Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward...

45
COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 1 Pursuing Perfection or Pursuing Protection?: Self-Evaluation Motives Moderate the Behavioral Consequences of Counterfactual Thoughts Maurissa P. Tyser Sean M. McCrea University of Wyoming Kristin Knüpfer University of Konstanz Address for correspondence: Sean McCrea Department of Psychology University of Wyoming Dept. 3415 1000 E. University Ave. Laramie, WY 82071 USA Tel.: (307) 766-6149 Fax: (307) 766-2926 Preprint of Tyser, M. P., McCrea, S. M., & Knuepfer, K. (2012). Pursuing perfection or pursuing protection? Self-evaluation concerns and the motivational consequences of counterfactual thinking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 372-382. Full version

Transcript of Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward...

Page 1: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 1

Pursuing Perfection or Pursuing Protection?: Self-Evaluation Motives Moderate the Behavioral

Consequences of Counterfactual Thoughts

Maurissa P. Tyser

Sean M. McCrea

University of Wyoming

Kristin Knüpfer

University of Konstanz

Address for correspondence:

Sean McCrea

Department of Psychology

University of Wyoming

Dept. 3415

1000 E. University Ave.

Laramie, WY 82071 USA

Tel.: (307) 766-6149

Fax: (307) 766-2926

Preprint of Tyser, M. P., McCrea, S. M., & Knuepfer, K. (2012). Pursuing perfection or pursuing

protection? Self-evaluation concerns and the motivational consequences of counterfactual

thinking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 372-382.

Full version

Page 2: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 2

Author Note

Maurissa P. Tyser, Sean M. McCrea, Department of Psychology, University of Wyoming,

Laramie, WY, 82071, USA.

Kristin Knüpfer, Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany.

The authors wish to thank Asuka Ogawa for serving as an experimenter, Benjamin Ernst and

Alex Jaudas for programming the Swahili task, and members of the University of Wyoming

Experimental Brownbag and University of Konstanz Social Colloquium for their insightful

comments. This research was supported by a grant to the second author from the German

Science Foundation (DFG MC68/2-1).

Page 3: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 3

Abstract

Counterfactual thoughts identifying how a past performance could have been better (e.g., “If

only I had studied for the exam, I would have gotten an A!”) have been shown to increase effort

and performance on future tasks. The present work examines whether current self-evaluation

motives moderate this link between past and future behavior. In two studies, we demonstrate that

the preparatory benefits of counterfactual thoughts are limited to situations in which individuals

pursue a self-improvement motive. When individuals are instead motivated by self-protection

concerns, counterfactuals can be used to excuse poor performance, undermining any desire to

improve in the future. The behavioral consequences of counterfactual thoughts are therefore

dependent upon active self-evaluation motives.

Page 4: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 4

Pursuing Perfection or Pursuing Protection?: Self-Evaluation Motives Moderate the Behavioral

Consequences of Counterfactual Thoughts

Thinking about the past offers the opportunity to evaluate our progress towards important

goals, improve upon past mistakes, and learn about our personal strengths and weaknesses.

Imagine the thoughts of a student who has just learned of a poor grade on an exam. Perhaps the

student would say to him or herself “if I had studied, I would have passed the test” or “if I hadn’t

been sick and missed so many classes, the test would have been easy.” Upward counterfactual

thoughts such as these identify how an outcome could have been better, and are strongly evoked

by difficulties that arise during goal pursuit (Epstude & Roese, 2008). Because these thoughts

identify factors that hindered a past performance, they have important consequences for future

goal-directed action. Consistent with this view, past work has shown that upward counterfactual

thinking facilitates the formation of future intentions (Smallman & Roese, 2009), increases self-

efficacy (Tal-Or, Boninger, & Gleicher, 2004), and improves task performance (Roese, 1994).

Because counterfactual thoughts involve the evaluation of goal progress, they should also be

relevant to judging the self (Epstude & Roese, 2008; McCrea, 2008; Sanna, Chang, & Meier,

2001). Therefore, we suggest that self-evaluation motives are an important moderator of the

consequences of counterfactual thinking for subsequent affect and behavior. We focus in the

present work on the operation of self-improvement and self-protection motives in counterfactual

thinking. A person concerned with self-improvement and personal growth may use

counterfactuals to determine how a failure occurred and identify corrective actions to be taken in

the future. In contrast, individuals concerned with self-esteem protection may use counterfactuals

to shift the blame for failure, with the consequence that no corrective actions are taken.

Returning to the example of the failing student, thinking “if I had studied, I would have passed

Page 5: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 5

the test” may reflect disappointment and increase intentions to change one’s behavior.

Alternatively, the very same thought may suggest an excuse for a performance that would

otherwise be attributed to a lack of ability. In this case, there would be little incentive for the

student to change his or her behavior. Our research was designed to test whether overarching

self-evaluation motivations moderate the consequences of counterfactual thoughts for subsequent

preparatory effort and performance in this manner.

Models of Self-Evaluation

Self-evaluation is the process by which individuals make judgments about and modify the

self-concept (Sedikides & Strube, 1997), and has been identified as a critical aspect of self-

regulation (Taylor, Neter, & Wayment, 1995). Two of the primary motives in self-evaluation are

self-improvement, the desire to consider one’s past shortcomings in order to grow in the future,

and self-enhancement and protection, the desire to positively judge one’s performance and

ability in order to maintain or increase self-esteem (Crocker & Park, 2004; Greenwald, 1980;

Sedikides & Strube, 1997; Taylor et al., 1995; Trope, 1986). The existence of these motives has

been demonstrated in a wide range of contexts, including social comparison (Taylor et al., 1995),

task choice (Trope, 1986), information search (Butler, 1993; Trope, Ferguson, & Raghunathan,

2001), and causal attribution (Zuckerman, 1979). For example, attributing failure to a lack of

effort has different motivational consequences, depending on whether the individual is concerned

with self-improvement or self-protection. Effort attributions for failure on threatening tasks have

been shown to protect mood and self-esteem, thereby serving self-protection concerns

(Covington & Omelich, 1979; Jones & Berglas, 1978; Weiner, 1985). When individuals are

instead motivated by self-improvement, attributions of failure to a lack of effort maintain

expectations of success and increase subsequent task persistence (Bandura, 1977; Weiner, 1985).

Page 6: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 6

Which motive an individual pursues in a given situation depends on a number of factors. For

example, when ability in a domain is perceived as malleable rather than fixed (Dunning, 1995;

Dweck & Leggett, 1988), when individuals adopt mastery-approach rather than performance-

avoidance goals (Elliot & Church, 2003; Elliot, Cury, Fryer, & Huguet, 2006), when self-worth

is not staked on the outcome of the task (Butler, 1993; Crocker & Wolfe, 2001), or when self-

protection concerns have already been addressed (D. K. Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Trope et al.,

2001), long-term self-improvement motivations are stronger than more short-term self-protection

motivations. Both motives arise to some degree in many situations (Taylor et al., 1995), and as a

result, they can often come into conflict. For example, self-protection concerns might lead one to

discount or ignore potentially useful negative feedback, whereas self-improvement concerns

might require attention to information that could be damaging to mood and self-esteem (D. K.

