Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

26
 Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdiscipl inary Journal Copyright © 2013 ASCR Publishing House. All rights reserved.  ISSN: 1224-8398 Volume XVII, No. 4 (Decembe r), 289-313 PERCEIVED AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE TEACHING, ACADEMIC SELF-PERCEPTIONS AND ENGAGEMENT IN LEARNING: TOWARD A PROCESS MODEL OF ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Viorel MIH 1*  , Codru  ţ a MIH 2  1 Department of Psychology, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania 2 Department of Educational Sciences, Babeş-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania  ABSTRACT Self-determination theory was used to determine the impact of perceived autonomy – supportive teaching and academic self-perception on engagement in learning and school performance of 174 10 th  graders. Multilevel structural equation modeling tested the model in which teacher-provided autonomy support  perception first nurtures students’ academic self perception; the extent of self  perception then predicts the extent of classroom engagement. Findings reveal that  provision of autonomy support within classrooms predicted students’ self-efficacy and academic self concept. The conceptualization of engagement includes behavioral (effort, task persistence) and emotional components. These components were assessed and their antecedents and consequences examined. Results indicated that behavioral engagement was grounded in expectancy for success and academic  self efficacy and emotional engagement in academic self-concept, expectancy for  success and academic self-efficac y. Both behavioral and emotional engagement  facilitated academic performance. Implications for the achievement and adjustment of adolescents in school are discussed. The conceptual model and  subsequent findings established in this study provide clues for further theoretical development and practical applications concerning the mediating mechanism between perceived autonomy-supportive teaching and academic performance.  KEYWORDS: autonomy support perception, academic self-perception, academic  self-concept, s chool engagement Understanding how particular aspects of the high-school environment perceptions relate to both adaptive patterns of academic self-perception and school adjustment of students has become an increasingly important topic in the field of educational  psychology (Eccles, Wigfield, Midgley, Maehr, & Anderman, 1993; Midgley, *  Corresponding author: E-mail: [email protected] 

description

PERCEIVED AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE TEACHING,

Transcript of Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    1/26

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    Copyright 2013 ASCR Publishing House. All rights reserved.ISSN: 1224-8398Volume XVII, No. 4 (December), 289-313

    PERCEIVED AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE TEACHING,

    ACADEMIC SELF-PERCEPTIONS AND

    ENGAGEMENT IN LEARNING: TOWARD A PROCESS

    MODEL OF ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT

    Viorel MIH1*, Codrua MIH

    2

    1

    Department of Psychology, Babe-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania2Department of Educational Sciences, Babe-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania

    ABSTRACT

    Self-determination theory was used to determine the impact of perceived

    autonomy supportive teaching and academic self-perception on engagement in

    learning and school performance of 174 10th graders. Multilevel structural

    equation modeling tested the model in which teacher-provided autonomy support

    perception first nurtures students academic self perception; the extent of self

    perception then predicts the extent of classroom engagement. Findings reveal that

    provision of autonomy support within classrooms predicted students self-efficacy

    and academic self concept. The conceptualization of engagement includes

    behavioral (effort, task persistence) and emotional components. These components

    were assessed and their antecedents and consequences examined. Results indicated

    that behavioral engagement was grounded in expectancy for success and academic

    self efficacy and emotional engagement in academic self-concept, expectancy for

    success and academic self-efficacy. Both behavioral and emotional engagement

    facilitated academic performance. Implications for the achievement and

    adjustment of adolescents in school are discussed. The conceptual model andsubsequent findings established in this study provide clues for further theoretical

    development and practical applications concerning the mediating mechanism

    between perceived autonomy-supportive teaching and academic performance.

    KEYWORDS: autonomy support perception, academic self-perception, academic

    self-concept, school engagement

    Understanding how particular aspects of the high-school environment perceptionsrelate to both adaptive patterns of academic self-perception and school adjustment

    of students has become an increasingly important topic in the field of educationalpsychology (Eccles, Wigfield, Midgley, Maehr, & Anderman, 1993; Midgley,

    *Corresponding author:

    E-mail: [email protected]

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    2/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    290

    1993; Reuman, Maclver, Feldlaufer, & 1993; Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996;Urdan, Midgley, & Wood, 1995). For instance, there is evidence that perception of

    academic autonomy support and self perception increase during the early adolescentperiod (Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). In this study, we focus on how perceived

    autonomy-supportive teaching, adolescents' self-perceptions which are emphasizedin their school (self-efficacy, expectancy for success and academic self-concept)and their school engagement affect academic performance during tenth grade.Research on the autonomy support suggests that students sense of autonomy

    increases when teachers minimize coercion and interference, show understandingfor students perspective and feelings, provide a relevant rationale for the task, and

    offer choice by allowing students to participate in task and goal selection (Katz &Assor, 2007). Instead, close surveillance and frequent intrusions undermine feelings

    of autonomy (Assor, Kaplan, & Roth, 2002; Assor, Kaplan, Roth, & Kanat-Maymon, 2005). Support for autonomy can be manifested in the classroom in atleast three ways: procedurally (encouraging student ownership of form, e.g., lettingstudents select the media in which to present ideas), organizationally (encouraging

    student ownership of the environment - e.g., letting students select due dates forassignments), and cognitively (encouraging student ownership of learning, e.g.,

    asking students to generate their own paths to a solution) (Stefanou, Perencevich,DiCinto, & Turner, 2004, as cited in, Katz & Assor, 2007).

    A large corpus of empirical evidence based on self-determination theory

    (SDT) suggests that perceived autonomy-supportive teaching is conducive toengagement and optimal learning in educational contexts (Deci & Ryan, 2000;Reeve, 2012; Su & Reeve, 2011). Autonomy support is the interpersonal behaviorone person provides to involve another persons intentions to act, such as when a

    teacher supports a students psychological needs (e.g., autonomy, competence),self-efficacy, expectancy for success, school valuating and academic self-concept

    (Reeve & Jang, 2006). Providing student a rationale to explain why a rule exists orwhy an apparently uninteresting activity is truly worth students attention is an

    autonomy-supportive behavior because it allows students sense of valuing to guidetheir classroom activity and consequently academic self-efficacy. Likewise, asking

    students what they want (e.g., asking for their input for the lesson plan) is anautonomy-supportive behavior because the teacher seeks to identify students

    psychological needs to integrate them into the days lesson and this compatibilitydevelops student perception of autonomy-supportive teaching and academic

    self-efficacy (Reeve & Jang, 2006). Overall, autonomy support revolves aroundfinding ways to support and increase students inner endorsement of their classroomactivity (Reeve, 2009; Reeve, Deci, & Ryan, 2004). When students perceive their

    teachers to be autonomy supportive (Rigby, Deci, Patrick, & Ryan, 1992) studentsreport high levels of self-determination (Vallerand, 1997). These motivationalresources, when supported and nurtured in the classroom, provide students with the

    motivational foundation they need to become highly engaged in school and

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    3/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    291

    committed to graduating (Hardre & Reeve 2003; Vallerand, 1997). Compared tostudents who perceived controlled motivations, students with greater perceived

    autonomy experience a wide range of educationally and developmentally importantbenefits. These benefits include not only greater psychological need satisfaction

    during learning activities but also greater classroom engagement, a preference foroptimal challenge over easy success, greater persistence in school tasks and higheracademic achievement (Black & Deci, 2000; Reeve, 2009; Reeve, Jang, Carrell,Barch, & Jeon, 2004). Other studies have shown that the benefits of perceived

    autonomy motivations are manifold, including deep-level learning, achievementand behavioral persistence (e.g., Buff, Reusser, Rakoczy, & Pauli, 2011; Reeve,

    2009). Recognizing this, we consider useful to investigate the extent to whichperceived autonomy-supportive teaching predict the subsequent engagement in

    school, and they do so in a way that is over and above the influence of schoolperformance. At the same time we are interested if some of cognitive self resources,as represented by perceived expectancy for success, academic self-efficacy, andacademic self-concept mediate the relationship between perceived autonomy-

    supportive teaching and students engagement in school and student performance.

