Japanese Wives

download Japanese Wives

of 18

Transcript of Japanese Wives

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    1/18

     Psychological Consequences of Occupational Conditions Among Japanese Wives

    Author(s): Michiko Naoi and Carmi Schooler

    Source: Social Psychology Quarterly , Vol. 53, No. 2, Special Issue: Social Structure and the

    Individual (Jun., 1990), pp. 100-116

    Published by: American Sociological Association

    Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2786673

    Accessed: 08-04-2016 15:05 UTC

     

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available athttp://about.jstor.org/terms

     

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted

    digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about

    JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    Sage Publications, Inc., American Sociological Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,preserve and extend access to Social Psychology Quarterly 

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    2/18

     Social Psychology Quarterly

     1990, Vol. 53, No. 2, 100-116

     Psychological Consequences of Occupational Conditions

     among Japanese Wives*

     MICHIKO NAOI

     Tokyo Gakugei University

     CARMI SCHOOLER

     National Institute of Mental Health

     This paper examines how Japanese women's occupational conditions affect their

     psychological processes. We find that self-directed work increases their intellectual

     flexibility and the self-directedness of their orientations; this finding replicates earlier

     findings about these important psychological outcomes of self-directed work, even in a

     culture where self-directedness for women is particularly disvalued culturally. Self-directed

     work also leads to less traditional attitudes towards the elderly, whereas working in a

     traditional industry makes such attitudes more traditional. This finding shows that Japanese

     women's work experiences can affect even their acceptance of traditional norms. Our

     evidence also shows that Japanese women are substantially less likely than their husbands to

     do self-directed work on the job. The resultant occupationally induced lessening of

     self-directed orientation may contribute to women s accepting cultural norms that> keep them

     in subservient positions. Thus the culturally and social structurally determined occupational

     experiences of Japanese women clearly affect how they confront major social and personal

     problems.

     This paper examines how Japanese wives'

     occupational conditions affect their psycho-

     logical functioning. In doing so, it asks a

     series of questions about the interrelationship

     of culture, socioeconomic structure, gender,

     and psychological functioning: What are the

     social and psychological factors related to

     Japanese wives' working for pay? Do occupa-

     tional conditions, particularly occupational

     self-direction, have the same effects on

     Japanese women as on other people? Do

     occupational conditions affect traditional val-

     ues about the Japanese wife's role, as

     exemplified by their accepting responsibility

     for caring for elderly parents in the home? If

     occupational conditions have effects on Japa-

     nese wives similar to those found for their

     husbands, do these differences in the nature

     * Please direct all correspondence to Carmi Schooler,

     Laboratory of Socio-Environmental Studies, NIMH,

     Room BIA-14 Federal Building, 7550 Wisconsin Ave.,

     Bethesda MD 20892. This work could not have been

     carried out without Carrie Schoenbach, who was

     indispensable to the project at every stage, from the

     editing of the data through data analysis to the writing of

     the final manuscript. We are also very grateful to Zita

     Givens, who provided valuable assistance at many times;

     Hiroko Hayasi and Kiyoko Okamura of the Tokyo

     Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology, who played

     important roles in collecting and coding the data; Junsuke

     Hara of Yokohama University and Hideo Kojima of

     Ibaragi, who were very helpful in the data collection; and

     Melvin Kohn, who gave us a very helpful critique.

     of husbands' and wives' occupational condi-

     tions contribute to the acceptance of societal

     norms about how women should behave?

     With the exception of wives of small urban

     (shitamachi) shopkeepers, Japanese women

     traditionally have not been employed after

     marriage. In recent years, however, the

     proportion of Japanese wives in the work

     force has increased dramatically (National

     Institute of Employment and Vocational

     Research 1989). We shall examine whether

     there are social and psychological characteris-

     tics that distinguish wives who work for pay

     from those who do not.

     The consequences of the trend toward paid

     employment among Japanese women are

     interesting in themselves for what they tell us

     about the interaction of culture, gender, and

     social structure in Japan. They also provide a

     stringent test of the generalizability of earlier

     findings by affording the opportunity to see

     whether social structurally determined occu-

     pational conditions have the same psycholog-

     ical effects on Japanese women-persons for

     whom paid employment is not traditional-as

     on those for whom such employment is an

     accepted tradition. The particular psychologi-

     cal effects of occupational conditions whose

     generalizability we investigate are those

     which were found among men in the United

     States and were replicated for Polish and

     Japanese men: Substantively complex self-

     100

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    3/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 101

     directed work characteristic of the more

     advantaged social strata leads to intellectual

     flexibility and to a self-directed orientation to

     self and society (Kohn, A. Naoi, Schoen-

     bach, Schooler and Slomczynski 1990), Kohn

     and Schooler 1983; J. Miller, Slomczynski,

     and Kohn 1985; A. Naoi and Schooler 1985;

     Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a; Slomczynski, J.

     Miller, and Kohn 1981).

     It is noteworthy that even among Japanese

     men, whose cultural setting does not particu-

     larly emphasize individual autonomy or

     psychological self-directedness (A. Naoi and

     Schooler 1985, Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a),

     self-directed occupational conditions lead to a

     self-directed orientation to self and society, as

     well as to increased intellectual flexibility. It

     would, however, strengthen substantially the

     case that social structurally determined differ-

     ences in conditions of daily life affect

     psychological functioning, if it were found

     that occupational conditions affect Japanese

     women's psychological functioning similarly,

     despite self-directed values running counter to

     that society's traditional norms for women's

     roles and demeanor.

     Until now, all of the empirical findings

     about the psychological effects of occupa-

     tional conditions and culture in Japan have

     been based on men. In the United States,

     occupational conditions have been shown to

     have the same effects on employed women as

     on employed men. J. Miller, Schooler, Kohn,

     and K. A. Miller (1979) found that occupa-

     tional self-direction increases intellectual flex-

     ibility and self-directed orientations of em-

     ployed women as well as of employed men.'

     1 Moreover, an analysis of the effects of dimensions of

     household work on American women shows that

     substantively complex housework increases the intellec-

     tual flexibility and the self-directedness of orientation

     among American women in the same ways as does

     substantively complex work done for pay (Schooler et al.

     1983; Schooler et al. 1984). In analyzing the data for

     Japanese wives, just as in the case of American women,

     we cannot consider the conditions of housework as

     alternatives to those of paid employment, to be

     substituted into our analyses as conditions of work for

     women who are not employed. Not only are the codes for

     the two types of work conditions different; in addition,

     some of the conditions of paid employment (e.g.,

     bureaucratization) have no analogs in the household

     situation. In addition, employed women also do house-

     work; thus they would have two sets of scores, whereas

     the housewives would have only one. Following the

     general scheme of analysis used with the U.S. data, we

     plan a further paper on the psychological effects of

     housework itself that will include both the employed and

     The question remains whether the similarity

     found in the United States between the effects

     of occupational conditions on men and on

     women also exists in Japan.

     Women's and men's roles overlap less in

     Japanese culture than in American culture.

      Furthermore, the difference between Japa-

     nese and American women's culturally pre-

     scribed roles is even greater than that between

     the roles of Japanese and American men.

     Self-directedness, for example, is valued even

     less for Japanese women than for Japanese

     men (Ackroyd 1959; Lebra 1984; Schooler

     and Smith 1978).

     There is evidence that some social back-

     ground conditions which increase individual-

     istic, self-directed orientations of women in

     the United States (Schooler 1972, 1984) have

     similar effects on Japanese women. Such

     evidence comes from a study of the effects of

     social structure and culture on Japanese

     women's attitudes towards their roles as

     mothers and wives and on their performance

     of those roles (Schooler and Smith 1978;

     Smith and Schooler 1978). Just as in the

     United States, where such conditions lead to

     individualistic values (Schooler 1972, 1984),

     social structural conditions that are linked to

     being raised in a complex environment, such

     as coming from urban settings, having fathers

     with high-status occupations, and being well

     educated, lead Japanese women to emphasize

     the importance of the individual in their views

     of the roles of wife and mother. In terms of

     values for their children, women from such

     backgrounds tend to value behavior reflecting

     self-direction rather than conformity to exter-

     nal standards.

