jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force...

22
DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") > W..: : Hearing and Communication Laboratory Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences Indiana University DTIC Bloomington, Indiana 47405 EET MAR12,1993 1119 12 1262 3 J.AN~ 199D

Transcript of jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force...

Page 1: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

DTIC FILE COPY

N

Final Technical Report~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research

N AFOSR-87-0300

I November, 1989

jjT-mitid

'.") >

W..: :

Hearing and Communication Laboratory

Department of Speech and Hearing SciencesIndiana University DTIC

Bloomington, Indiana 47405 EET

MAR12,1993

1119 12 1262 3 J.AN~ 199D

Page 2: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

6. .A

C:URITY CLASSIF1ICATIO6NOF TH-T~T 0r nREPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE OMSNo, 070.OIN

. REPORT SECURITY CLASSIFICATION lb. RESTRICTIVE MARKINGS

Unc lass if iedSECURITY CLASSIFICATION AUTHORITY 3. DISTRIBUTION/AVAILABILITY OF REPORT

DECLASSIFICATION / DOWNGRADING SCHEDULE t Pi L l, n UI.

4. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER(S) 5. MONITOPING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER(S)

AFO6JI. T . 9 0 - 0 80 I6s. NAME OF PERFORMING ORGANIZATION 1 6b. OFFICE SYMBOL 7&. NAME OF MONITORING ORGANIZATIONCharles S. Watson, Dept. of j (if applicable)

Speech & Hearing, Indiana Univ. _ _"_ AFOSR/NL6c. ADDRESS (City. State, and ZIP Code) 7b. ADDRESS (City, State, and ZIP Code)

"'loomington, IN 47405 Building 410Bolling AFB, DC 20332

8a. NA.,vIE OF FUNDING/SPONSORING 8b. OFFICE SYMBOL 9. PROCUREMENT INSTRUMENT IDENTIFICATION NUMBER

OR3ANIZATION Air Force Office (If applicable)

of S ientific Research [ NL AFOSR-87-030081c. ADDRESS (City, State, and ZIP Code) 10. SOURCE OF FUNDING NUMBERS

PROGRAM IPROJECT ITASK IWORK UNIT

Boling Air Force Base, DC 20332-6446 ELEMENT NO. NO. NO. ACCESSIONNO.

1 -. (Include Security Classification) 61102F 2313 A6

Perception of Complex Auditory Patterns

12. PERSONAL AUTHOR(S),iatscn, Charles S. (Indiana University), Kidd, Gary R. (Indiana University)13a. TYPE OF REPORT 13b. TIMECOVERED 14. DATE OF REPORT (Year, Month, Oay) IS. PAGE COUNTFinal IFROM _/1,/8L TO 0 '8 -,1989, November 19

6. SUF,LEMENTARY NOTATION

17. COSATI CODES 18. SUBJECT TERMS (Continue on reverse if necessary and identify by block nwmber)FIELD GROUP SUB-GROUP

complex sounds, auditory patterns, psychoacoustics

19. ABST, (Continue on reverse if necessary and identify by block number)

See other side.

20. DISIRIBUTION /AVAILABILITY OF ABSTRACT 21, ABSTRACT SECURITY CLASSIFICATION

.0 UNCLASSIFIED/UNLIMITED E SAME AS RPT. C OTIC USERS Unc lass izfiedAME OF RESPONSIBLE INDIVIDUAL 22b. TELEPHONE (Onclude Area Code) I, OFFI| SlfmiO

avow,')HNF mrr-Y 202 76Z;.50210D Fc m 1473, JUN 86 Previnut editioirje otilete, trI'ROV\ ci

Page 3: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

4 .

This report describes the results of research in four areas: (1) the perceptionof complex sounds, including tonal sequences, multidimensional complex sounds,and Gaussian noige;. (2) information ntegration; (3) multi-stage decisionmaking; (4) studies of the relation between auditory abilities measured withspeech and nonspeech stimuli. Major accomplishments during this funding periodinclude: (a) the discovery of the proportion-of-the-total-duration (PTD)principle: Each individual component of a complex sound is resolved with anaccuracy that is a function of its proportion of the total duration of thesound; (b) the demonstration that even small degrees of logarithmic frequencytransposition of tonal patterns severely degrades the detectability of patternchanges in novel sequences, but not in familiar sequences; (c) a,vances inresearch showing that temporal integration of auditory informati', is limited bytwo distinct types of internal noise, one that is added at the periphery beforea decision statistic is formed, and "central," or post-decision, noise; (d) thedevelopment of a theory that incorporates these limiting factors; (e) thecompletion and publication of studies of categorical perception for speech andnon-speech sounds, demonstrating that enhanced discrimination performance in theregion of certain categorical boundaries does not, as previously theorized,reflect either "hard-wired" feature detectors in the auditory nervous system,nor psychoacoustic boundaries determined by acoustic peculiarities of complexwaveforms.

Page 4: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

FinalTech ical eporAir Force Office of Scientific Research

AFOSR-87-0300

November, 1989

Page 5: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

The following technical report describes work accomplished between 1 September 1987 and 30

September 1989. Work completed prior to this period was described in the original proposal for the

current award and the HCL Progress Reports No.s 1, 2, and 3 which which have been previously

circulated.

- ABSTRACT

This report describes the results of research in four areas: K1) the perception of complex sounds,

including tonal sequences, multidimensional complex sounds, and Gaussian noise; ,(2) information

integration; ,(3) multi-stage decikon making; X4) studies of the relation between auditory abilities

measured with speech and nonspeech stimuli. Major accomplishments during this funding period

include: .(a) the discovery of the proportion-of-the-total-duration (PTD) principle: Each individual

component of a complex sound is resolved with an accuracy that is a function of its proportion of the

total duration of the sound; Pb) the-demonstration-that even small degrees of logarithmic frequency

transposition of tonal patterns severely degrades the detectability of pattern changes in novel

sequences, but not in familiar sequences; (c) advahces-in-research showing-that-temporal integration

of auditory information is limited by two distinct tyes of internal noise, one that is added at the

periphery before a decision statistic is formed, and entralA, or post-decision, noise; fd) the

development of a theory that incorporates these limiting factors; e) the completion and publication of

studies of categorical perception for speech and non-speech sounds, demonstrating that enhanceddiscrimination performac in the region of certain categorical boundaries does t as previously

theorized, reflect either - hard-wired feature detectors in the auditory nervous system, nor

psychoacoustic boundaries determined by acoustic peculiarities of complex waveforms. F .'