    Academic self-perception

    Researchers in educational psychology have long been interested in the role of self-related perceptions in academic contexts (Bong& Skaalvik, 2000; Lent, Brown, &

    Gore, 1997). Students who are otherwise similar feel differently about themselvesand choose different courses of action, depending on how they construe themselves- what expectancy for success they have, what attributes they think they possess,what they believe they are capable of, how they view they fare in comparison with

    others. Without doubt, these are beliefs and perceptions about the self that areheavily rooted in ones past achievement and reinforcement history. Yet it is these

    subjective convictions about oneself, once established which play a determiningrole in individuals further growth and development (Bandura, 1997; Markus &

    Nurius, 1986). During the past couple of decades, numerous studies in educationalresearch have resorted to either self-concept or self-efficacy to explain the function

    of self in school contexts (Bong & Skaalvik, 2000). Studies have demonstrated thatpositive perception of self generates many desirable outcomes (Reeve & Jang,

    2006; Vallerand, 1997). Thus, strong self-efficacy and positive self-concept leadstudents to persist longer on difficult tasks, feel less anxious in achievement

    settings, enjoy their academic work more, and feel better about themselves as astudent. Consequently, we analyzed the process of how academic self-efficacy

    beliefs, self-concept, and expectancy for success affect subsequent academic

    engagement (behavioral and emotional) and performance.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    4/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    292

    Academic self-efficacy concept

    According to Bandura (1986, 1997) self-efficacy contributes to engagement in one

    action in two ways, namely by (a) determining the amount of effort andperseverance one will expend in a given endeavor; and (b) shaping the outcomes

    expected from one's efforts. People who perceive themselves as highly efficaciouswill expect favorable outcomes, whereas those with less confidence in their

    performance capabilities will envision negative outcomes. Therefore, self-efficacy(a) can enhance ones functioning through elevated levels of effort and persistence

    and (b) can also enhance ones ability to deal with a problem situation byinfluencing cognitive and emotional processes related to the situation. Students with

    low self-efficacy tend to well on their deficiencies and view situations as moredifficult than they really are (Bandura, 2005). Following these points, it can be

    hypothesized that students high in self-efficacy are more likely to engage in aschool task that is characterized by positive, proactive, and solution-focusedorientations. Self-efficacy has been proven to be remarkably useful as a theoreticalmethod to comprehend a number of behaviors because of its influence on a

    students choice, effort, and persistence in an activity (Bandura, 1997). Thereby, theinfluence of efficacy beliefs within academic contexts is pervasive as a significant

    predictor of academic performance (Bandura, 1997; Chemers, Hu, & Garcia, 2001)

    and as a mediating variable influencing students levels of effort, persistence, andperseverance (Schunk, 1991; Zeldin & Pajares, 2000).

    Expectancy for success

    Research guided by the self-determination theory (SDT, Reeve, 2002; Ryan &Deci, 2000, 2002) has shown that the autonomous motivation support has important

    consequences for the quality of school adjustment. Because it is postulated thathigher levels of self-determination are associated with better psychological

    functioning, SDT proposes that the extent to which an individual is self-determinedis strongly reflected in the quality of his or her experience. For example, it has been

    shown that higher levels of self-determined motivation are related to severalpositive outcomes, such as expectancy for success (Reeve, 2002; Ryan & Deci,

    2000). Students need to believe they can be successful if they are going to try. Theirexpectancy for success influences their desire to engage in learning and school

    adaptation.Various factors can contribute to students expectancy for success. Research

    suggests that students who have a high level of academic self-efficacy or who valuelearning a particular skill or topic are more likely to engage in learning and hardworking (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). At the same time,

    numerous studies have shown that students with confidence in their abilities tosucceed on a task work harder, persist longer, and perform better than their lessefficacious peers (Bandura, 2005; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Eccles, Wigfield, &

    Schiefele, 1998; Schunk, 1991). Expectancy-value theory (Feather, 1988; Wigfield

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    5/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    293

    & Eccles, 2002) links performance, persistence, and effort directly to individualsexpectancy for success. According to expectancy-value theory (Feather, 1988;

    Wigfield & Eccles, 2002), expectancies and values directly influence persistenceand effort. Expectancy for successis conceptualized as a task-specific belief about

    success in a future academic task. According to theorists of expectancy-valuemodels, effort, persistence, and performance in a given task depend on value-relatedvariables and the individuals expectancy for success (Bandura, 1997; Wigfield &Eccles, 2000). For example, a student may show more persistence in mastering a

    difficult task when his or her success expectancies are high. This increasedpersistence may ultimately result in a superior outcome. Expectancy of success is

    closely related to other conceptions of self-beliefs (e.g., academic self-concept;Marsh, 2007; self-efficacy; Bandura, 1997; 2005) and these constructs form the

    self-perception concept.

    Academic self- concept

    From a social-cognition perspective, academic self-conceptcan be defined as how a

    student feels about himself or herself within a school or academic setting, or inrelation to his or her academic progress (Bracken, 2009). If academic self efficacyrefers to students convictions that they can successfully perform given academic

    tasks at designated levels (confidence perception) (Schunk, 1991), academic self-concept refers to students knowledge and perceptions about themselves in

    achievement situations (competence perception) (Shavelson & Bolus, 1982;Wigfield & Karpathian, 1991). Thus, academic self-concept is typically morestable, past orientated and refers to specific school subjects, whereas self-efficacy ismore malleable, future orientated and most often refers to specific tasks (Bong &

    Skaalvik, 2003; Marsh & Hau, 2003).Researchers have emphasized the multidimensionality of self-concept and

    the specific components of self-concept most appropriate to a particular setting.Because the same person can have a positive self-concept in one domain (e.g.,

    academic) and a negative self-concept in another domain (e.g., social), globalmeasures cannot adequately describe self-concepts in different domains (Marsh &

    Hau, 2003). Thus, academic self-concept is particularly important in educationalsettings that are the focus of the present investigation. Academic self-concept is

    hierarchically organized, and is multifacete: math self-concept, verbal self-concept,and school self-concept (Marsh & Yeung, 1997), and these together constitute

    academic self-concept in the current study. Students academic self-concept hasreceived a lot of attention in educational research during the last two decades

    because this concept has a predictive power for many academic outcomes such as

    interest, persistence, coursework selection, and academic achievement (Craven &Marsh, 2008; Marsh & Craven, 2006; Marsh & OMara, 2008). A high level ofacademic self-concept is thus seen as a desirable outcome in itself and as a mediator

    leading to other favorable educational outcomes. Thus, academic achievement,

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    6/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    294

    persistence, coursework selection, and long-term educational aspirations aresystematically related to academic self-concept, they are nearly uncorrelated (or

    even negatively related) to nonacademic (social and physical) self-conceptresponses (Marsh & Craven, 1997; Marsh & Yeung, 1997, 1998). This research

    demonstrated that self-concept has an important influence on educational decisionslike students persistence, effort, and emotional engagement. This research isimportant in that it has established that increases in academic self-concept lead toincreases in subsequent school engagement (Arens, Yeung, Craven, & Hasselhorn,

    2011).