     Despite the cross-national similarities in the

     effects of some socioenvironmental condi-

     tions on women, striking cultural differences

     exist in the way women carry out their roles.

     Husband-wife (Schooler and Smith 1978) and

     mother-child (Smith and Schooler 1978)

     relationships are a case in point. In compari-

     son with women in the United States,

     Japanese women regard the couple relation-

     ship as much less important than the mother-

     child relationship.

     In the present study we test whether social

     structurally determined occupational condi-

     tions have the same psychological effects on

     Japanese women as on other relevant popula-

     the unemployed wives in our sample and will take into

     account the fact of employment.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    4/18

     102 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     tions (i.e., Japanese men and American

     women). More precisely, we test whether

     occupational self-direction increases Japanese

     women's intellectual flexibility and self-

     directedness of orientation, just as it increases

     the levels of these psychological characteris-

     tics for Japanese, Polish, and American men

     and for American women. We also test

     whether oppressive working conditions lead

     to distress. There is suggestive evidence that

     such a relationship exists among American

     women (J. Miller et al., 1979). The evidence

     for men is more substantial; linear structural

     equation analyses of reciprocal effects show

     that oppressive working conditions lead to

     distress and anxiety among American men

     (Kohn and Schooler 1982) and Japanese men

     (A. Naoi and Schooler 1985). In a further test

     of how traditional Japanese culture may affect

     Japanese working women, we examine

     whether the finding of higher levels of

     traditional values among workers in tradi-

     tional (as compared to modem) industries

     holds true for Japanese women, as it does for

     men (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a).

     Our analyses go well beyond replication to

     investigate how the whole range of occupa-

     tional conditions affects an area of traditional

     values whose occupational determinants have

     not been examined previously: attitudes

     towards living with and taking care of one's

     elderly parents and towards living with and

     being cared for by one's children when one is

     older. The care and living arrangements of

     elderly parents are particularly important

     issues to Japanese wives, because the burden

     of both caring for the elderly and of resolving

     intergenerational disputes about household

     matters probably would fall more on them

     than on their husbands. Such burdens may

     weigh particularly heavily on the already

     complicated lives of working wives, for

     whom the value of the household help that

     elderly parents might provide may be less

     than the added work and the potential for

     interpersonal conflict that the presence of

     elders in the household would bring.

     Our hypothesis is that women who work in

     traditional Japanese industries will tend to

     have traditional values, just as they tend to

     develop traditional orientations in other realms

     (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a). Thus women

     working in traditional industries should ex-

     pect to accept the responsibility of caring for

     their parents in their own homes and also

     should expect to be cared for similarly by

     their own children. We also hypothesize that

     occupational conditions which foster a self-

     directed orientation should decrease the like-

     lihood that traditional values will be main-

     tained, and should increase the importance

     that workers attach to autonomy and indepen-

     dence in family relationships. As a result, the

     experience of occupational self-direction

     should reduce the belief that it is appropriate

     to take one's elderly parents into one's own

     home; this experience also should reduce the

     feeling that one will want to live with one's

     own children during one's own old age.

     Finally we will examine the nature of the

     differences between the work done by em-

     ployed Japanese women and by their hus-

     bands. The purpose of this analysis will be

     not so much to document the well-known sex

     discrimination in matters of promotion and

     tenure as to explore the implications of

     perhaps a more subtle form of discrimination:

     namely, the differential effects of Japanese

     men's and women's respective occupational

     conditions on their psychological develop-

     ment.

     SAMPLE

     Data for the analyses reported here were

     gathered from the 246 working (20 hours or

     more per week) wives of the employed men

     interviewed in the A. Naoi and Schooler

     study (1985). The men's sample of 629

     subjects, drawn in 1979-1980 through ran-

     dom probability sampling of employed males

     26 to 65 years old in the Kanto plain of Japan,

     represented a 74.6 percent completion rate of

     the original sample. When the women's

     survey was begun in 1983, 521 of these men

     were married and still lived in the Kanto area

     with their wives. Of the 418 wives who were

     interviewed (80.2%), 246 (59%) were work-

     ing. Although this procedure does not provide

     an absolutely representative sample of all

     Japanese working wives, it corresponds to the

     method used by J. Miller et al. (1979) to

     select a sample of working women in the

     Kohn and Schooler (1983) studies in the

     United States.

     As in this earlier American study, one may

     question the appropriateness of such a sam-

     ple; our findings may not be generalizable to

     unmarried employed Japanese women. Nev-

     ertheless, because the great majority of

     Japanese women marry and stay married, and

     because the age of marriage is remarkably

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    5/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 103

     uniform, our findings probably are applicable

     to nearly all Japanese working women above

     28 years of age.2 In any case, married women

     are an appropriate population on which to test

     the hypothesis that sex-role differences do not

     alter the effects of occupational conditions on

     psychological functioning, because employed

     wives are especially likely to be subject to

     actual and potential conflicts among occupa-

     tional, conjugal, and maternal roles (Miller et

     al. 1979).

     The interview consists primarily of ques-

     tions from the original American survey,

     translated and used for Japanese men by A.

     Naoi and Schooler. (For more detail about the

     procedures see A. Naoi and Schooler 1985.)

     Additional questions were of two types. One

     set centered on housework and child rearing.

     These questions also were based on questions

     from Kohn and Schooler (1983), which were

     translated and pretested for appropriateness in

     Japan. The other set of questions pertained to

     care of the elderly, and were developed by the

     staff of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of

     Gerontology.

     Interviewing was carried out by trained

     undergraduate and graduate students of Tokyo,

     Yokohama, Ibaragi, and Chiba National

     Universities and Tsuda Women's College.

     MEASURES

     Occupational Conditions

     The occupational conditions in the model

     include substantive complexity, closeness of

     supervision, and routinization of work, which

     are constituents of a single concept: occupa-

     tional self-direction, and bureaucratization.

     Information about the occupational conditions

     of earlier jobs was acquired through retrospec-

     tive questioning.

     Substantive complexity of work is defined

     as the degree to which performance of the

     work requires thought and independent judg-

     ment. This index is based on seven measures

     (see Table 1) derived from a detailed inquiry

     2 For Japanese women who married in 1985 the

     average age of marriage was 25.4 years. The average age

     of marriage for our sample is 24; 91 percent of the sample

     was married by age 28. The divorce rate in Japan

     (number of divorces per 1000 people) was 1.39 in 1985.

     Of the 544 men in our original sample who were married

     in 1979-1980, 12 (2%) were divorced by 1983.

     about precisely what people do when working

     with data, with things, and with people.

     Closeness of supervision is a condition that

     limits a worker's occupational self-direction.

     This variable is measured by the worker's

     assessment of how closely she is supervised,

     her freedom to disagree with her supervisor,

     and the extent to which her supervisor gives

     her direct orders. (For earlier jobs, we

     obtained only the last of these measures.)

     Routinization, which limits occupational self-

     direction by restricting possibilities for initia-

     tive, thought, and judgment, is measured by

     the respondent's rating of her job along a

     single dimension that ranges from being

     invariably repetitive to being unpredictable

     and requiring different things to be done in

     different ways.

     We developed the measures of occupa-

     tional self-direction through latent variable

     linear structural equation modeling (Joreskog

     and Sorbom 1976a, 1976b), using the MILS

     program developed by Ronald Schoenberg.

     Because the three principal determinants of

     occupational self-direction-substantive com-

     plexity, closeness of supervision, and routini-

     zation-are theoretically interrelated, they are

     modeled as indicators of a single second-

     order factor, namely occupational self-

     direction.

