Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex of ...raghav/pdfs/animalbehavior/Reading...In...

8
Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex of echolocating bats M Ko ¨ ssl 1 , JC Hechavarria 1 , C Voss 1 , S Macias 2 , EC Mora 2 and M Vater 3 Computational brain maps as opposed to maps of receptor surfaces strongly reflect functional neuronal design principles. In echolocating bats, computational maps are established that topographically represent the distance of objects. These target range maps are derived from the temporal delay between emitted call and returning echo and constitute a regular representation of time (chronotopy). Basic features of these maps are innate, and in different bat species the map size and precision varies. An inherent advantage of target range maps is the implementation of mechanisms for lateral inhibition and excitatory feedback. Both can help to focus target ranging depending on the actual echolocation situation. However, these maps are not absolutely necessary for bat echolocation since there are bat species without cortical target-distance maps, which use alternative ensemble computation mechanisms. Addresses 1 Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe University, Frankfurt, Max-von-Laue-Str. 13, 60439 Frankfurt, Germany 2 Department of Animal and Human Biology, Faculty of Biology, Havana University, calle 25 No. 455 entre J e I, Vedado, CP 10400, Ciudad de La Habana, Cuba 3 Institute for Biochemistry and Biology, University of Potsdam, Karl Liebknecht Str. 26, 14476 Golm, Germany Corresponding author: Ko ¨ ssl, M ([email protected]) Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:6875 This review comes from a themed issue on Neural maps Edited by David Fitzpatrick and Nachum Ulanovsky For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial Available online 17th September 2013 0959-4388/$ see front matter, # 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.016 Introduction Sensory brain maps consist of topographically continuous neuronal representations of a certain stimulus feature. Such a representation can already be generated at the sensory surface and either reflects spatially continuous sensory input or properties of sensory filtering along the receptor surface like the cochlear hair cells. The other type of map is computational in the sense that it is created in the brain by extracting behaviourally relevant stimulus information [1]. For both types of maps, wiring optimiz- ation and economy regarding projections between mapped areas are an inherent advantage. In this sense, a topographically ordered wiring of brain areas should also require less genetic information than other wiring arrangements [2]. In addition, on a functional level, spatially restricted local neuronal interactions like lateral inhibition can be implemented easily within a spatial parameter gradient as provided by a map [3,4]. Within a map, topological substructures like clusters or pin- wheels can be created to optimize local function [5]. As pointed out by Schreiner and Winer [6], map topo- graphies and their connectional metric can also provide a stable basis for efficient functional transformations and dynamic remodelling during development, like changing head related transfer functions during head growth or neuromodulatory control of cortical plasticity [7,8 ]. Unlike the visual or somatosensory system where import- ant spatial relationships are already mapped on the re- ceptor surface, spatial auditory information has to be calculated de novo by comparing response properties of both ears and in some species is then represented in midbrain auditory space maps [1]. In the forebrain such type of continuous spatial map is no longer prominent and clustered types of representation prevail [e.g. 9]. This is also true for bat auditory cortex where clustered binaural interactions [10] and a clustered representation of dynamic spatial receptive fields could be demonstrated [11]. In the cortex of bats, there are computational maps that contain target-relevant information extracted from returning echoes [review: 12]. There are two major types of such maps: first, the delay (D) between emitted bio- sonar signal and returning echo is mapped to derive target range (R) with R = D*C/2 (C = sound velocity) (Figure 1). Within such a map individual neurons are most sensitive to a specific echo delay that is defined as the characteristic delay (CD). In the mustached bat, Pteronotus parnellii, a widely used bat model for auditory processing, three target distance maps have been demonstrated in the FM-FM, dorsal fringe and ventral fringe (DF, VF) cor- tical areas, respectively [13 ,1416], second, relative velocity between bat and object is mapped in form of Doppler-induced echo frequency shifts [17]. In contrast to any other receptor-surface-dominated or compu- tational map, input into these maps is actively controlled by the animal through its echolocation signal emission. Chronotopic target range maps in different bat species Target range maps were initially discovered by Suga, O’Neill and colleagues in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat P. parnellii by using passive auditory stimulation with pairs of frequency modulated (FM) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:6875 www.sciencedirect.com

Transcript of Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex of ...raghav/pdfs/animalbehavior/Reading...In...

  • Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex ofecholocating batsM Kössl1, JC Hechavarria1, C Voss1, S Macias2, EC Mora2 and M Vater3

    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

    ScienceDirect

    Computational brain maps as opposed to maps of receptor

    surfaces strongly reflect functional neuronal design principles.

    In echolocating bats, computational maps are established that

    topographically represent the distance of objects. These target

    range maps are derived from the temporal delay between

    emitted call and returning echo and constitute a regular

    representation of time (chronotopy). Basic features of these

    maps are innate, and in different bat species the map size and

    precision varies. An inherent advantage of target range maps is

    the implementation of mechanisms for lateral inhibition and

    excitatory feedback. Both can help to focus target ranging

    depending on the actual echolocation situation. However,

    these maps are not absolutely necessary for bat echolocation

    since there are bat species without cortical target-distance

    maps, which use alternative ensemble computation

    mechanisms.