Scientific Goals

The goals of the project were essentially unchanged from those described in the original proposal.

We continued to pursue several lines of experimentation with complex (non-speech) auditory patterns

and also extended this work in lines of research begun when the current grant was awarded in

September, 1987: (a) studies of immediate auditory memory as a limiting factor in the discriminationFor

of complex auditory i atterns; (b) replication of our pattern discrimination studies with real and

synthetic speech stimuli; and (c) multi-dimensional pattern processing.

0

By-

o' " Dstribution/Availabi'lity Codes

*Avail and/or

-Dist Speojal

Page 6: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

Research Summaries

I. Complex Auditory-Pattern Perception

A. Tonal Sequences

1. Proportional target-tone duration as a factor in auditory pattern discrimination. C. Watson, Kidd,and Washburne

Results of previous experiments with isochronous tonal-patterns have appeared to reflect an

informational limit on pattern processing, a limit that appeared to be reasonably close to the"magicdl" 7+/-2 (components in discriminable patterns). However, these results are also consistent

with the alternative hypothesis, that target tones are equally well resolved if they occupy equalproportional durations of the patterns in which they occur. The results of a new series of experiments

revealed that the target-tone's proportion of the total pattern duration accounts for most of the

variance, while n, total duration, and individual component duration account for relatively little. Theseresults were obtained for patterns ranging in duration from 100 msec to 2 sec. Additional

experiments were designed to test the limits and generality of the proportion-of-the-total-duration

(PTD) rule. These experiments have shown that 1) pattern discrimination is not significantly affected

by the ratio of context to target tones, except when the context tones are very brief in absoluteduration as well as relative duration, 2) discrimination is primarily determined by PTD for different

target positions in spite of increases in target-position uncertainty, 3) PTD still accounts fordiscrimination performance when two non-adjacent target tones are used, but performance levels areslightly lower than in the single-target case for a given PTD. (A manuscript describing this work is in

preparation.)

2. Detection of pattern repetition in continuous tone patterns. Watson, Kidd, Washburne.

Additional data have been collected in experiments designed as variations on those reported by

Guttman and Julesz (1963, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America), but using tonal sequences

rather than noise samples. Subjects are presented with repeating or non-repeating tone patterns, using

a tracking paradigm that increases or decreases the number of tones in a pattern depending on a

subject's ability to detect repetition. The data show predicted strong effects of tone duration andbandwidth, as well as a significant interaction (due to a slightly greater effect of bandwidth at the50-msec tone duration). Listeners were able to detect the repetition of patterns consisting of more

tones with the shorter tone duration and the wider bandwidth. Despite our attempts to minimize the

occurrence of "unique events", subjects' reports indicated that judgments were often based on the

recurrence of particular events rather than detection of whole-pattern repetition.

3. Perception of Frequency- and Time-Transposed Auditory Patterns. Kidd, C. Watson.

a. Transposition in frequency and time.

This series of experiments examined the effect of transposition in frequency and in time on thedetection of single-tone frequency changes in unfamiliar tonal patterns. In the first experiments,listeners' abilities to discriminate between correct and incorrect transpositions of randomly generated

-2-

Page 7: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

five-tone patterns were investigated. Frequency transposition resulted in very large decrements inperformance, which occurred with the smallest amount of transposition, with a tendency for additionalreduction in performance with increases in the amount of transposition. Time transposition (i.e.,increases in tone duration) resulted in little or no performance decrements after 2-3 hours of training.

b. Frequency transposition with smaller transposition ratios.

In the frequency-transposition experiment described above, the smallest amount of transposition(a 1.15 frequency ratio) resu!ted in performance decrements that were nearly as great as the largertransposition intervals. A second experiment was run to determine whether there is a gradual or anabrupt change in performance for smaller transposition intervals. The frequency-transpositionexperiment was repeated using transposition ratios of 1.00, 1.03, 1.06, 1.09, 1.12, 1.15 (i.e., five logsteps from 1.0 to the smallest interval used in the previous experiment). Performance was found todecrease gradually over this range for all but one of the four subjects who showed a rather abruptdecrease in performance.

c. Transposition with reduced uncertainty.

i. Reduced transposition uncertainty. To test the extent to which transposition performance isadversely affected by transposition uncertainty (i.e., uncertainty with respect to the amount oftranspositici on a given trial), the frequency-transposition experiment was run again with a singletransposition ratio (1.52). Even after 10 hours of practice at this transposition ratio, little or noimprovement in performance was obtained.. Considerable trial-to-trial stimulus variation remained in

this experiment, however, and may be responsible for some of the poor performance.

ii. Reduced pattern uncertainty. To test the extent to which transposition performance is affectedby pattern uncertainty (or pattern familiarity), another frequency-transposition experiment was run inwhich the same pattern (randomly generated) was presented on every trial. Under these conditions,transposition performance improved dramatically; In many cases, transposition effects were completelyabsent.

d. Frequency transposition with 2-tone patterns.

A related line of investigation has been conducted using two-tone patterns (i.e., single intervals).These experiments were designed to provide a more direct test of the basic ability to detect changes inrelative frequency by using the simplest possible stimuli required to test this ability. Performanceunder high-uncertainty conditions was similar to that for 5-tone patterns, although complexinteractions among interval size, direction of transposition, and direction of frequency changes wereobserved. Results under low uncertainty conditions indicate that transposition effects and interactionare diminished and performance is often as accurate as in non-transposed conditions.

