Journal of Experimental Psychology: 1999, Vol. 25, No...

19
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1999, Vol. 25, No. 1,172-190 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/99/S3.00 Eye Movement During Skill Acquisition: More Evidence for the Information-Reduction Hypothesis Hilde Haider University of the Federal Armed Forces Peter A. Frensch Max Planck Institute for Human Development Two experiments tested H. Haider and P. A. Frensch's (1996) information-reduction hypothesis that people learn, with practice, to distinguish between task-relevant and task-redundant information and to limit their processing to task-relevant information. Participants verified alphabetic strings (e.g., "E [4] J K L") containing task-relevant and task-redundant information. In Experiment 1, the positioning of task-relevant information within the strings and the consistency of positioning were manipulated. Degree of information reduction as reflected in reduced reaction times was not affected by the positioning of the relevant information and was only slightly affected by consistency of the positioning. In Experiment 2, eye movements were recorded. Results suggest that task-redundant information is ignored at a perceptual rather than a conceptual level of processing. Thus, existing theories of skill acquisition should include mechanisms that capture the practice-related increase in the selective use of information. Practice improves both the speed and quality of perfor- mance. This relation between task practice and performance appears to hold across a wide variety of cognitive (e.g., Anderson, 1982, 1983, 1987, 1992, 1993; Lassaline & Logan, 1993; Logan & Etherton, 1994; Logan & Klapp, 1991) and motor (e.g., K. Newell, 1991; Schmidt, 1987) tasks; frequently, speed-of-performance improvements fol- low a power law of practice (e.g., A. Newell & Rosenbloom, 1981). Typically, the relation between task practice and task improvement is explained with reference to practice-related (a) qualitative changes in the effective task structure (i.e., strategy changes; Cheng, 1985; Logan, 1988, 1990, 1992), (b) increases in the efficiency of performing individual task components (e.g., the concept of strengthening in Ander- son's 1982, 1987, and 1992 ACT* theory), (c) increases in the efficiency of performing sequences of task components (e.g., Anderson's 1982 and 1987 composition mechanism; see also Frensch, 1991, 1994; A. Newell & Rosenbloom's chunking mechanism), or (d) combinations of these mecha- nisms (e.g., Anderson, 1987). Hilde Haider, Institute for Cognitive Research, University of the Federal Armed Forces, Hamburg, Germany; Peter A. Frensch, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany. This research was supported, in part, by a German-American Commission on Collaborative Research grant from the German Academic Exchange Agency and the American Council of Learned Societies. We thank Gerd Liier and Jan Restat, University of Gottingen, Germany, for help with Experiment 2, and Eva Risch- kau and Lars Pelka for their assistance with data collection. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hilde Haider, Institute for Cognitive Research, University of the Federal Armed Forces, Holstenhofweg 85, D-22043, Hamburg, Germany, or to Peter A. Frensch, who is now at the Institut fur Psychologie, Humboldt-Universitat zu Berlin, Hausvogteiplatz 5-7, D-10117, Berlin, Germany. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected] or to peter.frensch©psychologic hu-berlin.de. We (Haider & Frensch, 1996) recently reintroduced a theoretical explanation for practice-related performance im- provements that differs from those mentioned above. Specifi- cally, we argued that changes in the speed and quality of performance are partially due to a reduction in the amount of task information that is processed. This information- reduction hypothesis holds that people learn, with practice, to become selective in their use of information, that is, to distinguish between task-relevant and task-redundant infor- mation and limit their processing to task-relevant informa- tion. Improvements in task performance, in this view, reflect an increased knowledge about which information has to, and which information does not have to, be processed. The theoretical notion of information reduction (for related proposals, see Anderson, Matessa, & Douglass, 1995; Logan & Etherton, 1994) is rooted in the traditional belief that selection of information is a necessary prerequi- site for action (e.g., J. J. Gibson, 1979; Hoffmann, 1993; Neisser, 1976; Neumann, 1985, 1990; van der Heijden, 1992) and has been introduced in many different areas of psychology. It can be found in the literature on perception (e.g., E. J. Gibson, 1963; J. J. Gibson & Gibson, 1955; Kaptein, Theeuwes, & van der Heijden, 1995), concept •formation (e.g., Regehr & Brooks, 1993), educational psy- chology (e.g., Bransford, Sherwood, Vye, & Rieser, 1986; Gaeth & Shanteau, 1984), and expertise (e.g., Shanteau, 1992; Shapiro & Raymond, 1989). Moreover, the idea of information reduction is consistent with empirical results observed in various areas of psychology (e.g., Christensen et al., 1981; Fisher & Tanner, 1992; Holding, 1985; LaBerge & Samuels, 1974; Lambert, Spencer, & Mohindra, 1987; Myles-Worsley, Johnston, & Simons, 1988; Strayer & Kramer, 1994a, 1994b). To test the information-reduction hypothesis for the domain of cognitive-skill acquisition, we asked participants to verify alphabetic letter strings (Haider & Frensch, 1996). Strings consisted of an initial letter-digit-letter triplet and a 172

Transcript of Journal of Experimental Psychology: 1999, Vol. 25, No...

Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition1999, Vol. 25, No. 1,172-190

Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.0278-7393/99/S3.00

Eye Movement During Skill Acquisition: More Evidence for theInformation-Reduction Hypothesis

Hilde HaiderUniversity of the Federal Armed Forces

Peter A. FrenschMax Planck Institute for Human Development

Two experiments tested H. Haider and P. A. Frensch's (1996) information-reductionhypothesis that people learn, with practice, to distinguish between task-relevant andtask-redundant information and to limit their processing to task-relevant information.Participants verified alphabetic strings (e.g., "E [4] J K L") containing task-relevant andtask-redundant information. In Experiment 1, the positioning of task-relevant informationwithin the strings and the consistency of positioning were manipulated. Degree of informationreduction as reflected in reduced reaction times was not affected by the positioning of therelevant information and was only slightly affected by consistency of the positioning. InExperiment 2, eye movements were recorded. Results suggest that task-redundant informationis ignored at a perceptual rather than a conceptual level of processing. Thus, existing theoriesof skill acquisition should include mechanisms that capture the practice-related increase in theselective use of information.

Practice improves both the speed and quality of perfor-mance. This relation between task practice and performanceappears to hold across a wide variety of cognitive (e.g.,Anderson, 1982, 1983, 1987, 1992, 1993; Lassaline &Logan, 1993; Logan & Etherton, 1994; Logan & Klapp,1991) and motor (e.g., K. Newell, 1991; Schmidt, 1987)tasks; frequently, speed-of-performance improvements fol-low a power law of practice (e.g., A. Newell & Rosenbloom,1981). Typically, the relation between task practice and taskimprovement is explained with reference to practice-related(a) qualitative changes in the effective task structure (i.e.,strategy changes; Cheng, 1985; Logan, 1988, 1990, 1992),(b) increases in the efficiency of performing individual taskcomponents (e.g., the concept of strengthening in Ander-son's 1982, 1987, and 1992 ACT* theory), (c) increases inthe efficiency of performing sequences of task components(e.g., Anderson's 1982 and 1987 composition mechanism;see also Frensch, 1991, 1994; A. Newell & Rosenbloom'schunking mechanism), or (d) combinations of these mecha-nisms (e.g., Anderson, 1987).

Hilde Haider, Institute for Cognitive Research, University of theFederal Armed Forces, Hamburg, Germany; Peter A. Frensch, MaxPlanck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.

This research was supported, in part, by a German-AmericanCommission on Collaborative Research grant from the GermanAcademic Exchange Agency and the American Council of LearnedSocieties. We thank Gerd Liier and Jan Restat, University ofGottingen, Germany, for help with Experiment 2, and Eva Risch-kau and Lars Pelka for their assistance with data collection.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed toHilde Haider, Institute for Cognitive Research, University of theFederal Armed Forces, Holstenhofweg 85, D-22043, Hamburg,Germany, or to Peter A. Frensch, who is now at the Institut furPsychologie, Humboldt-Universitat zu Berlin, Hausvogteiplatz5-7, D-10117, Berlin, Germany. Electronic mail may be sent [email protected] or to peter.frensch©psychologichu-berlin.de.

We (Haider & Frensch, 1996) recently reintroduced atheoretical explanation for practice-related performance im-provements that differs from those mentioned above. Specifi-cally, we argued that changes in the speed and quality ofperformance are partially due to a reduction in the amount oftask information that is processed. This information-reduction hypothesis holds that people learn, with practice,to become selective in their use of information, that is, todistinguish between task-relevant and task-redundant infor-mation and limit their processing to task-relevant informa-tion. Improvements in task performance, in this view, reflectan increased knowledge about which information has to, andwhich information does not have to, be processed.

The theoretical notion of information reduction (forrelated proposals, see Anderson, Matessa, & Douglass,1995; Logan & Etherton, 1994) is rooted in the traditionalbelief that selection of information is a necessary prerequi-site for action (e.g., J. J. Gibson, 1979; Hoffmann, 1993;Neisser, 1976; Neumann, 1985, 1990; van der Heijden,1992) and has been introduced in many different areas ofpsychology. It can be found in the literature on perception(e.g., E. J. Gibson, 1963; J. J. Gibson & Gibson, 1955;Kaptein, Theeuwes, & van der Heijden, 1995), concept

•formation (e.g., Regehr & Brooks, 1993), educational psy-chology (e.g., Bransford, Sherwood, Vye, & Rieser, 1986;Gaeth & Shanteau, 1984), and expertise (e.g., Shanteau,1992; Shapiro & Raymond, 1989). Moreover, the idea ofinformation reduction is consistent with empirical resultsobserved in various areas of psychology (e.g., Christensen etal., 1981; Fisher & Tanner, 1992; Holding, 1985; LaBerge &Samuels, 1974; Lambert, Spencer, & Mohindra, 1987;Myles-Worsley, Johnston, & Simons, 1988; Strayer &Kramer, 1994a, 1994b).