     The measurement model is presented in

     Table 1. It contains one problematic aspect:

     although closeness of supervision is a power-

     ful indicator of the occupational self-direction

     of the earlier job, it is not a meaningful

     indicator of current occupational self-

     direction. Nevertheless, the model as it stands

     seems a reasonable measure of occupational

     self-direction; the overall fit to the data, based

     on a chi-square per degree of freedom ratio, is

     good (3.38). The values obtained through this

     measurement model were fixed in the subse-

     quent causal analyses.

     The only measure of women's organiza-

     tional position included in our final models is

     the bureaucratization of the firm or organiza-

     tion in which they were employed. This

     position is indexed on the basis of the number

     of formal levels of supervision and the size of

     the organization (see Kohn 1971; Kohn and

     Schooler 1983, Chapter 2).

     Traditionalism of Industry

     An industry's traditionalism is measured on

     a three-point scale developed in conjunction

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    6/18

     104 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     Table 1. Measurement Model of Occupoational Self-Direction

     Standardized path from

     oncept/ndcators concept to ndcators*

     First-Order Concepts:

     1. Substantive complexity of current job

      ours wth things . .............................................................. -.38

     Hurs wthdta . 46

     Hours wth people ............................................................... .36

     Complexity of work with things .................................................... .39

     Complexity of work wth data ............ ......................................... .79

     Complexity of work wth people ........... ........................................ .72

      verall complexity . ............................................................. .98

     2. Closeness of supervision (current job)

     Coseness of supervision .......................................................... .59

      reedomto disagree . ............................................................ -.63

     Boss tells R what to do ........................................................... .69

     3. Routinization (current job)

     Does same thng in same wy ............ ......................................... 100

     4. Substantive complexity of earlier job

     Hours wth things . .............................................................. -.55

    Hours wth data ................................................................. .51

     Hours wth people ............................................................... .32

     Complexity of work with things .................................................... .67

     Complexity of work wth data ............ ......................................... .94

     Complexity of work wth people .......... ......................................... .48

     5. Closeness of supervision (earlier job)

      oss tells R what to do ........................................................... 100

     6. Routinization (earlier job)

     Does same thing in same wy ........... .......................................... 100

     Second-Order Concepts:

     1. Occupational self-direction (current job)

     ubstantive complexity ........................................................... .39

      oseness of supervision .......................................................... .05

     Routinization . .................................................................. -.45

     2. Occupational self-direction (earlier job)

     ubstantive complexity ........................................................... .51

     Coseness of supervision .......................................................... -.59

     Routinization . .................................................................. -.35

     * Chi-square = 459.60, df = 136, ratio = 3.30.

     Note: Correlations of residuals are not shown. Those modeled include matching pairs of indicators for current and

     for earlier jobs as well as some pairs of intratime indicators, chosen on the basis of first-order partial derivatives.

     with Ken'Ichi Tominaga of the University of

     Tokyo (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a).

     Industries included in the Japanese Industrial

     Code are rated as most traditional if they

     manufacture products or provide services that

     existed in Japan during the Tokugawa era,

     before the Meiji restoration in 1868. Indus-

     tries are rated as least traditional if they

     manufacture products or provide services that

     did not exist in the pre-Meiji era. Industries

     are rated as intermediate if they provide

     products and services of both the pre- and

     post-Meiji eras. (The complete index is

     presented in Schooler and A. Naoi 1988b.)

     Psychological Functioning

     Our measures of psychological functioning

     are based on confirmatory factor analytic

     measurement models that parallel the models

     developed by A. Naoi and Schooler (1985)

     for Japanese men (fully described in that

     paper and the appendix available from its

     authors). These psychological variables are

     intellectual flexibility (Appendix IA) and

     seven facets of orientation to self and to

     others: 1) authoritarian conservatism, 2)

     personally responsible standards of morality,

     3) self-confidence, 4) self-deprecation, 5)

     idea-conformity, 6) anxiety, and 7) trust (see

     Appendix IB). (Unlike the measures of job

     conditions, for which we have ratings for both

     current and earlier jobs, we have measures

     only for current psychological functioning.)

     The measure of intellectual flexibility is

     based on four indicators: 1) the respondents'

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    7/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 105

     solutions to a seemingly simple but revealing

     cognitive problem involving a well-known

     issue, 2) their performance on a portion of the

     Embedded Figures Test (Witkin, Dyk, Fater-

     man, Goodenough, and Karp 1962), 3) their

     propensity to agree when asked agree-

     disagree questions, and 4) the impressions

     they made on the interviewers during a long

     session that required a great deal of thought

     and reflection. The Japanese women's model

     fits the data quite well and is presented in

     Appendix IA.

     The seven measures of social orientations

     and self-conceptions are based on a battery of

     57 questions, mainly of the agree-disagree

    and how often type. Principal-components

     factor analysis was initially used to examine

     the factor structure and to establish its general

     similarity to that found for the Japanese men.

     Confirmatory factor analysis was then used to

     develop measures of self-conceptions and

     orientation purged of measurement error. The

     resultant measures fit the data quite well and

     are presented in Appendix IB.

     Attempting to deal with all seven facets of

     self-conception and social orientation is

     unwieldy. Therefore we proceeded to perform

     a second-order confirmatory factor analysis

     based on the hypothesis already confirmed for

     Japanese, Polish, and American men (Kohn

     et al. 1990, Kohn and Schooler 1983) and for

     American women (Schooler, J. Miller, K. A.

     Miller and Richtand 1984). According to this

     hypothesis there are two principal underlying

     dimensions: self-directedness versus confor-

     mity to external authority, and a sense of

     distress versus a sense of well-being. The

     results of this analysis (presented in Appendix

     IC) show that the same two dimensions

     underlie the self-conceptions and social orien-

     tations of Japanese wives. Therefore we used

     these second-order dimensions in the present

     investigation.

     Attitudes towards the Elderly

     These measures are based on a set of 13

     items that literally ask what the respondents

     think about having elderly parents live with

     them, as well as about living with their own

     children when they themselves are older. An

     extensive series of exploratory and confirma-

     tory factor analyses showed that this complex

     of attitudes could best be described in terms

     of four factors (the individual items and the

     final models are presented in Appendix II).

     These four factors are 1) general belief that

     living with elderly parents is good (parents-

     general), 2) belief that one should not live

     with one's parents if this leads to problems

     (parents-problem), 3) willingness to take in

     parents if they are ill (parents-ill), and 4)

     desire to live with one's own children when

     one is older (respondent with children). The

     first three of these factors can be made into a

     second-order factor measuring willingness to

     live with and care for elderly parents; this

     factor fits the data well (see Appendix II).

     WHICH JAPANESE WIVES WORK?

     Before examining how the conditions of

     paid employment affect Japanese women, we

     look to see which Japanese wives go to work.

     We do so by using multiple regression

     analysis to determine which background and

     family conditions distinguish wives in our

     sample who work for pay from those who do

     not. The predictors of paid employment that

     we include in our equation are age less than

     35, age 55 or more (age 35-54 is the omitted

     category), respondent's education, urbanness

     of background, father's education, father's

     occupational status, traditionalism of hus-

     band's industry, peripherality of the sector of

     the economy in which the husband works

     (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988a), husband's

     income from job, and husband's educational

     and occupational status. Of these variables,

     only age 55 or more (Beta= -.24), age less

     than 35 (Beta= - 15), urban background

     (Beta = - .16), husband's education (Beta =

     - . 13), and peripherality of husband's sector

     of the economy (Beta= .23) have significant

     independent effects on whether women work

     for pay. Women who work tend to be in their

     middle years, to come from rural settings, and

     to have husbands who are less well educated

     and who work in the economic periphery.

     Thus the wives of classic salary men would

     not seem to be among those most likely to

     work.

     Further analyses revealed no significant

     independent relationships between the wives'

     working and the psychological variables we

     examine in this paper. When these variables

     are included together with the social variables

     that we found to be significant predictors of

     working, none of the psychological variables

     had a significant Beta weight. Thus the

     probability of a wife's working is affected

     neither by her psychological functioning, nor

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    8/18

     106 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     her educational attainment or her husband's

     income level. Instead, the primary determi-

     nants of whether a Japanese wife works for

     pay are her stage of life, whether she comes

     from a rural background, and the peripheral-

     ity of her husband's occupation in the

     Japanese economic structure.