    School engagement

    The construct of school engagement has received increasing research attention as

    educators need to find solutions for problems such as declining academicmotivation and achievement, increasing student alienation, and elevated schooldrop-out rates (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004). School engagement has beenseen as a potential answer to these problems because it encompasses processes that,

    theoretically, serve to promote learning and achievement and, practically, can befostered in students. A key hypothesis in research on school engagement is that, for

    students to profit from schooling, they must do more than simply attend school orbe present in classrooms. Rather, they must engage the classroom environment inways that promote learning.

    Because researchers studying the effects of school engagement onachievement have differed in their definitions and measures of engagement, it isdifficult to integrate findings across studies. Often, researchers incorporated a widevariety of constructs in their measurement of engagement, an inclusiveness that

    makes it difficult to determine the unique precursors and consequences of differenttypes of engagement. In the current study, we assess both behavioral and emotional

    engagement to involvement in learning (Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008).Behavioral engagement refers to participation in the learning environment, and

    although defined in different ways, has often been operationalized in terms of theeffort and persistence aspects of involvement in instructional activities. Such

    involvement includes trying hard, not giving up in the face of difficulty, anddirecting ones attention to instructional activities (Buhs & Ladd, 2001; Rothbart &

    Bates, 1998).Emotional engagement refers to students affective reactions in the

    classroom, which engage them in learning requires positive or negative emotionalexperiences. These emotional reactions, such as boredom, happiness, sadness, andanxiety contribute to a classroom climate that forms the foundation for teacher-

    student relationships and interactions necessary for motivation to learn (Connell &Wellborn, 1991; Meyer & Turner, 2006; Skinner & Belmont, 1993). Someresearchers assess emotional engagement by measuring emotional reactions to the

    school and the teacher (Lee & Smith, 1995; Stipek, 2002). Some conceptualize it as

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    7/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    295

    identification with school (Finn, 1993; Voelkl, 1997). Finn defines identification asbelonging (a feeling of being important to the school) and value (an appreciation of

    success in school-related outcomes). The emotions included in these definitionsduplicate an earlier body of work on attitudes, which examined feelings toward

    school and included survey questions about liking or disliking school, the teacher,or the work; feeling happy or sad in school; or being bored or interested in the work(Epstein & McPartland, 1976).

    These aspects of engagement have been investigated as a potential precursor

    of students academic achievement.

    THE PRESENT RESEARCH

    In line with past theorizing, and in an attempt to overcome some of the limitationsof past research, our objective was to investigate the extent to which cognitive selfresources, as represented by perceived expectancy for success, academic self-efficacy and academic self-concept mediate the relationship between perceived

    autonomy-supportive teaching and students engagement in school and studentperformance. Our question was, does the perceived expectancy for success,academic self-efficacy and academic self-concept serve as mediators of engagement

    in learning? Previous research has examined these variables separately, and thisstudy takes the research a step further by investigating direct and indirect predictors

    of these cognitive self resources together using path analysis. We expected thatthese cognitive resources are predicted by perceived autonomy-supportive teachingand predict the subsequent engagement in school, and they do so in a way that isover and above the influence of school performance. As a consequence, this study

    expands research on teaching autonomy perception, academic self beliefs,engagement in learning and performance. Figure 1 presents the model and

    constructs examined. First, we theorize that student' perceptions of the autonomysupport associated with school relate to variables such as academic self-efficacy,

    expectancy for success and school self-concept. Students' self-efficacy and schoolself-concept in turn are examined in relation to psychological outcomes associated

    with school such as effort, persistence, and emotional engagement. In the last stepof the model, the association of students' engagement with their final semester

    academic achievement is examined. In looking at each set of these relations, we usea path model. Although the causal direction of the relations among these constructs

    is not yet well established, the hypothesized direction from the perceived schoolcontext measures to self perception, engagement, and school achievement follows

    previous empirical research in classrooms and schools (Arens, et al., 2011; Hughes,

    et al., 2008; Jang, Kim, & Reeve, 2012; Marsh & Hau, 2003; Reeve & Jang, 2006).In this study, we examine the relationships flow among the constructs depicted inFigure 1 based upon theoretical considerations and prior research, all of which are

    discussed below. On the basis of the arguments presented, the following specific

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    8/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    296

    predictions were advanced: (a) autonomy support perception will predict schoolself-efficacy, expectancy for success and academic self-concept, (b) self perception

    will directly predict behavioral and emotional engagement, (c) effort, persistence,and emotional engagement will direct predict school performance.

    Context Self perception School engagement School achivement

    (Autonomy support

    perception)

    Figure 1.

    Integrated theoretical model of core teacher autonomy support for learning, academic self-

    perception elements, engagement, and learning outcomes

    METHODS

    Participants

    The students who participated in this study were attending three high schools. Thesample consisted of 174 students. Approximately equal numbers of girls (n=89,

    51.1%) and boys (n=85, 48.9%) participated in the study. Surveys wereadministered to students during one class period (of approximately 35 min).Students were given instructions in the use of Likert-type scales and were

    encouraged to ask questions during survey administration if anything was unclear.

    Academicself-efficacy

    Academic

    self-

    concept

    Behavioral

    engagement

    Emotional

    engagement

    Autonomy

    support

    perception

    School

    achievement

    Expectancy for

    success

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    9/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    297

    Participants were assured that the information they provided would be confidential.All participants were in the 10th grade. Participation was voluntary, and scores

    were confidential and anonymous. We collected the questionnaire data during thesemester and the achievement data (semester grade) after the semester ended

    Instruments

    Participants completed a questionnaire packet which included a demographic

    information form, the Students perception of autonomy-supportive teachingscale(Assor et al., 2002),Academic Self-Concept Scale (Reynolds, 1988),Academic Self-

    efficacy(PALS; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Roeser et al., 1996), Expectancy forsuccess (Wigfield & Eccles, 2002), Effort (Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999 &

    Pintrich & Groot, 1990), Persistence (Elliot et al.s, 1999), The EmotionalEngagement Scale(Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008), Participantswere required to respond on a five-point Likert-type rating scale; for example: 1(strongly disagree) to 5 or 7 (strongly agree); and 1 (not at all true of me) to 5 or 7

    (very true of me).Students Perception of Autonomy-Supportive Teaching Scale. The scale

    assessing this variable was a shortened version of a scale developed and validated

    by Assor et al. (2002). The18-item scale assessed students perceptions of theirmain teachers behavior and students indicated the extent to which they agreed with

    each response using a 4-point scale. The scale evaluate three aspect: (a) the extentto which the teacher was seen as allowing choice as part of the learning process andas conversing with students about choices, (b) the extent to which the teacher wasseen as enabling free and open expression of critical thoughts and independent

    opinions and (c) the extent to which the teacher is seen as talking to students aboutwhy it is important to learn the subject matter and do the assignments, as well the

    relevance and value of the subject matter and the assignments to their personalgoals and interests.