Sherman, Nelson, & Steele, 2000; Trope et al., 2001). This conflict has been noted by

researchers who argue that the pursuit of self-esteem can have negative implications for

performance and self-regulation (Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Crocker &

Park, 2004), and that attempts to cope with controllable events solely through the regulation of

emotion are less effective than more problem-focused approaches (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

Counterfactual Thinking

Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese,

2008; Roese, 1994). Epstude and Roese (2008) postulated two pathways through which

counterfactuals affect subsequent behavior: one specific to thought content and one neutral with

regard to thought content. The content-specific pathway involves the identification of corrective

actions (Smallman & Roese, 2009). Because upward counterfactuals suggest why a better

performance did not occur, individuals can convert these thoughts into behavioral intentions. For

Page 7: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 7

example, the thought “if I had studied more, I could have done better on the exam” can easily be

translated into the strategy “I will study more for the next exam.” Smallman and Roese (2009)

found substantial support for the notion that counterfactual thoughts facilitate the spontaneous

formation of corresponding future intentions. In contrast, the content-neutral pathway involves

benefits of counterfactual thinking for performance that are independent of any specific strategy

mentioned in the thought (Epstude & Roese, 2008). Content-neutral effects could involve

increased self-efficacy (Tal-Or et al., 2004), facilitation of flexible processing (Galinsky &

Moskowitz, 2000), or heightened task motivation (Markman, McMullen, & Elizaga, 2008). The

present work particularly focuses on the consequences of counterfactual thought for task

motivation. For example, the Reflection-Evaluation Model (REM, Markman & McMullen, 2003)

proposes that affect resulting from counterfactual thinking influences judgments of whether one

has met the goal for a task. Negative affect resulting from upward counterfactual thinking can be

taken as evidence that goal progress has been insufficient, and so task effort is increased (see

also Gendolla, 2000; Hirt, Levine, McDonald, Melton, & Martin, 1997). Similar predictions are

made by a number of classic models of motivation. For example, both control theory (Carver &

Scheier, 1999) and social cognitive theory (Bandura & Cervone, 1983; Bandura & Locke, 2003)

hold that goal-directed effort is increased only when the person perceives a discrepancy between

the goal and current performance. Markman, McMullen, and Elizaga (2008) found evidence for

these predictions, demonstrating that benefits of counterfactual thinking for task persistence and

performance were mediated by negative affect. Thus, upward counterfactual thinking seems to

involve a trade-off of negative affect in the present for improved performance in the future.

Counterfactual Thinking and Self-Evaluation

Page 8: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 8

Another way to conceptualize the consequences of counterfactual thinking is to consider the

implications of these thoughts for self-evaluation. Because of their role in evaluating progress

toward important goals (Epstude & Roese, 2008) and in guiding judgments of causality (Wells &

Gavanski, 1989), counterfactuals have important implications for judging the self (McCrea,

2008; Sanna et al., 2001). Indeed, previous research has demonstrated that self-evaluation

motives affect the generation of counterfactual thoughts (McCrea, 2007; Roese & Olson, 1993;

Sanna et al., 2001). We believe that self-evaluation motives also moderate the affective and

behavioral consequences of counterfactual thinking. However, there have been only a few

studies testing this contention. For example, Shalvi, Dana, Handgraaf, and De Dreu (2011)

recently examined whether the availability of upward counterfactuals could be used to justify

unethical behavior. Participants in these studies tended to report the best outcome of a series of

die-rolls made in private, even when only the first roll of the die was supposed to be counted. In

other words, the relative ease of imagining that one “could have” rolled a six made it easier for

participants to claim that they had in fact rolled a six. The motive to justify their deceitful action

therefore led participants to report the counterfactual alternative as the real outcome.

McCrea (2008) examined the role of self-evaluation in counterfactual thinking within the

context of self-handicapping behavior. Self-handicapping is a self-protective strategy in which

the individual creates an obstacle to success (e.g., not preparing adequately for a test) in order to

shift blame away from the self, and onto the handicap, should failure ensue (Jones & Berglas,

1978). In attribution terms, failure is attributed to low effort (an unstable cause) rather than to

low ability (a stable cause, Covington & Omelich, 1979; Weiner, 1985). From the perspective of

counterfactual thinking, self-handicapping facilitates the generation of upward counterfactual

thoughts that can be used to excuse failure (S. J. Sherman & McConnell, 1995). That is,

Page 9: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 9

individuals may find it easier to excuse a poor performance if they are able to say “I could have

succeeded” if not for the handicap. To test this prediction, McCrea (2008) examined the

consequences of upward counterfactual thinking in the context of a threatening test of

intelligence. Using a modified induced-compliance procedure, participants were led to practice

insufficiently for the intelligence test, and they subsequently received failure feedback. Half of

the participants were asked to consider the thought “if I had practiced more, I would have gotten

a better score,” whereas the remainder of participants considered a neutral statement. All

participants then completed a measure of state self-esteem and a second version of the test.

Individuals who considered the upward counterfactual reported more positive self-esteem, but

lower effort and performance on the subsequent test, than did individuals in the control

condition. When faced with a threatening performance, individuals used the upward

counterfactual as an excuse for failure. As would be expected from attributional models

(Covington & Omelich, 1979; Jones & Berglas, 1978), self-esteem was maintained, but

motivation to improve on the subsequent task was undermined. Consistent with the REM

(Markman & McMullen, 2003), upward counterfactuals did not increase task effort or

performance when they were processed in a manner that maintained or increased positive affect.

In other words, when the upward counterfactual is used as an excuse, goal progress is considered

sufficient and there is little motivation to take corrective action.

These findings suggest that the same counterfactual thought can have different behavioral

consequences, dependent on the self-evaluation motives of the individual. However, McCrea

(2008) did not explicitly compare the consequences of upward counterfactual made in the service

of self-protection to those made in the service of self-improvement. The present work was

designed to do just that. Specifically, we test the extent to which achievement goals and the prior

Page 10: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 10

satisfaction of self-protection motives via self-affirmation influence the consequences of an

upward counterfactual thought for subsequent preparation and performance. We examine these

questions in the context of insufficient effort, which could serve as a self-handicap when

participants seek to protect self-esteem (Jones & Berglas, 1978; McCrea, 2008), or motivate

additional effort when individuals seek to improve their performance (Bandura, 1977; Markman

et al., 2008; Roese, 1994; Weiner, 1985). We predicted that, when individuals pursue self-

improvement, upward counterfactuals would lead to increased preparative effort and

performance. When individuals pursue self-protection, we expected that upward counterfactual

thoughts would be used to excuse failure, thereby undermining preparative effort and

performance.

Study 1 – Prior Satisfaction of Self-Protection Motives

In an initial study, we examined the moderating role of self-improvement motives in

determining the consequences of upward counterfactual thinking for preparatory effort on an

ego-threatening task. Participants were told they would be taking memory tests predictive of

intelligence and academic success. They received failure feedback on the first memory test.

Preparatory effort for the subsequent test was the key measure of interest. To manipulate self-

evaluation motives, we relied on past work demonstrating that individuals are more likely adopt

a self-improvement motive if their self-protection concerns have been previously addressed.