General Conclusions: Resolution of the pattern of relative frequency changes in transposed tonal

sequences is severely degraded under conditions of high stimulus uncertainty for patterns of five ormore components. Resolution of relative frequencies within transposed patterns is nearly as accurateas for non-transposed patterns a) under conditions of minimal stimulus uncertainty, or b) when thepatterns contain only two components. These effects of stimulus uncertainty and complexity are the

-3-

Page 8: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

same as those previously reported in pattern discrimination experiments without frequency

transposition.

4. Discrimination of multiple-tone targets in tonal sequences.

We have completed an initial investigation of the ability to discriminate and identify multiple-tone

targets embedded within tonal sequences. This work extends earlier Experiments with single-tone

targets to cases in which the target itself is a pattern. In this initial experiment, one of four

three-tone targets is presented on each trial in the middle of a nine-tone sequence in which all tone

durations are set at 100 ms. A same/different task is used in which listeners must determine whether

the three-tone target in the temporal center of the pattern is the same in both patterns presented on

a trial. The six context tones are selected randomly for each pattern on every trial. Thus, the two

patterns on each trial are always different, but the three-tone target segment may be the same. On

"different" trials the target tones are permuted (thus preserving the distribution of spectral energy).

The level of the context tones is initially set 40 dB down from the target-tone level (75 dB SPL) so

that target tones can be easily "heard out". An adaptive tracking procedure is used to adjust the

level of the context tones according to listeners' ability to detect the target sequence. The results

have revealed large differences across listeners and patterns, but in the best cases the level of the

context tones is increased from roughly 40-to-50 dB to 0-to-10 dB below the target tones over the

course of 150 hours of training (over 6000 total trials). In the worst cases there is little or no

improvement even after 150 hours of training. (Dr. Kathy Barsz was a visiting scientist during July

and August, 1989, and participated in the design of this experiment.)

Results with lower levels of uncertainty (using a single target sequence instead of four) have shown

no additional improvement compared to ending levels under higher uncertainty. However, the

performance of a new listener with no prior experience in the high-uncertainty experiment suggeststhat learning occurs at a faster rate with lower uncertainty. Additional experiments using patterns with

other component and total durations will be conducted to examine the role of those variables in the

detection of complex multi-component targets.

5. Perception of salient auditory events or figures. C. Watson, Kidd, Washburne.

We continued to use our auditory figure-identification procedure to study factors that may be

systematically related to the emergence of auditory figures or "targets" from various backgrounds.

6. Perception of complex auditory patterns by humans. Espinoza-Varas, C. Watson.

A chapter, writtten by B. Espinoza-Varas and C.S. Watson, was contributed to "The comparative

psychoacoustic of complex acoustic perception," co-edited by S. Hulse and R. Dooling (in press,

Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, N.J.). The thesis proposed is that, for the case of mammals,measured phylogenetic differences in pattern perception ability appear to result more from differences

in information processing abilities (i.e., central factors), than from structural and/or physiological

differences in the peripheral auditory system. (A review of this book commented favorably on the

chapter by Espinoza-Varas and Watson (Scienc, December 1, 1989).)

-4-

Page 9: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

B. Spectrally-Complex Sounds

1. Perception of multidimensional complex sounds. Kidd, C. Watson, Washbume.

This project extended our work on listeners' abilities to categorize, or classify, complex sounds,

based on information in three independent acoustic "features", studying abilities to 1) allocate

attention simultaneously to three complex auditory dimensions, and 2) accumulate complex

multidimensional information over a series of sound pulses. The stimuli consisted of sequences 1, 3, 5,

or 7 100-msec sound pulses. The features were spectral shape, temporal envelope, and harmonicity of

generating sinusoids. Listeners can integrate information in sequences of sound pulses, with little or no

loss of efficiency with increasing sequence length. Although listeners' absolute efficiency in this task is

fairly high, comparable performance levels are achieved in spite of a variety of individual patterns of

attention to the features of the stimuli. Attempts to train listeners to attend to features to which they

initially do not attend met with little or no success.

C. Gaussian-noise Stimuli

1. Discriminability of noise samples. Robinson, Fallon, Rickert.

Our work on the discriminability of noise samples was continued. This work employs pairs of

complex auditory waveforms that listeners are asked to discriminate. The waveforms are samples of

broad-band, white, Gaussian noise. All experiments made use of a same-different paradigm.Research reported in previous years has investigated several parameters that affect the discriminability

of noise samples. In our initial work in this area, we demonstrated that if only a small segment of anoise burst is replaced with a new sample of noise, the temporal position of the new segment is of

critical importance in determining discriminability. Independent of total burst duration, changes made

at the end of a burst are more discriminable than those in the middle, which are more discriminable

than changes made at the beginning. In that work, we also observed a 'Weber's law' relationship

between the duration of the altered segment and total burst duration. As total burst duration wasvaried from 25-msec to 150-msec, the ratio of the duration of the altered segment to total burst

duration remained constant within each of the three conditions: beginning, middle, and end. In two

additional experiments, we investigated the effects of decorrelating the noise samples .n "different"

trials. This was done in two ways: first, by time delaying the noise presented in the second interval

relative to that in the first interval and, second, by adding an independent sample of noise to the

sample presented in the first interval to generate the sample presented in the second interval. In both

cases, increasing the correlation of the samples in the two intervals reduces discriminability. However,

the functions relating performance to correlation are different depending on the method used to vary

the correlation. In other work, we have attempted to improve performance by reducing the size of

the set of noise samples presented to the subjects. We found a negligible effect of set size. We have

previously reported on the effects of introducing temporal gaps in the noise bursts. In the last year,

we have extended the work on temporal gaps and have investigated the effects of reducing the level

of portions of the noise bursts.

a. Effect of gap duration and position. Robinson, Fallon.

In this experiment, the overall duration of the bursts of noise in a pair was 150 msec and either a

25-msec segment of new noise was inserted at the end of the burst or a 50 msec sample was

-5-

Page 10: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

appended to the beginning of the burst. A silent interval or gap replaced a portion of the repeated

segment either immediately following or immediately prior to the appended segment. The duration of

the gap was gradually increased until only 5-msec of the repeated or inserted segment remained.