To test the information-reduction hypothesis for thedomain of cognitive-skill acquisition, we asked participantsto verify alphabetic letter strings (Haider & Frensch, 1996).Strings consisted of an initial letter-digit-letter triplet and a

172

INFORMATION REDUCTION 173

varying number of additional letters (e.g., "D [4] I," "E [4] JK L," "D [4] J K," and "E [4] K L M N"). Participantsjudged whether the strings followed the alphabet or not. Thedigit in brackets was to be interpreted as the number ofletters skipped in the alphabet between the two letterscomposing the triplet (e.g., "D [4] I" could be expressed as"D [E, F, G, H] I"). Strings were either correct (i.e., theyfollowed the alphabet, e.g., "D [4] I") or incorrect (i.e., theydid not follow the alphabet, e.g., "D [4] J"). It is importantto note that errors in incorrect strings always occurred atString Position 3 (i.e., the letter to the right of the digit).Therefore, letters in String Positions 4 and higher werealways correct and were redundant for the verification taskrequired of participants.

Because the total number of letters varies across strings inthe alphabet verification task, one can infer whether or notparticipants ignore the task-irrelevant information from themagnitude of the string-length effect for correct strings. Ifparticipants verify the entire string, then verification timeshould increase systematically with string length. If, on theother hand, participants have learned to limit their taskprocessing to the relevant letter-digit-letter triplet, thenverification time should be independent of string length. Themagnitude of the string-length effect for correct strings isthus an indirect measure of the degree to which task-irrelevant information is ignored.

Three main results were reported in the initial set ofstudies (Haider & Frensch, 1996). First, the effect of stringlength on the verification times for correct strings disap-peared altogether after approximately 500 trials. Second,when errors were suddenly introduced at the formerlyredundant string positions, participants' error rates corre-lated positively with the amount of practice they hadreceived on the original task. Third, the magnitude of thestring-length effect was unaffected by changes in the stimu-lus material (i.e., new letters).

These results are suggestive of a two-phase information-reduction process, in which task-relevant and task-redundantinformation are identified in the first phase. In the secondphase, task-relevant information is actively selected forprocessing (e.g., Allport, 1987; Neumann, 1990), and task-redundant information is actively ignored (e.g., Neisser &Becklen, 1975). Primarily on the basis of the first and thirdmain results summarized above, we reasoned that noticingthe distinction between relevant and redundant informationmay be driven by both bottom-up and top-down processes(i.e., that it may be the result of implicit or, explicit learningmechanisms, or both), whereas the active selection ofrelevant information and ignoring of redundant informationmay be the result of a conscious and voluntary decision to nolonger attend to and perceive those structural components ofthe task that contain redundant information. Additionalsupport for the latter view comes from a new series ofexperiments (Haider & Frensch, in press), in which it wasshown that speed and accuracy instructions affect the rate ofinformation reduction, even when instructions are givenhalfway through training. In general, the two-phase view isconsistent with arguments that have been made in other

psychological contexts (e.g., J. J. Gibson & Gibson, 1955;Neisser, 1976; Trabasso & Bower, 1968).

The primary goal of the present set of experiments was toaddress two issues. The first issue concerns the generalizabil-ity of the earlier findings. The second issue addresseswhether information reduction operates at a perceptual or"later" conceptual level.

In regard to the first issue, one might wonder to whatextent the original findings might have been due to specificcharacteristics of the experimental task (i.e., the alphabetverification task). In principle, the issue of generalizabilitycan be addressed by either (a) replicating the earlier findingswith entirely new experimental tasks or (b) manipulatingthose aspects of the previously employed task that mighthave yielded information reduction. Given the methodologi-cal difficulties that are involved in assessing information-reduction directly, we choose the latter approach.

One potentially interesting task parameter is the locationof the task-relevant and task-irrelevant information withinthe alphabetic strings. At least two arguments support theview that positioning task-relevant information at the begin-ning of the strings—as we had done in previous research—might have favored, or perhaps even induced, informationreduction. Both arguments assume that there may be a"natural" tendency for participants in a relatively long andboring experiment to come to pay increasingly less attentionto later string parts and to "fire off' their responses on thebasis of increasingly less information (e.g., Gratton, Coles,& Donchin, 1992). The first argument holds that because ofsuch a tendency, participants may be more likely to noticethat errors always occur at the same position when thatposition is processed earlier rather than later in a trial.According to the second argument, responding on the basisof increasingly less information would minimize the re-sponse latency difference between strings of different lengths,which we interpreted in terms of information reduction. Todeal with these and other potential arguments concerning thelocation of the task-relevant and task-redundant information,we manipulated whether the task-relevant information waslocated at the beginning or end of the strings in Experiment 1.

A second task parameter that could have increased thechances of our finding evidence for information reduction inthe earlier studies is the fact that we had kept the position ofthe relevant information constant during the entire trainingphase. It is conceivable that such consistency in positioningmay be a necessary precondition for information reductionto develop. A second goal of Experiment 1 was therefore toexamine the effect of consistency of positioning. For thispurpose, participants in a third condition received strings inwhich the task-relevant information (i.e., the triplet) couldrandomly occur either at the beginning or the end of thestrings.1

1 Another potentially interesting task parameter that could havefavored the occurrence of information reduction is the difference in,difficulty and perceptual salience between the task-relevant andtask-redundant information. Lincourt, Hoyer, and Cerella (1997)equalized the complexity and perceptual salience of the task-relevant information and task-redundant information in the alpha-

174 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

EXPERIMENT 1

Experiment 1 consisted of three experimental conditionsin which participants evaluated alphabetic strings of the typeused by Haider and Frensch (1996). In the fixed relevant-first condition, the task-relevant information (i.e., the letter-digit-letter triplets) was consistently presented at the begin-ning of the alphabetic strings; in the fixed relevant-lastcondition, the task-relevant information was consistentlypresented at the end of the strings; and in the relevant-mixedcondition, the task-relevant information randomly occurredeither at the beginning or end of the strings. For allparticipants, the experiment was divided into two phases, atraining phase and a transfer phase. The two phases were notdistinguishable for participants and differed only in terms ofthe location at which errors in incorrect strings occurred.Errors in the training phase (i.e., Trial Blocks 1-7) occurredonly at the unique digit-letter transition of the string; errorsin the transfer phase occurred either inside the triplet or, onsome trials, in the formerly redundant letters.

Degree of information reduction was assessed in twoways in all three conditions—first, in terms of the string-length effect for correct strings, which was expected todecline with practice, and, second, in terms of the error ratefor incorrect strings with errors outside the triplet. The latterwas expected to be higher than the error rate for incorrectstrings with errors inside the triplet during the transfer phase.More specifically, the expected findings for Experiment 1were as follows.

above should be found only for participants in the relevant-first condition.

Consistency of Positioning

Comparing performance in the two fixed positioningconditions with that in the two types of positioning withinthe relevant-mixed condition makes possible a fair assess-ment of the influence of consistency of positioning that isunconfounded by potential positioning effects. If consis-tency of positioning is not a necessary precondition forinformation reduction, then we should find a similar declineof the string-length effect in the two fixed positioningconditions and the relevant-mixed condition. In addition, theerror rate for incorrect strings with errors outside the triplet(the transfer phase) should not differ between conditions. If,on the other hand, information reduction depends on consis-tency of positioning, then the effects described above shouldbe found only for participants in the two fixed conditions.

Method

Participants

One hundred seven male students at the University of the ArmedForces, Hamburg, Germany, served as participants in the experi-ment. Participants ranged in age from 20 to 27 years (M = 24.1,SD = 1.96) and received course credit in introductory psychology.

Positioning of Task-Relevant Information

First, if information reduction is unaffected by the position-ing of the task-relevant information, then participants in therelevant-first and the relevant-last conditions should show asimilarly declining string-length effect for correct strings inthe training phase but should differ in terms of theirstring-length effects for incorrect alphabetic strings. Becauseof the fixed location of errors in incorrect strings at the laststring position, participants in the relevant-last conditionshould show a declining string-length effect for incorrectstrings as well as for correct strings. In contrast, participantsin the relevant-first condition should show no string-lengtheffect for incorrect alphabetic strings because in this condi-tion errors occurred always at the third string position.Second, regardless of positioning of task-relevant informa-tion, the error rate for incorrect strings with errors outsidethe triplet (e.g., the transfer phase) should be higher than theerror rate for incorrect strings with errors inside the tripletduring the transfer phase. Alternatively, if indeed informa-tion reduction depends on locating task-relevant informationat the beginning of the strings, then the effects described

bet verification task by using strings of the form triplet-triplet-triplet. These authors' demonstration of information reduction withthe modified version of our original task shows that the differencein complexity and perceptual salience between the task-relevantand task-redundant information inherent in our earlier research(Haider & Frensch, 1996) and in our present research is not anecessary factor to obtain information reduction.

Stimuli and Apparatus

In total, 50 correct and 70 incorrect alphabetic strings were usedin the experiment. Correct strings contained a letter-digit-lettertriplet (e.g., "E [4] J") that began with 1 of 10 letters, E, F, G, H, I,J, K, L, M, or N. The 10 possible correct triplets were eitherfollowed (relevant-first condition) or preceded (relevant-last condi-tion) or, within each block, followed in half of the trials andpreceded in the other half of the trials (relevant-mixed condition)by 0, 1, 2, 3, or 4 additional letters such that the entire stringfollowed the alphabet. The digit in brackets was always equal to 4.In all three experimental conditions, there were thus 5 X 10 = 50correct letter strings that varied in total length from three to sevensymbols.

The construction of 50 of the 70 incorrect letter strings wasidentical to the construction of the correct letter strings, except thatthe letter immediately following the digit 4 was the letter thatfollowed the correct one in the alphabet (e.g., "E [4] K" instead of"E [4] J"). In addition, 20 incorrect strings were constructed thatcontained the error outside the triplet (five errors at each of the fourlogically possible string positions in the two fixed positioningconditions and two to three errors at each of the eight logicallypossible positions in the relevant-mixed condition).