    Examples of items were: The teacher encourages me to work in my ownway (providing choice), The teacher allows us to talk about things that we find

    unacceptable inn school (allowing criticism and encouranging independentthinking) and The teacher explains why it is important to study certain subjects in

    school (fostering understanding and relevance). Factor analysis with varimaxrotation was conducted on all 18 items, and one factor was extracted, accounting for

    61% of the variance. Thus, the factor analysis indicates that the three sets of items(for providing choice, for allowing criticism and encouranging independentthinking and fostering understanding and relevance) do not constitute distinctive

    sub-scales. Cronbachs alpha for the whole scale was .71.Academic Self-Concept Scale. In the current study, self-concept was

    measured using the ASCS (Reynolds, 1988), which is a 40-item self-report

    measure. The questionnaire used a 4-point Likert-type scale that ranges from

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    10/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    298

    strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (4), to measure aspect of academic self-concept. Thus, within the ASCS, the seven constructs of academic self-concept

    include: grade and effort dimension, study habits/organization self-perceptions, peerevaluation of academic ability, self-confidence in academics, satisfaction with

    school, self-doubt about ability, and self-evaluation with external standards.Reynolds, Ramirez, Magrina, and Allen (1980) formulated the seven constructs ofacademic self-concept and the ASCS has been utilized in repeated studies onvarious student populations (Lent, Brown, & Gore, 1997). Lent et al. (1997)

    performed studies on undergraduate students using the ASCS along with other self-concept measurements that established a correlation between academic self-concept

    and academic achievement. The results of the study concluded that academic self-concept related to the overall academic achievement of their participants and the

    measurement of academic self-concept may be useful in forecasting aggregateacademic performance (Lent et al., 1997). Reynolds et al. (1988) utilized the ASCSto determine the self-concept of college students; however, this scale was adapted toevaluate high school students in this study. A revision of specific terms was

    necessary to explore the unique experiences of junior high students. Therefore theterm exams was replaced with tests in item 5, college was replaced withschool in items 8, 14, 15, and 37, the terms courses or major was replaced with

    classes in items 10, 24, 33 and 39, and instructors was replaced with teachersin item 13. Item 35 was reworded to reflect the end of the semester or grading

    period instead of finals week, which was previously used to refer to the end of acourse for college students. Factor analysis of the ASCS yielded a seven-factorsolution that accounted for 48.6% of total variance (Reynolds, 1988). The ASCSyields one global score and seven subscale scores. The items are keyed in a positive

    direction for academic self-concept. These data lend support to the reliability andvalidity of the use of the ASCS as a measurement of academic self-concept. Sample

    items include: (a) .If I try hard enough, I will be able to get good grades|; (b) Ienjoy doing my schoolwork. The overall ASCS alpha coefficient was .86.

    Academic Self-efficacy Scale consisted of five items regarding perceivedcompetence and confidence in performance of class work. Subject-level academic

    self-efficacy items were adapted from the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey(PALS; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Roeser et al., 1996) and the Self-Efficacy

    subscale of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich &De Groot, 1990). Students report answers on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all

    true of me (1) to very true of me (5). The five self-efficacy items were "I can mastereven the hardest material in this class if I try," "I can do almost all the work in classif I don't give up," "I'm certain that I can do an excellent job on the problems and

    tasks assigned for a class," "I know that I will be able to learn the material for theclass," and "I'm confident that I will receive a good grade this semester." Principalcomponents analysis of the items verified that the items formed a single factor. The

    self-efficacy score was computed as the mean score of the scale items. Item and

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    11/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    299

    reliability analysis indicated acceptable scale properties, with item -totalcorrelations ranging from .46 to .78 and =.84.

    Expectancy for success. Expectancy for success consisted one item (Wigfield& Eccles, 2002) and was assessed by asking participants to rate on a scale of 0 ( no

    chance) to 100 (complete certainty) their confidence in achieving a grade of ten inthe course. This type of measure is similar to measures of self-efficacy for grades(Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994).

    Behavioral engagement (Effort + Persistence). Effort was measured using

    two items from Elliot, McGregor, and Gable (1999) and two items from the MSLQ(Pintrich & Groot, 1990). Four items from Elliot et al.s (1999) scales were used for

    the persistence variable. Reliability estimates (Cronbachs alpha) were .76 for theeffort scale and .89 for the persistence scale. Two items from effort scale were I

    put a lot of effort into preparing for the exam and I always work as hard as I canto finish my school assignments.Persistence, which included four items, reflectedstudents beliefs that they completed work for their math class even when facedwith distractions, boredom, or difficulty (=.72) Two items from this scale were I

    get distracted very easily when Im studying (reverse coded) and I get started ondoing my work for school but often dont stick with it for very long (reversecoded). Some preliminary evidence of the validity of these measures was provided

    by Wolters (1999). Participants indicated their responses on a 1 (not at all true ofme) to 7 (very true of me) scale for the persistence items and a 1 (strongly disagree)

    to 7 (strongly agree) scale for the effort items. A principal-components factoranalysis with varimax rotation was performed and this analysis yielded the twohypothesized factors with each item loading on its designated factor. The twofactors (persistence and effort) accounted for 65 % of the total variance (Elliot et

    al.s, 1999).The Emotional Engagement Scale was designed to measure students

    emotional involvement during learning activities. Each student reported on his orher own (a) emotional engagement, using six items tapping their emotions

    indicating motivated involvement during learning activities (such affectivereactions in the classroom, attitudes towards school and teachers, appreciation of

    success in school) (Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008). Examples ofitems include When we start something new in school, I feel interested. and

    I enjoy learning new things in class.) (=.74).Academic performance. To assess academic achievement, we used the actual

    school record of each students overall semester grade, scored at the end of thesemester on a 10-point scale.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    12/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    300

    RESULTS

    Prior to conducting the analyses, we examined the data for univariate andmultivariate outlying cases by using the procedure devised by Tabachnik and Fidell

    (2007). Before conducting the main analyses, all of the major variables werechecked for missing data. Since the pattern of missing values was random for the

    present data, cases with missing values more than 5% were deleted (Tabachnick &Fidel, 2001). Among 174 participants, 9 data were detected with missing values

    more than 5% of the total endorsement. Hence, 165 data were left for the mainanalyses after this deletion. In order to prevent additional subject loss, cases with

    missing data less than 5% were replaced with mean of the given variable. Secondfor the preliminary analyses, outlier analyses over the data were conducted. In this

    respect, in order to check the univariate outlier, the data was converted into z-scoreand 3problematic outlier values higher or smaller than

    3.29 (Tabachnick & Fidel, 2001) was detected. As a result 3 cases weretreated as outlier and excluded from the data set. Hence, the analyses were

    performed with data obtained from 162 cases.Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for the autonomy

    support perception, various academic self perception and engagement factors and

    the matrix of correlations between them and school achievement. Bivariatecorrelations were computed to depict the interrelationships among all of the study

    variables. The correlation matrix on the Table 1 showed the relationships among thepredictors, mediator and criterion variables. The relationships also assess thepresence of multicollinearity. The results showed that none of the partialcoefficients exceeded .50 that the multicollinearity among the study variables was

    not severe (Tabachnick & Fidel, 2001).The path analyses that were employed in the present investigation rely on

    assumptions including linearity, causal closure and unitary variable. Overall theassumption checks were conducted in the frame of preliminary analysis. In this

    respect, linearity assumption was controlled by conducting the correlation analysis.As suggested by Wright (1968) all relationships between variables should be linear.