     MODELING THE PSYCHOLOGICAL

     EFFECTS OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS

     AND PSYCHOLOGICAL FUNCTIONING

     Any causal model that includes an exami-

     nation of the psychological effects of occupa-

     tional conditions must take into account the

     possibility of reciprocal effects of various job

     conditions and psychological functioning. We

     cannot rule out on a priori grounds the

     possibility that an individual's personality

     affects her job through processes such as

     occupational selection or job molding. Fur-

     thermore, there is empirical evidence that in

     both the United States and Japan, not only do

     job conditions affect personality; personality

     also affects job conditions (Kohn and Schooler

     1982, 1983, Chapter 6; A. Naoi and Schooler

     1985; Schooler and A. Naoi 1988b). There-

     fore, we should test our hypotheses with

     models that include the reciprocal effects of

     occupational conditions and psychological

     functioning.

     We estimate these models involving recip-

     rocal causation through latent variable linear

     structural equation modeling (Joreskog and

     Sorbom 1976a, 1976b), using the MILS

     program developed by Ronald Schoenberg.

     The general model for the analyses is

     presented in Figure 1. To gain instrumenta-

     tion for the path from psychological function-

     ing to the occupational self-directedness of

     the present job, we postulate that characteris-

     tics of the individual's family of origin affect

     directly the individual's present psychological

     functioning and the occupational characteris-

     tics of her earlier job, but not of her present

     job. Such a restriction is consonant with

     previous research findings, which show that

     in Japan the direct effects of social back-

     ground indeed are limited to the first job (A.

     Naoi 1980). We obtain instrumentation for

     the reciprocal path from occupational self-

     directedness of the present job to psychologi-

     cal functioning by postulating that early

     occupational self-direction directly affects

     CURRENT

     BACKGROUND EARLER JOB PSYCHOLOGCAL

     CHARACTERISTICS JOB CONDITIONS CONDITIONS FUNCTIONING

     oc c n3-=UES

     oa Bkgounh

      L = ~ ~ ~ 1 I D >

     L_L4 L4

     , ofChdreninC/X

      ~~ .2

     >) ism off//) ismf.

     Social Backgroundc

     -U

     Occupational Occupationial c

     Sef-Sef-

     Educationdrectiondrection0

     C D

    -n

     C-

    Urban Childhood Bureaucracy Bureaucracy -l

     of Children in

     Family of Origin Traditional- Traditional-

    smofsmofL

     Industry Industry

     A ge

     Figure 1. Reciprocal Effects Model

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    9/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 107

     later occupational self-direction but not later

     psychological functioning.

     The background characteristics in the

     analyses include mother's education, father's

     education, and father's occupational status;

     because of their very high intercorrelations,

     these three items are modeled as a single

     construct, namely parental background. The

     other characteristics are respondent's age,

     education, urban background, and number of

     children in family of origin. Age is postulated

     to affect psychological functioning and condi-

     tions of both current and earlier job. All other

     conditions are postulated to affect directly

     current psychological functioning and the

     conditions of the earlier job, but to affect the

     conditions of the present job only indirectly.

     The occupational conditions in the model

     are occupational self-direction, bureaucratiza-

     tion, and traditionality of the occupational

     setting of the present and the earlier job. The

     occupational conditions of the current job, as

     well as the traditionality of the industry of the

     current job, are modeled as affecting psycho-

     logical functioning contemporaneously, but

     psychological functioning is modeled as

     affecting only occupational self-direction.

     This latter limitation occurs because by

     definition a change in job setting would mean

     a change in job (Kohn and Schooler 1982). In

     the modeling of present and of past job

     conditions, however, both bureaucratization

     and traditionality of industry affect occupa-

     tional self-direction.3

     RESULTS

     What do our models reveal about the

     3With certain exceptions, these models are generally

     similar to those used by Schooler and A. Naoi in their

     examination of the psychological effects of occupational

     conditions and job settings on Japanese men (1988a,

     1988b). For women, data were available on two

     jobs-the present and the previous jobs-whereas the

     Schooler and A. Naoi model for men includes data on

     three jobs-present job, job held 10 years ago, and first

     job. Four other variables that were included in the

     Schooler and A. Naoi model of Japanese men could not

     be included in the model of Japanese women. Data on

     sector of the economy were not available for the women's

     jobs; ownership and hierarchical position could not be

     included because too few Japanese women were owners

     or held supervisory positions to provide a statistical

     distribution that could be modeled appropriately. Finally,

     we omitted time pressure from the women's models.

     Although we tried various alternative models, we could

     not include time pressure successfully in the women's

     models of reciprocal effects, possibly because we lacked

     the extra identification provided in the men's model by

     information about a third job.

     effects of social structurally determined envi-

     ronmental conditions on Japanese working

     women? We begin by describing how such

     conditions affect three important aspects of

     their psychological functioning: intellectual

     flexibility, self-directedness of orientation,

     and level of distress. Then we examine how

     those conditions affect the traditionalism of

     their attitudes towards caring for elderly

     parents and towards living with their children

     in their own old age.

     Intellectual Flexibility

     The first thing we note when we examine

     our model of the reciprocal effects of

     occupational self-direction and intellectual

     flexibility (see Table 2A), is that, while the

     path from occupational self-direction to intel-

     lectual flexibility is positive (.45), as pre-

     dicted, and significant (t = 3.66), the recipro-

     cal path from intellectual flexibility to

     occupational self-direction is negative (-.20)

     and not significant (t = 0.94). Such a pattern

     is strongly indicative of multicollinearity

     (Farrar and Glauber 1967; Gordon 1968;

     Kohn and Schooler 1978, 1983).

     Since multicollinearity may inflate path

     coefficients artifactually, a more conservative

     assessment of the effect of occupational

     self-direction on intellectual flexibility may

     be obtained by fixing at zero the statistically

     nonsignificant contemporaneous path from

     intellectual flexibility to occupational self-

     direction.4 In such a nonreciprocal model, the

     path from occupational self-direction to intel-

     lectual flexibility drops to .40 but remains

     highly significant (t = 3.45). The other

     significant path to intellectual flexibility in

     this model shows that higher educational level

     increases intellectual flexibility directly (.27).

     Whereas education increases intellectual flex-

     ibility directly, other background characteris-

     tics, such as parental socioeconomic back-

     ground, may influence intellectual flexibility

     indirectly by affecting education and occupa-

     tional self-direction. Nevertheless, among the

     potentially relevant variables we have mod-

     eled, occupational self-direction has by far

     the greatest direct effect on Japanese wom-

     en's intellectual flexibility.

     4 On this and all subsequent occasions, when we omit

     a reciprocal path, the path that is omitted in our revised

     models will be nonsignificant and the estimate of the

     other will be exaggerated.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    10/18

     108 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     Table 2. One-way and Reciprocal Effects of Occupational Self-Direction and Psychological Functioning

     Standardized Path Coefficients

     Reciprocal Modeling Nonreciprocal Modeling

     Occupational Psychological Occupational

     Self-Direction Functioning Self-Direction Distress

     t t t t

    Psychological Occupational Psychological Occupational

     Functioning Self-Direction Functioning Self-Direction

     A. Psychological Functioning

     1 Ideational fexibility.45* -.20.40*

     2. Self-directedness of orientation .44* -.18 .32*

     3 Dstress-2035 24

    B. Attitudes toward Parents and

     Children Living Together

     1 Respondent andparents together-29*

     2 Respondent wth children-.22 t= 184)

     * =p

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    11/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 109

     The one occupational condition that signif-

     icantly affects Japanese working wives' sense

     of distress is working in a bureaucratic

     setting. The path from bureaucracy to distress

     is .16 (t = 2.41). This finding-that Japanese

     women who work in a bureaucratic setting

     tend to become more distressed than other

     women-is contrary to earlier findings for

     Japanese men. Japanese men who work in

     bureaucracies are more self-confident and less

     anxious than those who do not (A. Naoi and

     Schooler 1985; Schooler and A. Naoi 1988b);

     both of these psychological characteristics

     indicate an absence of distress. This differ-

     ence between the sexes may well reflect the

     different positions they hold in bureaucratic

     Japanese businesses. In such businesses,

     women generally are not fully accepted;

     rather they are seen as short-term employees

     who are ineligible to embark on the career

     promotion ladders available to men or to

     receive many of the benefits that the compa-

     nies offer.