Strings were presented at the center of a 17-in. (43.2-cm)diagonal video screen controlled by a PC-AT80386. The letterswere approximately 1.0 X 0.8 cm. Consecutive letters appearedapproximately 0.8 cm apart on the screen. Response keys were the" < " or the "—" key on the second row from the bottom on aPC-AT keyboard. Half of the participants were instructed to use the" < " key to indicate that a string was correct and the "—" key toindicate that the string was incorrect; for the other half, the keyassignment was reversed.

INFORMATION REDUCTION 175

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three experi-mental conditions, fixed relevant-first, fixed relevant-last, andrelevant-mixed, and were tested in groups of up to 3 people in amoderately lit room containing three PC-ATs. The experimentbegan with computerized instructions. Participants were instructedthat their task was to verify alphabetic strings. They were told whatconstituted a correct string, and were shown examples of correctand incorrect strings. Then, a short practice session followed inwhich participants evaluated 10 strings. For the practice strings,errors in incorrect strings occurred inside as well as outside theletter-digit-letter triplet. If participants made more than threeerrors on the practice strings, the instructions and practice sessionwere repeated.

When they had successfully completed the practice session,participants received additional instructions and began with thetraining phase. They were told to pay attention to the entire stringbecause errors could occur anywhere in the string. Both speed andaccuracy of responding were stressed. During the training phase, inall conditions the 50 correct and 50 incorrect alphabetic stringswere presented seven times each, once in each of seven trainingblocks.

The transfer phase consisted of a single block of trials andimmediately followed the training phase without further instruc-tions. Here, the 50 correct strings, 30 of the 50 incorrect strings ofthe practice phase and 20 incorrect strings with errors outside thetriplet, were presented. The order of strings was randomly deter-mined for each participant in each trial block.

Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation cross at thecenter of the screen for 500 ms. The disappearance of the fixationcross was followed by the presentation of a string that remained onthe screen until a response was made. The screen then went blankfor 1,000 ms, and the next fixation cross appeared. When theparticipants responded incorrectly, an error prompt with a 600-msduration appeared on the screen and a 440-Hz tone was sounded.

In the transfer phase, only half of the participants in therelevant-first and the relevant-last conditions and all participants inthe relevant-mixed condition received error feedback for trials inwhich errors occurred outside the letter-digit-letter triplet. Theremaining participants did not receive feedback.

After every trial block, participants received feedback abouttheir mean response time and their error rate for the preceding trialblock and were allowed to take a short break. For all conditions, thesummary feedback given for the transfer block included mistakesfor incorrect strings with errors outside the triplet. The entireexperiment lasted between 90 and 120 min.

Design

Dependent variables were the individual median reaction times(RTs) and the mean error rates per trial block. The only between-subjects factor was triplet position (relevant-first vs. relevant-lastvs. relevant-mixed). Within-subjects factors were trial block (1through 8), string length (3 through 7), string type (correct vs.incorrect), and error type (inside vs. outside triplet).

Results

For each participant and each trial block, mean error rateswere computed first to ensure that only participants withreasonable error rates in the training were entered in the dataanalysis. Two participants showed error rates higher than10% in each training block (the a priori criterion for

excluding participants) and were excluded from further dataanalyses. There were 45 participants in the relevant-firstcondition, 38 participants in the relevant-last condition, and22 participants in the relevant-mixed condition. Mean errorrates per trial block were generally low (Ms = 3.5%, 3.8%,and 3.4% in the relevant-first, the relevant-last, and therelevant-mixed conditions, respectively). For all partici-pants, median RTs for correct responses to correct andincorrect strings were computed separately for each trialblock and each string length.

In all analyses reported here and in Experiment 2, theadopted significance level was .05. For significant effects,individual probability values are not reported.

The discussion of the main results is divided into twosections. First, we describe the overall learning effects in thetraining phase of the experiment. Second, we discuss thefindings obtained for the transfer phase.

Training Phase

Overall RTs

Figure 1A (fixed relevant-first vs. fixed relevant-lastconditions) and Figure IB (relevant-first vs. relevant-laststrings of the relevant-mixed condition) display the meanRTs per trial block for correct and incorrect strings as afunction of triplet position.2 As can be seen from Figure 1A,the two fixed triplet conditions showed very similar RTdeclines over training. A closer look reveals that thedifference between correct and incorrect strings differed forthese two conditions, at least for the first few trial blocks.Specifically, in the relevant-first condition, incorrect stringswere initially verified more quickly than correct strings. Incontrast, in the relevant-last condition, correct strings wereinitially verified more quickly than incorrect strings. Thedifferences between correct and incorrect strings graduallydisappeared, however, in both conditions. Moreover, com-parison of Figures 1A and IB shows that the relevant-mixedcondition produced longer latencies for both relevant-first and relevant-last strings than the two fixed positionconditions.

To examine the effect of positioning of the task-relevantinformation and of consistency of positioning on overallRTs, we computed a 2 (triplet position: relevant-first vs.relevant-last) X 7 (training block: 1-7) X 2 (string type:correct vs. incorrect strings) mixed-design analysis of vari-ance' (ANOVA) and two separate 2 (consistency: fixedrelevant-first vs. mixed relevant-first and fixed relevant-lastvs. mixed relevant-last) X 7 (training block: 1-7) X 2 (stringtype: correct vs. incorrect strings) mixed-design ANOVAs.

Positioning of task-relevant information. The fixed posi-tioning ANOVA yielded significant main effects of trainingblock, F(6, 486) = 218.3, MSE = 703,404, and of stringtype, F(l , 81) = 5.33, MSE = 132,290. In addition, theinteraction between triplet position and string type, F(l, 81) =

2 The confidence intervals shown here and in subsequent figureswere constructed separately for each experimental group (Loftus &Masson, 1994).

176

7000 A

HAIDER AND FRENSCH

5000-

4000-

3000-

2000-

Fixed-first / correct strings

Fixed-last /correctstrings

Fixed-first / incorrect strings

Fixed-last / incorrect strings

7000-•

6000-

5000-

3000-

1000-

Mixed-first / correct strings

Mixed-last / correct strings

Mixed-first / incorrect strings

-A—- Mixed-last / incorrect strings

4 5Practice Block

4 5Practice Block

Figure 1. Mean RTs (reaction times) for correct and incorrect strings in Experiment 1 (A: fixedconditions; B: mixed conditions). The error bars represent 95% within-subject confidence intervals asdefined by Loftus and Masson, 1994.

28.43, MSE = 132,290, and the interaction between practiceblock and string type, F(6, 486) = 4.88, MSE = 33,488,were significant, as was the three-way interaction,F(6,486) = 4.26, MSE = 33,488. At present, we do not havea reasonable explanation for the puzzling three-way interac-tion but interpret the overall results of this initial analysis asdemonstrating few clear-cut differences between the twofixed triplet position conditions.

Consistency of positioning. Both the relevant-first stringand the relevant-last string ANOVAs revealed significantmain effects of consistency, F(l , 65) = 10.46, MSE =9,759,054, and F(l , 58) = 7.71, MSE = 12,559,836, for thetwo relevant-first and the two relevant-last conditions,respectively, and of practice block, F(6, 390) = 134.8,MSE = 742,662, and F(l , 58) = 116.99, MSE = 762,427,for the two relevant-first and the two relevant-last condi-tions, respectively. In addition, the relevant-first stringANOVA yielded an additional significant interaction be-tween practice block and string type, F(6, 390) = 10.1,MSE = 41,107. In contrast, the relevant-last string ANOVArevealed a significant main effect of string type, F(l , 58) =5.97, MSE = 136,463, as well as significant interactionsbetween consistency and string type, F(l, 58) = 14.01,MSE = 136,463; between practice block and string type,F(6, 348) = 4.57, MSE = 44,104; and between consistency,

string type, and practice block, F(6, 348) = 2.21, MSE =44,104. Thus, consistency of positioning affected the overalllatencies in that varying the triplet position slowed downparticipants' verification times. More important, neither thetwo relevant-first conditions nor the two relevant-last condi-tions differed substantially with regard to their practice-related decline of overall latencies, indicating that consis-tency of positioning appears to have had, at best, minoreffects on learning rate.

String-Length Effect

In order to capture the effects of the string-length manipu-lation, we computed the slopes of the best fitting linearregression lines across the five string lengths separately foreach participant in each trial block (see Haider & Frensch,1996).

Positioning of task-re levant information. As mentionedabove, if the positioning of the task-relevant informationdoes not affect information reduction, then the mean slopesfor correct strings should not differ between the fixedrelevant-first and the fixed relevant-last conditions. In addi-tion, in the fixed relevant-last condition, the mean slopes forincorrect strings should be similar to the slopes for correctstrings; in the fixed relevant-first condition, however, they

INFORMATION REDUCTION 177

should be flatter and, ideally, should not change withpractice.

The reason for this expected difference is that participantsin the fixed relevant-first condition, for whom errors alwaysoccurred at String Position 3, should stop their processing ofthe incorrect strings as soon as they discover the errors.Therefore, a string-length effect should not develop in thiscondition. For participants in the fixed relevant-last condi-tion, in contrast, errors in incorrect strings occurred at thevery last string position. Thus, early in training, the entirestring had to be processed regardless of whether the stringturned out to be correct or incorrect. Consequently, astring-length effect should be observed in this conditionearly in practice but then should disappear, because partici-pants should increasingly be able to distinguish task-relevant from task-redundant information.

Figure 2A displays the average slopes for correct andincorrect strings over the seven training blocks for the fixedrelevant-first and the fixed relevant-last conditions. With theexception of the first training block, the general pattern ofresults depicted in Figure 2A fits the predictions outlinedabove. As can be seen, three of the four functions for the twofixed-position conditions were very similar to each other andshow a decline over practice; the fourth function (i.e.,

incorrect strings in the fixed relevant-first condition) wascomparatively less affected by practice. As can be seen fromFigure 2B, the slopes of the relevant-first and the relevant-last strings in the relevant-mixed condition also declinedwith training (again, with the exception of the first trainingblock). This decline, however, was somewhat flatter thanthat observed for the two fixed positioning conditions. Inaddition, the effect of positioning of task-relevant informa-tion on the difference between correct and incorrect stringswas less pronounced than for the two fixed positioningconditions. Because three of the four conditions showedunexpectedly flat slopes in the first training block, this blockwas excluded from all following analyses.

