How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past...

14
How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of Grammatical Categories? The Aspect Hypothesis Revisited Jekaterina Mažara and Sabine Stoll 1. Introduction Verbs are not only the semantic center of information in an utterance denoting and locating events in time and space but also form the morphosyntactic anchor point of information in a sentence. The verbal system of a language thus plays a pivotal role in language acquisition. Learning about the complex features and functions of verbs is one of the main challenges in early first language acquisition. The earliest verb forms in children’s production are grammatically and lexically specific constructions, which are presumably rote learned (Lieven et al., 1997; Pine & Lieven, 1997; Tomasello, 1992, 2000, 2003). During this phase, children hardly display any flexibility of usage; but soon after using the first rote-learned constructions children start to produce new forms and apply them to new contexts. So far, relatively little is known about the details of this generalization process. Here we focus on the development of flexibility in the use of Russian verb morphology. We analyze the development of combinations of grammatical markers and lexical elements from the earliest phase of verb production to the point where children are approaching the flexibility displayed by surrounding adult speakers. A central category of the Russian verbal system is grammatical aspect, which expresses the viewpoint onto the temporal structure of an action or event. Perfective aspect indicates a temporally bounded, external view of a completed event (Comrie, 1976), e.g. on s’’el jabloko ‘He finished eating an apple’, whereas imperfective aspect puts the focus on the internal process of a temporally extended action, e.g. on el jabloko ‘He was eating an apple’. In Russian, aspect interacts in complex ways with tense morphology and lexical aspect (also called Aktionsart). There is enormous cross-linguistic variation in whether and how languages mark aspectual relations. However, results from a number of languages have * Jekaterina Mažara, [email protected], Sabine Stoll, Department of Comparative Language Science and Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zurich. This research is part of the project ‘Acquisition processes in maximally diverse languages: Min(ding) the ambient languages (ACQDIV)’ that has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7-2007-2013) (Grant agreement No. 615988; PI Sabine Stoll). Author contributions: JM and SST designed the study and performed the research. JM did the computations and analysis. JM and SST wrote the paper. Proceedings of the 44th Boston University Conference on Language Development, ed. Megan M. Brown and Alexandra Kohut, 363-375. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Transcript of How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past...

Page 1: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use ofGrammatical Categories? The Aspect Hypothesis Revisited

Jekaterina Mažara and Sabine Stoll

1. Introduction

Verbs are not only the semantic center of information in an utterance denoting and locating events in time and space but also form the morphosyntactic anchor point of information in a sentence. The verbal system of a language thus plays a pivotal role in language acquisition. Learning about the complex features and functions of verbs is one of the main challenges in early first language acquisition. The earliest verb forms in children’s production are grammatically and lexically specific constructions, which are presumably rote learned (Lieven et al., 1997; Pine & Lieven, 1997; Tomasello, 1992, 2000, 2003). During this phase, children hardly display any flexibility of usage; but soon after using the first rote-learned constructions children start to produce new forms and apply them to new contexts. So far, relatively little is known about the details of this generalization process.

Here we focus on the development of flexibility in the use of Russian verb morphology. We analyze the development of combinations of grammatical markers and lexical elements from the earliest phase of verb production to the point where children are approaching the flexibility displayed by surrounding adult speakers. A central category of the Russian verbal system is grammatical aspect, which expresses the viewpoint onto the temporal structure of an action or event. Perfective aspect indicates a temporally bounded, external view of a completed event (Comrie, 1976), e.g. on s’’el jabloko ‘He finished eating an apple’, whereas imperfective aspect puts the focus on the internal process of a temporally extended action, e.g. on el jabloko ‘He was eating an apple’. In Russian, aspect interacts in complex ways with tense morphology and lexical aspect (also called Aktionsart).

There is enormous cross-linguistic variation in whether and how languages mark aspectual relations. However, results from a number of languages have

* Jekaterina Mažara, [email protected], Sabine Stoll, Department of Comparative Language Science and Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Language Evolution (ISLE), University of Zurich. This research is part of the project ‘Acquisition processes in maximally diverse languages: Min(ding) the ambient languages (ACQDIV)’ that has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7-2007-2013) (Grant agreement No. 615988; PI Sabine Stoll). Author contributions: JM and SST designed the study and performed the research. JM did the computations and analysis. JM and SST wrote the paper.