Although discriminability increased as gap duration increased, the presence of a brief repeated

segment temporally separated from the appended segment by 90-120 msec caused a large decrement

in performance. For example, when each burst in a pair consisted of a 5-msec repeated segment

followed by a 120-msec gap and a 25-msec appended segment, the average P(C) is 0.72. If the

5-msec repeated segment was not present and the pair of 25-msec bursts was presented in isolation

the overall P(C) increased to 0.88. It would appear, then, that interactions occurring after such a

long sil-'nt interval are unlikely to be due to peripheral sensory interactions, as was suggested by

Hanna (Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 409-416, 1984).

b. Effect of varying the level of the repeated segment. Robinson, Fallon.

The effects of decreasing the level of the repeated segment on the discriminability of pairs of

150-msec bursts of noise was examined in two conditions. In the first condition a 15-msec sample ofnew noise was appended to the end of the second sample of pairs presented during "different" trials.

A 75-msec sample of new noise was inserted at the beginning of the second sample of a pair in the

second condition. Prior to varying the level of the repeated segment, the discriminability of the burstspresented in the two conditions was similar. Discriminability tended to gradually increase as the level

of the repeated segment was decreased. Performance did not greatly improve until the level of the

repeated segment was reduced from 50 to at least 28 dB SPL, suggesting that peripheral maskingcannot account for the effect of the temporal position of the appended or inserted segment of noiseon discriminabiaty.

c. Discriminability of Noise Burst under Dichotic Listening Conditions. D. Robinson and M. Rickert.

Several binaural phenomena (e.g., the precedence effect) suggest that under some conditions

interaural differences occurring at the beginning of a brief acoustic event are more effective cues thanthose occurring later. In the context of our previous work concerning the discriminability of dioticnoise samples, these phenomena suggest that the advantage of the 'end' condition might not be found

with dichotic presentations. Thus one goal of extending our research to include binaural listeningtasks was to determine whether the factors affecting performance for diotic presentations act in a

similar fashion under dichotic conditions. In this experiment, performance was measured using asingle-interval, yes-no procedure. The subject's task was to detect the presence of an interaurally

uncorrelated segment of noise within a binaural broadband noise burst. Three overall stimulusdurations (25, 75, and 150 ms) were used. Psychometric functions were then obtained bysystematically varying the duration of the segment of uncorrelated noise at each of three temporalpositions (beginning, middle, and end). The results can be summarized as follows. First,

independent of overall stimulus duration, detectability improves as the duration of the uncorrelatedsegment is increased; percent correct is a monotonic increasing function of the duration ofuncorrelated noise. Second, for most durations of uncorrelated noise, detectability is best when the

segment occurs at the end of a burst. Relative to diotic presentations, however, the performancedecrements obtained by varying the temporal position of the uncorrelated segment are much smaller.

This result is not easily accounted for by most models of binaural processing. For example, the EC

-6-

Page 11: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

model [Durlach, N.I. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35, 1206-1218 (1963)] assumes that performance isdetermined by the power of the difference in the waveforms in the two auditory channels. Sincepower is computed over the total stimulus duration, it is independent of the temporal position of theuncorrelated noise. Third, for each stimulus combination used in this study, performance for dichoticpresentations greatly exceeds performance for diotic presentations.

d. Molecular Psychophysics. Robinson, Fallon

In their work concerning the role of stimulus uncertainty in the discrimination of complex tonalpatterns, Watson, Kelly and Wroton (1976) and Watson and Kelly (1981) distinguish between centraland peripheral factors that limit the processing of sensory information. They argue that the limits oninformation processing that can modified by manipulating stimulus uncertainty (i.e., smaller set size,over-training, etc.) are central, whereas those that cannot be modified are peripheral. The main goalof the research described here is to determine whether the discriminability of different 'target'segments (i.e., beginning, middle, and end) within a random, time-varying waveforrr can be alteredby minimizing stimulus uncertainty.

Previously, we have used free-running samples of noise such that new waveforms were generatedprior to each trial. This method made it impossible for subjects' to base their decisions on long-terminformation about the spectral-temporal characteristics of the noise samples. If, however, the factorsaffecting discriminability of deterministic tonal sequences and stochastic waveforms are similar, then itmay be possible to eliminate the effects of temporal position by (1) reducing the size of the stimulusset from which samples are drawn and by (2) over-training.

Two separate experiments, both employing a two-interval, same-different method were conducted.All stimuli used were samples of computer-generated broad-band Gaussian noise, 150 msec in totalduration. The first experiment was designed to estimate the hit and false alarm rates for each oftwenty 'Same' pairs and twenty'Different' pairs. This experiment measured subjects' relativeperformance for each of the individual samples. The data indicate that all pairs of noise samples arenot equally discriminable. There is considerable scatter when the points are plotted in ROC space.Although these samples have identical long-term statistical properties, it is likely that the observedvariability among samples is due to differences in their short-term power or energy spectra. Thussome pairs of noise samples are more discriminable than others. A subset of these samples was thenused in the second experiment. The samples which were selected were chosen to maximize theirseparation in ROC space. In this experiment only two noise bursts (rne 'Same' and one 'Different')were run in each experimental block. Moreover, the same noise bursts were used in successive blocks.Several experimental sessions were conducted for each pair of noise bursts in order to monitorchanges in discrimination performance that might have resulted from over-training. We were unable tofind evidence for improved performance with this reduced stimulus set and a large number of trials.

D. Information integration: Multiple observations and internal noise. Robinson, Berg.

The work described in this section began with two major goals. The first was to understand theprocesses by which humans integrate information over time or over channels. The "multiple look"

-7-

Page 12: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

problem is the basis for our initial work in this area. The basic question is, "How much addiaonal

information is gained by allowing observers more than one observation in a detection or discrimination

task?" The second goal was to develop and evaluate models of "internal noise." The amount and rate

of improvement in performance with an increasing number of observations will depend not only upon

the amount of internal noise, but upon the level of processing at which the internal noise is added.