The effect of positioning of the task-relevant informationon information reduction was assessed by a 2 (tripletposition: fixed relevant-first vs. fixed relevant-last) X 6(training block) X 2 (string type) mixed-design ANOVA onparticipants' slope values. This ANOVA yielded significantmain effects of triplet position, F(l, 81) = 9.87, MSE =36,057.7; training block, F(5,405) = 27.5, MSE = 9,278.2;and string type, F(l, 81) = 21.75, MSE = 11,327.5. Inaddition, the interactions between triplet position and stringtype, F(l, 81) = 7.93, MSE = 11,327.5, and betweentraining block and string type, F(5, 405) = 4.79, MSE =

300T A

200-

100-

0-

-100 •

Fixed-first / correct strings

Fixed-last / correct strings

Fixed-first / incorrect strings

Fixed-last / incorrect strings

4 5

Practice Block

200-

-100-

— Mixed-first / correct strings

— Mixed-last / correct strings

* Mixed-first / incorrect strings

— Mixed-last /incorrectstrings

4 5

Practice Block

Figure 2. Means of best fitting regression slopes for correct and incorrect strings in Experiment 1(A: fixed conditions; B: mixed conditions). The error bars represent 95% within-subject confidenceintervals as defined by Loftus and Masson, 1994.

178 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

7,638.5, were both significant, as was the three-way interac-tion between triplet position, practice block, and string type,F(5,405) = 3.34, MSE = 7,638.5.

To more cleanly assess the effect of positioning of thetask-relevant information on information reduction, wecomputed a Linear -X Linear Interaction contrast betweentriplet position and training block for correct strings only.This analysis yielded no significant effect, F(l, 81) = 2.61,MSE = 9,984, p = . 11, indicating that positioning had only anegligible effect on the decline of the slopes for correctstrings.

On the whole, the pattern of results from the two fixedpositioning conditions is consistent with the assumption thatthe positioning of the triplets itself had little effect oninformation reduction. Participants in the fixed relevant-firstand the fixed relevant-last conditions showed similar de-clines of the string-length effect over practice for correctstrings but differed with regard to their performance onincorrect strings, F(l, 81) = 15.91, MSE = 26,476, indicat-ing that task processing of the incorrect strings was affectedby error position. If errors occurred at String Position 3, aswas the case in the relevant-first condition, participants wereable to stop processing of the incorrect strings as soon asthey discovered the errors. If, in contrast, errors occurred atthe very last position of the strings, as was the case in therelevant-last condition, the entire string had to be processeduntil participants were able to distinguish relevant fromredundant task information. Therefore, the string-lengtheffect should not develop in the relevant-first condition andshould have declined in the relevant-last condition, which isexactly what was observed.

Consistency of positioning. To assess the effect ofconsistency of positioning on degree of information reduc-tion, we conducted two separate 2 (consistency, fixedrelevant-first vs. mixed relevant-first and fixed relevant-lastvs. mixed relevant-last) X 6 (training block) X 2 (stringtype) mixed-design ANOVAs on participants' slope values.The relevant-first ANOVA yielded significant main effects ofconsistency, F(l, 65) = 23.57, MSE = 21,250.1; of practiceblock, F(5, 325) = 16.37, MSE = 9,082.7; and of stringtype, F(l , 65) = 26.01, MSE = 21,552.4. In addition, theinteraction between practice block and string type wassignificant, F(6, 325) = 6.41, MSE = 8,736.9 (all otherps > .1). The relevant-last ANOVA revealed significantmain effects only of practice block, F(5, 290) = 12.91,MSE = 13,030, and of string type, F(l, 58) = 9.37, MSE =14,530.4 (all otherps> .1).

Thus, this pattern of results suggests that consistency ofpositioning does influence information reduction. The de-cline of the string-length effect in the relevant-mixedcondition was flatter than that in the two fixed positioningconditions, indicating that information reduction was some-what reduced when triplet position was manipulated withintraining blocks. However, information reduction was notcompletely absent; the effect of practice block wasF(5, 105) = 3.54, MSE = 16,324, and F(5, 105) = 4.37,MSE = 14,720, for relevant-first and relevant-last strings inthe relevant-mixed condition, respectively.

Transfer Phase

The discussion of the main results from the transfer phaseis divided into two sections. First, the effect of errors in theformerly redundant letters on overall error rates is reported.Second, we discuss the correlation between error rate andstring-length effect.

Whereas all participants in the mixed-relevant conditionand about half of the participants in the two fixed positioningconditions (23 participants in the relevant-first and 15participants in the relevant-last conditions) consistentlyreceived error feedback in the transfer phase, the remaininghalf of the participants in the two fixed-relevant conditionsdid not receive error feedback when they erroneouslyaccepted a string with an error outside the triplet as correct.The omission of error feedback in the transfer block wasdeliberate because we were concerned that error feedbackmight artificially reduce the theoretically important errorrate.

Overall Error Rate

Table 1 contains the mean error rates for the last trainingblock and the transfer block for the two error types (errorsoccurring inside vs. outside the triplet). The error rates arereported separately for the three feedback conditions (rel-evant-first, relevant-last, and mixed-relevant) and the twono-feedback conditions (fixed relevant-first and fixed rel-evant-last). As argued above, occurrence of informationreduction can be inferred if the error rate for incorrect stringswith errors outside the triplet is higher than the error rate forincorrect strings with errors inside the triplet. Table 1 shows,first, that error rates for strings with errors inside the tripletdid not differ between conditions.

Second, and more important, in all conditions the errorrate for strings with errors outside the triplet was signifi-cantly higher than the error rate for strings with errors insidethe triplet, although this difference was affected by feedback.Third, although positioning of task-relevant informationitself did not affect error rates, neither with nor withoutfeedback, consistency of positioning did. Error rates in therelevant-mixed condition were smaller than in the two fixedpositioning conditions with feedback, particularly for rel-evant-last strings.

Table 1Error Rates for Strings With Errors Outside theTriplet (Transfer Block)

Condition

Relevant-first/feedbackRelevant-last/feedbackMixed-first/feedbackMixed-last/feedbackRelevant-first/no feedbackRelevant-last/no feedback

Lasttrainingblock(insidetriplet)

2.562.732.913.092.592.96

Transferblock(insidetriplet)

2.933.313.073.183.012.45

Transferblock

(outsidetriplet)

28.9136.0028.6415.0063.8657.39

INFORMATION REDUCTION 179

Two 2 (consistency: fixed relevant-first vs. mixed relevant-first and fixed relevant-last vs. mixed relevant-last; allconditions with feedback) X 3 (type of error: inside-triplet/last-training-block vs. inside-triplet/transfer-block vs. outside-triplet/transfer-block) mixed-design ANOVAs were com-puted separately. The relevant-first ANOVA yielded a reliablemain effect only of error type, F{2, 86) = 61.3, MSE =164.19. The relevant-last ANOVA, however, revealed reli-able main effects of consistency, F(l , 35) = 5.44, MSE =235.64, and of error type, F(2,70) = 28.23, MSE = 211.82,as well as a significant interaction between consistency anderror type, F(2,70) = 6.26, MSE = 211.82.

A third 2 (triplet position: relevant-first vs. relevant-last;both conditions without feedback) X 3 (type of error)mixed-design ANOVA on participants' error rates revealed asignificant main effect only of type of error, F(2, 86) =80.17, MSE = 626.40. Neither the main effect of tripletposition nor the two-way interaction was significant in thisanalysis.

Taken together, the error rate data support the resultsobtained above with overall RTs and the string-length effect,namely, that consistency of positioning reduces degree ofinformation reduction.

Error Rate Distribution for Errors Outside Triplets

The fact that half of the participants in the fixed relevant-first and the fixed relevant-last conditions received nofeedback in the transfer block allows for an additionalanalysis, the correlation between error rates for strings witherrors outside the triplet and the magnitude of the string-length effect for correct strings. If indeed the error rate forstrings with errors outside the triplet and the magnitude ofthe string-length effect are both measures of information

reduction, as we have argued previously (Haider & Frensch,1996), then the two measures ought to be correlated.

Figure 3 relates the string-length effect for correct strings(averaged over the two last blocks of the training phase) tothe error rate in the transfer phase. As can be seen, therelation between string-length effect and error rate wassignificant in both conditions, r(22) = - .49 and r{22>) =— .73 for the relevant-first and the relevant-last conditions,respectively.

Nevertheless, the results were, at first glance at least, quitesurprising. First, the error rate frequency distributions werequite different for the two experimental conditions. Specifi-cally, participants in the relevant-last condition could bedivided into two groups. Of the 23 participants in thiscondition, 10 committed a maximum of 10% errors onincorrect strings with errors in the redundant string part andshowed virtually no string-length effect by the end of thetraining phase. Twelve participants committed a minimumof 95% errors on the same strings and exhibited rather largestring-length effects by the end of training. The error ratefor 1 participant lay in between the two peaks of thedistribution.

Participants in the relevant-last condition thus behaved inone of two ways: First, information reducers (« = 12)recognized the relevant and redundant task information withtask practice and, consequently, selected the relevant infor-mation for processing and ignored the redundant informa-tion altogether. Second, nonreducers (n = 10), in contrast,processed rather consistently the entire task informationthroughout the experiment.

By comparison, in the relevant-first condition, the relationbetween string-length effect and error rate was relativelylinear, but the distribution of error rate was much more

Relevant-First300-1

200-

100-o

55

o-

-1000 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

Error Rate

Relevant-Last300-1

200-

g; ioo

55

o-

-1000 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

Error Rate

Figure 3. Scatter plots: error rate for strings with errors outside the triplet versus mean slope(averaged over the last two training blocks) in Experiment 1.