    Addresses1 Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe University,

    Frankfurt, Max-von-Laue-Str. 13, 60439 Frankfurt, Germany2 Department of Animal and Human Biology, Faculty of Biology, Havana

    University, calle 25 No. 455 entre J e I, Vedado, CP 10400, Ciudad de La

    Habana, Cuba3 Institute for Biochemistry and Biology, University of Potsdam, Karl

    Liebknecht Str. 26, 14476 Golm, Germany

    Corresponding author: Kössl, M ([email protected])

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    This review comes from a themed issue on Neural maps

    Edited by David Fitzpatrick and Nachum Ulanovsky

    For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial

    Available online 17th September 2013

    0959-4388/$ – see front matter, # 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rightsreserved.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.016

    IntroductionSensory brain maps consist of topographically continuousneuronal representations of a certain stimulus feature.Such a representation can already be generated at thesensory surface and either reflects spatially continuoussensory input or properties of sensory filtering along thereceptor surface like the cochlear hair cells. The othertype of map is computational in the sense that it is createdin the brain by extracting behaviourally relevant stimulusinformation [1]. For both types of maps, wiring optimiz-ation and economy regarding projections betweenmapped areas are an inherent advantage. In this sense,a topographically ordered wiring of brain areas should also

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    require less genetic information than other wiringarrangements [2]. In addition, on a functional level,spatially restricted local neuronal interactions like lateralinhibition can be implemented easily within a spatialparameter gradient as provided by a map [3,4]. Withina map, topological substructures like clusters or pin-wheels can be created to optimize local function [5].As pointed out by Schreiner and Winer [6], map topo-graphies and their connectional metric can also provide astable basis for efficient functional transformations anddynamic remodelling during development, like changinghead related transfer functions during head growth orneuromodulatory control of cortical plasticity [7,8��].

    Unlike the visual or somatosensory system where import-ant spatial relationships are already mapped on the re-ceptor surface, spatial auditory information has to becalculated de novo by comparing response properties ofboth ears and in some species is then represented inmidbrain auditory space maps [1]. In the forebrain suchtype of continuous spatial map is no longer prominent andclustered types of representation prevail [e.g. 9]. This isalso true for bat auditory cortex where clustered binauralinteractions [10] and a clustered representation ofdynamic spatial receptive fields could be demonstrated[11]. In the cortex of bats, there are computational mapsthat contain target-relevant information extracted fromreturning echoes [review: 12]. There are two major typesof such maps: first, the delay (D) between emitted bio-sonar signal and returning echo is mapped to derive targetrange (R) with R = D*C/2 (C = sound velocity) (Figure 1).Within such a map individual neurons are most sensitiveto a specific echo delay that is defined as the characteristicdelay (CD). In the mustached bat, Pteronotus parnellii, awidely used bat model for auditory processing, threetarget distance maps have been demonstrated in theFM-FM, dorsal fringe and ventral fringe (DF, VF) cor-tical areas, respectively [13��,14–16], second, relativevelocity between bat and object is mapped in form ofDoppler-induced echo frequency shifts [17]. In contrastto any other receptor-surface-dominated or compu-tational map, input into these maps is actively controlledby the animal through its echolocation signal emission.

    Chronotopic target range maps in differentbat speciesTarget range maps were initially discovered by Suga,O’Neill and colleagues in the auditory cortex of themustached bat P. parnellii by using passive auditorystimulation with pairs of frequency modulated (FM)

    www.sciencedirect.com

    [email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/09594388/24http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.008http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.016http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/09594388

  • Target range maps in echolocating bats Kössl et al. 69

    Figure 1

    R = D*C/2

    R = target rangeD = echo delayC = sound velocity

    call

    echo

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    Echolocating bat that computes target range (R) from the echo delay (D).

    sweeps that mimic the FM components of echolocationsignal and echo. Neurons that preferentially respond to aspecific echo delay (Figure 2: examples of delay tuningcurves from different bat species) are arranged in approxi-mately rostrocaudal direction such that neurons respond-ing to short echo delay and hence short target distancesare represented more rostrally than neurons responding tolong echo delays (Figure 3). The mustached bat is a new

    Figure 2

    P. parnellii

    echo-d

    Ech

    o-le

    vel [

    dB S

    PL]

    90

    70

    50

    90

    70

    50

    30

    90

    70

    50

    30

    90

    70

    50

    30

    (a) (c)

    (b) (d)0 5 10 15 20

    0 5 10 15 20 0 5 10

    0 5 10

    P. qu

    Examples of receptive fields of delay-sensitive neurons in 3 bat species: P. pa

    of FM sweeps separated by a specific delay that represents sonar pulse and e

    delay are varied. Normalized neuronal response strength is color coded, red

    50% of maximal activity. The response area can be echo level invariant (a,c

    approaching objects on a single neuron basis (see text).