������������������!��������������������Proceedings of the 44th Boston University Conference on Language Development, ed. Megan M. Brown and Alexandra Kohut,

363-375. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Page 2: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

shown that there are consistent regularities in how tense-aspect categories evolve in early development. Correlations between verbs denoting a defined endpoint (telic verbs) and grammatical marking of past tense and perfective aspect as well as between verbs without a defined endpoint (atelic) and non-past imperfective marking were found in early acquisition of a number of different languages (cf. e.g., Bloom, Lifter, and Hafitz, 1980; Harner, 1981; Shirai and Anderson, 1995; Clark, 1996; Johnson and Fey, 2006 for English; Bronckart and Sinclair, 1973 for French, Antinucci and Miller, 1976 for Italian; Li and Bowerman, 1998; Shirai 1998; Li & Shirai, 2000 for Japanese; Stoll & Gries, 2009; Gagarina, 2000; Bar-Shalom, 2002 for Russian; Li, 1990; Li & Shirai, 2000 for Mandarin; Aksu-Koç, 1998 for Turkish; Stephany, 1985 for Greek; Weist et al., 1984; for Polish; as well as self-organizing feature map models, cf. Li, 2000; Li & Shirai, 2000).

These correlations and a possible cause are summarized in the Aspect Hypothesis (Shirai & Anderson, 1995). The Aspect Hypothesis proposes generalizations of children’s early verb form use in terms of preference for specific combinations of tense, grammatical aspect, and inherent semantic properties of the verb. According to Shirai and Anderson (1995), children form prototypes of categories based on semantic properties of verbs. They are able to use the matching morphology with these prototypes first, before they extend it to other verbs. Two predictions of the Aspect Hypothesis are applicable to Russian (Shirai & Anderson 1998: 282f):

1. In languages that encode the perfective/imperfective distinction, imperfective past develops later than perfective past. Imperfective past marking is initially used mostly with states and activities.1

2. Past marking is at first used predominantly on achievement and accomplishment verbs (verbs denoting actions/events with an inherent end point); only later it is extended to activities and states (actions/events without an inherent end point).

The Aspect Hypothesis predicts that these generalizations hold during the early phase of verb use as well as during early development of flexibility. The child should move through an intermediary phase of higher flexibility within these categories than across them.

Interestingly, these correlations are not restricted to child language but are also found in child-directed speech, but to a lesser degree (Shirai & Anderson, 1995). During development, these correlations in child speech gradually decrease to the level of adults. This is a problem for the proposed prototype account that was already addressed by the authors themselves (as well as Andersen & Shirai 1996; Li et al. 2001; Boland 2006, Bertinetto et al. 2015). If the input distributions

1 The four time schemata Vendler (1967) developed for English verbs are states, activities, accomplishments, and achievements. Examples for each of the four are love, know (states), read, sleep (activities), run a mile, eat a bowl of soup (accomplishments), find, discover (achievements).

364

Page 3: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

line up with the initial production, then there is not enough evidence to posit the cognitive preference of the prototypes. Bertinetto et al. (2015) propose that there are no universal cognitive preferences; instead, children build the categories of their language based on the distributions of overt markings in their target language.

Evidence against prediction 1 was provided in an analysis of longitudinal data from 4 Russian children. Verbs of both aspects with the past tense start increasing in flexibility around the same time (Mažara & Stoll, 2019). Even in the earliest productions, past morphology is found across both aspects. Here, we address prediction 2 and test whether Russian children’s use of morphology reflects a preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without an inherent endpoint. 2. The Russian verb 2.1. Verb morphology in Russian

Russian is a language with relatively complex verbal morphology including asemantically and morphologically complex category of grammatical aspect which interrelates with tense. Grammatical aspect in Russian is a binary perfective/imperfective distinction; each verb form is either perfective or imperfective.2 In contrast to English, which has one single aspectual marker(-ing), Russian has multiple grammatical markers for the perfective aspect (mainlyprefixes and one suffix), and one suffix (with various allomorphs) for theimperfective aspect as well as suprasegmental and zero marking. The morphological marking of aspect is subject to several constraints which might help the learner make generalizations about the complex morphology. However, for every constraint there are exceptions (Stoll, 1998). Morphologically, perfectives are typically derived from imperfectives by prefixation (with a few exceptions). A simplified overview is presented in Table 1. Table 1: Example of derivation of aspectual forms

imperfectivesimplex

perfectivederivation(s)

imperfectivederivation(s)

čitat’ ’to read’ + prefix pročitat’ ’readthrough’

+ suffix -yv-pročityvat’

’read throughrepeatedly’

Prefixes typically carry an additional lexical meaning in addition to providing aspectual marking. The resulting meaning of the new perfective verb cannot be derived via simple rules, since there is no one-to-one relationship between prefixes and the resulting meaning change in the verb they are attached to.