Previous research (Swets, Shipley, McKey, and Green, 1961; Swets and Birdsall, 1978; Swets,

Green, Getty, and Swets,1978)' has demonstrated that performance in signal-in-noise detection tasks

improves as listeners are allowed more observations. According to signal detection theory, d' should

increase as the square-root of the number of observations. This result assumes that the decision

statistic is any monotonic transformation of the mean of the n likelihood ratios obtained from the n

observations. In some versions of the derivation, internal noise is added prior to the formation of the

decision statistic in order to account for the less than optimal performance at n=1. This does not alter

the square-root-of-n prediction. Previous research, usually involving the detection of repeated signals

in a noise background, has supported the square-root-of-n prediction. However, the earlier work

provided only limited tests, since the use of signals in a noise background precluded exact

specification of the noise and signal-plus-noise distributions on an observer's decision axis. Further,

only small values of n were used. Our research has extended this work by developing a unique

paradigm that allows exact specification of the distributions on an observei's decision axis (Berg and

Robinson, 1987). In this paradigm, the observer's task is to judge from which of two distributions on

frequency a sequence of n tones was bampled. Results from several experiments indicate that listeners

can approach the theoretical d' for n=1, but do not follow the square-root-of-n rule, even for smalln.

In order to account for the failure of the square-root-of-n prediction, we have assumed an

additional source of internal noise (Robinson and Berg, 1986; Berg and Robinson, 1987; Berg, 1987).

This type of internal noise may be thought of as additional variance introduced by uncertainty of the

decision criterion, changes in response bias, or memorial factors associated with the decision statistic.

Briefly, then, in the "partitioned variance" model, as we have termed it, internal noise is added at

two stages: (1) at the periphery, before a decision statistic is formed and (2) centrally, after the

statistic is formed. As we have previously reported, the model does an excellent job of describing the

data of the sequential tone experiments, as well as those of a similar experiment in which the tones

are presented simultaneously (Grantham, 1987). We have also developed techniques for estimating the

amount of internal noise added at each of the two stages and tcchniques for determining the degree

to which each tone in the sequence contributes to the observers' final decision (Sorkin, Robinson, and

Berg, 1987). Our results indicate that subjects are highly idiosyncratic in the degree to which various

portions of a temporal sequence contribute to their decision: some subjects appear to show a

primacy-recency effect while others show roughly equal weighting of the tones in a sequence.

1. Distribution of internal noise over the tonal sequence.

An important question raised by our general model is whether information from each tone in a

tonal sequence is equally weighted in determining an observer's decision. We have developed a

technique fo7 assessing how internal noise is distributed over the n-elements of a tonal sequence. In

terms of the model, the amount of information obtained from different tones in an n-element

-8-

Page 13: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

sequence will be reflected in the variance of the internal noise added at each temporal position. If a

particular temporal position contributes little to the final decision, that position will be found to have a

large amount of internal noise associated with it. If, on the other hand, a particular element

contributes a great deal, that element will have less internal noise associated with it. Data from an

auditory experiment were analyzed to assess how internal noise is distributed over successive temporal

positions. Over many thousands of trials we store the frequency of the tones actually presented in the

ith temporal position (i = 1, 2, ... n; where n is the number of tones in the sequence). We then

partition these stored frequencies into bins of arbitrary width. The purpose of this analysis is to keep

track of the number of trials on which the frequency of the ith element was in each frequency bin.

For each bin and each temporal position, we then compute the probability that the subject responds

that the sequence came from the lower distribution. Cumulative normal distributions are then fit to

the resulting ogives. The standard deviation of the best fitting normal distribution is then an estimate

of the standard deviation of the total internal noise limiting performance at each display position.

Results have revealed a tendency for tones near the beginning and end of the sequence to contribute

more to the final decision than tones in the middle.

E. Multi-stage decision making. Robinson.

In this work we considered a person-machine system consisting of an automated alarm and a

human monitor. The task of the human is to monitor a noisy channel on which information about a

potentially dangerous condition may appear. The alarm system monitors an independently noisy

channel for information about the same threatening condition. Using basic concepts of statistical

decision theory, the Contingent Criterion Model of such a per."-machine system has been

developed. According to the model, the human should establish two criteria for responding: one

contingent on an alarm from the automated detector and one, on no-alarm. The model shows large

gains in performance compared to either detector alone. In Progress Report No. 1, we reported

experimental support for the model. In the last two years, we have continued to develop the

Contingent Criterion model, to investigate the effects of additional variables on the performance of the

model, and to expand the model to include target identification.

1. Effects of inter-channel correlation. In our initial development of the Contingent Criterion

Model, we assumed that the noise in the alarm-system channel is uncorrelated with that in the

channel monitored by the human operator. Such an assumption is probably unrealistic. We have

recently investigated the degradation in performance that occurs with iLcreased correlation between

the two channels. The predictions of the 'del indicate that, although there may be a considerable

performance decrement when the correlatior. near unity, the model is quite robust, and system

performance can exceed that of either detector alone even with correlations as high as 0.50.

2. Effects of signal probability and values and costs. Other parameters of importance in

determining the performance of combined detection systems are the a priori probability of the signal

and the values and costs of the possible outcomes of a decision. In our past work, we have evaluated

system performance using measures derived directly from the Receiver Operating Characteristic

(ROC), such as P(C) and d'. These measures are not affected by changes in a priori probability or

-9-

Page 14: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

values and costs. Thus, we turned to measures based on Expected Value. Analyses b,.sed on

Expected Value suggest that the advantage of a multi-stage decision makin2 system over a single-stage

one is highly dependent on a priori probability and values and costs. In fact, under some

circumstances a single-stage system should be used.

3. Target identification. We expanded the Contingent Criterion Model to include signalclassification (identification) as well as signal detection. This effort draws on the work of Nolte(JASA, 42, 773-777, 1967) Nolte and Jaarsma (JASA, 41, 497-505,1967), Green and Birdsall

(Psych. Rev., 85, 192-206, 1978), and Starr, Metz, Lusted, and Goodenough (Radiology, 116,

533-538 ,1975). Our efforts to date suggest that the performance of a system consisting of a human

operator and an automatic signal classifier can show significant improvement compared to either

subsystem operating alone.