    www.sciencedirect.com

    world long-CF–FM bat where the FM component whichis important for target range estimation is preceded by aconstant frequency (CF) component that is used by thebat to exploit echo Doppler-shifts to derive informationon relative velocity. Velocity sensitive neurons are alsoarranged in form of a computational map (P. parnellii: CF–CF area, see Figure 3; [17]). Remarkably, chronotopy hasevolved convergently both in old and new world batfamilies. Rhinolophus rouxi, a bat species from the familyRhinolophidae that is widely distributed in the old worldpossesses a target range map located in the dorsal auditorycortex ([18], Figure 3). However, in the auditory cortex ofbats, that only employ FM biosonar signals, delay sensi-tive neurons are not necessarily arranged in form of targetdistance maps [19,20��,21]. In Eptesicus fuscus they formclusters that are located mainly within a high frequencycortex region where cortical tonotopy reverses ([21];Figure 3). Only recently were target maps discoveredfor a frugivorous FM bat, Carollia perspicillata [22��] andfor the insectivorous short-CF–FM bat Pteronotus quad-ridens [23�]. Interestingly, in C. perspicillata, delay-sensi-tive neurons occur in dorsal high frequency areas andwithin a region where tonotopy reverses in primary audi-tory cortex, as in E. fuscus (Figure 3).

    It is still open if the presence of a short or long CFcomponent in the echolocation signal and the accompa-nying added cortical computational complexityencourages the formation of a mapped target range pro-

    elay [ms]

    70

    50

    30

    70

    50

    30

    (e)

    (f)

    15 20 25

    15 20 0 5 10 15 20

    0 5 10 15 20

    25

    adridens C. perspicillata

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    rnellii, P. quadridens, and C. perspicillata. The stimulus consists of a pair

    cho. The call level is held constant at 70 or 80 dB SPL, the echo level and

    indicates maximal number of action potentials, the black line indicates

    ,e) or tilted (b,d,f). Tilt can provide for a certain amount of tracking of

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

  • 70 Neural maps

    Figure 3

    Rhinolophus rouxi FM-FM

    FM-FM

    FM-FM

    FM-FM

    CF/CF

    FA

    CF/CF

    Ala

    VF

    HFII

    HFI

    DP

    Al

    AAF

    RA

    AI

    AII

    AAF

    CF/CF

    FM-FM

    FADF

    DP

    DSCF

    HF

    RA

    VF

    primary auditory cortex

    secondary auditory cortex

    anterior auditory field

    CF/CF areaFM-FM area

    fovea area

    dorsal fringe

    dorsoposterior field

    highfrequency field

    rostral area

    ventral fringe

    doppler-shifted constantfrequency field

    AlI

    DSCFAlp

    DF

    D

    R C

    V

    AI

    Pteronotus parnellii

    Pteronotus quadridens

    Carollia perspicillata

    Myotis lucifugus

    Eptesicus fuscus

    100fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]fr

    eque

    ncy

    [kH

    z]

    50

    25 ms

    20 ms

    2 ms

    2 ms

    0

    100

    50

    0

    100

    50

    0

    100

    50

    0

    2 ms

    2 ms 1 mm 1 mm

    100

    50

    0

    100

    50

    0

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75 www.sciencedirect.com

  • Target range maps in echolocating bats Kössl et al. 71

    cessing area. However, the FM bat C. perspicillata hasimplemented a prominent target range map in the cortex.There are no CF components in the call of C. perspicillata(Figure 3). This could suggest that chronotopy is a verybasic feature at the root of the evolutionary tree of thesister groups of Phyllostomatidae, to which C. perspicillatabelongs, and the Mormoopidae with the genus Pteronotus[see 23�]. This hypothesis is strengthened by the pre-sence of chronotopic cortex organization in other phyl-lostomids [24�].

    Generation of cortical chronotopyIn bat species that have cortical chronotopy, the gener-ation of echo delay maps takes place through spatialsorting, and hence transformation of neuronal projectionsfrom inferior colliculus (where there is no map, see below)to auditory thalamus and cortex.

    The building blocks of cortical chronotopic maps aredelay-sensitive neuronal interactions occurring at subcor-tical levels. For P. parnellii it has been demonstrated thatfacilitatory delay-sensitivity in the ascending auditorysystem first emerges at the level of the central nucleusof the inferior colliculus (ICc; [25,26, review: 27��]).Paradoxically, the main components of creation of facil-itatory delay-sensitivity in ICc are glycinergic inputs[28,27��]. In addition, the ICc inherits a delay-tunedinhibition from the intermediate nucleus of the laterallemniscus (INLL), conveyed via an excitatory glutami-nergic input [28–32]. Within the ICc, delay tuned neuronsare integrated in the tonotopic representation and are notarranged according to CD [33], and they also can be tunedto sound duration [34]. The tectothalamic projectioncreates spatially discrete assemblies of delay-tunedneurons in two regions of the rostral half of the medialgeniculate body (MGB) that are organized according toharmonic frequency bands (FM2, 3, 4). Furthermore,there is a crude representation of characteristic delay inMGB [35, for further references see 27��].

    Thalamocortical projections feed three discrete rangingareas in AC of P. parnellii. The FM-FM-area and the VFreceive overlapping projections from rostral MGB derivedmainly from lateral parts, whereas the DF receives inputfrom medial parts [36]. Since there are massive cortico-thalamic backprojections, the cortex could also imprint itschronotopic organization onto its main input structure. Sofar, specific functional roles have not been assigned to themultiple delay representations.