     Being young is the final significant deter-

     minant of distress in our model. The path

     from age to distress is - .19 (t= 2.89). This

     finding for Japanese working wives is similar

     to that for their husbands, among whom the

     younger also are more anxious than the older

     (A. Naoi and Schooler 1985). Finally,

     Japanese wives who hold traditional jobs tend

     to feel less distressed than other women

     (-.12; t= 1.81 p< .075), although this

     finding does not reach the .05 level of

     significance when a two-tailed probability test

     is used. This result is consonant with the

     significant effect of traditional jobs on

     distress among their husbands (Schooler and

     A. Naoi 1988b). Such findings accord with

     both Durkheim's and Marx's beliefs that

     modem industry leads to anomie and alien-

     ation.

     Traditional Values towards the Elderly

     It seems unlikely that a woman's attitudes

     towards responsibilities for the older genera-

     tion would affect her level of occupational

     self-direction, once she has entered the work

     force. Thus we assumed that the causal

     direction would be from occupational self-

     direction to values towards the elderly.

     Accordingly we modified the model presented

     in Figure 1 by eliminating the reciprocal path

     from the psychological variable to occupa-

     tional self-direction.

     When we test such models (see Table 2B),

     the path from occupational self-direction to

     our second-order factor measuring willing-

     ness to care for elderly parents in one's home

     is significant; the path from occupational

     self-direction to wanting to live with one's

     children when one is older is negative and

     very close to significant (t = 1.84). Thus we

     confirm our hypothesis that the self-directed

     orientation resulting from doing self-directed

     work would decrease people's willingness to

     tolerate the constraints involved in living with

     the older generation.

     We also find support for our hypothesis

     that working in a traditional industry in-

     creases the traditionalism of family attitudes.

     Although traditionalism of industry is not

     related significantly to the second-order

     measure of traditionalism of attitude towards

     care of elderly parents, it is related signifi-

     cantly to two of the component factors.

     Working in a traditional industry increases

     Japanese women's willingness to take elderly

     parents into their homes, both if the parents

     were sick (.15) and if doing so would cause

     problems (.17). In addition, women working

     in traditional industries are significantly more

     likely than those working in nontraditional

     industries to want to live with their own

     children when they themselves are elderly

     (.15). These findings-that Japanese women

     working in traditional industries retain tradi-

     tional attitudes towards the elderly-are

     congruent with earlier findings for Japanese

     men (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988b). Those

     earlier findings show that Japanese men who

     work in traditional industries tend to be

     traditional in their orientations and values.

     When we look at the effects of the other

     variables in our model, we see that coming

     from a high-social-status background de-

     creases directly the acceptability of older and

     younger generations of adults living together.

     This finding is reflected both in a decreased

     willingness to take elderly parents into one's

     home, as measured by the second-order factor

     (- .19), and in a decreased desire to live with

     one's own children when one is older

     (respondents with children= -.19).

     Working in a bureaucracy is another

     occupational variable that affects attitudes

     towards the elderly. Such a work setting leads

     to less willingness to take elderly parents into

     one's home (- .34) and to less desire to live

     with one's own children during one's own old

     age (- .16). One possible explanation is that

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    12/18

     10 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     bureaucracies may be less likely to permit the

     flexible work arrangements that sometimes

     are needed to care for the elderly at home.

     The findings also can be explained by the

     hypothesis that working in a bureaucratic

     setting leads to a more general bureaucratic

     view of life, in which problems are solved

     through bureaucratic rather than personal

     arrangements. If this is so, working in a

     bureaucracy should lead individuals to believe

     that impersonal agencies rather than families

     should bear the responsibility for care of the

     aged.

     Another finding seems more puzzling:

     Women from large families are less likely to

     want to live with their children during their

     own old age than are those from smaller

     families (-.12). One possible explanation is

     that such women are tired of the interpersonal

     complexities that can arise in large house-

     holds and thus would rather stay by them-

     selves than become involved again in their old

     age in potentially complicated family situa-

     tions.

     In our evaluation of the meaning of these

     results, the type of analysis employed (see

     Figure 1) searches for and controls statisti-

     cally the effects of several other potentially

     relevant background variables. Thus although

     respondent's age, education, and urban back-

     ground have no significant direct effects on

     attitudes towards the elderly, these variables

     are controlled statistically, so that the findings

     we report take their effects into account.

     Therefore the differences in traditionality of

     attitudes between respondents working in

     self-directed and in nonself-directed jobs, or

     between respondents working in nontradi-

     tional and in traditional industries, are not due

     to differences in characteristics such as the

     age of these different groups.

     Husband's birth rank is another possible

     cause of differences in attitudes towards

     caring for elderly parents in one's home.

     According to the traditional Japanese ie

     family system, the oldest son not only inherits

     the major share of his parents' wealth but also

     the responsibility of caring for them in their

     old age. Consequently it might be expected

     that women who are married to first-born sons

     would feel a special obligation to care for

     their husbands' parents.

     There is some evidence that in the past,

     husband's birth rank may have affected the

     likelihood of living with the husband's

     parents, but that the pattern has changed. M.

     Naoi, Okamura and Hayashi (1984), in

     examining the actual patterns in which elderly

     parents lived with their children, found more

     women whose husbands were first-born

     among respondents whose parents had lived

     with them but then had died. Among those

     living with their elderly parents at the time of

     the interview in 1983, however, husband's

     birth rank did not affect the likelihood of such

     living arrangements. In this group the major

     demographic predictor of living with one's

     parents is whether the parents own a home

     that provides comfortable accommodation for

     the children.

     We also modified the present analyses to

     check whether the husband's birth rank

     affects the wife's attitude about caring for

     elderly parents. We did so by adding to the

     model presented in Figure 1 a new variable-

     whether the husband was first-born-and

     estimating the path from that variable to

     traditional attitudes towards living with the

     elderly. Both for attitudes about living with

     elderly parents or about living with ones

     children, such a path, which serves as a

     control for the effects of husband s birth rank,

     was not significant and did not affect the

     other values in the model.5

     DIFFERENCES IN OCCUPATIONAL

     CONDITIONS

     Having shown that occupational conditions

     affect the psychological functioning of Japa-

     nese wives in ways not too dissimilar to those

     in which their husbands are affected, we now

     examine the differences between the occupa-

     tional conditions to which working wives and

     their husbands are exposed to learn whether

     such differences in themselves may have

     sociocultural consequences. Table 3 presents

     a comparison of Japanese working wives and

     their husbands on the mean levels of various

     occupational dimensions included in our

     models. Because there can be some difficul-

     ties in comparing mean levels of multiple-

     indicator concepts when the item loadings on

     the factors are somewhat different in the two

     populations involved, we compare directly

     S We also tried to develop confirmatory factor analysis

     models based on the possibility that attitudes towards

     one's husband's parents were different from attitudes

     towards one's own parents. After a long series of

     attempts, however, we were forced to conclude that such

     a hypothesis was not supported by any well-fitting

     confirmatory models.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    13/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 111