Affirming the overall integrity of the self reduces a variety of self-protective behaviors, including

downward social comparison (Tesser & Cornell, 1991), rejecting threatening feedback (D. K.

Sherman et al., 2000), and self-handicapping (McCrea & Hirt, 2011; Siegel, Scillitoe, & Parks-

Yancy, 2005). Based on these findings, we predicted that the prior satisfaction of self-protection

concerns via self-affirmation would result in the adoption of a self-improvement motive. As a

Page 11: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 11

result, participants in this condition were expected to invest more effort preparing for the

subsequent task if they considered an upward counterfactual thought indicating their failure was

due to inadequate effort than if they considered a neutral thought. Conversely, those facing an

ego-threatening task who had not previously affirmed the self were expected to pursue a self-

protection motive. Under a self-protection motive, the same counterfactual should be used to

excuse failure, thereby undermining any benefit of the thought for subsequent task motivation.

Thus, we predicted that within the no affirmation condition, preparatory effort in the

counterfactual thought condition would be equivalent to or lower than preparatory effort in the

control thought condition.

Method

Participants and Design

Participants were 80 students at the University of Konstanz who participated in the study in

exchange for research credit. Five participants were dropped from the final analyses because they

reported either that they did not believe that practice helped on the Swahili task or that doing

well on the two memory tasks was not important to them. The remaining participants were 44

women and 31 men, 89% of whom were age 25 or younger. Participants were randomly assigned

to conditions in a 2 (affirmation vs. no affirmation) x 2 (upward counterfactual vs. control)

between subjects design.

Materials and Procedure

Participants completed the Rosenberg (1965) measure of trait self-esteem as part of a

prescreening questionnaire prior to registering for the study. Upon arriving to the lab for the

main experiment, participants were informed that they would be completing several memory

tests during the study. They were then asked to provide basic demographic information,

Page 12: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 12

including their age and gender. Participants were told the first task was a “subjective” test of

memory. This task constituted the affirmation manipulation (see Cohen, Aronson, & Steele,

2000). Those assigned to the self-affirmation conditions selected one of eleven values (e.g.,

sense of humor, relationships with family and friends) that most exemplified their character.

None of the values were related to academic performance or general intelligence. They were then

asked to write a few sentences explaining the importance of the value and to remember three

instances in which the value had played a significant role in their life. Participants in the no

affirmation conditions were asked to list everything they had had to eat in the past 48 hours in as

much detail as possible.

Following the affirmation manipulation, participants were informed that the next task

they would complete was an objective measure of working memory and had been found to be

related to both intelligence and academic performance. They were also told that practice had

been shown to have a significant effect on performance on the memory test. Specifically,

participants were told that they needed at least three minutes of practice on the memory task to

achieve their optimal performance. After ensuring that participants understood the importance of

practice to the task, they were notified that a computer programming error had occurred which

assigned too many participants to the practice condition. Participants were asked if they would

not mind being in the “no practice” condition. They were told that the choice to practice was

their own. This induced compliance technique (adapted from McCrea, 2008) was used to

reinforce the notion that not practicing was freely chosen. All participants agreed to be in the no-

practice condition. Next, participants were administered an adapted version of the Computation

Span Task (Oberauer, Sueß, Schulze, Wilhelm, & Wittmann, 2000), a measure of working

memory. Participants had to decide within a two-second window whether the solution to a math

Page 13: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 13

problem was true or false (e.g., 4 + 3 = 8?). After a series of 4 to 8 items, participants were asked

to recall the digits that appeared on the right-side of each equation, in the order in which they had

been presented. Participants completed fifteen trials of the computation span task (three trials of

four-digit sequences, three trials of five-digit sequences, etc.). Order of trial presentation was

randomized. Following the task, all participants received false feedback that they had scored in

the bottom 40% of participants.

Next, participants were asked to consider a thought that had been supposedly written by a

previous participant in reaction to his performance on the computation span task. Those assigned

to the counterfactual condition were presented with an upward counterfactual statement (i.e., “If

I had practiced more, I would have done better.”), whereas those assigned to the control

condition were presented with a neutral statement (i.e., “The test items were similar to each

other.”). All participants were asked to consider how the statement related to their own

performance and re-typed the statement word-for-word three times before moving on to the next

task. In this manner, the content of the thought considered by participants was held constant (see

also McCrea, 2008).

The final task was described as a second type of objective memory test in which

participants had to learn the German translation of Swahili words. None of the participants knew

Swahili, and the words used were actually created by the researchers. The “correct” translations

were counterbalanced across participants. On a given trial, a Swahili word (e.g., “mjinala”) was

presented for five seconds, surrounded by four possible translations (e.g., career, pattern, gesture,

abyss). Participants were to select the correct translation for the Swahili word. Following their

selection, the answer was presented in the middle of the screen (in green for supposedly correct

answers, or in red for supposedly incorrect answers). Participants then pressed a key to advance

Page 14: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 14

to the next trial, or the next trial automatically began after 20 seconds. There were 20 word-

pairs, and participants were instructed to repeat the trials as many times as they wanted without a

time limit in order to learn the correct translations of all the Swahili words. They could quit the

practice session at any point, and the computer recorded the time in seconds participants spent

learning the translations. Following the practice session, participants were told they did not need

to complete the Swahili test. They were then asked to indicate using five-point scales to what

extent they thought that practice was helpful for the Swahili test (1 = not at all to 5 = very

helpful), and to what extent it was important for them to perform well on each memory test (1 =

not at all to 5 = very important).

Results

Manipulation Checks

Participants retained in the analyses believed that practicing for the Swahili test was helpful

(M = 4.75), and rated the importance of the computation span (M = 3.53) and Swahili (M = 3.72)

tests as moderately high. Participants did not significantly differ on these measures across

experimental conditions (all Fs < 2.76, ps > .10), and controlling for scores on these measures

did not alter the results. Among those given the opportunity to self-affirm, the majority (62.5%)

of participants chose to write about social skills or relationships, followed by sense of humor and

artistic sensibilities (22.5%).

Practice Time

Because individuals high in self-esteem have more resources with which to affirm the self

(Steele, Spencer, & Lynch, 1993), we controlled for trait self-esteem in our analysis. A 2 (self-

affirmation vs. control) x 2 (upward counterfactual vs. control) ANCOVA was conducted on the

practice measure, with self-esteem scores included as a covariate. This analysis revealed that

Page 15: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 15

high self-esteem individuals practiced significantly longer than did low self-esteem individuals,

F(1,70) = 4.52, p < .05. There was also a main effect of self-affirmation, F(1, 70) = 15.32, p <

.001, η2 = .18. Participants who had the opportunity to self-affirm practiced significantly longer

on the Swahili task, relative to those in the control condition. This effect was qualified by a

significant Self-affirmation x Thought interaction, F(1, 70) = 6.56, p < .05, η2 = .09, see Figure

1. To probe this interaction, the effect of counterfactual thought within each affirmation

condition was tested while controlling for trait self-esteem. Within the self-affirmation condition,

exposure to counterfactual thoughts increased practice effort relative to the control thought,

F(1,34) = 4.99, p < .05, η2 = .13. However, within the no-affirmation control condition, practice

was non-significantly lower in the counterfactual condition than in the control condition, F(1,35)

= 1.90, p = .17, η2 = .05. There was no main effect of thought condition, F <1, p = .45, η

2 < .01.