180 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

continuous. As Figure 3 shows, the extreme upper end of theerror rate distribution (i.e., 100%) was oversampled relativeto the remainder of the distribution. That is, there were 9participants with error rates of 100% who, for the most part,exhibited relatively small string-length effects. In addition,the lower end of the distribution was undersampled relativeto the relevant-last condition.

Participants in thejelevant-first condition thus behaved inone of three different ways. First, some participants wereinformation reducers (those participants who showed errorrates close to 100%; n = 9). Second, some were nonreducers(those who showed error rates close to 0%; n = 3). Third,some were mixed reducers (n = 10), who initially ignoredredundant information but who at some time during thetransfer phase began noticing the errors in the formerlyredundant task component. Once noticed, the formerlyredundant information was no longer ignored. Eight of the10 participants in this group behaved in this way, if one takesthe rather crude measure of correctly identifying threeconsecutive errors in the formerly redundant task componentas a measure of when participants first noticed that theredundant information had become relevant. The 2 remain-ing participants behaved erratically; for a while, theyperformed as if they knew that the information outside of thetriplets was relevant, but then they returned to ignoring thisinformation nevertheless.

Discussion

Experiment 1 produced three main results regarding thequestion of generalizability of previously obtained findingson information reduction (Haider & Frensch, 1996). First,varying the positioning of task-relevant information (i.e., theletter-digit-letter triplet) within the strings between subjectshad little effect on overall response times, the initialmagnitude of the string-length effect, and the overall likeli-hood of participants' making mistakes when errors in theformerly redundant task part were suddenly introduced.

Second, varying the positioning of task-relevant informa-tion within subjects appears to reduce information reduction.The results obtained for the relevant-mixed condition sug-gest that manipulating positioning of task-relevant informa-tion within blocks reduced the decline of slopes overpractice and the overall likelihood of errors on strings witherrors outside the triplet relative to the two "pure" condi-tions. Nevertheless, and worth noting, information reductionstill operates under mixed positioning conditions. In sixadditional single-case studies, we trained participants over30 practice blocks (Haider & Frensch, 1997). Three of theseparticipants received consistently relevant-last strings, and 3received either relevant-first or relevant-last strings (ran-domly varied from trial to trial). The results confirmed thatinformation reduction occurs with both consistent andmixed positioning of task-relevant information, although itwas again reduced in the mixed positioning condition.

Third, the distribution of errors for new incorrect stringsin the transfer phase differed for the relevant-first and therelevant-last conditions. The main difference between theerror distributions for the two conditions lies in the finding

that, in the transfer phase, a substantial number of partici-pants in the relevant-first condition, but only 1 participant inthe relevant-last condition, initially ignored redundant infor-mation but eventually came to process it. There are severalpossible reasons for this finding. For example, the differencebetween the relevant-first and the relevant-last conditionsmight have been due to overleamed left-to-right readinghabits, making it very difficult for participants in therelevant-first condition to abruptly stop their processing ofthe strings once the relevant task component had beenprocessed. This difficulty might not have existed in therelevant-last condition because there the relevant task infor-mation was located at the end of the strings, and right-to-leftmovement of eye fixations might have required extra effort.

Whatever the reason for this difference in error pattern,the results of Experiment 1 give very little support to theassumption that the positioning of the task-relevant informa-tion at the beginning of the strings might be necessary forinducing information reduction. Indeed, with the exceptionof the error distributions, there were no hints in our dataindicating that positioning itself had any effect on informa-tion reduction.

EXPERIMENT 2

The main issue addressed in Experiment 2 was whetherinformation reduction operates at a perceptual or laterconceptual level. According to the information-reductionhypothesis (Haider & Frensch, 1996), redundant informa-tion, once identified, is no longer perceived and conse-quently is not processed any longer at any level of thecognitive system. However, there exist no data as of yet thatwould refute the alternative argument, namely, that informa-tion recognized as redundant continues to be perceived but isignored at a later processing stage.

The main focus of Experiment 2, therefore, was onparticipants' eye fixations while they were performing thealphabet evaluation task. More specifically, the emphasiswas on the frequency and duration of participants' eyefixations on both the task-relevant and task-redundant infor-mation. If redundant information, once identified, is nolonger perceived, then we should find that participants' eyefixation frequencies show a clear decrease for the redundantpositions of the strings with increasing practice. Alterna-tively, if redundant information is ignored at a later stage ofprocessing, then participants' eye fixation frequencies shouldnot show a decline for redundant string positions, althoughfixation durations might well show such an effect.

Experiment 2 was essentially a replication of the twofixed positioning conditions used in Experiment 1. Partici-pants in the relevant-first condition received strings in whichthe relevant letter-digit-letter triplet was positioned consis-tently at the beginning of the strings (i.e., String Positions1-3). Participants in the relevant-last condition evaluatedstrings in which the relevant information always occurred atthe end of the strings. In contrast to Experiment 1, notransfer block was given in Experiment 2.

Consistent with our finding little support for the effects ofpositioning on information reduction in Experiment 1, we

INFORMATION REDUCTION 181

expected the string-length effect, as our main measure ofinformation reduction in Experiment 2, to be predicted bythe number of times with which participants fixated theredundant task components. Participants showing smallstring-length effects should fixate the task-redundant compo-nent less often than participants showing large string-lengtheffects, irrespective of triplet position. In contrast, weexpected the string-length, effect to be less well predicted bythe duration of the eye fixations.

Method

Participants

Participants were 16 male and 14 female students at theUniversity of Gottingen, Germany, who were paid DM 20 ($12) forparticipating in the experiment. The participants ranged in age from19 to 37 years (Af = 24.2, SD = 4.69). All participants had normalor corrected-to-normal vision. Because of technical problemsconcerning the calibration of eye movements, the data from 2participants had to be discarded.

3.8+6cm.A * *

Materials

Apparatus

A projection screen (70 X 45 cm) was placed about 1.5 m infront of the participants. Presentation of strings was controlled by aPower Macintosh computer. Participants' responses and responsetimes were assessed and stored by the Power-Mac. Eye movementswere monitored by a DEMEL-Recorder (Debic 84) and recordedon a PC-AT80386 that was attached to the Power-Mac. TheDEMEL-Recorder generated 50 samples per second.

Stimulus Materials

Thirty correct and 30 incorrect alphabetic strings were used. Asin Experiment 1, these strings were composed by 10 differentcorrect and 10 different incorrect letter-digit-letter triplets andwere either followed (relevant-first condition) or preceded (relevant-last condition) by 0, 2, or 4 additional letters.

Strings were displayed at the center of the screen. Letters wereapproximately 2.8 cm in length and 3.8 cm in height. Consecutiveletters appeared approximately 6 cm apart. The horizontal visualangle between two consecutive letters was equal to 3°. Participantsresponded by pressing either the left or right key on an externalkeypad with two keys. Half of the participants were instructed touse the right key to indicate that a string was correct and the left keyto indicate that the string was incorrect; for the other half, the keyassignment was reversed.

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two experimen-tal conditions, relevant-first and relevant-last, and were individu-ally tested in a dimly lit room that contained the video screen. Eachparticipant was seated in a movable reclining chair with a headrest.The headrest was adjusted for each participant in order to reducehead movements.

The experimental session began with computerized instructionsfollowed by a short practice session (see Experiment 1). Afterparticipants successfully completed this part of the experiment,their eye fixations were calibrated at 20 points of the screen

A B C D E C4) I

Figure 4. Assignment of absolute eye-fixation screen positions tostring letters in Experiment 2.

determining the upper, lower, left, and right boundaries of thescreen. During the entire experimental session, the calibration ofparticipants' eye fixations was observed on-line by a trainedexperimenter. If accuracy of calibration fell below a prespecifiedthreshold, participants' eye fixations were recalibrated. This oc-curred only two times during the entire experiment, once for eachof 2 participants.

After calibration, the training phase began. The training phasecontained 240 correct and 240 incorrect alphabetic strings that wereequally divided into eight blocks of 60 strings each. In each of thetraining blocks, the 30 correct and 30 incorrect strings werepresented once, randomly ordered for each participant in eachtraining block. Eye movements were registered from the appear-ance of the letter string until the response was given.

After every trial block, participants were given feedback abouthow well they were doing in terms of their mean response time andtheir error rate for the preceding trial block and were allowed totake a short break. The entire experiment lasted between 60 and 90

Design

As in Experiment 1, the main dependent variables were themedian response time per trial block and the mean error rate pertrial block. Concerning the eye movements, the main dependentvariables were the mean number and mean duration of fixations onthe relevant and redundant string positions. There were fourindependent variables, triplet position (relevant-first vs. relevant-last; between-subjects), string type (correct vs. incorrect strings;within-subjects), trial block (1-8; within-subjects), and stringlength (3,5,7; within-subjects).

Preliminary Data Analysis of Eye Movements

The coordinates of all stored eye fixations were first transformedinto video screen positions. Then, absolute eye fixation screenpositions were converted to string positions. Figure 4 summarizesthe assignment of absolute screen positions to string letters.3 Theunit of analysis was defined as fixations that fell into one of theseven position categories and had a minimum duration of 80 ms (cf.Rayner, Slowiaczek, Clifton, & Bertera, 1983).

Results

As in Experiment 1, a first check ensured that onlyparticipants with reasonable error rates were entered in thedata analysis. Therefore, mean error rates were computed for

3 Letter boundaries were constructed such that they were notexactly in between adjacent letters. The reason for this was that apilot study had shown that after training, participants did not fixatethe letters themselves any longer but rather the space betweenadjacent letters.

182 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

each participant and each trial block. No participant showedconsistently high error rates (higher than 10% errors in eachtraining block). Mean error rates per training block were4.7% and 4.8% in the two experimental conditions, relevant-first and relevant-last, respectively. There were 12 partici-pants in the relevant-first condition and 16 participants in therelevant-last condition. For all participants, median RTs forcorrect responses to correct and incorrect strings werecomputed separately for each trial block and each stringlength.

