Korperstellung)- Julius Springer, Berlin(English translationpublished by US Department of Commerce, 1987)--- (1924) Korperstellung (Body Posture) - US Department of Commerce, New Delhi, Englishtranslation (1987)--- (1925) Animal posture- Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B.Vol 98339-353

    --- (1926a) Some results of studies in the physiology of posture, Part I- The Lancet,Vol 208531-536--- (1926b) Some results of studies in the physiology of posture, Part II- The Lancet,Vol 208585-588--- (1930) Lane lectures on experimental pharmacology and medicine- Stanford University Press,StanfordMcCOMAS, A. J.(1996) Skeletal Muscle: form and function- Human Kinetics, Champaign, Illinois

    MITTELSTAEDT, H., MITTELSTAEDT, M.-L. From interdependent to independent control of

    head and trunk BERTHOZ, A., VIDAL, P.GRAF, W.(1992)

    PROCHAZKA, A., CLARAC, F., LOEB, G. E., et al(2000) What do reflex and voluntary mean?

    Modern views on an ancient debate- Experimental Brain Research130417-432

    SHERRINGTON, C. (1906) The integrative action of the nervous system - Cambridge University

    Press, Cambridge(1947 edition)

  • 7/21/2019 Posture Health Alexander Technique

    48/48