There are two important observations that may be drawn from this work. First, although

combined system performance (human-plus-automated detectors) may be less thar. optimum withuntrained operators, it is possible that human operators can 1-e trained to use the availableinformation more efficiently. A second observation is that system perf.rmance is dependent net only

on the benavior (sensitivity and criterion placement) of the human operator and the criterion(threshold or alarm set-point) of the automated alarm system, but also on the a priori signal

probability and the values and costs. System performance may be altered by changing any of theseparameters of the system. W. ael strongly that the designers of automated ala.m systems must take

into account the complex interactions which may occur when human operators are involved.

All of the work on multi-stage detection systems was done in collaboration with Dr. Robert D.Sorkin of Purdue University. We take this opportunity to again acknowledge his many contributionsto this, and other projects, both at ARL and HCL. Portions of the work described here have beenreported previously [Sorkin and Robinson (N.T.I.S. Report No. DOT/OST/p-34/85/021, 1985) and

Robinson and Sorkin (In Trends in Ergonomics/Human Factors II, Eberts and Eberts, North-Holland:

Elsevier, 1985)]. Additional support for this work was provided by contracts with the U. S.Department of Transportation and with the U. S. Naval Weapons Center, China Lake, CA.

F. Multiple Targets: Detection and identification of auditory signals in noise. Robinson, Rickert

The multiple target problem, and the consequent requirement for target categorization, has been

simplified to the case of equally detectable, orthogonal signals. The set of potential signals used in this

experiment consisted of four sinusoids (500, 1100, 1900, and 2700 Hz). In order to satisfy thecondition of equal detectability across channels, the signal-to-noise ratio for each the four frequencies

was adjusted to achieve a P(C) of approximately 0.75. We assumed that the wide frequency

separation between signals was sufficient to meet the condition of orthogonality. That is, each signalfrequency is processed independently. These assumptions allow application of a theorem relating target

detection and identification (Starr, Metz, Lusted, and Goodenough, 1975). In a combined

detection-identification task, subjects were askecd to listen during a specified observation interval andto make two decisions: (1) report the presence or absence of a tonal signal in a broad-band gated

-10-

Page 15: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

noise, and (2) indicate which one of the four signals occurred. The overall a priori probability ofsignal-plus-noise and noise-alone trials was balanced at 0.50. Further, each of the four frequencieswas equally likely to occur on a signal-plus-noise trial. Group mean data indicate that detectionperformance exceeds identification performance by approximately 14 percent (0.77 to 0.63). Althoughmore elaborate analyses are required to evaluate statistical fit to the data, the predictions made by thetheorem are consistent with observed performance.

1. Effect of changing the distribution of targets over channels. Robinson, Rickert

We have completed a pilot study to investigate the effects of changing target probability overchannels within the detection-recognition task. The experimental procedures described in the

previous section were also used in this study. The a priori probability of a signal occurring wasmaintained at 0.50. However, on signal trials the probability of the 500 Hz signal was reduced to 0.10while the probability of the remaining signals was increased to 0.30. Preliminary results indicate thatidentification performance increases relative to identification performance when the a priori

probabilities of the four signals are equal.

II. Studies of the relation between auditory abilities measured with speech and nonspeech stimuli.

A. Categorical perception. Kewley-Port, C. Watson, Foyle

In March, 1988, we published a manuscript on the categorical perception of speech andnon-speech sounds (Kewley-Port, Watson and Foyle, 1988). Conclusions were, 1) that a categoryboundary for these sounds was not obtained in tasks with reduced levels of stimulus uncertainty, 2)that this implies the categorization of familiar sounds is not a result of a psychoacoustic threshold, 3)that the functions obtained for the discrimination of temporal-onset differences for the speech andnon-speech sounds were basically the same.

These conclusions were challenged by Richard Pastore in a Letter to the Editor of the Journal ofthe Acoustical Society of America (1988). A reply to Pastore's letter (Watson and Kewley-Port, inpress) was published in the same issue. New data and analyses presented in our response furthersupport the conclusions listed above, and directly contradict arguments raised by Pastore.

B. Detection thresholds for isolated vowels. Kewley-Port

A series of experiments on the detectability of vowels in isolation has been completed. Stimuli

consisted of three sets of 10 vowels, one synthetic, one from a male talker and one from a femaletalker. Vowel durations ranged from 20 to 160 msec. Thresholds for detecting the vowels in isolationwere obtained from well-trained, normal-hearing listeners using an adaptive-tracking paradigm.Dete...ion thresholds for vowels calibrated for equal sound pressure at the earphones differed by asmuch as 20 dB across vowels. However, the same patterns of thresholds obtained for the vowel setwere obtained for all vowel durations. An orderly decrease in vowel thresholds was obtained forincreased duration, as predicted from temporal integration for pure tones. Several different analyses

-11-

Page 16: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

have been conducted in an attempt to explain the differential detectability for the various vowels

within each set. The most detailed of these analyses involved a model developed by Moore and

Glasberg (1987) for calculating excitation patterns and the corresponding loudness level in phons.

Although that model provided improved explanatory power over other models examined, further

refinements of perceptual models of loudness will be needed to explain these data. (A manuscript

describing these data has been submitted to the Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.)

C. Vowel Sequence Studies: High versus Minimal Uncertainty Detection using a Pattern Catalogue.Kewley-Port, C. Watson, Hackett.

Previous research in our lab has investigated complex auditory stimuli consisting of a series of

brief tones presented one after the other to form single "word-length" tonal patterns. We have nowreplicated some of those experiments with speech stimuli. The first experiment examined the

detectability of vowels, presented in sequences, under very high-uncertainty. Thresholds differed only

slightly across temporal position in the sequence, but differed considerably for different subjects and

vowel types. In a new study, a catalogue of 48 ten-vowel patterns was constructed. These stimuli were

presented under high uncertainty, where all 48 patterns were randomly presented, and under minimal

uncertainty where only a single pattern was presented. Results obtained for the detectability of vowelsin the pattern catalogue under high uncertainty were similar to those obtained in the first experiment.