The discussion of the results is divided into two mainsections. First, we describe the results of the string-lengtheffect analysis. Then, we report the results of the eye-fixationanalysis.

String-Length Effect

As in Experiment 1, the slopes of the best fittingregression lines across the three string lengths were com-puted for each participant and each trial block. Figure 5displays the average slopes in the two triplet positionconditions for correct and incorrect strings over the eighttraining blocks.4 As can be seen in the figure, the results

350'

300'

250'

200'

S 150'

100-

50-

0-

-50'

Relevant-first / correct strings

Relevant-first / incorrect strings

Relevant-last / correct strings

Relevant-last / incorrect strings

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Training Block

Figure 5. Means of best fitting regression slopes for correct andincorrect strings in Experiment 2. The error bars represent 95%within-subject confidence intervals as defined by Loftus andMasson, 1994.

closely replicated those reported for the two fixed position-ing conditions of Experiment 1 (cf. Figure 2). Three of thefour slopes were very similar to each other and declined withtraining. The fourth slope, for incorrect strings in therelevant-first condition, was considerably smaller than theremaining ones and was relatively unaffected by practice.

A 2 (triplet position) X 8 (training block) X 2 (string type)mixed-design ANOVA on participants' slope values yieldedsignificant main effects of triplet position, F(l , 26) = 11.01,MSE = 72,864.76; training block, F(7,182) = 6.29, MSE =15,888.5; and string type, F(l , 26) = 10.77, MSE =20,287.48. In addition, the interaction between triplet posi-tion and string type was significant, F(l, 26) = 10.01,MSE = 20,287.48. Because of the relatively small samplesize in Experiment 2, the three-way interaction was notsignificant. Nevertheless, the pattern of results depicted inFigure 5 was sufficiently similar to the pattern depicted inFigure 2 to indicate replication of the findings obtained inExperiment 1.

Eye-Fixation Analyses

Training Effects: Eye-Fixation Frequency

Figures 6A and 6B display the frequency of eye fixationson the relevant and redundant string components across theeight blocks of training in the two experimental conditions,separately for correct (Figure 6A) and incorrect (Figure 6B)strings. The data are averaged over string length and excludeincorrect responses. As stated above, fixations were countedonly if they (a) fell into one of the seven position categoriesdisplayed in Figure 4 and (b) had a minimum duration of 80ms. As can be seen, the patterns of results were very similarfor correct and incorrect strings. The main finding conveyedby the two figures is that the fixation frequencies for therelevant-last condition showed the exact pattern we hadexpected. That is, the fixation frequencies were initiallyidentical for task-relevant and task-redundant information,but with increasing training participants fixated the redun-dant information less frequently than the relevant informa-tion. The pattern in the relevant-first condition was less clear.Here, the fixation frequencies differed markedly for the firstblock of training already, and the decline over trainingwas roughly parallel for task-relevant and task-redundantinformation.

A 2 (triplet position) X 2 (string type: correct vs. incorrectstrings) X 8 (training block) X 2 (relevance: relevant vs.redundant) mixed-design ANOVA on participants' eye-fixation frequencies yielded results consistent with theimpressions conveyed by Figures 6A and 6B. The maineffects of training block, F(7,182) = 39.64, MSE = 0.11; ofrelevance, F(l , 26) = 50.54, MSE = 0.797; and of stringtype, F(l , 26) = 25.46, MSE = 0.04, were all significant, aswere the interactions between triplet position and relevance,F(l , 26) = 13.8, MSE = 0.797; between triplet position andstring type, F(l , 26) = 29.82, MSE = 0.04; and between

4 Two outlier RT values, one each in Trial Blocks 1 and 2,occurred in the triplet-first/incorrect strings condition and werereplaced by their respective mean values.

INFORMATION REDUCTION 183

Incorrect Strings

ZOO-

Correct Strings

1.60-

&• 1.20-c

£ I.OO-

0.80-

0.60-

0.20-

0.00

L60-

1.40-

Relevant-first (relevant)

Relevant-first (redundant)

Relevant-last (relevant)

Relevant-last (redundant)

0.80

0.60-

0.40-

0.20-

0.00

] — Relevant-first (relevant)

I*"' Relevant-first (redundant)

>— Relevant-last (relevant)

•*•• Relevant-last (redundant)

3 4 5Practice Block

3 4 5

Practice Block

Figure 6. Means of eye-fixation frequencies for relevant (triplet) and redundant (letters) taskcomponents for correct strings (A) and for incorrect strings (B) in Experiment 2. The error barsrepresent 95% within-subject confidence intervals as defined by Loftus and Masson, 1994.

relevance and string type, F(l, 26) = 60.94, MSE = 0.03. Inaddition, the three-way interactions between triplet position,relevance, and string type, F(l , 26) = 36.38, MSE = 0.03,and between triplet position, training block, and relevance,F(7,182) = 6.96, MSE = 0.064, were significant.

Separate follow-up analyses for the relevant-first and therelevant-last condition indicated significant interactions be-tween relevance and training block in both the relevant-first,F(7, 77) = 4.57, MSE = 0.061, and the relevant-lastcondition, F(7,105) = 3.28, MSE = 0.067. However, as canbe seen in Figures 6A and 6B, the pattern of resultsunderlying the significant interactions differed for the twoconditions. In the relevant-last condition, the interaction wasdue to a steeper decline of the fixation frequencies fortask-redundant than task-relevant information; in contrast,the interaction in the relevant-first condition was due to asteeper decline of the fixation frequencies for task-relevantinformation.

There are at least two different possible explanations forour not finding the pattern of results we had expected in therelevant-first condition. First, it is conceivable that themechanisms underlying information reduction are differentin the two triplet position conditions. For example, it might

be that participants in the relevant-first condition, due tooverlearned left-to-right reading habits, found it difficult tonot fixate the redundant task information at all and insteadtried to minimize the amount of time spent looking at theredundant letters. If this is the case, then the analysis of eyefixation durations presented below should reveal this.

Second, it also seems plausible that the pattern underlyingthe interaction between relevance and training block in therelevant-first condition might have been caused by a flooreffect for eye fixations on task-redundant information. Giventhat the fixation frequency on task-redundant information inthat condition was very low in the first training blockalready, there simply was not much room left for a decline.

Concerning the latter, the floor-effect argument, the twoexperimental conditions were divided, by means of a mediansplit, into those showing large (nonreducers) and thoseshowing small string-length effects (reducers) by the end ofthe training phase (Block 8). Figures 7A and 7B display themean fixation frequencies on the redundant and the relevanttask component for reducers and nonreducers in the relevant-first (Figure 7A) and relevant-last (Figure 7B) conditions(correct alphabetic strings only). As can be seen, the datapresented are indeed consistent with this argument.

184 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

Relevant-First Relevant-Last

1.75-

1.50-

0.25-

0.00

— • — Reducers (relevant)

— O — Reducers (redundant)

0— Nonreducers (relevant)

Nonreducers (redundant)

2.00-

1.75-

1.25-

IO 1.00-

0.75-

p" l" I (-1

1 1 1 " 1

0.00-

— • — Reducers (relevant)

-"D"** Reducers (redundant)

• — Nonreducers (relevant)

—O"- Nonreducers (redundant)

4 5 6

Practice Block

4 5

Practice Block

Figure 7. Means of eye-fixation frequencies for relevant (triplet) and redundant (letters) taskcomponents for reducers versus nonreducers for correct strings (A: relevant-first condition; B:relevant-last condition) in Experiment 2. The error bars represent 95% within-subject confidenceintervals as defined by Loftus and Masson, 1994.

In the relevant-first condition, the fixation frequencies forrelevant information (i.e., the triplet) were very similar forreducers and nonreducers. The fixation frequencies forredundant information, in contrast, diverged for reducersand nonreducers, beginning with Block 4. Put differently,the difference between fixating relevant and fixating redun-dant task information was more pronounced for reducers bythe end of the training phase than for nonreducers.

Similarly, and as shown in Figure 7B, in the relevant-lastcondition, nonreducers showed basically no difference foreye fixations on task-redundant and task-relevant informa-tion; reducers, however, did. Taken together, these findingssuggest that the reason for our not finding the expectedpattern of results in the relevant-first condition might indeedhave been a floor effect in that condition.

Training Effects: Eye-Fixation Duration

Figures 8A and 8B display the mean duration of eyefixations on the relevant and redundant task componentsacross the eight blocks of training in the two experimentalconditions, separately for correct (Figure 8A) and incorrect(Figure 8B) alphabetic strings. The mean fixation durationsshown are the averages of the individual duration means,which, in turn, were based on fixations to the seven

categories shown in Figure 4 lasting at least 80 ms. It isimportant to note that the mean durations were computed onthe basis of eye fixations; they were thus not affected byfixation frequency. The data are averaged over string lengthand exclude incorrect responses, and again the patterns ofresults were essentially identical for correct (Figure 8A) andincorrect (Figure 8B) strings.

Three aspects of Figures 8A and 8B are important. First,in both conditions the mean duration of eye fixations fortask-redundant information was much shorter than the meanduration for task-relevant information. Second, the meanduration for task-relevant information was longer in therelevant-last than the relevant-first condition. Third, andmost important, in both conditions training did not decreasethe eye fixation durations more for redundant than forrelevant task information.