     Table 3. Means and Differences of Means for Husbands' and Wives' Work Conditions

      eans Difference

     ofSignfcance

     Wves Husbands Means t-Value Level

     A. Complexity of Work

     1 Comlexityofdata 256 309 -053 424 .0001

     2 Comlexityofthings 358 490- 132 9.07 .0001

     3 Comlexityofpeople 333 426 -093 525 .001

     4 Overall comlexity268 456 -187 2063 .0001

     B. Closeness of Supervision

     1. Not free to disagree wth boss 2.51 2.13 0.39 2.22 .029

     2 Closelysupervised276 236 040252 .013

     3 Boss tells what todo192 167 024 209 .040

     C. Routinization

     Nonvariable, easily predictible work 4.66 4.31 0.35 4.99 .0001

     D. Hierarchical Position

     Numerofsubordnates 113 236 - 124 1109 0001

     E. Working in Bureaucracy

     Bureaucracy index (including owners) 1.91 2.31 -0.40 3.50 .001

     F. Pressures

     1. Held responsible for things outside

    ones contro 184 223 -039 514 0001

     2 Frequencyoftim pressure 324 355 -031 287 004

    3 Drtiness 194 223 -029 475 001

     4. Riskofloss ofjob 1.41 1.32 009 1.01 ns

     G Incom 1 = 10000Yen348 8.17 -467 1456 .0001

     Note: Work conditions compared here are those at current job.

     the mean levels of the individual items for

     husbands and for wives. When we examine

     such measures of the conditions of the current

     job, we find that in every instance women's

     conditions are less favorable than men's.

     Thus in terms of the components of occupa-

     tional self-direction, Japanese women score

     significantly lower than their husbands on

     complexity of work with data, things, and

     people as well as on overall complexity.

     Japanese wives' work also is significantly

     more routinized than their husbands', and

     they are significantly more likely to be

     supervised on each of our three indices of that

     concept. In addition, they are less likely than

     their husbands to work in large bureaucratic

     organizations, the generally preferred work

     setting in Japan.6

     When we look at the psychological effects

     6 Hours of work with things, data, and people also are

     part of the model of occupational self-direction; hours of

     work with data and with people are positive indicators of

     occupational self-direction, and number of hours of work

     with things is negative. The Japanese men work longer in

     each of these three types of work than do their wives, a

     difference reflecting the men's generally longer hours of

     employment. This difference in hours of work also may

     account for part of the sex difference in pay (unfortu-

     nately, we do not have directly comparable measures of

     the total number of hours a week worked by the two

     sexes). The men earn 2.35 times as much as the women

     for perhaps one and one-half times as many hours.

     of such work conditions on women, we find

     that working in a bureaucracy has a moderate

     negative effect on intellectual flexibility, but

     that self-directed work has a strong positive

     effect (.41). Thus the nature of their work

     puts Japanese women in a less advantaged

     position than that of their husbands in terms

     of developing such flexibility. Working in a

     bureaucracy has a nonsignificant effect on

     self-directed orientations, but occupational

     self-direction has a strong positive effect

     (.44). The fact that Japanese women are less

     self-directed at work than are their husbands

     means that they are less likely to develop

     self-directed orientations from their work

     experience. Thus we see the possibility of a

     feedback loop between cultural expectations

     and the development of gender differences in

     self-directed orientations. Japanese cultural

     norms suggest that even if women work, they

     should be in generally subservient, nonself-

     directed positions. Occupying such positions

     may serve to reduce the self-directedness of

     their orientations in comparison to those of

     their husbands; thus they become even more

     amenable to the culture's norms.

     DISCUSSION

     In this paper we have examined the effects

     of occupational conditions and job settings on

     the psychological functioning of Japanese

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    14/18

     12 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     women. We have done so partially in the

     belief that replication among Japanese women

     of the findings for Japanese men-that the

     experience of occupational self-direction in-

     creases both intellectual flexibility and self-

     directedness of orientation-would add sub-

     stantially to the robustness and theoretical

     implications of those earlier findings. Such a

     replication obviously also increases the gener-

     alizability of similar findings for Polish men

     and for American men and women. Of at

     least equal importance is the evidence that our

     results provide about the intricacies of the

     causal interconnections among the psycholog-

     ical, social structural, and cultural levels of

     phenomena in which Japanese working wives

     are enmeshed.

     This web of interconnections extends to the

     very question of which Japanese wives work.

     Our findings suggest that whether a Japanese

     wife goes to work is determined primarily not

     by her social background, her educational

     attainment, her husband's income, or even

     her psychological orientation, but rather by

     her stage in the life course, whether she

     comes from a rural background, and the

     degree to which her husband works in the

     periphery of the economy. Socioeconomic

     incentive to work is determined not by the

     husband's earning power, but by financial

     uncertainty and possibly by the actual need

     for the wife's labor in small business and

     rural settings. Who works and who does not

     work seems to be determined strongly by

     cultural expectations about what women at

     certain stages of their lives and in certain

     positions in the socio-economic system are

     supposed to do.

     Among women who work, our analyses

     show that self-directed work in fact increases

     intellectual flexibility and self-directedness of

     orientation. Thus in a society in which

     self-directedness generally is not valued, even

     those for whom self-directedness is particu-

     larly disvalued provide evidence the Kohn

     and Schooler (1983) hypotheses about the

     ways in which the psychological effects of

     self-directed work generalize beyond the

     workplace. In terms of orientation towards

     Japanese cultural norms, our findings support

     the hypothesis that the experience of occupa-

     tional self-direction increases the importance

     that Japanese wives place on being self-

     directed and independent, while reducing

     their adherence to traditional values that

     emphasize the importance of family obliga-

     tions over individual autonomy.

     We also sought to examine whether

     findings about the effects on Japanese men of

     working in a traditional Japanese industry

     (Schooler and A. Naoi 1988b) also hold true

     for Japanese women. Here the replication was

     only partial. We expected on the basis of both

     Marx's and Durkheim's reasoning that Japa-

     nese women working in traditional industries

     would be more psychologically comfortable

     and less distressed than those working in

     modem industries, as is the case for Japanese

     men (A. Naoi and Schooler 1985). For their

     wives, the same trend just misses reaching the

     accepted standard for level of statistical

     significance. Not even marginally replicated

     among the women is the finding for Japanese

     men that working in a traditional industry

     leads to traditional, nonself-directed psycho-

     logical orientations.

     On the other hand, we found evidence in

     data that were not available for Japanese

     men-traditionality of attitudes towards the

     care of the elderly-that among Japanese

     women, working in a traditional industry

     results in traditional ways of thought. Even

     when the effects of all other variables in our

     model are controlled statistically, women

     working in traditional industries have a more

     traditional approach to assuming responsibil-

     ity for the care of the older generation than do

     those working in modem industries. Another

     occupational variable that significantly affects

     attitudes towards the elderly is working in a

     bureaucracy. Here too we see some evidence

     of modernization. Working in a bureau-

     cratic setting seems to lead to the belief that

     caring for the elderly should be done through

     bureaucratic means rather than personally by

     family members.

     Most important, in terms of the effects of

     occupational conditions on attitudes towards

     the elderly, we were able to confirm our

     hypothesis that occupational self-direction

     would lead to less traditional attitudes. Our

     findings demonstrate that women who are

     more self-directed in their jobs are signifi-

     cantly less likely to consider caring for elderly

     parents. We also find that coming from a

     high-status family background makes women

     less willing to take their elderly parents into

     their homes and also leads them to reject the

     idea of living with their children in their own

     old age. Coming from a high-status social

     background previously was linked empirically

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    15/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 113

     to a nontraditional, individualistic orientation

     among Japanese women (Schooler and Smith

     1978; Smith and Schooler 1978). These

     findings have several explanations, not neces-

     sarily incompatible. One plausible explana-

     tion hinges on the fact that being raised in a

     high-status family is linked to experiencing

     high levels of environmental complexity

     during childhood. Such exposure seems to

     result in a self-directed orientation among

     people in general (Schooler 1972, 1984) and

     among Japanese women in particular (Schooler

     and Smith 1978). As a consequence, such

     childhood experience may lead (as does

     self-directed work) to the rejection of tradi-

     tional values which emphasize obligations to

     others, including the family, at the expense of

     the satisfaction of the individual. Another

     possibility is that the greater resources

     available to higher-status families make

     alternative arrangements for the care of the

     elderly more viable.