    In order to perform a path analysis he also suggested causal closure in that all directinfluences of one variable on another must be included in the path diagram. Final

    specific assumption for conducting path analysis includes unitary variables forwhich variables should not be composed of components that behave in different

    ways with different variables.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    13/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    301

    Table 1.

    Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among all the measures

    M(SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 71. Autonomy support

    perception124.47(17.3) -

    2. Academic

    self-efficacy3.67(.91) .39** -

    3. Expectancy

    for success73.21(22.56) .14** 0.4 -

    4. Academic

    self-concept2.78(.57) .26** .11* .07 -

    5. Behavioral

    engagement9.37(2.22) .07 .34** .29** .19** -

    6. Emotionalengagement 2.82(.42) .11* .27** .22** .25** .12* -

    7. School achievement 7.68(1.12) .08 .07 .12* .11* .31** .24** -

    * = p

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    14/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    302

    As seen on the Table 2, the hypothesized model that did not meet the criteria. In

    other words, the goodness of fit indexes were evaluated and found to be 2

    /df ratiowas higher than 5.0; GFI, and NFI values were smaller than .90; and RMSEA value

    was found to be higher than .08. However, our review of the modification indicesindicated that the fit of the model can be substantially improved if some of the

    pathways were added and some were eliminated. In this respect, some of thesuggested pathways were added to the hypothesized model besides removing non-

    significant paths from the model. The suggested path model is depicted in Figure 2,with non-significant paths in italic arrows type and suggested paths in dotted arrows

    type. After making the modification to the path model, the analysis was performed.The fit statistics obtained from the final path analysis showed that the value of

    2(5,

    N = 162) was 5.82, p > .05 which indicated a good fit. Besides the 2value, its ratioto degrees of freedom was also calculated. The value of this ratio was

    2/df = 5.82 /

    5 = 1.14 which implied a good fit given that generally values less than 2 areaccepted to be good fit (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). The other important goodness

    of fit statistics that were calculated for the present study was RMSEA, GFI, AGFI,and NFI. The results of the present analysis showed that RMSEA value was .052(90% CI: .047-.064), GFI values was .98, AGFI was .98 and NFI was found to

    be .99. These multiple indices also confirmed the adequacy of the model fit.

    Figure 2.

    Final model depicting the relationships among autonomy support perception, academic self

    perception, engagement in learning, and school achievement. Solid path coefficients are

    standardized regression coefficients, and all paths represent significant effects (p < .05 at

    minimum, except that autonomy support perception was unrelated to expectancy for success.

    This nonsignificant path coefficient is in italic type. Dotted paths represent significant path

    which not-figure in first model.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    15/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    303

    The final model we obtained using the tests of the nested models, includingthe standardized path loadings, is shown in Figure 2. This model suggests that the

    effect of teacher autonomy support on performance is mediated by school self-efficacy, academic self-concept, behavioral and emotional engagement, but not by

    expectancy for success.In accord with the posited structural model, perceived teachers autonomy

    support predicted school efficacy and academic self-concept. The effect ofautonomy support perception on behavioral engagement was totally mediated by

    self-efficacy and academic self-concept. Thus, students who perceive teacherautonomy support has a high level of self-efficacy and a more consistent self-

    concept, and this will lead to a deep engagement in learning. Also, school efficacywas distally linked to behavioral engagement both directly and through the

    mediation of emotional engagement. Similarly, a high sense of academic selfconcept predicted school achievement by supporting emotional and behavioralengagement. As we further hypothesized, the impact of perceived academic self onschool performance was mediated through different type of engagement. Overall,

    behavioral engagement in learning (effort + persistence) and academic emotionalengagement accounted for 28% of the variance in performance.

    DISCUSSION

    The goals of this investigation were to determine whether perceived teacherpractices predicted students academic self-efficacy, academic self-concept, andexpectancy for success and to determine whether these self-related cognitions

    predicted their engagement in learning and implicit academic performance. Priorresearch has shown that perceptions of autonomy are related to academic success

    (Perry, Hladkyj, Reinhard, Clifton, & Chipperfield, 2005; Skinner, 1990) and thatperceived teacher practices are related to academic engagement and academic

    success (Durkin, 1995; Fulton & Turner, 2008). The current study extends theliterature by examining the pathway from perceived teacher practices (such as

    warmth, listening, creating time for independent work, giving the studentopportunities to talk, praising signs of improvement and mastery, encouraging thestudents effort, offering progress-enabling hints when the student seemed stuck,

    being responsive to the students questions and comments, and acknowledging the

    students perspective and experiences and supervision) which function asantecedents of students school achievement.

    The present study specifically examined students perceptions of teachers

    autonomy-supportive activities as an important factor that may predict studentsdeeper understanding of academic self perception and students engagement inlearning. The findings indicated that students perceptions of teachers provision of

    rationale and relevance, together with perceptions of the teachers attempts to take

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    16/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    304

    the students perspective, appeared to predict self-efficacy and academic self-concept by supporting students development of identification with the school

    values and requirements.Our findings indicate that perceived teacher practices are a significant

    predictor of high school students perceptions of self, and that self-relatedcognitions are related to school engagement. These findings are important becausethey indicate that an autonomy supportive climate contributes to students beliefsystems (perceptions), which then impact their academic efforts, persistence, and

    outcomes in school. This can be explained by the fact that, when teachers andschools respect students need for autonomy via the dialogue they engage in, and by

    allowing students opportunities for self expression, students may experiencepositive feelings in the classroom also in ages when oppositional attitudes toward

    social institutions and adult authorities are common. This view is also consistentwith Eccles and Midgleys (1989) stage fit theory, according to which adolescentsoften show low levels of motivation and positive affect toward their junior highschools because they experience these schools as frustrating and inconsistent with

    their increased need for autonomy.Also, according to SDT, people (in our case students) feel autonomous when

    they understand the value or relevance of the task (learning) in which they are

    engaged, and therefore can identify with it. Feelings of autonomy are particularlystrong when the learning is perceived as being closely connected to the values,

    interests, and goals that constitute the core of ones self-concept (Assor, Cohen-Melayev, Kaplan, & Friedman, 2005; Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003). The study ofAssor, Kaplan, and Roth (2002) suggests that what students perceive as beinghighly valuable is probably not the mere act of choosing, but mostly the value of the

    options to the participants self. As for the context in which choice is offered(learning autonomy context), the teaching autonomous mode should support the

    development of a firm sense of self-efficacy and solid expectancy for success(Flowerday & Schraw, 2003). For example, the teacher may do well to consider the

    type of feedback provided to students (see Butler, 1987). Feedback that isresponsive to the students questions and comments provides information for

    judging progress, and encouraging the students efforts is more beneficial fordevelopment a sense of autonomy than feedback comparing the students ability

    with that of other students (Brophy, 1981). In such autonomy-supporting contexts,students can devote themselves to the task learning they have chosen, without

    worrying about their performance level and the possibility of negative evaluations(Katz & Assor, 2006).