    Figure 3 Legend Target range maps in different bat species. Left: represent

    position of auditory cortex. Right: detailed view of chronotopic maps within

    give direction of representation of decreasing echo delay. Black arrows indi

    that in E. fuscus and M. lucifugus, delay-sensitive neurons are not arranged

    Cortex data are from Schuller et al. [18], Suga [12], Hechavarria et al. [23�], Ha

    rouxi were kindly provided by D. Leipert, calls for M. lucifugus by B. Fenton

    www.sciencedirect.com

    Emergent features within target distancemapsNeural processing to create delay tuning in P. parnelliiappears largely complete at subcortical levels. The sharp-ness of delay tuning (50% width) is similar in IC, MGBand AC [15,26,35,37]. Furthermore, the range of CDs issimilar in IC, MGB, and AC with an overrepresentation ofdelays from 1 to 10 ms [15,26].

    Cortical delay-tuned responses, on the other handhave certain response features and show interactionsthat are not yet present at lower levels and could havebeen implemented with the help of a chronotopicgradient:� Among those are a higher specificity regarding stimulus

    type in P. parnellii for FM stimulus pairs and lessresponsiveness to single FM components or to puretone stimuli than in the IC and MGB [14,27��,37,38].However, in this respect cortical delay-tuned neuronsin C. perspicillata are clearly less specific and they allrespond vigorously to single pure tones [39�].

    � Mechanisms of lateral inhibition and excitatoryfeedback that sharpen or shift response tuning areone of the major advantages of a map, and could beused to provide dynamic plasticity of receptive fieldsduring learning or arousal [40]. There is ampleevidence from the work of Suga and colleagues onplasticity of neuronal tuning both in tonotopic and intarget range sensitive cortical areas in P. parnellii.They showed that a combination of widespread lateralinhibition in the cortex and highly focused excitatoryfeedback via projections to other cortical or sub-cortical areas creates a self-organizing map [8��,41].Cortical neurons within this map that code beha-viourally important stimuli, can augment the activityof neurons with a similar, ‘matched’ CD in the targetarea and also recruit additional neurons while activityof unmatched neurons is reduced. For the intra-cortical interaction between the 3 target distancemaps, this type of ‘egocentric selection’ has beendemonstrated by local cortical electrical activation[42–45]: The FM-FM area predominantly maintainsa strong suppressive influence on unmatchedneurons in the other two areas and on thecontralateral FM-FM area. This suppression canalso result in a shift of the CD of the unmatchedneurons away from the CD of the FM-FM neuron(centrifugal CD shift). In contrast, the DF and VFareas have a mostly augmenting influence onmatched neurons in the FM-FM area. This can

    ative spectrograms of echolocation calls. Middle: brain overview with the

    auditory cortex. Target range computing areas are in blue, white arrows

    cate increasing characteristic frequency in tonotopic areas. Please note

    in a chrontopic map but are interspersed within the tonotopic cortex.

    gemann et al. [22��], Dear et al. [21], Wong and Shannon [19]. Calls for R.

    , calls from E. fuscus by M. Gadziola.

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

  • 72 Neural maps

    shift the CD of matched neurons closer to the CD ofthe DF or VF neuron (centripetal CD shift). Thelatter could produce a focussing on and sharpeningof tuning to short echo delays in the FM-FM area,since the mapped delay range in DF and VF isrestricted to shorter delays in comparison to the FM-FM area [see 8��].This form of self-organizing feedback interaction is aquite powerful general neuronal organization principleand is also found in the interaction between cortex andICc and MGB [46,47]. In general, the dominance ofcentrifugal plasticity could shape the selective neuralrepresentation of a specific target distance and producecontrast enhancement. Dominance of centripetalactions could result in strong clustering and expandthe representation of a selected specific targetdistance.

    � In P. parnellii, P. quadridens, and C. perspicillata, asubstantial proportion of cortical delay-tunedneurons, in particular those responding to longerdelays at threshold have a tilted receptive field thatcould allow a certain degree of target tracking(Figure 2, [39�,23�]). When the bat approaches itstarget, echo intensity increases due to decreasingtarget-range. As a consequence, during the end stageof approach, only those neurons with appropriatelytilted receptive field will continue to respond tolouder echoes at shorter delays. In this respect tiltedreceptive fields loose specificity in terms of staticobject distance but gain specificity in terms ofresponding to echo series that are typical for approachto target — and may facilitate target tracking. We notethat some bat species reduce call intensity [48], andthen this sort of tracking at the level of individualneurons would not work. For P. parnellii tiltedresponse areas are present in the FM-FM area butare not found in the IC [49�]. They could be generatedwithin the topographic gradient of the map if there is alevel-dependent asymmetry of input convergence orinput integration in cortical neurons.

    � Map topography could also provide a more effectivemeans to exert local and delay-specific gain regulationby modulation through local GABAergic interneuronsor external modulatory systems.

    � Additional parameter representation: A 2D repres-entation of a single parameter (echo delay) in principleallows arranging additional axes orthogonal to the mapto represent/process other features. In the FM-FM andDF areas of P. parnellii, the three relevant frequencybands of the echo harmonics (FM2, 3, 4) are separatedand projected orthogonal to the delay axis [14,15]. Sucha harmonic dissociation is not found in P. quadridens orC. perspicillata and in R. rouxi there are no relevantmultiple harmonics in the echo.