     Finally, let us note an unexpected but

     probably not artifactual finding: among our

     sample of Japanese working wives, distressed

     women are more likely to seek out self-

     directed jobs or to do their jobs in self-

     directed ways than are those who are not

     distressed. The reasons underlying this pat-

     tern of behavior are obscure. As we have

     noted, women with self-directed orientations

     are not particularly more likely than other

     women to seek self-directed work. Neverthe-

     less, women who are distressed because they

     are not in tune with Japan's cultural climate

     may be more likely to go against cultural

     norms by seeking self-directed work.

     Just as the effect of a psychological

     variable such as distress on an occupational

     condition such as self-direction takes place in

     the cultural context of Japan, the effects of all

     occupational conditions on psychological func-

     tioning do not occur in a social vacuum. As

     we have seen, the differences between

     Japanese women's occupational conditions

     and those of their husbands may well

     contribute to the continuation of cultural

     norms about differences in sex roles. Paid

     employment of Japanese women is signifi-

     cantly less self-directed than that of Japanese

     men. Less self-directed work leads to less

     intellectual flexibility and to less self-directed

     orientations. It may well be that this occupa-

     tionally induced lessening of self-directed

     orientation contributes to Japanese women's

     apparent acceptance of cultural norms that

     keep them in subservient positions.

     Taken together, our results portray a

     complex pattern of interrelationships among

     cultural, socioeconomic, and psychological

     levels of phenomena. The likelihood of a

     Japanese wife's working seems to be affected

     strongly by cultural norms about what is

     appropriate for a woman in her stage of the

     life course, whose family is in the position

     that hers occupies, in terms of its centrality to

     the Japanese economic structure. If and when

     a Japanese wife works, the occupational

     conditions that she experiences-particularly

     the level of occupational self-direction-

     affect her psychological functioning and

     social attitudes in ways generally similar to

     those in which similar conditions affect her

     husband. Differences between spouses in the

     nature of their occupational conditions-

     particularly the lower level of occupational

     self-direction experienced by women-may

     contribute to the perpetuation of cultural

     norms that limit the opportunities open to

     Japanese women.

     Appendix IA.

     Measurement Model of Intellectual Flexibility

     Standardized

     path from

     concept to

    Concept/Indicators indicators:*

     Intellectual Flexibility (chi-square = .74,

     df= 1, ratio=.74)

     Reasons for/against cigarette ads ......... .23

     Embedded Figures Test ........ ........ .64

     Propensity to agree .................... -.51

     Interviewer's estimate of R's intelligence. . .45

     Notes:

     1. The model has one correlation of residuals, that

     between estimate of intelligence and cigarette ads

     (.12).

     2. Intellectual flexibility corresponds to the ideational

     component of a two-factor model for U.S. men (the

     other component is perceptual), which was repli-

     cated for Polish and Japanese men and for U.S.

     women. Because in all those populations the

     ideational component was the only component

     whose causal relationships with occupational condi-

     tions could be modeled successfully, we replicate

     only that factor for Japanese women.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    16/18

     114 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     Appendix IB.

     1st-Order Measurement Models of Self-Conception and Social Orientation

     Standardized path from

     oncept/ndcators concept to indcator

     1. Authoritarian conservatism

     (chi-square = 24.78, df= 22, ratio = 1.13)

     The most important thing to teach children is absolute obedience to their parents ......... .64

     People who question the old and accepted ways of doing things usually just end up causing

     troube ................................................................... .45

     In this complicated world, the only way to know what to do is to rely on leaders and experts ....... 47 ...

     No decent man can respect a woman who has had sex relations before marriage ......... .33

     Prison is too good for sex criminals; they should be publicly whipped or worse .......... .30

     Any good leader should be strict with people under him in order to gain their respect ..... .58

     It's wrong to do things differently from the way our forefathers did ..... .............. .55

     One should always show respect to those in authority ............................... .58

     2. Personally responsible criteria of morality

     (chi-square= 1.39, df= 1, ratio= 1.39)

     It's all right to do anything you want as long as you stay out of trouble ..... ........... -.55

     If something works, it doesn't matter whether it's right or wrong ..... ................ -.62

     It's all right to get around the law as long as you don't actually break it ................ -.64

     Do you believe that it's all right to do whatever the law allows or are there some things

     that are wrong even if they are legal? ........ ................................. -.33

     Self-esteem two-factor model

     (chi-square = 19.74, df = 17, ratio = 1.16)

     3. Self-confidence:

     I take a positive attitude toward myself ........................................... .54

     I feel that I'm a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others ..... ........... .71

     I am able to do most things as well as other people can .58

     I generally have confidence that when I make plans I will be able to carry them out ...... .34

     4. Self-deprecation:

      t times I thnk I amno good at all .............. ............................... .56

      feel useless at times ......................................................... .54

     There are very few things about which I'm absolutely certain ..... ................... .31

     (Correlation: self-confidence/self-deprecation) (0.56)

     5. Idea-conformity

     (chi-square= 1.85, df= 1, ratio = 1.85)

     According to your general impression, how often do your ideas and opinions about impor-

     tant matters differ from those of your relatives? .................................. -.62

     How often do your ideas and opinions differ from those of your friends? ..... .......... -.67

     How about from those of other people with your religious background? ..... ........... -.49

     Those of most people in the country? ............. ............................... -.43

     6. Anxiety (chi-square = 56.25, df= 37, ratio = 1.52)

     How often do you feel that you are about to go to pieces? ....... .................... .64

     How often do you feel downcast and dejected? ......... ........................... .55

     How often do you find yourself anxious and worrying about something? ..... .......... .58

     How often do you feel uneasy about something without knowing why? ..... ........... .67

     How often do you feel so restless that you cannot sit still? ........................... .35

     How often to you find that you can't get rid of some thought or idea that keeps running

    through your mnd? ......................................................... .51

     How often do you feel bored with everything? ..................................... .68

     How often do you feel powerless to get what you want out of life? .................... .50

     How often do you feel that the world just isn't very understandable? ..... ............. .34

     How often do you feel that there isn't much purpose to being alive? ................... .52

    t

     Notes:

     1. A high score on the indicator generally implies agreement or frequent occurrence; where alternatives are posed,

     the first alternative is scored high.

     2. In several of the models, some error correlations are allowed, which are not shown in the table.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    17/18

     PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF OCCUPATIONAL CONDITIONS 115

     Appendix IC.

     Second-Order Measurement Model of Self-Conception and Social Orientation

     Standardized path from

     oncept/ndcators concept to indcator

     Second-order two-factor model (chi-square = 8.23, df= 6, ratio = 1.37)

     Self-directedness (vs. conformity)

    Authoritarian conservatism ...................................................... -.48

      ersonally responsible criteria of morality ........ ................................. .96

     Trustfuness .................................................................. .16

     Self-deprecation . .............................................................. -.17

     Distress (vs. sense of well-being)

    Trustfulness ................................................................... -.25

     Self-confidence . .............................................................. -.21

     elf-deprecation . .............................................................. .42

     Anxiety..................................................................... .96

     Idea-conformty ............................................................... .35

     Correation sef-drectedorentation/dstress) -11)

     Notes:

     1. A high score on the indicator generally implies agreement or frequent occurrence; where alternatives are posed,

     the first alternative is scored high.

     2. In several of the models, some error correlations are allowed, which are not shown in the table.

     3. Because the measurement model of trustfulness, shows that this factor can be measured by a single indicator we

     use that indicator alone as our index of trust in this second-order model: Do you think most people can be trusted?

     Appendix II.