A 2 (triplet position) X 2 (string type: correct vs. incorrectstrings) X 8 (training block) X 2 (relevance: relevant vs.redundant) mixed-design ANOVA on participants' fixationdurations yielded significant main effects of training block,F(7,182) = 33.05, MSE = 15,083.8; of relevance, F(l, 26) =75.27, MSE = 244,018.2; and of string type, F(l, 26) =5.82, MSE = 10,546.9. Furthermore, the interactions be-tween triplet position and relevance, F(l , 26) = 6.24,

INFORMATION REDUCTION 185

Correct Strings Incorrect Strings

900

700-

.2 600-

S

400-

300-

200-

100-

i 1 T ? • • • • • • • • • • « • . • *'""-•

± »

Relevant-first (relevant)

Relevant-first (redundant)

Relevant-last (relevant)

Relevant-last (redundant)

3 4 5Practice Block

900-

3Q

I<3 400-

300-

100-

— Relevant-first (relevant)

*— Relevant-first (redundant)

— Relevant-last (relevant)

—- Relevant-last (redundant)

3 4 5

Practice Block

Figure 8. Means of eye-fixation durations for relevant (triplet) and redundant (letters) taskcomponents for correct strings (A) and for incorrect strings (B) in Experiment 2. The error barsrepresent 95% within-subject confidence intervals as defined by Loftus and Masson, 1994.

MSE = 244,018.2; between training block and relevance,F(7,182) = 8.1, MSE = 10,875; and between relevance andstring type, F(l, 26) = 4.86, MSE = 3,248, were significant,as was the three-way interaction, F(l, 26) = 12.96, MSE =3,248.

The findings conveyed by Figures 8A and 8B point to thefollowing interpretation. First, the mean duration for task-relevant information might have been consistently longerthan for task-redundant information because of a differencein complexity for the two types of information. It was simplymore demanding to process the triplet than the additionalletter strings. Second, the shorter duration of triplets in therelevant-first condition is consistent with the higher fixationfrequency found for this condition and points to a possiblestrategy difference between the relevant-first and relevant-last conditions mentioned above. Third, and most important,there was no evidence in support of the view that eye-fixation durations decline differentially with training fortask-relevant and task-redundant information.

Predicting the Magnitude of the String-Length Effect

Of primary importance in Experiment 2 was the predic-tion of participants' string-length effects on the basis of theireye-fixation frequencies and durations for task-relevant and

task-redundant information. Figures 7A and 7B, of course,demonstrate already that the fixation frequency for task-redundant information was related to the magnitude of thestring-length effect. To address this issue more directly, wefitted two separate multiple regression models to the datafrom the relevant-first and the relevant-last condition (cor-rect and incorrect alphabetic strings were both included).Predictors in both models were participants' mean eye-fixation frequencies for task-relevant and task-redundantinformation (averaged over Blocks 7 and 8) and their meanfixation durations for task-relevant and task-redundant infor-mation, also averaged over Blocks 7 and 8. The dependentvariable was participants' mean string-length effect (aver-aged over Blocks 7 and 8), expressed as the slope of theindividual best fitting linear regression across the threestring lengths. The results of the two analyses are summa-rized in Table 2.

As can be seen in the table, the string-length effect was, inboth conditions, significantly predicted by participants'eye-fixation frequencies on task-redundant information. Thedirection of the beta weights was as one would expect. Thatis, the lesser the frequency with which participants fixatedthe task-redundant information, the smaller were theirstring-length effects. In the relevant-last condition, a second

186 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

Table 2Results of Regression Analyses PredictingIndividual String-Length Effect

Predictor variable

Gaze frequencyTripletLetters

Gaze durationTripletLetters

Accumulated eye-fixation durationsTripletLetters

Condition

Relevant-first

.06

.62*

- .26.12

- .38.84*

Relevant-last

- .49*.71*

.36

.12

- .15.80*

Note. Numbers are standardized regression coefficients.*p < .05.

significant predictor was the fixation frequency for task-relevant information. Here, the greater the frequency withwhich participants fixated task-relevant information, thesmaller was the string-length effect. Fixation durations, incontrast, did not significantly predict the magnitude of thestring-length effect.

Because the two predictors, eye-fixation frequency andeye-fixation duration, were highly correlated in the multipleregression analyses reported above, we computed a secondset of regressions, again separately for the relevant-first andrelevant-last conditions. Here, predictors were the accumu-lated eye-fixation durations (fixation frequency multipliedby fixation duration) for task-relevant and task-redundantinformation (averaged over Blocks 7 and 8). The dependentvariable was again participants' mean string-length effect(averaged over Blocks 7 and 8). Table 2 contains theresulting beta weights of this additional analysis.

The results of the regression analyses are consistent withthe training effects described above. In both conditions,participants' string-length effect was driven mainly by theireye-fixation frequencies for task-redundant information. Putsimply, participants with small string-length effects did notfixate the task-redundant information. Participants' behavioris thus consistent with the information-reduction hypothesisand supports the assumption that information, once it hasbeen identified as task-redundant, is no longer perceived.

The practice-related change in the fixation frequency fortask-redundant information, particularly evident in the rel-evant-last condition, is impressively underscored by Figures9A (correct strings) and 9B (incorrect strings). These figuresdisplay the mean of first-gaze frequencies across the sevenstring positions (only data from the string-length = 7 condi-tion were used) for the two experimental conditions early(Block 1) and late (Block 8) in training. In other words, thefigures display how many times participants' first gaze oneach trial fixated the first, second, third, and so forth stringposition.

Participants in each of the two experimental conditionswere again divided, by means of a median split, into thoseshowing large (nonreducers) and those showing smallstring-length effects (reducers). What Figures 9A and 9B

show is a clear shift in the preferred first-gaze position forparticipants in the relevant-last condition from early to latein training, but only for reducers. These participants initiallyfixated the beginning of the strings but by the end of thetraining phase had come to fixate primarily the end of thesequence, that is, the task-relevant information. Nonreducersin the same experimental condition did not show such ashift. In addition, participants in the relevant-first conditiondid not show such an effect either, as indeed they should nothave. This finding underscores the interpretation that smallstring-length effects reflect the perceptual ignoring of task-redundant information rather than the inhibition of process-ing task-redundant information at a later cognitive process-ing stage.

Discussion

The main goal of Experiment 2 was to assess whetherinformation, once identified as redundant, is ignored at theperceptual level or is "suppressed" at a later processingstage. The most important conclusion suggested by theresults of Experiment 2 is that task-redundant informationappears to be ignored at the perceptual level. This conclu-sion is most impressively demonstrated by the findings forthe relevant-last condition in which participants seem toinitially have processed the task information in a strictlyserial manner, moving from the first to the last stringposition. After practice, they behaved in complete accor-dance with the information-reduction view, and, as inExperiment 1, neatly fell into one of two categories.Participants showing small string-length effects by the endof the training phase did not fixate task-redundant informa-tion any longer; instead, they concentrated exclusively onthe task-relevant information. Participants who still showedlarge string-length effects by the end of the training phase, incontrast, fixated task-relevant and task-redundant informa-tion proportionally to exactly the same extent as they did atthe beginning of training.

The conclusion that task-redundant information is ignoredat the perceptual level is somewhat less well supported forthe relevant-first condition. Participants in the relevant-firstcondition, although they clearly "noticed" the task-redundant information at some level, did not show theexpected Relevance X Training Block pattern for fixationfrequencies overall, although the interaction became visiblewhen information reducers and nonreducers were separated.We have argued that the weaker pattern in this conditionmight have been due to a floor effect that, in turn, might havebeen caused by the influence of highly overlearned left-to-right reading habits on participants' behavior in the alphabetverification task.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Haider and Frensch (1996) argued that practice-relatedchanges in the speed and quality of task performance arepartially due to a reduction in the amount of information thatis processed. The information-reduction hypothesis holdsthat participants learn, with practice, to distinguish between

INFORMATION REDUCTION 187

Correct Strings

—••— Relevant-first / reducers

••"••— Relevant-first / nonreducers

—O— Relevant-last / reducers—f.... Relevant-last / nonreducers

Position in String

2 3 4 5

Position in String

75-

25-

Incorrect Strings

Relevant-first / reducers

Relevant-first / nonreducers

Relevant-last / reducers

Relevant-last / nonreducers

2 3 4 5

Position in String

2 3 4 5

Position in String

Figure 9. Percentage of first-gaze frequency across seven string positions in the first and lasttraining blocks for correct strings (A) and for incorrect strings (B) in Experiment 2.

task-relevant and task-redundant information and to limittheir processing to task-relevant information. The presentexperiments address two issues raised by previous research:(a) the generalizability of the original findings and (b) thequestion of whether task-redundant information is ignored ata perceptual or conceptual level of processing.

Taken together, the results from Experiment 1 indicatethat the positioning of the task-relevant information withinthe alphabetic strings evaluated by participants in the Haider

and Frensch (1996) task does not affect information reduc-tion. In addition, inconsistency of positioning appears toslow down, but does not completely eliminate, the processof information reduction. Thus, the findings reported earlierby Haider and Frensch can be said to be, to some extent,general effects that do not depend on the specifics of the taskas it was used earlier.

In addition, and perhaps more important, the findings ofExperiment 2 indicate that task-redundant information ap-

188 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

pears to be ignored at the perceptual level. These findings donot necessarily imply, of course, that task-redundant informa-tion is exclusively ignored at the perceptual level; rather,they suggest that redundant information is perceptuallyignored whenever this is possible. Our current thinking isthat participants, once they have identified redundant infor-mation, try to minimize the amount of processing theyextend to this information. Preferably, they do so by notperceiving redundancies any longer. If this is not a viableoption, they might use a different strategy, such as minimiz-ing the duration of time spent on the redundant information.

This view suggests that how participants deal withredundant information is based on a conscious strategicdecision. A similar argument has been recently advanced byStrayer and Kramer (1994a, 1994b), who have argued thatparticipants can strategically adjust their response criteria,that is, how much of the information given by a task isverified before a response is selected (e.g., Fisk & Schneider,1983; LaBerge & Samuels, 1974; Strayer & Kramer, 1994a,1994b). Alternatively, one could assume, of course, thatinformation reduction is dealt with implicitly, that is, by wayof some automatic (e.g., Jacoby, 1991) learning mechanism,perhaps one similar to the mechanism that is responsible forthe overshadowing or blocking effect in Pavlovian condition-ing experiments (e.g., Kamin, 1969; Rescorla, 1968). Astimulus-response association in the blocking paradigm isnot learned if it does not contain information beyond thatinherent in another stimulus-response compound that hasbeen learned earlier (e.g., Hinchy, Lovibond, & Ter-Horst,1995; Lovibond, Siddle, & Bond, 1988). The blocking effectdoes not occur if both stimuli carry unique informationregarding the appearance of the unconditioned stimulus.