Discussion

As predicted, the prior opportunity to affirm the self moderated the behavioral

consequences of counterfactual thinking. Based on prior research (McCrea & Hirt, 2011; D. K.

Sherman et al., 2000), we assumed that self-affirmation would free participants to pursue a self-

improvement motive. As a result, upward counterfactuals would be processed in a manner that

facilitated improvement. Indeed, individuals in this condition practiced longer on a subsequent

task as a result of considering an upward counterfactual thought, relative to a control thought.

These findings are consistent with past work (e.g., Markman et al., 2008) showing that upward

counterfactual thoughts increase persistence on subsequent (nonthreatening) tasks.

Without a chance to affirm the self, the threatening nature of the task was expected to

induce a self-protection motivation among participants. As a result, we expected that upward

counterfactuals would be processed in a manner that excuses poor performance and undermines

Page 16: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 16

motivation to improve. As predicted, individuals in the no affirmation condition did not practice

more as a result of considering an upward counterfactual thought, relative to the control

condition. These findings are consistent with past work showing that upward counterfactual

thoughts concerning a prior self-handicap protect self-esteem, but can undermine subsequent

persistence (McCrea, 2008). Thus, the operation of different self-improvement motives explains

the apparent discrepancy between work showing preparatory benefits of upward counterfactual

thoughts (e.g., Roese, 1994) and work showing that the same thoughts can undermine effort

(McCrea, 2008).

Study 2 – Manipulation of Motives Through Achievement Goals

Study 2 was designed to replicate and extend these findings. First, we added a measure of

performance. Second, we utilized a different manipulation of self-evaluation motive, namely

achievement goals. Achievement goals provide both the standard and valence for evaluating

competence on a task. As a result, they have a number of important implications for how

individuals pursue a task and how they respond to feedback on that task (Elliot, 1999; Elliot &

Dweck, 1988). Mastery goals are those in which an individual uses his or her own past

performance as a standard for evaluating competence, whereas performance goals are those in

which the performance of others serves as a standard for evaluating competence (Elliot &

McGregor, 2001). These goals can be further divided into approach or avoidance goals,

depending on whether the goal is framed in terms of meeting the standard or avoiding failure to

meet the standard. Indeed, it is not possible to define a goal standard as mastery or performance

without at least implicitly referencing the approach-avoidance dimension as well (Elliot &

McGregor, 2001). Importantly for our purposes, past work has shown that mastery-approach

goals are most closely tied to the motive to improve and develop new skills, whereas

Page 17: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 17

performance-avoidance goals maximize the evaluative threat of a task and thus attempts to

protect the self from the negative implications of failure (Elliot, 1999; Elliot & McGregor, 2001).

For example, individuals who hold mastery-approach goals are more likely to attempt to learn

task-relevant material, engage in deep processing and preparatory effort, persist when faced with

failure, and they exhibit heightened self-determination, intrinsic motivation, need for

achievement, positive emotionality, and self-efficacy, compared to those pursuing other goals

(Butler, 1993; Elliot, 1999; Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Grant & Dweck,

2003; van Yperen, 2006). In contrast, individuals holding performance-avoidance goals engage

in more shallow processing, put forth less preparatory effort, adopt the perfectionistic standards

of others, and they exhibit more learned helplessness, extrinsic motivation, fear of failure,

negative emotionality, and worry, compared to those pursuing other goals (Butler, 1993; Elliot,

1999; Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Grant & Dweck, 2003; van Yperen,

2006).

Because the self-handicapping strategy is central to our predictions concerning the effects

of upward counterfactual thoughts on subsequent behavior, it is important to consider links

between achievement goals and this self-protective strategy. Consistent with the pattern of

findings outlined above, individual differences in the tendency to self-handicap are negatively

related to the adoption of mastery goals, but positively related to the adoption of performance-

avoidance goals (Elliot & Church, 2003; Rhodewalt, 1994; Urdan, 2004). Moreover, individuals

assigned a performance-avoidance goal exhibit more self-handicapping behavior than do

individuals assigned a mastery or no goal (Elliot et al., 2006; Lovejoy & Durik, 2010).

Additionally, self-handicapping is maximized in public settings (Hirt, McCrea, & Kimble, 2000;

Kolditz & Arkin, 1982) and under conditions of prevention regulatory focus (Hendrix & Hirt,

Page 18: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 18

2009). Thus, both normative standards for evaluating competence and avoidance framing seem

to increase the self-protection motive (and thus self-handicapping behavior in particular), relative

to intrapersonal standards and approach framing.

In contrast to the effects of performance-avoidance and mastery-approach goals,

performance-approach and mastery-avoidance goals appear to reflect motivational hybrids

(Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002). For example, performance-approach

goals are associated with both need for achievement and fear of failure, whereas mastery-

avoidance goals are related to both fear of failure and higher class engagement (Elliot &

McGregor, 2001). Findings from studies examining the relationship between achievement goals

and self-handicapping behavior are also quite mixed. For example, performance-approach goals

have been found to either predict increased self-handicapping (Midgley, Arunkumar, & Urdan,

1996), to predict self-handicapping only in certain contexts (Lovejoy & Durik, 2010; Midgley &

Urdan, 2001), or not to predict self-handicapping at all (Elliot et al., 2006; Midgley & Urdan,

1995). Mastery-avoidance goals are a relatively late addition to theories of achievement goals

and so there is little empirical evidence concerning their impact on self-handicapping behavior.

However, Lovejoy and Durik (2010) recently reported that individuals with a mastery-avoidance

goal orientation were more likely to claim self-handicaps, but not to behaviorally self-handicap.

Based on this past work, we elected to manipulate underlying self-evaluation motives in

Study 2 by assigning individuals to pursue either a mastery-approach or a performance-

avoidance goal. Given that our primary interest was in manipulating underlying self-evaluation

motivations, we chose to focus on these two goal orientations because they most clearly foster

self-improvement and self-protection motives, respectively. We predicted that, when individuals

pursue a mastery-approach goal (and thus self-improvement), upward counterfactual thoughts

Page 19: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 19

will lead to increased preparatory effort and performance on a subsequent task, compared to a

control thought. In contrast, when individuals pursue a performance-avoidance goal (and thus

self-protection), the same upward counterfactual thought should undermine motivation to

improve, resulting in the same or reduced preparatory effort and performance on a subsequent

task compared to a control thought.

Method

Participants and Design

Participants were 101 undergraduate students from the University of Wyoming who

participated in the study in exchange for partial course credit. Data from four participants were

excluded from the analyses for the following reasons: one participant had an extremely low

practice time (107 ms), indicating inadvertent responding; two participants did not follow

directions on the second memory task; one participant indicated that performing well on the two

memory tasks was not important. Thus, 97 (56 female, 41 male) participants were retained in the

final analyses, 90.7% of whom were age 25 or younger. Participants were randomly assigned to

conditions in a 2 (mastery-approach vs. performance-avoidance goal) x 2 (upward counterfactual

vs. control) between subjects design.