2 There is a number of biaspectual verbs that use the same form for both aspects. However, they represent a specific aspect according to context.

365

Page 4: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

Furthermore, many verbs can combine with multiple prefixes, while others are restricted in their combinability (Timberlake, 2004). Therefore, a certain degree of rote learning is required.

On the functional level several temporal and contextual features influence the use of the two aspects. Imperfectives are the unmarked form and can appear in a greater variety of contexts. Put simply, Russian imperfective verbs are used when the duration of an action is highlighted, without placing focus on the beginning or end border of the action (on čital knigu ‘he was reading a book’) or if the action is presented as a complete event sometime in the past (on čital etu knigu ‘he has read this book (at some point)’). Perfective verbs are used when a boundary of a verb is in focus (Bickel, 1997; Breu 1985), be it the beginning, end or result of the action or both (on pročital knigu ‘he (has) finished reading the book’). However, in practice there are finer semantic and pragmatic distinctions between contexts.

Russian has three tenses. Present tense occurs exclusively with imperfective verbs. Past tense occurs on verbs of both aspects. Future tense is marked on perfective verbs through the morphemes that mark present tense on imperfectives. Imperfective verbs use a periphrastic construction (inflected form of ‘to be’ + infinitive of the main verb). In this study we focus on the acquisition of synthetic morphology. 3 Table 2 provides examples of the synthetic tense-aspect morphology Russian children must master to be fully proficient users of language. Table 2: Examples of synthetic tense-aspect morphology

imperfective verbs perfective verbs

presentja čita-ju

‘I am reading’ –

futureanalytic construction

ja budu čitat’‘I will read’

ja pročitaju ètu knigu‘I will read/finish reading this

book’

pastja čitala

‘I read/was reading’ja pročitala ètu knigu

‘I finished reading this book’

imperative čitaj!‘read!’

pročitaj!‘read!’

3 We exclude reflexive marking, because it does not occur systematically on all verbs (Timberlake 2004). For the sake of completeness, we also must mention that Russian has a number of participle forms. These were also excluded from this study because they are hardly ever used in spoken Russian, and their use differs from that of other verb forms.

366

Page 5: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

2.2. Semantic verb classes

The lexical temporal categories are known under a number of different terms such as Aktionsart (Agrell, 1908), time schema (Vendler, 1967) or lexical aspect (Timberlake, 1985; Smith, 1983). To avoid confusion, we will use aspect for the grammatical aspect of verbs and Aktionsart for the inherent temporal properties.

Many studies of aspect, both in studies of general linguistic work and especially also in child language acquisition, use the Vendlerian classification (Vendler 1967) of inherent temporal properties of verbs (cf. Shirai & Anderson, 1995). This classification distinguishes four categories: states, activities, accomplishments, and achievements. For Russian, however, this distinction is both insufficient and inappropriate. Therefore, rather than following the same classification for all languages, we adopt a different approach taking language-specific distinctions into account (Stoll, 1998, 2001). The classification is based on a set of primitives: boundaries and phases, which can combine in various ways to create the categories needed for each language (cf. Bickel, 1996, 1997; Breu 1985). We use the classification outlined in Table 3, which proposes five Aktionsarten for Russian (Stoll 1998): Table 3: Aktionsarten in Russian

Aktionsart example aspect

telicsevent/action includesan inherent endpoint

umeret’‘to die’

imperfective or perfective

durativesstate/action withoutan inherent endpoint

igrat’’to play’ always imperfective

ingressives beginning ofan event/action

zagovorit’‘start to talk’

always perfective

delimitatives temporally boundedactions/states

počitat’’to read for a while’

always perfective

semelfactivesaction that happenspunctually and only once

stuknut’’to knock once’

always perfective

A semantic core meaning might belong to different Aktionsart(en) after

derivation. For example, govorit’ ‘to speak’ is durative in its core form and becomes telic as dogovorit’ ‘finish speaking’ or delimitative as pogovorit’ ‘to talk for a while’. A specific prefix used for derivation can lead to different outcomes, depending on the stem it is combined with, e.g. the same surface form of the prefix za- can result in an ingressive or a telic verb. Combined with the verb govorit’ to zagovorit’ ‘to start talking’ the meaning is ingressive, whereas in combination

367

Page 6: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

with prygnut’ ‘to jump’, the resulting meaning of zaprygnut’ ‘to jump somewhere/onto something’ is telic. To make matters even more complex, the same Aktionsart can have multiple prefixes associated with it. 3. Data