Under minimal uncertainty, the detectability for vowels improved a great deal, but not as much as fortones in analogous tonal-pattern studies. Further experiments suggest that more energetic masking may

be obtained for complex vowel spectra in vowel sequences than for pure tones. These experiments arenow focusing on determining the parameters of vowel masking.

D. Effects of response uncertainty on the intelligibility of nonsense syllables. Espinoza-Varas, C.Watson, C. Parks.

Syllable intelligibility in the CUNY Nonsense Syllable Test (Dubno, J.R., and Levitt, H.J. (1981).

J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 69, 249-261) was studied with a response format consisting of either the usual

7-9 alternatives or only 3- alternatives: the correct alternative plus the two most likely confusions.With a few exceptions, the average intelligibility of each of the syllables was consistently higher (about

15% on the average) in the 3-alternative condition, but there was a strong correlation between the

performance in the two response formats.

E. Rate and number as limiting factors in the perception of sequences of syllables. C. Watson,Espinoza-Varas.

A test was developed to measure the effects of the number of syllables and, independently, those

of syllable rate, on listener's abilities to process syllable sequences. The sequences consist of digitized

and edited natural tokens of the spoken letters b, c, d, g, p, t, or v, produced as CV's ending in the

vowel /i/. Results for an initial test group showed some individual's performance to be more limited byrate than by number, and vi-e versa. Some listeners show a much more severe drop when the number

of syllables is increased from 2 to 4, than others. Basically, however, these "high capacity listeners"

-12-

Page 17: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

(those who do not show a marked change in performance as n is increased from 2 to 4) are also the

ones who do the best on the shortest sequences.

F. Informational limits in speech processing by normal and impaired listeners. Espinoza-Varas, C.Watson, Kyle.

Using a variation of the speech test described in the previous section, we compared the

information capacity of normal and impaired listeners (moderate-to-severe losses). Percent

information transmission, IT%, was estimated for each temporal position within the sequences, number

of syllables, and syllable rate, for equal-sensation-level listening. For the normal listeners, 6 or moresyllables could be identified, on the average, but for the best of the impaired listeners, the maximum

number seems to be only 4-5. This would imply a loss of information capacity of as much as 30-40

peicent. Additional Studies of this type are in progress.

0. Low communality between tests of auditory discrimination and of speech perception.Espinoza-Varas, C. Watson.

Factor analyses of the performance of normal listeners on the Test of Basic Auditory Capabilities,TBAC collected in a field (n=127) and using earphones (n=119) revealed similar structures for both

groups, consisting essentially of two factors loaded on the nonspeezh tests (complex sound/pitch and

duration/intensity) and a third that is mainly a spee..h iactor. In addition, multiple regression analyses

showed that only a small portion of the variance in the speech tests can be accounted by the optimallinear combinations of the tonal test scores.

III. Partially Supported Projects

A. Response latency and decision criterion in psychophysical decisions. Espinoza-Varas, C. Watson,Patterson, Kyle.

A manuscript ha6 oeen prepared for submission to Perception and Psychophysics describing earlierdata on the effects of decision criterion on response latencies in psychophysical decisions. Theresponse latency to a given stimulus was found to vary inversely with the distance between the stimulusand the current response criterion, and with the probability of the response. These effects wereobserved for both visually presented two-digit numbers and pure tones under three different decisiontasks. (Original & experimental work supported by an NIH grant to the Central Institute for the Deaf,data analyses and manuscript preparation supported by NIH and AFOSR grants to IndianaUniversity).

B. Development of norms and factor analysis of TBAC performance obtained in 398 listenersEspinoza-Varas, Watson

Since first developed (Watson et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1982, 71, S73), the eight-subset Test ofBasic Auditory Capabilities (TBAC) has been administered to approximately 600 listeners, in 10

-13-

Page 18: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

different studies carried out at the Boys Town National Institute, the University of Nebraska, Lincoln,

University of Wisconsin, Madison, and the Speech and Hearing Center, Indiana University. This large

data base offers a unique opportunity to standardize and develop norms for the TBAC and to perform

factor and cluster analyses. We have recently consolidated the data from the various studies, which

were previously stored in several different formats and media. We have developed a coherent

structure for this data base and currently have TBAC results for 398 listeners entered in the CDC-856

Indiana University computer, which has available appropriate statistical packages. Preliminary analyses

have confirmed that the TBAC is a reliable test, that the three-factor partitioning of the variance on

the TBAC is a stable result, and that performance on these tests has an unexpectedly strong

association with certain intellectual measures (WAIS scores, SAT's).

C. Relations between auditory capabilities and phoneme perception. Espinoza-Varas, C. Watson,Srygler.

A manuscript describing this work is in preparation for submission to the Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America.

D. Interaural comparison of discrimination abilities in monaural hearing impaired listeners.Espinoza-Varas, C. Watson.

We continued to collect data to compare, interaurally, the discrimination abilities of monaural

hearing impaired listeners. The goal is to assess the differential roles of central and peripheral

processing in speech and non-speech tasks.

E. Articulatory-Rate context effects in phoneme identification. G. Kidd

These experiments demonstratud that the pattern of changes in articulatory rate over several

syllables prior to a target phoneme can have a substantial influence on identification of the target

phoneme. A manuscript describing this work was published in the Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

-14-

Page 19: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

Personnel

Name Position Title Department

Charles S. Watson, Ph.D. Professor & Chair Speech & Hearing Sci.

Donald E. Robinson, Ph.D. Professor Psychology

Diane Kewley-Port, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Speech & Hearing Sci.

Gary R. Kidd, Ph.D. Assistant Scientist Speech & Hearing Sci.

Bias Espinoza-Varas, Ph.D. Visiting Assistant Speech & Hearing Sci.Professor

Kathy Barsz, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Psychology(Visiting Scientist) SUNTY Geneseo

Advanced Degrees Awarded

Bruce G. Berg, Ph.D., 1987; "Internal noise in auditory detection tasks."