Materials and Procedure

Upon arriving to the lab, participants were informed that they would be taking two memory

tests and were asked to provide basic demographic information including age and gender.

Achievement goals were manipulated based on the work of Elliot and McGregor (2001). In the

mastery-approach goal condition, participants were told that performance on the memory tests

predicts academic ability and future career success, and that people can improve their basic

memory skills through practice and effort. Participants were given the goal to try their best to

Page 20: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 20

improve their performance on the two memory tests. In the performance-avoidance goal

condition, participants were told that, though most college students are proficient on the memory

tasks, some students lack memory ability and are more likely to struggle in academics and their

future careers. These participants were told that the researchers were only interested in whether

they could manage to avoid performing worse than other participants. They were given the goal

to demonstrate that they do not lack memory ability relative to other students.

Following the introduction of the goals, participants were given the same instructions

concerning the adapted computation span task as in Study 1, including the importance of prior

practice. They were induced not to practice, completed the computation span task, and received

false feedback that they achieved a score of 66% on the memory test. In addition, participants in

the performance-avoidance goal condition were told that they had scored below average in

memory ability.

Next, a counterfactual statement (i.e., “If I had asked to practice the memory test, I would

have done better”) was presented to participants in the counterfactual condition, whereas a

neutral statement (i.e., “The test items were pretty similar to each other”) was presented to

participants in the control condition. All participants were asked to consider how the statement

related to their own experience and re-typed the statement word-for-word three times before

moving on to the next task.

The second memory test involved learning a series of 15 pictures (i.e., stones, statuettes,

painted flowers, etc.) that later had to be serially recalled. Participants were told that the memory

skills needed on the picture test were the same as those required for the prior computation span

test. They were instructed to try to learn the picture sequence during a practice session. All of the

pictures would be presented in sequence for ten seconds each. Following the initial presentation,

Page 21: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 21

participants would have the option of reviewing the sequence up to seven additional times. They

were instructed to review the sequence until they felt they could recall the pictures in the correct

order. Participants in the mastery-approach condition were reminded that their goal was to

improve their performance and beat their score from the first test, whereas those in the

performance-avoidance condition were reminded that their goal was to demonstrate that they

were not below average in memory ability. They were then shown the entire sequence of pictures

once, and were subsequently given the opportunity to review the sequence an additional seven

times. After each picture was presented, participants had the opportunity to quit the practice

session and complete the memory test, or to continue practicing. Time spent and the number of

pictures viewed were recorded by computer.

Upon completing the practice session, the memory test was administered. Participants were

asked to recall the pictures (using a brief description) in the exact order they had been presented.

Following the memory test, participants completed several manipulation checks. They were

presented with a list of goals and asked to indicate which goal(s) they had pursued on the

memory tasks, checking all that applied (to do better than everyone else; to do better on the

picture test than on the math test; to try my best no matter what; other). Participants were also

asked to indicate to what extent practice was helpful for learning the picture sequence, using a

five-point scale (1 = not at all to 5 = very helpful). They were also asked how important it was

for them to perform well on the computation span task and the picture sequence task, using a

five-point scale (1 = very unimportant to 5 = very important).

Results

Manipulation Checks

Page 22: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 22

Participants retained in the analyses believed that practicing for the picture sequence task

was helpful (M = 4.18), and rated the importance of the computation span (M = 3.58) and picture

sequence (M = 3.60) tasks as moderately high. Participants did not significantly differ on these

measures across experimental conditions (all Fs < 1.69, ps > .19), and controlling for scores on

these measures did not alter the results.

To determine whether the goal manipulation was effective, we created a goal index as

follows: Those indicating they were trying to outperform others were assigned a score of 1, those

indicating they were trying to improve across the tasks were assigned a score of -1, and those

selecting both or neither goal were assigned a score of 0. A 2 (mastery-approach vs.

performance-avoidance) x 2 (upward counterfactual vs. control) ANOVA revealed only a main

effect of goal condition, F(1,91) = 25.70, p < .001, η2 = .22, indicating that individuals were

more likely to adopt a performance goal and less likely to adopt an improvement goal in the

performance-avoidance condition (M = -0.34) than in the mastery-approach goal condition (M =

-0.91).

Practice Index

Overall practice time (M = 69.43 sec, SD = 69.48) and number of pictures reviewed (M =

24.89, SD = 21.48) were square-root transformed to eliminate positive-skew. These scores were

highly correlated (r = .82). Consistent with past work (e.g., Hirt et al., 2000; McCrea, 2008), a

summary index was calculated by standardizing and summing the transformed scores

(Cronbach’s α = .90).1 A 2 (mastery-approach vs. performance-avoidance) x 2 (upward

counterfactual vs. control) ANOVA conducted on the practice index revealed a significant Goal

x Thought condition interaction, F(1, 93) = 4.78, p < .05, η2 = .05, see Figure 2. Within the

performance-avoidance goal condition, those exposed to the upward counterfactual practiced

Page 23: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 23

significantly less than did those in the control condition, t(48) = 2.28, p < .05, η2 = .10. Within

the mastery-approach goal condition, there was no effect of thought condition, t(45) < 1, p = .35.

There were no main effects of thought or goal condition, F(1, 93) < 1, ps > .48.

Memory Performance

Performance on the memory test was assessed by how many pictures participants correctly

recalled. Scoring criteria were strict in that, in order to be correct, participants had to accurately

describe the picture in the correct serial position. A 2 (mastery-approach vs. performance-

avoidance) x 2 (upward counterfactual vs. control) ANOVA revealed a significant Goal x

Thought condition interaction, F(1, 93) = 4.50, p < .05, η2 = .05, see Figure 3. Within the

mastery-approach goal condition, those exposed to the upward counterfactual performed better

than did those in the control condition, t(45) = 2.02, p = .05, η2 = .08. Within the performance-

avoidance goal condition, this effect was not significant, t(48) = 1.01, p = .32, η2 = .02.

There were no main effects of thought or goal condition, F(1, 93) < 1, ps > .47.

Mediation Analyses

Following the guidelines of Muller, Judd, and Yzerbyt (2005) for testing mediated

moderation, we next examined whether the practice index mediated the Goal x Thought

condition interaction on memory performance.2 Effect coding was utilized for the dichotomous

condition variables (i.e., performance-avoidance goal = 1, mastery-approach goal = -1;

counterfactual thought = 1, control thought = -1), and the practice index was centered (see also

Aiken & West, 1991). The goal, thought condition, and Goal x Thought condition interaction

terms were entered in the first step of a regression model predicting memory performance. The

practice index and Goal x Practice index interaction terms were entered in the second step of the

model. According to Muller et al. (2005), mediated moderation would be evidenced by a

Page 24: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 24

significant practice index effect, and a nonsignificant Goal x Thought condition interaction, in

this model. As seen in Table 1, the practice index effect was significant, whereas the Goal x

Thought condition interaction was no longer significant.3 A Sobel test revealed a significant

indirect effect, z = 2.03, p < .05. Thus, the observed performance effects were due to the degree

of preparatory effort exhibited by participants.

Discussion

Study 2 demonstrated that upward counterfactual thoughts are beneficial for subsequent

tasks when individuals pursue a self-improvement motive, but that these same thoughts can

undermine efforts on subsequent tasks when individuals are motivated by self-protection

concerns. When individuals pursue a mastery-approach goal, thoughts about how a past

performance could have been better are likely to be used as an assessment of goal progress.