    Also, according to SDT, perceived teacher autonomy support facilitates

    engagement in learning because a rationale, when communicated in an autonomy-supportive way, reveals an activitys value and personal benefit (Ryan & Deci,2000, 2002). Such personal relevance information helps students identify with and

    internalize the value of the task and this internalization allows students to engage

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    17/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    305

    volitionally in the learning activity, through effort and persistence. Additionally,results of this study supported previous research findings indicating that self-

    efficacy correlated positively with students performance. Researchers havesuggested that students with high self-efficacy tend to learn and achieve more than

    students with low self-efficacy even when actual ability levels are the same(Bandura, 1986). This is partly because efficacious students tend to engage incognitive processes that promote learning, such as effort to paying attention,

    persisting longer at difficult tasks, and organizing and elaborating new information

    (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002; Tellefson, 2000).Another goal of this study was to demonstrate the important relationship

    between academic self perception and emotional engagement. This researchdemonstrated that self perception has an important influence on students - how they

    feel about themselves in approaching learning, their effort, persistence, andemotional engagement. This study is important in that it has established thatincreases in academic self-perception lead to increases in subsequent academicengagement and other desirable educational outcomes. Hence, not only is self

    perception an important outcome variable in itself, but it also plays a central role inmediating the effects of other desirable educational outcomes (task persistence,emotional engagement). These findings have significant implications for

    educational policy and practice. Also, it appears that students who experience anincreased academic self-concept were more likely to report behavioral and

    emotional engagement in learning. Student engagement is a motivationally enrichedclassroom quality that has clear implications for student achievement (Skinner,Kindermann, Connell, & Wellborn, 2009). By engaging themselves actively andenthusiastically in academic activities, students learn, develop skills, and generally

    make academic progress. Consequently, both the extent and quality of studentsclassroom engagement have been shown to predict various aspects of achievement,

    including course grades and improved standardized test scores (Jansen & Bruinsma,2005; Ladd & Dinella, 2009; Marsh, Trautwein, Ldtke, Kller, & Baumert, 2005;

    Mih, 2013; Wouters, Germeijs, Colpin, & Verschueren, 2011).Surprisingly, perceived teacher autonomy support was not a significant

    predictor of expectancy for success. Numerous studies have shown autonomysupport to be related to positive outcomes among adolescent students (e.g., Ratelle

    et al., 2005; Soenens, Vansteenkiste, Luyckx, & Goossens, 2006). In the pathmodel, autonomy support was a positive predictor of expectancy for success, but it

    did not reach significance. Sample size may be an issue in this lack of statisticalsignificance. This is a clear limitation of this study. Also, information about teacherwas gathered only from the students and was retrospective. Clearly, multiple

    measures (e.g., from teachers reports) or observational measures would addstrength to this research (Fulton & Turner, 2008).

    However, we found that student expectancy for success predicted their

    behavioral engagement in learning. An explication for this prediction is that

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    18/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    306

    students who are more confident about their academic success are more interestedin their schoolwork and more convinced of the usefulness of their education.

    Students who attach little value to academic success may exert little effort on theirschoolwork and then lower their expectancy for success. Conversely, students with

    low expectancies for success may devalue academic activities (Dweck & Leggett,1988). These hypotheses could be evaluated directly with a longitudinal design inwhich measures of expectancies, and achievement were obtained at multiple times.

    Implications for Practice

    The study has some implications for educational practices. First, taken together with

    previous research, this study underscores the idea that, how engaged students areduring instruction and how much they develop themselves as autonomous learners

    depends, in part, on the autonomy supportive quality of the teacher. From this pointof view, students' engagement is an interpersonally coordinated process betweenteachers and students. When teachers support students' autonomy rather thancontrol their behavior - teachers function both as a guide to structure students'

    learning opportunities and to help them develop constructive sources ofautonomous motivation, such as internalized values. The implication for teachers is

    that of supporting students' autonomy and creating the conditions during learningactivities in which students can experience an engagement-fostering congruence

    between what they want to do and what they actually do during class.

    The second implication suggests that a priority for schools should bebuilding the quality of childrens perception of academic self and learning.Research from the areas of teaching suggests that qualities such as warmth, caring,sensitivity, dedication of attention and time, and emotional availability may be

    important to the development of accurate perception of academic self (seeGoldstein, 1999).

    The third implication suggests that, one aspect that seems particularlysensitive is adolescents emotional experience when involved in learning activities.

    Students enthusiasm, happiness, and comfort during engagement in academic tasksseem to be shaped by their perception of academic self and implicit by their

    perception of autonomous support.

    Study Limitations

    The present study has some of limitations. A first limitation is related to the design

    of the current study. Our theoretical model implies a temporal order, withautonomous support perception impacting self perception and these self beliefsimpacting academic engagement and engagement impacting academic success. The

    model suggests that the relationships are unidirectional. Although these paths aretheoretically defensible, it is also possible that the relationships are bidirectional.For example, adolescents with high perceptions of self may elicit different teacher

    behaviors. Future longitudinal studies may help to examine the direction of the

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    19/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    307

    effects and also lead one to better foresee the future implications of ones currentstudying. The second limitation was the application of a self-report instrument to

    measure students perception of behavioral engagement in learning and use oflearning strategies. In particular, social desirability bias is considered a significant

    threat to the construct validity of any self-report instrument. Thus, the students self-report may not have been entirely accurate in their reporting. Although the largesamples used in this study precluded the use of such methods, future researchshould explore the comparability of information obtained from self-report and

    observation (e. g., behavioral indicators of individuals effort and perseveration).However, the fact that the results of this study substantiated many of the

    relationships previously reported in the literature gives us some confidence that ourresults might be generalizable beyond the specific subjects and measures we used.

    CONCLUSION

    In general, the relations among variables involved in this study support ourhypotheses derived from SDT and expectancy-value theory. Specifically, we foundthat the academic self beliefs held by students largely mediate the relations between

    autonomy support perception, on the one hand, and behavioral, and emotionalengagements, on the other hand. Our empirical model also showed that behavioral

    and emotional engagements were predictive of students achievement outcome.Specifically, the deep effort and persistence learning were found to be positive

    predictors, respectively, of students outcome achievement. Behavioral andemotional engagement in turn played a mediating role in the relations between self

    perception and achievement outcome. Finally, teachers closeness in theirrelationships with individual students has been found to be good distal antecedent

    of tenth graders' school performance. From this perspective, teachers autonomysupport is not just a by-product of doing well in school; a sense of autonomy plays

    an important role in academic self development and in adolescents academicengagement.

    REFERENCES

    Arens, A. K., Yeung, A. S., Craven, R. G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2011). The twofold

    multidimensionality of academic self-concept: Domain specificity and separation

    between competence and affect components. Journal of Educational Psychology,

    103,970-981.

    Assor, A., Cohen-Melayev, M., Kaplan, A., & Friedman, D. (2005). Choosing to stay

    religious in a modern world: Socialization and exploration processes leading to an

    integrated internalization of religion among Israeli Jewish youth. In M. L. Maehr &

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    20/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    308

    S. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement, Vol. 14: Religion

    and motivation(pp. 105150). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good but relevance is excellent:

    Autonomy affecting teacher behaviors that predict students engagement in

    learning.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 261-278.