    � Chronotopic maps could also serve non-target relevantpurposes: In P. parnellii, neurons in the FM-FM areanot only respond well to FM pulse-echo pairs of

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    specific delay but also to the specific temporal syntax ofsyllables within communication sounds [50,51].

    Chronotopic maps as interface to spatial-memory, decision-making, and motor-controlsystems?Chronotopic maps of target distance that, depending onthe echolocation situation, plastically adjust to mostrelevant input features (see above) could also providean efficient interface to other cortical processing systems.Completely unknown is the transfer of spatial infor-mation from target distance maps to hippocampal placecells that in bats have features comparable to those inrodents [52,53��].

    A chronotopically organized target distance representa-tion could provide an efficient interface to the motorsystem, in particular since there are target-distancespecific behaviours. Most notable is the switch fromlow call repetition rates during the approach phase ofecholocation to high call repetition rates in the finalphase shortly before the insect is caught. The wingcontrol breaking behaviour close to obstacles (alarmhypothesis, [18]) is also target-distance specific. Bothtypes of behaviour should be triggerable by short echodelays, and the above mentioned intracortical positivefeedback systems that enhance activity to short echodelays may be especially efficient for inducing thosebehaviours. There are also specific reactions to conspe-cifics if they fly close by [54].

    A chronotopic representation may also offer advantagesfor efficient input from decision-making systems. Theneurons in the FM-FM area that respond both tocall-echo pairs of specific delay and to complex communi-cation signals may have input from auditory regions offrontal cortex [55,56]. It is still to be tested if frontal cortexcould initiate a switching between both processing modesin the FM-FM area.

    But how are bats without target-distancemaps coping?In two insectivorous bat species that exclusively use FMecholocation signals and that predominantly hunt in openuncluttered space, target distance maps have not beendemonstrated. In E. fuscus (Figure 3) delay-sensitiveneurons are clustered mainly between two tonotopic areasand are interspersed with neurons sensitive to single puretones. In M. lucifugus the cortical location of those neuronsoverlaps with tonotopically arranged neurons. Theabsence of a delay map in E. fuscus has inspired research-ers to develop an alternative model for cortical repres-entation of target distance and acoustic scenes based onensemble coding over a larger number of neurons[20��,57,58��,59��]. It also has been demonstrated thatthe population of cortical neurons tuned to echo-delaycould integrate information from objects with different

    www.sciencedirect.com

  • Target range maps in echolocating bats Kössl et al. 73

    space-depths for the formation of acoustic images. Thelatter mechanism can manage a quite powerful processingof realistic echolocation call/echo sequences [59��] and itcould also be present in the cortex of bat species that havea delay map [60]. Therefore, at present there is still anopen discussion if delay maps are strictly necessary forefficient extraction of spatial target distance informationor if important features of such a processing also could beobtained by other cortical mechanisms.

    Innate cortical chronotopy in batsSharply tuned delay-sensitive cortical neurons arealready present in neonate P. parnellii and C. perspicil-lata, and their topography is comparable to those of adultmaps [61��]. In particular the neurons tuned to shortecho delay have receptive fields that are quite similar tothose of adults. In this respect the delay-tuned dorsalauditory cortex matures earlier than the tonotopicprimary auditory cortex [62]. The establishment ofthose first basic maps of target distance takes placewithout prior experience since the young bats do notyet echolocate [63,64] and therefore the maps seem tobe hardwired in early prenatal developmental stages. Ofcourse we expect that during ongoing postnatal devel-opment the above described feedback mechanismsexert a fine-tuning and adaptation of certain featuresof delay-tuned neurons. However, basic implementa-tion of target-distance sensitive neurons and maps isprobably of high evolutionary value such that a prewir-ing that is genetically determined takes place. It isnoteworthy that the a priori implementation of activespace perception in bats has its complement in passivespace perception: in young rodents, head-direction cellsand hippocampal place cells are already implementedbefore their first use [65,66].

    ConclusionActive space perception by means of echo-delay tunedneurons is essential for echolocating bats. In manyspecies, these neurons are arranged in cortical mapsthat possess a rather unique chronotopic neuronalrepresentation. Basic features of such target-distancemaps are innate and most probably hardwired. This apriori implementation of spatial perception emphasizestheir behavioural relevance for bats. Importantly, oncetarget-distance maps are established they seem to beconserved during evolution of bat families and theywere implemented at least two times independentlyin convergent evolution in old and new world bats.However, the functional relevance of such maps is stilldiscussed since there are other coding principles thatcould extract echo delay information from auditoryscenes. In addition, an ordered time representation onthe cortical surface could also be exploited for extractingsignal features that are not related to echolocation but tocommunication.

    www.sciencedirect.com

    References and recommended readingPapers of particular interest, published within the period of review,have been highlighted as:

    � of special interest�� of outstanding interest

    1. Knudsen EI, du Lac S, Esterly SD: Computational maps in thebrain. Annu Rev Neurosci 1987, 10:41-65.

    2. Weinberg RJ: Are topographic maps fundamental to sensoryprocessing? Brain Res Bull 1997, 44:113-116.

    3. Durbin R, Mitchison G: A dimension reduction framework forunderstanding cortical maps. Nature 1990, 343:644-647.

    4. Kaas JH: Topographic maps are fundamental to sensoryprocessing. Brain Res Bull 1997, 44:107-112.

    5. Chklovskii DB, Koulakov AA: Maps in the brain: what can welearn from them? Annu Rev Neurosci 2004, 27:369-392.

    6. Schreiner CE, Winer JA: Auditory cortex mapmaking: principles,projections, and plasticity. Neuron 2007, 56:356-365.

    7. Ji W, Suga N: Serotoninergic modulation of plasticity of theauditory cortex elicited by fear conditioning. J Neurosci 2007,27:4910-4918.