Because participants in the present experiments did notknow at the beginning of training that the strings containedredundant information, they had to process both the redun-dant and the relevant task information initially. Thus, at leastat the beginning of training, the redundant letters carriedunique information for the required response beyond thatgiven by the triplet. At the present time, we therefore believeit to be unlikely that a blocking-like learning mechanism,and in general, any nonconscious, nonstrategic mechanism,could account for the findings presented here and in theearlier Haider and Frensch (1996) article, although such apossibility cannot and should not be excluded at the presenttime. For instance, one could argue that an automaticimplicit strengthening of attention occurs that eventuallyreaches a level at which participants become consciouslyaware of the attentional shift.5 Nevertheless, it is noteworthythat only 7 (out of 36) participants with large string-lengtheffects in the fixed relevant-first and the fixed relevant-lastconditions of Experiment 1 (the two conditions withoutfeedback in the transfer block) reported that they had noticedthe redundant information and yet continued checking theredundant letters because they expected errors to occur. Incontrast, all participants with close-to-zero string-lengtheffects by the end of the training phase (n = 37) reportedthat they had discovered that errors occurred only in thetriplet and, therefore, had stopped checking the additionalletters.

The results of the present experiments show that oneimportant effect of practice is that task processing becomesmore selective as has been suggested by, among manyothers, J. J. Gibson and Gibson (1955). Put differently, theresults of Experiments 1 and 2 support the contention thatthe rate of acquiring a skill is dependent on the amount of,and ability to, limit processing to relevant information. Theeffect of information reduction on learning rate cannot beexplained by assuming that participants optimize their taskprocessing by means of a transition from algorithmic-basedto memory-based processing (e.g., Logan, 1988,1992) or byoptimizing the sequence of procedures used to process thetask (e.g., Anderson, 1982,1987, 1992).

On the other hand, however, information reduction in andby itself cannot be the sole explanatory mechanism underly-ing skill acquisition. Task information can be safely ignoredonly when it is redundant. Otherwise, information reductionmust lead to incorrect task solutions. Clearly, many tasks inour daily lives, such as pure mental arithmetic tasks (Frensch& Geary, 1993), for instance, do not contain redundantinformation, yet they are learned. Thus, it is obvious that anyeffects of practice that are observed with these tasks cannotbe due to information reduction.

This is to say that it is necessary to extend the theoreticalmechanisms introduced by Anderson and his colleagues andby Logan and his colleagues to incorporate the presentfindings. For example, Anderson et al. (1995) recentlyargued that "information gathering" should be considered a"procedure" (p. 64). Similarly, Logan and Etherton (1994;Lassaline & Logan, 1993) have shown that what is learnedduring skill acquisition is dependent on what is attended to.Thus, although the main focuses of existing theories of skillacquisition are different from the phenomenon that we haveconcentrated on, it is clear that existing theories are, at leastin principle, capable of explaining information reduction—if they are extended to incorporate mechanisms that cap-ture the practice-related increase in the selective use ofinformation.

5 We are grateful to Bill Whitlow for pointing out this possibility.

References

Allport, A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral andneurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H.Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception andaction (pp. 395^19). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychologi-cal Review, 89, 369^06.

Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.

Anderson, J. R. (1987). Skill acquisition: Compilation of weak-method problem solutions. Psychological Review, 94, 192-219.

Anderson, J. R. (1992). Automaticity and the ACT* theory.American Journal of Psychology, 105, 165-180.

Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Anderson, J. R., Matessa, M., & Douglass, S. (1995). The ACT-R

theory and visual attention. In J. D. Moore & J. F. Lehman(Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Conference of theCognitive Science Society (pp. 506-511). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

INFORMATION REDUCTION 189

Bransford, J. D., Sherwood, R. D., Vye, N. J., & Rieser, J. (1986).Teaching thinking and problem solving. American Psychologist,41, 1078-1089.

Cheng, P. W. (1985). Restructuring versus automaticity: Alternativeaccounts of skill acquisition. Psychological Review, 92, 414-423.

Christensen, E. E., Murry, R.C., Holland, K., Reynolds, J., Landay,M. J., & Moore, J. G. (1981). The effect of search time onperception. Radiology, 138, 361-365.

Fisher, D. L., & Tanner, N. (1992). Optimal symbol set selection:An automated procedure. Human Factors, 34, 79-92.

Fisk, A. D., & Schneider, W. (1983). Category and word search:Generalizing search principles to complex processing. Journalof Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,9, 177-195.

Frensch, P. A. (1991). Transfer of composed knowledge in amultistep serial task. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 997-1016.

Frensch, P. A. (1994). Composition during serial learning: A serialposition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 20, 423-442.

Frensch, P. A., & Geary, D. C. (1993). The effects of practice oncomponent processes in complex mental addition. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,19, 433-456.

Gaeth, G. J., & Shanteau, J. (1984). Reducing the influence ofirrelevant information on experienced decision making. Organi-zational Behavior and Human Performance, 33, 263-282.

Gibson, E. J. (1963). Perceptual learning. Annual Review ofPsychology, 14, 29-56.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception.Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning:Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review, 62, 32-41.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing theuse of information. Strategic control of activation responses.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480-506.

Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (1996). The role of informationreduction in skill acquisition. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 304-337.

Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (1997). Lernmechanismen deskognitiven Fertigkeitserwerbs [Learning mechanisms in cogni-tive skill acquisition]. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psycholo-gie, 44, 521-560.

Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (in press). Information reductionduring skill acquisition: The influence of task instruction.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied.

Hinchy, J., Lovibond, P. F., & Ter-Horst, K. M. (1995). Blocking inhuman electrodermal conditioning. Quarterly Journal of Experi-mental Psychology, 48B, 2-12.

Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis [Prediction andknowing]. Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

Holding, D. H. (1985). The psychology of chess skill. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

Jacoby, L. L. (1991). Aprocess dissociation framework: Separatingautomatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory& Language, 30, 513-541.

Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and condi-tioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishmentand aversive behavior (pp. 279-296). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Kaptein, N. A., Theeuwes, J., & van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1995).Search for a conjunctively defined target can be selectivelylimited to a color-defined subset of elements. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,21, 1053-1069.

LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automaticinformation processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6,293-323.

Lambert, A. J., Spencer, M. B., & Mohindra, N. (1987). Automatic-ity and the capture of attention by a peripheral display change.Current Psychological Research and Reviews, 6, 136-147.

Lassaline, M. L., & Logan, G. D. (1993). Memory-based automatic-ity in the discrimination of visual numerosity. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,19, 561-581.

Lincourt, A. E., Hoyer, W. J., & Cerella, J. (1997). Selectiveattention in the acquisition of instance-based learning: Adultage differences. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervalsin within-participant designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,I 476-490.

Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization.Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.

Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Com-mon underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1-35.

Logan, G. D. (1992). Shapes of reaction-time distributions andshapes of learning curves: A test of the instance theory ofautomatization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 18, 883-914.

Logan, G. D., & Etherton, J. L. (1994). What is learned duringautomatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, andCognition, 20, 1022-1050.

Logan, G. D., & Klapp, S. T. (1991). Automatizing alphabetarithmetic: I. Is extended practice necessary to produce automa-ticity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,and Cognition, 17, 179-195.

Lovibond, P. F, Siddle, D. A. T, & Bond, N. (1988). Insensitivityto stimulus validity in human Pavlovian conditioning. QuarterlyJournal of Experimental Psychology, 40B, 377-410.

Myles-Worsley, M., Johnston, W. A., & Simons, M. A. (1988). Theinfluence of expertise on X-ray image processing. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,14, 553-557.

Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.Neisser, U., & Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: Attending to

visually-specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 480-494.Neumann, O. (1985). Die Hypothese begrenzter Kapazitat und die

Funktionen der Aufmerksamkeit [The hypothesis of limitedcapacity and the functions of attention]. In O. Neumann (Ed.),Perspektiven der Kognitionspsychologie [Perspectives of cogni-tive psychology] (pp. 185-229). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

Neumann, O. (1990). Visual attention and action. In O. Neumann &W.'Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action(pp. 227-268). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1981). Mechanisms of skillacquisition and the law of practice. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.),Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1-56). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

Newell, K. (1991). Motor skill acquisition. Annual Review ofPsychology, 42, 213-237.

Rayner, K., Slowiaczek, M. L., Clifton, C , & Bertera, J. H. (1983).Latency of sequential eye movements: Implications for reading.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception andPerformance, 9, 912-922.

Regehr, G., & Brooks, L. R. (1993). Perceptual manifestations ofan analytic structure: The priority of holistic individuation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 92-114.

190 HAIDER AND FRENSCH

Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence andabsence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative andPhysiological Psychology, 66, 1-5.

Schmidt, R. A. (1987). The acquisition of skill: Some modificationsto the perception-action relationship through practice. In H.Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception andaction (pp. 77-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shanteau, J. (1992). How much information does an expert use? Isit relevant? Acta Psychologica, 81, 75-86.

Shapiro, K. L., & Raymond, J. E. (1989). Training of efficientoculomotor strategies enhances skill acquisition. Acta Psycho-logica, 71, 217-242.

Strayer, D. L., & Kramer, A. F. (1994a). Strategies and automatic-ity: I. Basic findings and conceptual framework. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,20, 318-341.

Strayer, D. L., & Kramer, A. F. (1994b). Strategies and automatic-ity: II. Dynamic aspects of strategy adjustment. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,20, 342-365.

Trabasso, T. R., & Bower, G. D. (1968). Attention and learning.New York: Wiley,

van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1992). Selective attention in vision.London: Routledge.

Received October 16, 1996Revision received July 16, 1998

Accepted July 16, 1998 •