    Assor, A., Kaplan, H., Roth, G., & Kanat-Maymon, Y. (2005). Directly controlling teacher

    behaviors as predictors of poor motivation and engagement in girls and boys: The

    role of anger and anxiety. Learning and Instruction, 15, 397-413.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

    Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy.Harvard Mental Health Letter,13(9), 4-7.

    Bandura, A. (2005). The evolution of social cognitive theory. In K. G. Smith & M. A. Hitt

    (Eds.),Great minds in management(pp. 9-35). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Childrens interpersonal behaviors and the teacher-childrelationship.Developmental Psychology,34,934-946.

    Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors autonomy support and

    students autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-

    determination theory perspective. Science Education, 84,740756.

    Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different

    are they really?Educational Psychology Review, 15, 140.

    Bong, M., & Clark, R. E. (1999). Comparison between self-concept and self-efficacy in

    academic motivation research.Educational Psychology, 34,139154.Brophy, J. E. (1981). Teacher praise: A functional analysis. Review of Educational

    Research, 51, 5-32.

    Buff, A., Reusser, K., Rakoczy, K., & Pauli, C. (2011) Activating positive affectiveexperiences in the classroom: Nice to have or something more? Learning and

    Instruction, 21,452-466.

    Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an antecedent of young childrens

    school adjustment: An examination of mediating process. Developmental

    Psychology, 37, 550560.

    Butler, R. (1987). Task-involving and ego-involving properties of evaluation: Effects of

    different feedback conditions on motivational perceptions, interest, and

    performance.Journal of Educational Psychology, 79,474482.

    Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year

    college student performance and adjustment. Journal ofEducational Psychology,

    93, 5564.

    Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1994). Engagement Versus Disaffection: Motivated

    Patterns of Action In the Academic Domain. Rochester, NY: University of

    Rochester.

    Craven, R. G., & Marsh, H. W. (2008). The centrality of self-concept construct for

    psychological well-being and unlocking human potential: Implications for child

    and educational psychologists.Educational andChild Psychology, 25, 104118.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

    behavior.New York: Plenum Press.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs

    and the self determination of behavior.Psychologic Inquiry, 11,319338.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    21/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    309

    Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental social psychology: From infancy to old age. Malden,

    MA: Blackwell.

    Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and

    personality.Psychological Review, 95(2),256273.

    Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage environment fit: Developmentally appropriate

    classrooms for young adolescents. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on

    motivation in education: Vol. 3. Goals and cognitions (pp. 1344). New York:

    Academic Press.

    Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents

    achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social

    Psychology Bulletin, 21,215225.

    Eccles, J. S., Wigfeld, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). The development of achievement

    motivation. In N. Eisenberg, (Ed.),Handbook of child psychology(pp. 1017-1095,

    Vol. IV, 5th Ed.), New York: John Wiley.Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Midgley, C, Reuman, D., Maclver, D., & Feldlaufer, H. (1993).

    Negative effects of traditional middle schools on students' motivation. The

    Elementary School Journal,5,553-574.

    Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. L. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies,

    and exam performance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 91, 549563.

    Epstein, J. L., & McPartland, J. M. (1976). The concept and measurement of the quality of

    school life.AmericanEducational Research Journal, 13,1530.

    Feather, N. T. (1988). Values, valences, and course enrolment: Testing the role of personal

    values within an expectancy-valence framework. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 80, 381391.Finn, J. D. (1993). School engagement and students at risk. Washington, DC: National

    Center for Education Statistics.

    Flowerday, T., & Schraw, G. (2003). Effect of choice on cognitive and affective

    engagement.Journal of Educational Research, 96, 207215.

    Fredericks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C. & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of

    the concept, state of the evidence.Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59-109.

    Fulton, E. B., & Turner, L. A. (2008). Students academic motivation: relations with parental

    warmth, autonomy granting, and supervision.Educational Psychology, 28 (5), 521-

    534.

    Goldstein, L. S. (1999). The relational zone: The role of caring relationships in the co-

    construction of mind.American Educational ResearchJournal, 36, 647673.

    Hardre, P. L., & Reeve, J. (2003). A motivational model of rural students intentions to

    persist in, versus drop out of, high school.Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,

    347356.

    Hoyle, R. H., & Panther, A. T. (1995). Writing about structural equation models. In R.H.

    Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications(pp.

    158175). London: Sage

    Hughes, J. N., Luo, W., Kwok, O.-M., & Loyd, L. K. (2008). Teacher-student support,

    effortful engagement, and achievement: a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 100(1), 1-14.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    22/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    310

    Jang, H., Kim, E. J., & Reeve, J. (2012). Longitudinal test of self-determination theorys

    motivation mediation model in a naturally-occurring classroom context.Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 104, 1175-1188.

    Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in learning activities: its not

    autonomy support or structure, but autonomy support and structure. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 102,588-600.

    Jansen, P. W. A., & Bruinsma, M. (2005). Explaining achievement in higher education.

    Educational Research and Evaluation, 11, 235252.

    Katz, I. & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it does not. Educational

    Psychology Review, 19, 429-442.

    Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and change in early school engagement:

    Predictive of childrens achievement trajectories from first to eighth grade?Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 101,190206.

    Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. B. (1995). Effects of high school restructuring and size on early gainsin achievement and engagement. Sociology of Education, 68, 241270.

    Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Gore, P. A. (1997). Discriminant and predictive validity of

    academic self-concept, academic self-efficacy, and mathematics-specific self-

    efficacy.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 307-315.

    Maehr, M. L., & Andennan, E. M. (1993). Reinventing schools for early adolescents:

    Emphasizing task goals.The ElementarySchool Journal, 93,593-610.

    Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves.American Psychologist, 41, 954-969.

    Marsh, H. W. (2007). Self-concept theory, measurement and research into practice: The role

    of self concept in educational psychology. London, England: British Psychological

    Society.

    Marsh, H. W., & Craven, R. G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performancefrom a multidimensional perspective. Beyond seductive pleasure and

    unidimensional perspectives.Perspectives on PsychologicalScience, 1, 133-163.

    Marsh, H. W., & OMara, A. J. (2008). Self-concept is as multidisciplinary as it is

    multidimensional. In H. W. Marsh, R. G. Craven, & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Self-

    processes, learning, and enabling human potential.Dynamic new approaches (pp.

    87115). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

    Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K-T. (2003) Big fish little pond effect on academic self-concept: A

    crosscultural (26 country) test of the negative effects of academically selective

    schools.American Psychologist,58, 364-376.

    Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Ldtke, O., Baumert, J., & Kller, O. (2007). The big-fish

    little-pond effect: Persistent negative effects of selective high schools on self-

    concept after graduation.American EducationalResearch Journal, 44, 631669.

    Marsh, H. W., & Yeung, A. S. (1997). The causal effects of academic self-concept on

    academic achievement: Structural equation models of longitudinal data.Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 89, 4154.

    Meyer, D. K., & Turner, J. C. (2006). Reconceptualizing emotion and motivation to learn in

    classroom contexts.Educational Psychology Review, 18, 377-390.