    8.��

    Suga N: Tuning shifts of the auditory system by corticocorticaland corticofugal projections and conditioning. NeurosciBiobehav Rev 2012, 36:969-988.

    Comprehensive review on mechanisms for adaptive plastiticity andlearning in bat auditory system. Both frequency and delay tuning arediscussed in detail. The paper highlights the involvement of lateralinhibition and excitatory feedback and discusses the role of corticofugalprojections.

    9. Cohen YE, Knudsen EI: Maps versus clusters: differentrepresentations of auditory space in the midbrain andforebrain. Trends Neurosci 1999, 22:129-136.

    10. Razak KA, Fuzessery ZM: Functional organization of the pallidbat auditory cortex: emphasis on binaural organization.J Neurophysiol 2002, 87:72-86.

    11. Hoffman S, Schuller G, Firzlaff U: Dynamic stimulation evokesspatially focused receptive fields in bat auditory cortex. Eur JNeurosci 2010, 31:371-385.

    12. Suga N: Cortical computational maps for auditory imaging.Neural Netw 1990, 3:3-21.

    13.��

    Suga N, O’Neill WE: Neural axis representing target range in theauditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science 1979, 206:351-353.

    Classic on discovery of target-distance maps.

    14. O’Neill WE, Suga N: Encoding of target range information andits representation in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat.J Neurosci 1982, 2:17-31.

    15. Suga N, Horikawa J: Multiple time axes for representation ofecho delays in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat.J Neurophysiol 1986, 55:776-805.

    16. Edamatsu H, Suga N: Differences in response properties ofneurons between two delay-tuned areas in the auditory cortexof the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol 1993, 69:1700-1712.

    17. Suga N, O’Neill WE, Kujirai K, Manabe T: Specificity ofcombination-sensitive neurons for processing of complexbisonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat.J Neurophysiol 1983, 49:1573-1626.

    18. Schuller G, O’Neill WE, Radtke-Schuller S: Facilitation and delaysensitivity of auditory cortex neurons in CF–FM bats,Rhinolophus rouxi and Pteronotus p. parnellii. Eur J Neurosci1991, 3:1165-1181.

    19. Wong D, Shannon SL: Functional zones in the auditory cortex ofthe echolocating bat, Myotis lucifugus. Brain Res 1988,453:349-352.

    20.��

    Dear SP, Simmons JR, Fritz J: A possible neuronal basis forrepresentation of acoustic scenes in auditory cortex of the bigbrown bat. Nature 1993, 364:620-623.

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0005http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0005http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0010http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0010http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0015http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0015http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0030http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0030http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0035http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0035http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0035http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0040http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0040http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0040http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0045http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0045http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0045http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0050http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0050http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0050http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0055http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0055http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0055http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0060http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0060http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0065http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0065http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0070http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0070http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0070http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0075http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0075http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0075http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0080http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0080http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0080http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0090http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0090http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0090http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0090http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0095http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0095http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0095http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0100http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0100http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0100

  • 74 Neural maps

    Classic paper on neuronal computations that serve auditory sceneanalysis without the use of target distance maps.

    21. Dear SP, Fritz J, Haresign T, Ferragamo M, Simmons JR:Tonotopic and functional organization in the auditory cortex ofthe big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Neurophysiol 1993,70:1988-2009.

    22.��

    Hagemann C, Esser KH, Kössl M: A chronotopically organizedtarget distance map in the auditory cortex of the short-tailedfruit bat. J Neurophysiol 2010, 103:222-333.

    Discovery of delay sensitive neurons and a large target-distance map in abat species that is not specialized for insect hunting but is frugivorous.

    23.�

    Hechavarria JC, Macias S, Vater M, Mora EC, Kössl M: Evolutionof neuronal mechanisms for echolocation: specializations fortarget-range computation in bats of the genus Pteronotus. JAcoust Soc Am 2013, 133:570-578.

    The FM-bat P. quadridens uses heteroharmonic echolocation and cor-tical target distance maps just like its relative, the CF–FM bat P. parnellii.This suggests that target-distance maps and heteroharmonic echoloca-tion could have been subjected to positive Darwinian selection in theevolution of bats.

    24.�

    Hoffmann S, Warmbold A, Wiegrebe L, Firzlaff U: Spatio-temporal contrast enhancement and feature extraction in thebat auditory midbrain and cortex. J Neurophysiol 2013. (inpress).

    Binaural presentation of semi-natural echo sequences demonstrates thatdelay-sensitive cortical neurons are involved in extraction of direction anddistance of objects.