These thoughts indicate that performance was inadequate, suggest that improvement is possible,

and provide strategies for corrective action. As a result, performance should improve. Consistent

with this prediction, we observed that upward counterfactual thinking improved performance

within the mastery-approach condition. On the other hand, when individuals pursue a

performance-avoidance goal, they are likely to be concerned with protecting self-esteem. In this

context, upward counterfactuals can provide excuses for failure, shifting blame to unstable

causes. Dissatisfaction with performance is reduced and the motivation to improve is

undermined. Consistent with this prediction, we observed that upward counterfactual thinking

reduced preparatory effort when individuals pursued a performance-avoidance goal. Moreover,

the performance benefits of upward counterfactual thinking found in the mastery-approach

condition were eliminated.

Page 25: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 25

We chose to contrast the effects of pursuing a performance-avoidance goal with those of

pursuing a mastery-approach goal because they most cleanly relate to underlying self-protection

and self-improvement motives, respectively. In contrast, performance-approach and mastery-

avoidance goals likely represent a mix of these two motives (Harackiewicz et al., 2002) and are

therefore less directly relevant to testing the predictions of our model. Of course, this leaves open

the question of whether our findings were driven by the intrapersonal vs. normative nature of the

mastery-performance dimension or the strategic means suggested by the approach-avoidance

dimension. While additional research is required to definitively address this question, past work

on achievement goals (Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Harackiewicz et al., 2002; van Yperen, 2006)

and self-handicapping behavior (Elliot et al., 2006; Hendrix & Hirt, 2009; Hirt et al., 2000;

Lovejoy & Durik, 2010; Rhodewalt, 1994) suggests that both variables independently influence

self-evaluation motives, and therefore the likely consequences of counterfactual thinking for

affect and behavior. In other words, we suspect that both performance goals and avoidance

framing would independently contribute to the motive to protect the self, each making it more

likely that individuals would use upward counterfactuals to excuse poor performance.

Conversely, both mastery goals and approach framing should independently contribute to the

motive to improve, each making it more likely that individuals would use upward counterfactuals

to identify corrective actions and increase effort. This remains an interesting direction for further

study.

General Discussion

Counterfactuals help us to evaluate the past, thereby providing information relevant to

judging the self. The consequences of these thoughts for future behavior are therefore critically

dependent upon how individuals wish to view the self. Consistent with this view, self-evaluation

Page 26: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 26

motives influence the generation of counterfactual thoughts (McCrea, 2007; Roese & Olson,

1993; Sanna et al., 2001). Our findings extend this past work to demonstrate that the very same

counterfactual thought can have different consequences for subsequent behavior, dependent upon

the self-evaluation motive currently active.

Considering counterfactual thoughts within a self-evaluation framework allows for an

integration of apparently discrepant findings in the literature. On the one hand, upward

counterfactual thoughts have been shown to serve a preparatory function, leading to increased

persistence, performance, and the formation of intentions to take corrective action (Epstude &

Roese, 2008; Markman et al., 2008; Smallman & Roese, 2009). On the other hand, McCrea

(2008) found that the same thoughts can be used to excuse poor performance, maintaining self-

esteem in the face of failure, but eliminating any preparatory benefits. Our findings indicate that

overarching self-evaluation motives influence which of these two patterns emerges. When

individuals are pursuing improvement or have already addressed self-protection concerns,

upward counterfactuals concerning a lack of effort increase subsequent persistence and facilitate

performance. These findings are consistent with past work suggesting that attributing failure to a

lack of effort maintains self-efficacy and leads to persistence and improvement on tasks

(Bandura, 1977; Weiner, 1985). On the other hand, when individuals are faced with an ego-

threatening failure, they may generate and interpret upward counterfactuals in a manner that

excuses this poor performance. Upward counterfactuals concerning a self-handicap serve to shift

blame to relatively unstable causes of failure, which in turn can preserve mood and self-esteem

(Covington & Omelich, 1979; Jones & Berglas, 1978). Counterfactual excuses may be functional

in the short-term by blunting the negative consequences of failure for mood and self-esteem in

this manner, but the long-term consequences are less desirable. By reducing self-blame and

Page 27: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 27

negative affect, excusing upward counterfactuals undermine motivation to improve in the future.

Preparatory effort is reduced and subsequent performance suffers.

Limitations and Future Directions

The current findings add to a growing body of research demonstrating that the

consequences of counterfactual thought are not rigidly determined by thought content (e.g.,

upward or downward direction). For example, the REM (Markman & McMullen, 2003) suggests

that the consequences of these thoughts are determined by the manner in which they are

processed. Upward counterfactuals lead to more negative affect, increased persistence, and better

performance when they are used to evaluate actual performance against a counterfactual

standard. However, the same thought can lead to more positive affect, decreased persistence, and

worse performance when individuals vividly imagine (i.e., reflect on) the counterfactual

alternative (Markman et al., 2008). In a similar manner, self-evaluation motives moderate the

consequences of upward counterfactuals. Our findings are consistent with the REM in the sense

that upward counterfactuals do not result in negative affect when they are used to excuse failure,

and so persistence and performance suffer. Self-evaluation motives may in fact operate on

counterfactual thoughts in the manner envisioned by the REM. That is, self-evaluation motives

could determine whether individuals spontaneously engage in reflection or comparison when

processing counterfactual thoughts. Future research could test whether achievement goals or the

opportunity to self-affirm moderate the use of evaluation or reflection in processing

counterfactuals.

We examined the consequences of counterfactual thinking for task preparation and

performance with experimenter-provided thoughts in order to control for any effects of thought

content. It is important to note that similar effects have been observed in studies in which

Page 28: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 28

individuals generated their own counterfactual thoughts. For example, McCrea (2008) reported

that upward counterfactuals could undermine persistence and performance, even when

participants generated these thoughts. Likewise, Markman et al. (2008) found that processing

style determined the behavioral consequences of self-generated counterfactuals. Moreover, these

studies demonstrated similar consequences of counterfactual thinking using a variety of tasks and

measures, including intentions to study for a psychology exam, practicing for a nonverbal

intelligence test, and persisting on unsolvable anagrams. Thus, we do not believe that our

findings are limited to memory and learning tasks or to cases in which the counterfactual thought

is experimenter-provided.

It is worth considering however whether differences in the content of the counterfactual

thought would further moderate the pattern of results we observed. Our studies focused on the

consequences of upward counterfactual thoughts, as they appear to be more frequently generated

following failure (Epstude & Roese, 2008). What about the operation of self-evaluation motives

on downward counterfactual thoughts? Downward counterfactual thoughts indicate how an

outcome could have been worse (e.g., “At least I got a B, I could have gotten an even worse

grade”). Downward counterfactuals are more frequently generated following success, and

typically result in more positive affect (Markman, Gavanski, Sherman, & McMullen, 1993;

Roese, 1994). However, inducing individuals to focus on the fact that a negative outcome

“almost” occurred can also motivate efforts to improve (a so-called "wake-up call", McMullen &

Markman, 2000). According to our model, this “wake-up call” should be most likely to occur

when individuals are pursuing a self-improvement motive. In contrast, downward counterfactuals

should be used to boost mood and self-esteem when individuals are more concerned with self-

enhancement.