    Middleton, M. J., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of lack of ability: An

    under explored aspect of goal theory. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 89, 710-

    718.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    23/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    311

    Midgley, C. (1993). Motivation and middle level schools. In P. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr

    (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 8. Motivation in the

    adolescent years(pp. 217-294). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Mih, (2013). Role of parental support for learning, autonomous/control motivation,

    and forms of self-regulation on academic attainment in high school students: a path

    analysis. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, An Interdisciplinary journal, 17(1), 35-60.

    Perry, R.P., Hladkyj, S., Reinhard, H.P., Clifton, R.A., & Chipperfield, J.G. (2005).

    Perceived academic control and failure in college students: A three-year study of

    scholastic attainment.Research in HigherEducation, 46, 523569.

    Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning

    components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 82, 33-40.

    Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and

    applications (2

    nd

    Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall.Ratelle, C. F., Larose, S., Guay, F., & Senecal, C. (2005). Perceptions of parental

    involvement and support as predictors of college students persistence in a science

    curriculum.Journal of Family Psychology,19, 268293.

    Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educational settings. In E. L. Deci &

    R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 183203).

    Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how

    they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44, 159-

    178.

    Reeve, J. (2012). A self-determination theory perspective on student engagement. In S. L.

    Christenson, A. Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on Studentengagement (pp. 149-172). New York: Springer.

    Reeve, J., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Self-determination theory: A dialectical

    framework for understanding socio-cultural influences on student motivation. In S.

    Van Etten & M. Pressley (Eds.), Big theories revisited (pp. 3160). Greenwich,

    CT: Information Age Press.

    Reeve, J., & Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do to support students autonomy

    during learning activities.Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 209-218.

    Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J. (2004). Enhancing high school

    students engagement by increasing their teachers autonomy support. Motivation

    and Emotion,28, 147-169.

    Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of the experience of self

    determination in intrinsic motivation and the conundrum of choice. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 95, 375392.

    Reynolds, W. M. (1988). Measurement of academic self-concept in college students.

    Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(2), 223-240.

    Reynolds, W. M., Ramirez, M. P., Magrina, A., & Allen, J. E. (1980). Initial development

    and validation of the academic self concept scale. Educational and Psychological

    Measurement, 40, 1013-1016.

    Rigby, C. S., Deci, E. L., Patrick, B. C., & Ryan, R. M. (1992). Beyond the intrinsic-

    extrinsic dichotomy: Self-determination in motivation and learning.Motivation and

    Emotion, 16, 165185.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    24/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    312

    Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions of the school psychological

    environment and early adolescents psychological and behavioral functioning in

    school: The mediating role of goals and belonging. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 88(3), 408-422.

    Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Vol.

    Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality

    development (5thEd.) (pp 105176). New York: Wiley.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic

    motivation, social development, and well-being.American Psychologist, 55, 68-78.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). Overview of self-determination theory: An organismic-

    dialectical perspective. In E. L. Deci and R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-

    determination research(pp. 333). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychologist,

    Special Issue: Current Issues and New Directions in Motivational Theory andResearch, 26(3-4), 207-231.

    Shavelson, R. J., & Bolus, R. (1982). Self-concept: The interplay of theory and methods.

    JournalEducational Psychology, 74,317.Skinner, E. A. (1990). Development and perceived control: A dynamic model of action in

    context. In M. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota symposium on child

    development (pp. 167215). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Skinner, E., & Belmont, M. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of

    teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 85, 571581.

    Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and

    disaffection in the classroom: part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal ofEducational Psychology, 100(4), 765-781.

    Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., & Furrer, C. J. (2009). A motivational perspective on

    engagement and disaffection: Conceptualization and assessment of childrens

    behavioral and emotional participation in academic activities in the classroom.

    Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69, 493525.

    Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Luyckx, K., & Goossens, L. (2006). Parenting and

    adolescent problem behavior: An integrated model with adolescent self-disclosure

    and perceived parental knowledge as intervening variables. Developmental

    Psychology, 42, 305318.

    Stefanou, C. R., Perencevich, K. C., DiCinto, M., & Turner, J. C. (2004). Supporting

    autonomy in the classroom: Ways teachers encourage student decision making and

    ownership.Educational Psychology, 39,97110.

    Stipek, D. (2002). Good instruction is motivating. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.),

    Development of achievement motivation (pp. 309332). San Diego, CA: Academic

    Press.

    Su, Y., & Reeve, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of intervention programs

    designed to support autonomy.Educational Psychology Review, 23, 159-188.

    Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate analysis. Boston: Allyn and

    Bacon.

    Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (2nd

    Ed.). Boston:

    Pearson.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    25/26

    V.Mih, C.Mih

    Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    17 (2013) 289-313

    313

    Tellefson, N. (2000). Classroom applications of cognitive theories of motivation.

    Educational Psychology Review, 12, 63-84.

    Urdan, T. C, Midgley, C. M., & Wood, S. (1995). Special issues in reforming middle level

    schools.Journal of Early Adolescence, 15,9-37.

    Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In

    M. P. Zanna (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol. 29, pp. 271-

    360). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Vansteenkiste, M., Niemiec, C., & Soenens, B. (2010). The development of the five mini-

    theories of self-determination theory: an historical overview, emerging trends, and

    future directions. In T. Urdan, & S. Karabenick (Eds.). The decade ahead.

    Advances in motivation and achievement, Vol. 16 (pp. 105-166). London: Emerald

    Group Publishing Limited.

    Voelkl, K. E. (1997). Identification with school. American Journal of Education, 105, 204

    319.Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation.

    Contemporary Educational Psychology. Special Issue: Motivation and the

    Educational Process, 25(1), 68-81.

    Wigfield, A., & Karpathian, M. (1991). Who am I and what can I do? Childrens self-

    concepts and motivation in achievement situations. Educational Psychology, 26,233-261.

    Wolters, C. (1999). The relation between high school students motivational regulation and

    their use of learning strategies, effort, and classroom performance. Learning and

    Individual Differences, 11, 281299.

    Wouters, S., Germeijs, V., Colpin, H., & Verschueren, K. (2011). Academic self-concept in

    high school: Predictors and effects on adjustment in higher education.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52, 586594.

    Wright, B. D. (1968). Sample-free test calibration and person measurement. Proceedings of

    the 1967 Invitational Conference on Testing Problems (pp. 8510). Princeton, N.J.:

    Educational Testing Service.

    Zeldin, A. L., & Pajares, F. (2000). Against the odds: Self-efficacy beliefs of women in

    mathematical, scientific, and technological careers. American Educational

    Research Journal, 37, 215246.

    Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing

    course attainment.American Educational ResearchJournal, 31, 845862.

  • 5/21/2018 Perceived Autonomy-supportive Teaching,

    26/26

    C o p y r i g h t o f C o g n i t i e , C r e i e r , C o m p o r t a m e n t / C o g n i t i o n , B r a i n , B e h a v i o r i s t h e p r o p e r t y o f

    E d i t u r a A S C R a n d i t s c o n t e n t m a y n o t b e c o p i e d o r e m a i l e d t o m u l t i p l e s i t e s o r p o s t e d t o a

    l i s t s e r v w i t h o u t t h e c o p y r i g h t h o l d e r ' s e x p r e s s w r i t t e n p e r m i s s i o n . H o w e v e r , u s e r s m a y p r i n t ,

    d o w n l o a d , o r e m a i l a r t i c l e s f o r i n d i v i d u a l u s e .