     First-Order Measurement Models of Family Traditionalism

     Standardized path from

     oncept/ndcators concept to ndcators*

     1. Parents-General: Thinks that it would be a good thing to do to live with parents:

     If both of wife's parents were alive ................................................. .55

     f one of wfes parents were alive ......... ........................................ .74

     If both of husband's parents were alive .............................................. .43

     f one of husbands parents were alive ........ ...................................... .66

     (Indicators coded: 1 = parents live separately; 2 = parents live with other child; 3 = parents live with

     us.)

     2. Parents-Problems: Thinks one should live with parents even if:

      der parents were jealous ............ ............................................ .70

     There is trouble between couple about parents ........................................ .71

     Older parents spoil the grandchildren ......... ...................................... .59

      der parents are incontinent ........... ........................................... .38

     (Indicators coded: 1 = completely disagree; 2 = somewhat disagree; 3 = cannot choose; 4 = somewhat

     agree; 5 = completely agree.)

     3. Parents-Ill: Thinks that it is a good thing to do to live with parents:

     If only one of wife's parents is alive and is ill ........................................ .63

     If only one of husband's parents is alive and ill ....................................... .55

     (Indicators coded: 1 = parents live separately; 2 = parents live with other child; 3 = parents live with

     us.)

     4. Respondent with Children: When you are older would you want to live with your own chil-

     dren if:

     Both husband and wife were alive .................................................. .41

      ny one were alive . ............................................................ .96

     Only one were alive and were ill ................................................... .50

     (Indicators coded: First Question: 1 = live separately; 2= cannot choose; 3= live together; Second

     and Third Question s: 1 = live separately; 2 = live with other child; 3 = live with us.)

     * chi-square = 317.01, df= 56, ratio = 5.66, adjusted goodness-of-fit index = .999.

     Note: Correlations of residuals are not shown in the table.

    This content downloaded from 92.87.204.254 on Fri, 08 Apr 2016 15:05:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 Japanese Wives

    18/18

     116 SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY

     REFERENCES

     Ackroyd, Joyce. 1959. Women in Feudal Japan. Pp.

     31-68 in Transactions of the Asiatic Society of Japan,

     3d series. Tokyo: Asiatic Society of Japan.

     Farrar, Donald E. and Robert R. Glauber. 1967.

      Multicollinearity in Regression Analysis: The Prob-

     lem Revisited. Review of Economics and Statistics

     49:92-107.

     Gordon, Robert A. 1968. Issues in Multiple Regres-

     sion. American Journal of Sociology 73:592-616.

     Joreskog, Karl G. and Dag Sorbom. 1976a. Statistical

     Models and Methods for Analysis of Longitudinal

     Data. Pp. 285-325 in Latent Variables in Socioeco-

     nomic Models, edited by Dennis J. Aigner and Arthur

     S. Goldberger. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

     . 1976b. Statistical Models and Methods for

     Test-Retest Situations. Pp. 135-70 in Advances in

     Psychological and Educational Measurement, edited

     by D.N.M. deGruijter, L.J. van der Kamp, and H.F.

     Crombag. New York: Wiley.

     Kohn, Melvin L. 1971. Bureaucratic Man: A Portrait

     and an Interpretation. American Sociological Review

     36:461-74.

     Kohn, Melvin, Atsushi Naoi, Carrie Schoenbach, Carmi

     Schooler, and Kazimierz M. Slomczynski. 1990.

      Position in the Class Structure and Psychological

     Functioning in the United States, Japan, and Poland.

    American Journal of Sociology 95:964-1008.

     Kohn, Melvin L. and Carmi Schooler. 1978. The

     Reciprocal Effects of the Substantive Complexity of

     Work and Intellectual Flexibility: A Longitudinal

     Assessment. American Journal of Sociology 4:24-52.

     . 1982. Job Conditions and Personality: A

     Longitudinal Assessment of Their Reciprocal Effects.

    American Journal of Sociology 87:1257-1286.

     Kohn, Melvin L. and Carmi Schooler, in collaboration

     with Joanne Miller, Karen A. Miller, Carrie Schoen-

     bach, and Ronald Schoenberg. 1983. Work and

     Personality: An Inquiry into the Impact of Stratifica-

     tion. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

     Kohn, Melvin L., Kazimierz M. Slomczynski, and

     Carrie Schoenbach. 1986. Social Stratification and

     the Transmission of Values in the Family: A Cross-

     National Assessment. Sociological Forum 1:73-102.

     Lebra, Takie Sugiyama. 1984. Japanese Women: Con-

     straint and Fulfillment. Honolulu: University of

     Hawaii Press.

     Miller, Joanne, Carmi Schooler, Melvin L. Kohn, and

     Karen A. Miller. 1979. Women and Work: The

     Psychological Effects of Occupational Conditions.

    American Journal of Sociology 85:66-94.

     Miller, Joanne, Kazimierz M. Slomczynski, and Melvin

     L. Kohn. 1985. Continuity of Learning-Generalization:

     The Effect of Job on Men's Intellective Process in the

     United States and Poland. American Journal of

     Sociology 91:593-615.

     Naoi, Atsushi. The Status-Attainment Process: Results

     from the Japanese Data. Presented at the Japan-U.S.

     Conference on Social Stratification and Mobility,

     Maui, Hawaii, 1980.

     Naoi, Atsushi and Carmi Schooler. 1985. Occupational

     Conditions and Psychological Functioning in Japan.

    American Journal of Sociology 90:729-52.

     Naoi, M., K. Okamura and H. Hayashi. 1984. Living

     Arrangements of Old People and Some Comments

     about Future Change, Social Gerontology 21:3-21.

     National Institute of Employment and Vocational Re-

     search. 1989. Women's Careers in Japan. NIEVR

     Report #6. Tokyo:

     Schooler, Carmi. 1972. Social Antecedents of Adult

     Psychological Functioning. American Journal of

     Sociology 78:299-322.

     . 1984. Psychological Effects of Complex

     Environments during the Life Span: A Review and

     Theory. Intelligence 8:259-81.

     Schooler, Carmi, Melvin L. Kohn, Joanne Miller, and

     Karen A. Miller. 1983. Housework as Work. Pp.

     242-60 in Work and Personality: An Inquiry into the

     Impact of Social Stratification, edited by Norwood,

     NJ: Ablex.

     Schooler, Carmi, Joanne Miller, Karen A. Miller, and

     Carol Richtand. 1984. Work for the Household: Its

     Nature and Consequences for Husband and Wives.

    American Journal of Sociology 90:97-124.

     Schooler, Carmi and Atsushi Naoi. 1988a. The

     Psychological Effects of Traditional and of Economi-

     cally Peripheral Job Settings in Japan. American

     Journal of Sociology 94:335-55.

     . 1988b. The Psychological Effects of Tradi-

     tional and of Economically Peripheral Job Settings in

     Japan. Annals of Human Sciences 9:1-28.

     Schooler, Carmi and Karen C. Smith. 1978. '. . . And

     a Japanese Wife.' Social Structural Antecedents of

     Women's Role Values in Japan. Sex Roles 4:23-41.

     Slomczynski, Kazimierz M., Joanne Miller, and Melvin

     L. Kohn. 1981. Stratification, Work, and Values: A

     Polish-United States Comparison. American Sociolog-

     ical Review 46:720-44.

     Smith, Karen C. and Carmi Schooler. 1978. Women as

     Mothers in Japan: The Effects of Social Structure and

     Culture on Values and Behavior. Journal of Marriage

     and the Family 40:613-20.

     Witkin, Hernan A., Ruth B. Dyk, Hanna F. Faterson,

     Donald R. Goodenough, and Stephen A. Karp. 1962.

     Psychological Differentiation: Studies of Development.

     New York: Wiley.

     MICHIKO NAOI is Associate Professor at Tokyo Gakugei University. Her research interests include

     sociology of aging, family and women's attitudes.

     CARMI SCHOOLER is Acting Chief, Laboratory of Socio-Environmental Studies, National Institute of

     Mental Health. His research interests include social structural and cultural determinants of both normal

     and abnormal adult functioning throughout the life span.