    25. Mittmann DH, Wenstrup JJ: Combination-sensitive neurons inthe inferior colliculus. Hear Res 1995, 90:185-191.

    26. Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ: Delay-tuned neurons in the inferiorcolliculus of the mustached bat: implications for analyses oftarget distance. J Neurophysiol 1999, 82:1326-1338.

    27.��

    Wenstrup JJ, Portfors CV: Neural processing of target distanceby echolocating bats: functional roles of the auditorymidbrain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011, 35:2073-2083.

    Delay tuning but not chronotopy is created in the midbrain. This reviewgives comprehensive insight into cellular mechanisms and discussesrelevant processing strategies for generation of delay tuning.

    28. Sanchez J, Gans D, Wenstrup JJ: Glycinergic ‘inhibition’mediates selective excitatory response to combinations ofsounds. J Neurosci 2008, 28:80-90.

    29. Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ: Responses to combinations of tonesin the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol2001, 2:104-117.

    30. Peterson DC, Nataraj K, Wenstrup JJ: Glycinergic inhibitioncreates a form of spectral integration in nuclei of the laterallemniscus. J Neurophysiol 2009, 102:1004-1016.

    31. Peterson DC, Voytenko S, Gans D, Galazyuk A, Wenstrup JJ:Intracellular recordings from combination-sensitive neuronsin the inferior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 2008, 100:629-634.

    32. Yavuzoglu A, Schofield BR, Wenstrup JJ: Substrates of auditoryfrequency integration in a nucleus of the lateral lemniscus.Neuroscience 2010, 169:906-919.

    33. Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ: Topographical distribution of delay-tuned responses in the mustached bat inferior colliculus. HearRes 2001, 151:95-105.

    34. Macias S, Hechavarria JC, Kössl M, Mora EC: Neurons in theinferior colliculus of the mustached bat are tuned both toecho-delay and sound duration. Neuroreport 2013, 24:404-409.

    35. Wenstrup JJ: Frequency organization and responses tocomplex sounds in the medial geniculate body of themustached bat. J Neurophysiol 1999, 82:2528-2544.

    36. Pearson JM, Crocker WD, Fitzpatrick DC: Connections offunctional areas in the mustached bat’s auditory cortex withthe auditory thalamus. J Comp Neurol 2007, 500:401-418.

    37. Olsen JF, Suga N: Combination-sensitive neurons in the medialgeniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of targetrange information. J Neurophysiol 1991, 65:1275-1296.

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    38. O’Neill WE, Suga N: Target range-sensitive neurons in theauditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science 1979, 203:69-73.

    39.�

    Hagemann C, Vater M, Kössl M: Comparison of properties ofcortical echo delay-tuning in the short-tailed fruit bat and themustached bat. J Comp Physiol A 2011, 197:605-613.

    Receptive fields of delay-sensitive neurons are remarkably similarbetween insectivorous and frugivorous bat species.

    40. Weinberger NM: Dynamic regulation of receptive fields andmaps in the adult sensory cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 1995,18:129-158.

    41. Suga N, Yan J, Zhang Y: Cortical maps for hearing and egocentricselection for self-organization. Trends Cog Sci 1997, 1:13-20.

    42. Xiao Z, Suga N: Reorganization of the auditory cortexspecialized for echo-delay processing in the mustached bat.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004, 101:1769-1774.

    43. Tang J, Suga N: Modulation of auditory processing by cortico-cortical feed-forward and feedback projections. Proc NatlAcad Sci USA 2008, 105:7600-7605.

    44. Tang J, Suga N: Corticocortical interactions between andwithin three cortical auditory areas specialized for time-domain signal processing. J Neurosci 2009, 29:7230-7237.

    45. Tang J, Xiao Z, Suga N: Bilateral cortical interaction:modulation of delay-tuned neurons in the contralateralauditory cortex. J Neurosci 2007, 27:8405-8413.

    46. Yan J, Suga N: Corticofugal modulation of time-domainprocessing of biosonar information in bats. Science 1996,273:1100-1103.

    47. Yan J, Suga N: Corticofugal amplification of facilitativeauditory responses of subcortical combination-sensitiveneurons in the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol 1999, 81:817-824.

    48. Hiryu S, Shiori Y, Hosokawa T, Riquimaroux H, Watanabe Y: On-board telemetry of emitted sounds from free-flying bats:compensation for velocity and distance stabilizes echofrequency and amplitude. J Comp Physiol A 2008, 194:841-851.

    49.�

    Macias S, Mora EC, Hechavarrı́a JC, Kössl M: Properties of echodelay-tuning receptive fields in the inferior colliculus of themustached bat. Hear Res 2012, 286:1-8.

    Comparison of echo delay sensitive neurons between midbrain andcortex in P. parnellii.

    50. Esser K-H, Condon CJ, Suga N, Kanwal JS: Syntax processingby auditory cortical neurons in the FM–FM area of themustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA1997, 94:14019-14024.

    51. Kanwal JS, Rauschecker JP: Auditory cortex of bats andprimates: managing species-specific calls for socialcommunication. Front Biosci 2007, 12:4621-4640.