Page 29: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 29

Other features of the counterfactual thought may enhance or minimize their impact on

subsequent behavior. We focused on the effects of an “additive” counterfactual thought

concerning a lack of effort. That is, the counterfactual concerned what one should have done.

One might ask whether the same results would obtain when the counterfactual concerned what

one should not have done. Such “subtractive” counterfactuals are evoked by prevention failure

(Roese, Hur, & Pennington, 1999) and lead to greater persistence under prevention framing

(Markman, McMullen, Elizaga, & Mizoguchi, 2006; Spiegel, Grant-Pillow, & Higgins, 2004).

From this perspective, considering an additive counterfactual thought may have resulted in

increased persistence because of greater regulatory fit (Higgins, Idson, Freitas, Spiegel, &

Molden, 2003) or value placed on the task in the mastery-approach goal condition than in the

performance-avoidance goal condition in Study 2. Additional research is required to directly test

this explanation for our results, but several pieces of evidence speak against it. In Study 1, we

found that the consequences of counterfactual thought were moderated by prior affirmation of

the self, even when there was no manipulation of approach-avoidance framing. In addition,

McCrea (2008, Study 4) found reduced task motivation as a result of considering excusing

counterfactuals, even though these thoughts involved the subtraction of a handicap (i.e.,

participants considered thoughts including “If I did not have to listen to the distracting noises, I

would have done better”). Finally, differences in task importance did not account for our results,

contrary to what one would expect if regulatory fit had increased the value of the task (Higgins et

al., 2003). Taken together, we believe the results of the present studies are most consistent with

the view that the behavioral consequences of upward counterfactual thoughts are moderated by

whether the individual pursues a self-improvement or self-protection motive.

Page 30: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 30

Beyond framing of the thought, certain counterfactuals more easily come to mind or are

simply more compelling than others. For example, individuals tend to generate counterfactuals

concerning controllable features of the situation, particularly their own behavior (Markman,

Gavanski, Sherman, & McMullen, 1995). Thoughts concerning one’s own behavior are also

more likely to impact future behavior by identifying corrective actions (Smallman & Roese,

2009) and suggesting that improvement is possible (Tal-Or et al., 2004). As shown in many

studies of self-handicapping (e.g., McCrea, 2008; McCrea & Hirt, 2001), intentionally

dysfunctional behavior can also provide an effective excuse for failure. Beyond controllability,

counterfactuals can also differ in their potency (Petrocelli, Percy, Sherman, & Tormala, 2011).

Potency is a product of the perceived likelihood of the antecedent (if-part) having occurred and

the perceived likelihood that the antecedent would have led to the consequent (then-part).

Counterfactuals that have a higher perceived potency have a stronger impact on affect and

perceived responsibility for outcomes (Petrocelli et al., 2011). Although it remains to be tested,

more potent counterfactuals should also have a stronger impact on behavior. The more

compelling the counterfactual, the greater should be the desire to improve, and the more effective

should be the excuse. Thus, we suggest that the consequences of counterfactuals for persistence

and performance when pursuing a self-improvement or self-protection motive would be even

stronger to the extent that the counterfactual thought is potent or concerns a controllable

behavior.

A final avenue for future research would be to examine the operation of other self-

evaluation motives in counterfactual thinking. For example, self-verification is identified as

another common self-evaluation motive (Sedikides & Strube, 1997; Swann, 1987). In this case,

individuals may process counterfactuals in a manner that reinforces current views of the self.

Page 31: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 31

Similarly, the desire to justify past actions may also impact the consequences of counterfactual

thought. As discussed earlier, Shalvi et al. (2011) observed that the availability of an upward

counterfactual allowed greater justification of cheating behavior. Escalation of commitment is

another well-documented example of self-justification (Arkes & Blumer, 1985; Staw, 1976). In

this case, reluctance to admit a mistake leads to inappropriate persistence in a failing course of

action. We would predict that counterfactual thoughts increase persistence following large prior

investments, as individuals are pushed by a desire to justify those previous losses.

Conclusion

Counterfactual thinking provides an important bridge between the past and the future.

Reflecting on the past allows us to better understand the world and ourselves. However, the

implications of this knowledge for future action depend greatly on how we want to view the self.

Thus, one of the paradoxes of mental time travel is that it is remains wedded to the whims and

desires of the present.

Page 32: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 32

References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.

Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage Publications.

Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior &

Human Decision Processes, 35, 124-140.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.

Psychological Review, 54, 191-215.

Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the

motivational effects of goal systems. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 45,

1017-1028.

Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99.

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem

cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles?

Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1-44.

Butler, R. (1993). Effects of task- and ego-achievement goals on information seeking during task

engagement. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65, 18-31.

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Themes and issues in the self-regulation of behavior. In

R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral self-regulation (pp. 1-105). Mahweh, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 33: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 33

Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing

biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,

1151-1164.

Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1979). Effort: The double-edged sword in school

achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(2), 169-182.

Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin, 130,

392-414.

Crocker, J., & Wolfe, C. T. (2001). Contingencies of self-worth. Psychological Review, 108,

593-623.

Dunning, D. (1995). Trait importance and modifiability as factors influencing self-assessment

and self-enahcement motives. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1297-1306.

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and

personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.

Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational

Psychologist, 34, 169-189.

Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (2003). A motivational analysis of defensive pessimism and self-

handicapping. Journal of Personality, 71, 369-396.

Elliot, A. J., Cury, F., Fryer, J. W., & Huguet, P. (2006). Achievement goals, self-handicapping,

and performance attainment: A mediational analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise

Psychology, 28, 344-361.

Elliot, A. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5-12.

Page 34: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 34

Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of

Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 501-519.

Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Personality

& Social Psychology Review, 12, 168-192.

Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: Priming the

simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 36(4), 384-409.

Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a

review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378-408.

Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541-553.

Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history.

American Psychologist, 35(7), 603-618.

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision

of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 94, 638-645.

Hendrix, K. S., & Hirt, E. R. (2009). Stressed out over possible failure: The role of regulatory fit

on claimed self-handicapping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 51-59.

Higgins, E. T., Idson, L. C., Freitas, A. L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D. C. (2003). Transfer of

value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140-1153.

Hirt, E. R., Levine, G. M., McDonald, H. E., Melton, R. J., & Martin, L. L. (1997). The role of

mood in quantitative and qualitative aspects of performance: Single or multiple

mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(6), 602-629.

Page 35: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 35

Hirt, E. R., McCrea, S. M., & Kimble, C. E. (2000). Public self-focus and sex differences in

behavioral self-handicapping: Does increasing self-threat still make it "just a man's

game?". Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1131-1141.

Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-

handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachievement.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 4(2), 200-206.

Kolditz, T. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1982). An impression management interpretation of the self-

handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 43(3), 492-502.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

Lovejoy, C. M., & Durik, A. M. (2010). Self-handicapping: The interplay between self-set and

assigned achievement goals. Motivation & Emotion, 34, 242-252.

Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1993). The mental

simulation of better and worse possible worlds. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 29(1), 87-109.

Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1995). The impact of

perceived control on the imagination of better and worse possible worlds. Personality &

Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(6), 588-595.

Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative

thinking. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 7, 244-267.

Markman, K. D., McMullen, M. N., & Elizaga, R. A. (2008). Counterfactual thinking,

persistence, and performance: A test of the Reflection and Evaluation Model. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 421-428.

Page 36: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 36

Markman, K. D., McMullen, M. N., Elizaga, R. A., & Mizoguchi, N. (2006). Counterfactual

thinking and regulatory fit. Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 98-107.

McCrea, S. M. (2007). Counterfactual thinking following negative outcomes: Evidence for group

and self-protective biases. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1256-1271.

McCrea, S. M. (2008). Self-handicapping, excuse-making, and counterfactual thinking:

Consequences for self-esteem and future motivation. Journal of Personality & Social

Psychology, 95, 274-292.

McCrea, S. M., & Hirt, E. R. (2001). The role of ability judgments in self-handicapping.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1378-1389.

McCrea, S. M., & Hirt, E. R. (2011). Limitations on the substitutability of self-protective

processes: Self-handicapping is not reduced by related-domain self-affirmations. Social

Psychology, 42, 9-18.

McMullen, M. N., & Markman, K. D. (2000). Downward counterfactuals and motivation: The

wake-up call and the Pangloss effect. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5),

575-584.

Midgley, C., Arunkumar, R., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). "If I don't do well tomorrow, there's a

reason": Predictors of adolescents' use of academic self-handicapping strategies. Journal

of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 423-434.

Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1995). Predictors of middle school students' use of self-handicapping

strategies. Journal of Early Adolescence, 15(4), 389-411.

Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (2001). Academic self-handicapping and achievement goals: A further

examination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(1), 61-75.

Page 37: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 37

Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is mediated and mediation

is moderated. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 89, 852-863.

Oberauer, K., Sueß, H. M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W. (2000). Working

memory capacity: Facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality & Individual

Differences, 29, 1017-1045.

Petrocelli, J. V., Percy, E. J., Sherman, S. J., & Tormala, Z. L. (2011). Counterfactual potency.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 30-46.

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and

comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavioral Research Methods,

40, 879-891.

Rhodewalt, F. (1994). Conceptions of ability, achievement goals, and individual differences in

self-handicapping behavior: On the application of implicit theories. Journal of

Personality, 62(1), 67-85.

Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality &

Social Psychology, 66(5), 805-818.

Roese, N. J., Hur, T., & Pennington, G. L. (1999). Counterfactual thinking and regulatory focus:

Implications for action versus inaction and sufficiency versus necessity. Journal of

Personality & Social Psychology, 77(6), 1109-1120.

Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1993). Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking. Journal of

Personality & Social Psychology, 65(1), 199-206.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University

Press.

Page 38: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 38

Sanna, L. J., Chang, E. C., & Meier, S. (2001). Counterfactual thinking and self-motives.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1023-1034.

Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own

self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thy own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),

Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 209-269). New York:

Academic Press.

Shalvi, S., Dana, J., Handgraaf, M. J. J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2011). Justified ethicality:

Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior.

Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 115, 181-190.

Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory.

In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183-

242). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

Sherman, D. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten

the self: Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046-1058.

Sherman, S. J., & McConnell, A. R. (1995). Dysfunctional implications of counterfactual

thinking: When alternatives to reality fail us. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.), What

might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 199-231).

Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Siegel, P. A., Scillitoe, J., & Parks-Yancy, R. (2005). Reducing the tendency to self-handicap:

The effect of self-affirmation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 589-597.

Page 39: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 39

Smallman, R., & Roese, N. J. (2009). Counterfactual thinking facilitates the formation of

intentions: Evidence for a content-specific pathway in behavioral regulation. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 845-852.

Spiegel, S., Grant-Pillow, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). How regulatory fit enhances motivational

strength during goal pursuit. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 39-54.

Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: A study of escalating commitment to a chosen

course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 27-44.

Steele, C. M., Spencer, S., & Lynch, M. (1993). Self-image resilience and dissonance: The role

of affirmational resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 885-896.

Swann, W. B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 53, 1038-1051.

Tal-Or, N., Boninger, D. S., & Gleicher, F. (2004). On becoming what we might have been:

Counterfactual thinking and self-efficacy. Self and Identity, 3, 5-26.

Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality &

Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278-1287.

Tesser, A., & Cornell, D. P. (1991). On the confluence of self-processes. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 501-526.

Trope, Y. (1986). Self-enhancement and self-assessment in achievement behavior. In R. M.

Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations

of social behavior (pp. 350-378). New York: Guilford Press.

Trope, Y., Ferguson, M., & Raghunathan, R. (2001). Mood as a resource in processing self-

relevant information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition. (pp.

256-274). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Page 40: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 40

Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of academic self-handicapping and achievement: Examining

achievement goals, classroom goal structures, and culture. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 96, 251-264.

van Yperen, N. W. (2006). A novel approach to assessing achievement goals in the context of the

2 x 2 framework: Identifying distinct profiles of individuals with different dominant

achievement goals. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1432-1445.

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion.

Psychological Review, 92(4), 548-573.

Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality. Journal of Personality &

Social Psychology, 56(2), 161-169.

Zuckerman, M. (1979). Attribution of success and failure revisited: or The motivational bias is

alive and well in attribution theory. Journal of Personality, 47(2), 245-287.

Page 41: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 41

Footnotes

1 A summary index of practice effort is superior in our view because it is less affected by

practice speed (i.e., those who practice many items quickly or few items slowly will tend to have

moderate, rather than extreme, scores). When analyzed separately, significant Goal x Thought

condition interactions were found on both practice time and number of items practiced, Fs >

4.17, ps < .05.

2 Using a boot-strapping procedure (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) with the interaction contrast as

the independent variable, the recall measure as the dependent measure, and the practice index as

the mediator also revealed a significant indirect effect (Sobel test z = 2.03, p < .05).

3 The significant Goal x Practice index interaction revealed that the benefits of practice were

stronger within the performance-avoidance condition. However, given the absence of a main

effect of counterfactual thought condition on the practice index, this result is not consistent with

Muller et al.’s (2005) criteria for moderated mediation.

Page 42: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 42

Table 1. Mediation analysis (Study 2)

Initial Model Mediation model

Term β t p f2 β t p f2

Goal .072 <1 .48 .01 .082 <1 .34 .01

Thought condition .067 <1 .51 .01 .146 1.64 .10 .03

Goal x Thought condition -.214 2.12 <.05 .05 -.084 <1 .35 .01

Practice index .528 5.90 <.001 .28

Goal x Practice index .188 2.10 <.05 .05

Page 43: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 43

Figure 1. Self-affirmation x Thought interaction on Swahili task practice time (Study 1)

Page 44: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 44

Figure 2. Goal x Thought condition interaction on practice index (Study 2)

Page 45: Laramie, Wyoming | University of Wyoming - … mccrea docs...Counterfactual Thinking Upward counterfactuals have been shown to serve a preparative function (Epstude & Roese, 2008;

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING 45

Figure 3. Goal x Thought condition interaction on memory performance (Study 2)