Susan M. Falon, Ph.D., 1989; "Discriminability of Bursts of Reporducible Noise"

-15-

Page 20: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

Manuscripts and Publications

Berg, B.G. (1987). Internal noise in auditory detection tasks. Ph.D. Dissertation, Indiana University.

Espinoza-Varas, B. (1987). Levels of representation of phonemes and bandwidth of spectral andtemporal integration. In: M.E.H. Schouten (Ed.) The Psychophysics of Speech Perception, M.Nijhoff, The Netherlands, 80-90.

Espinoza-Varas, B. (1987). Involvement of the critical band in identification, perceived distance, anddiscrimination of vowels. In: M.E.H. Schouten (Ed.) The Psychophysics of Speech Perception,M. Nijhoff, The Netherlands, 306-313.

Sorkin, R.D., Robinson, D.E., & Berg, B.G. (1987). A detection theory method for the analysis ofvisual and auditory displays, Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Human FactorsSociety, Vol. 2, 1184-1188.

Watson, C.S. (1987). Uncertainty, informational masking and the capacity of immediate auditorymemory. In W.A. Yost and C.S. Watson (Eds.) Auditory Processing of Complex Sounds,Erlbaum Associ2tes, Hillsdale, NJ.

Yost W.A. & Watson C.S. (Eds.) (1987). Auditory Processing of Complex Sounds, ErlbaumAssociates, Hillsdale, NJ.

Kewley-Port, D., Watson, C.S., and Foyle, D. (1988). Auditory temporal acuity in relation tocategory boundaries: Speech and nonspeech stimuli. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 81, 1133-1145.

Kidd, G.R., & Greenwald, A.G. (1988). Attention, rehearsal, and memory for serial patterns.American Journal of Psychology, 101, 259-279.

Leek, M.R. & Watson, C.S. (1988). Auditory perceptual learning of tonal patterns. Perception &Psychophysics, 43 (4), 389-394.

Watson, C.S., and Kewley-Port, D. (1988). Some remarks on Pastore (1988). J. Acoust. Soc. Am.,84, 2266-2270.

Espinoza-Varas, B. and Watson, C.S. (1989). Perception of complex auditory patterns by humans.In S.H. Hulse and P. Dooling (Eds.) The Comparative Psychology of Complex AcousticPerception, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

Fallon, Susan M. (1989). Discriminability of bursts of reproducible noise. Ph.D. Dissertation,Indiana University.

Kewley-Port, D., and Atal, B. (1989). Perceptual differences between vowels located in a limitedphonetic space, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 85, 1726-1740.

Kidd, G. R. (1989) Articulatory-rate context effects in phoneme identification. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 736-748.

-16-

Page 21: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

Manuscripts in Progress

Espinoza-Varas, B. & Watson, C.S. Relations between auditory capabilities and phoneme perception.(in preparation for 1. Acoust. Soc. Am.).

Kewley-Port, D. (1989). Detection thresholds for isolated vowels. 1. Acoust. Soc. Am. (submitted).

Kidd, G.R. & Watson, C.S. Detection of changes in relative- frequency in tonal sequences. (inpreparation for J. Acoust. Soc. Am.).

Kidd, G.R. & Watson, C.S. Perception of multidimensional complex sounds. (in preparation forPerception & Psychophysics).

Watson, C.S., & Kidd, G.R. Discriminability of changes in tonal sequences is limited by theproportion of the total pattern duration that is changed. (in preparation for J. Acoust. Soc.Am.).

Watson, C.S., Foyle, D.C., & Kidd, G.R. Limited processing capacity for auditory patterndiscrimination. (in preparation for J. Acoust. Soc. Am.).

-17-

Page 22: jjT-mitid - DTIC Login · 2011-05-15 · DTIC FILE COPY N Final Technical Report ~UI Air Force Office of Scientific Research N AFOSR-87-0300 I November, 1989 jjT-mitid '.") >:W..:

Oral Presentations

Berg, B.G. & Robinson, D.E. (1987). Multiple observations and internal noise. J. Acoust. Soc.Am., 81, S33

Espinoza-Varas, B., and Watson, C.S. (1987). Informational limits in speech processing by normaland hearing impaired listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 82, S42.

Fallon, S.M. & Robinson, D.E. (1987). The effects of decorrelation on .he discriminability of noisesamples. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 81, S33.

Kidd, G.R., and Watson, C.S. (1987). Perception of multidimensional complex sounds. J. Acoust.Soc. Am., Suppl. 1, 81, S33.

Watson, C.S., Espinoza-Varas, B., and Kyle, M.S. (1987). Rate and number as limiting factors in theperception of sequences of syllables. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 82, S42.

Watson, C.S., and Kidd, G.R. (1987). Proportional target-tone duration as a factor in thediscriminability of tonal patterns. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 82, S40.

Espinoza-Varas, B., and Watson, C.S. (1988). Low communality between tests of auditorydiscrimination and of speech perception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 84, S143.

Kidd, G.R., and Watson, C.S. (1988). Detection of frequency- and time-transposed auditorypatterns. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 84, S141.

Fallon, S.M., & Robinson, D.E. (1989). Effects of a silent temporal interval on discriminability ofbursts of reproducible noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 86, S122.

Kewley-Port, D. (1989). Detection thresholds for isolated vowels. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 85,S51.

Kidd, G. R., & Watson, C. S. (1989) Detection of relative-frequency changes in tonal sequences. J.Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 86, S121.

Rickert, M.E., & Robinson, D.E. (1989). Effects of temporal position on the detectability ofinteraurally uncorrelated noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 86, S12.

Watson, C.S. (1989). A performance-oriented approach to the hearing of complex sounds:Nonspeech and speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 86, Sill.

Watson, C. S., & Kidd, G. R. (1989) Proportional target-tone duration as a limit on patterndiscriminability: multi-component targets. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Suppl 1, 86, S23.

-18-