    52. Ulanovsky N, Moss CF: Hippocampal cellular and networkactivity in freely moving echolocating bats. Nat Neurosci 2007,10:224-233.

    53.��

    Ulanovsky N, Moss CF: Dynamics of hippocampal spatialrepresentation in echolocating bats. Hippocampus 2011,21:150-161.

    Evidence that bats possess space-sensitive neurons in the hippocampus.The data show how spatial representation depends on echolocationbehavior and exploratory modes.

    54. Chiu C, Xian W, Moss CF: Adaptive echolocation behavior inbats for the analysis of auditory scenes. J Exp Biol 2009,212:1392-1404.

    55. Eiermann A, Esser KH: Auditory responses from the frontalcortex in the short-tailed fruit bat Carollia perspicillata.Neuroreport 2000, 11:421-425.

    56. Kanwal JS, Gordon M, Peng JP, Heinz-Esser K: Auditoryresponses from the frontal cortex in the mustached bat,Pteronotus parnellii. Neuroreport 2000, 11:367-372.

    57. Sanderson MI, Simmons JA: Selectivity for echo spectralinterference and delay in the auditory cortex of the big brownbat Eptesicus fuscus. J Neurophysiol 2002, 87:2823-2834.

    www.sciencedirect.com

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0110http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0110http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0110http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0115http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0115http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0115http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0115http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00226.2013http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00226.2013http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0125http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0125http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0130http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0130http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0130http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0135http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0135http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0135http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0140http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0140http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0140http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0145http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0145http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0145http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0150http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0150http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0150http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0155http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0155http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0155http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0160http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0160http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0160http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0165http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0165http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0165http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0170http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0170http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0170http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0175http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0175http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0175http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0180http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0180http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0180http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0180http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0185http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0185http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0185http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0190http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0190http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0195http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0195http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0195http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0200http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0200http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0200http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0205http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0205http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0210http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0210http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0210http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0215http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0215http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0215http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0220http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0220http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0220http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0225http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0225http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0225http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0230http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0230http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0230http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0235http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0235http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0235http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0245http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0245http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0245http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0255http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0255http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0255http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0260http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0260http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0260http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0265http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0265http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0265http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0270http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0270http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0270http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0275http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0275http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0275http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0280http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0280http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0280http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0285http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0285http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0285

  • Target range maps in echolocating bats Kössl et al. 75

    58.��

    Bates ME, Simmons JA, Zorikov TV: Bats exploit echo harmonicstructure to distinguish targets from clutter and suppressinterference. Science 2011, 333:627-630.

    Evidence for the efficiency of sonar imaging and separation of objectsfrom clutter by a bat species where neuronal auditory computations donot involve target distance maps.

    59.��

    Simmons JA: Bats use a neuronally implementedcomputational acoustic model to form sonar images. CurrOpin Neurobiol 2012, 22:311-319.

    Detailed discourse on neuronal computational mechanisms that cancreate an internal image of acoustic scenes for echolocation.

    60. Hechavarrı́a JC, Macı́as S, Vater M, Mora EC, Kössl M: Corticaltarget-distance maps of bats render biased point-to-pointrepresentations of space-depth. In abstracts of the 4thInternational Conference on Auditory Cortex; Lausanne,Switzerland: 2012:64.

    61.��

    Kössl M, Voss C, Mora E, Macı́as S, Foeller E, Vater M: Auditorycortex of newborn bats is prewired for echolocation. NatCommun 2012, 3:773 http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1782.

    Delay-sensitive neurons and cortical chronotopy are innate. This implies

    www.sciencedirect.com

    that basic features of space perception in bats are implemented a prioriand most likely by genetically determined mechanisms.

    62. Vater M, Foeller E, Mora EC, Coro F, Russell IJ, Kössl M: Postnatalmaturation of primary auditory cortex in the mustached bat,Pteronotus parnellii. J Neurophysiol 2010, 103:2339-2354.

    63. Vater M, Kössl M, Foeller E, Coro F, Mora E, Russell IJ:Development of echolocation calls in the mustached bat,Pteronotus parnellii. J Neurophysiol 2003, 90:2274-2290.

    64. Sterbing SJ: Postnatal development of vocalizations andhearing in the phyllostomid bat, Carollia perspicillata. JMammal 2002, 83:516-525.

    65. Wills TJ, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O’Keefe J: Development of thehippocampal cognitive map in preweanling rats. Science 2010,328:1573-1576.

    66. Langston RF, Ainge JA, Couey JJ, Canto CB, Bjerknes TL,Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB: Development of the spatialrepresentation system in the rat. Science 2010, 328:1576-1580.

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2014, 24:68–75

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0290http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0290http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0290http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0295http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0295http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0295http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0300http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1782http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0310http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0310http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0310http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0315http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0315http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0315http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0320http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0320http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0320http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0325http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0325http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0325http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0330http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0330http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0330http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4388(13)00175-X/sbref0330

    Neural maps for target range in the auditory cortex of echolocating batsIntroductionChronotopic target range maps in different bat speciesGeneration of cortical chronotopyEmergent features within target distance mapsChronotopic maps as interface to spatial-memory, decision-making, and motor-control systems?But how are bats without target-distance maps coping?Innate cortical chronotopy in batsConclusionReferences and recommended reading