We use data from an audio-visual longitudinal corpus of 5 Russian children (Stoll & Meyer, 2008). All data were collected in monolingual families in St. Petersburg, Russia. The recordings were made in naturalistic settings and include the focal child (or children), siblings and a varying number of surrounding adults. The children were recorded for one hour each week over several years.4 Since we examine the early stages of verb production here, we focus on the 4 children of the corpus, whose recordings began before the age of three. When making comparisons to adult speech, we exclude all utterances by children other than the focal children. The entire corpus consists of approximately 1.95 million transcribed and morphosyntactically annotated words. Table a e 4 summ riz s the

4

number of utterances, words, and verbs uttered by each focal child and surrounding adults as well as the age range during which the focal children were recorded. All verbs were annotated for Aktionsart manually according to the definitions and tests in Stoll (2001). Interrater reliability is at 93.5% agreement, with Cohen’s kappa at 0.85.

Table : Overview of focal children’s age range and production

Target Age Number of N (tokens)Child span recordings Produced by child Produced by adults

utterances words verbs utterances words verbs

1 1;8.10 - 4;8.21 130 124,497 241,948 38,843 118,024 301,418 60,9872 1;4.23 - 4;1.24 109 40,341 57,929 5,411 128,515 354,034 65,1733 1;3.24 - 4;9.29 123 45,412 74,926 10,733 148,093 423,078 84,6595 1;11.28 - 4;3.12 67 43,962 97,397 16,585 77,550 223,289 43,149

If the predictions made by the Aspect Hypothesis hold for Russian, development should follow these tendencies: (1) past forms are used predominantly on perfective telic verbs, and only later extended to imperfective telics and duratives, and (2) imperfective past develops later than perfective past.

4. Methods 4. 1. Computing phases of development

First, we want to find out whether there are different stages of verb use in the development of the verbal paradigm in Russian early language acquisition. To be able to compare verb forms produced during the earliest phase of children’s

4 Gaps in later months of data collection are due to the children's families being out of town for holidays without being able to record.

368

Page 7: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

production with that of the later phase, we performed a segmented regression on the additive growth of each child’s full verb form inventory. As previously stated, it has been claimed that early production is characterized by a small number of frequently repeated items. During this phase, children add few new forms to their inventories, keeping the growth curve flat. This is followed by a phase later in development, with a rapid increase in verb forms. Here we use segmented regression to calculate the point at which that change occurs by estimating significant changes in the slope of the regression.

With the exception of Child 55 all children exhibit a flat first phase with a steep increase later on. The estimated breaks happen for Child 1 at age 2;2, Child 2 at age 3;5, and Child 3 at age 2;3. 4.2. Analysis 1: Analysing verb form use in the two phases

Assessing the development of flexibility of form use in naturalistic corpora

is difficult, because perfect correspondence of context is never given. However, limiting the instances to a predefined set is also inadequate, since it artificially diminishes the range of produced forms. Therefore, we conduct two analyses. The first restricts adult production to compare the distribution of forms produced by the children and the surrounding adults within the same amount of tokens. To do this, we take the entire number of tokens produced by each child during their first phase and extract the lemmas these forms belong to. Then, we sample the same lemmas and an equivalent number of tokens from the adults as well as from each child’s second phase. This allows us to assess the flexibility for a controlled group of lexical items within a controlled sample size.

To compute the distance between the distributions of each child’s first phase and the sample from adult speech, we use Jensen-Shannon divergence (Lin 1991). This measures the distance between two probability distributions over the same elements. The closer the resulting number is to zero, the more alike the two distributions are.

4.3. Analysis 2: Development of flexibility over time

In the first analysis, the matching of lemmas and number of tokens allows us to make a statement about the diversity of forms used in the entire first phase. However, we cannot measure development in the same way. To measure development of flexibility from week to week, we must characterize the production during that week and compare it systematically to the production of

5 Child 5 is an early talker and shows a steady increase from the start of the recording period, which is similar in its shape to that of adults, albeit on a smaller scale. Essentially this means that we cannot know whether Child 5 has acquired and used many of the forms before or is acquiring them during the recording period, but doing so at a faster pace than the other children.

369

Page 8: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

adults within the same recording. This allows us to include the entire production of each session for both children and adults.

To assess the flexibility of form use, we compute the entropy (Shannon, 1948) of the production of each child and the corresponding surrounding adults. Entropy measures the rate at which a process generates information; it characterizes the balance of frequency distributions over a set of elements. The more equally the probability of produced elements is distributed, the less predictable, and hence more informative, the output. Early child language is usually characterized by the repeated use of a few forms, while other forms might appear only once. This would result in a skewed distribution, highly predictable output, and low entropy. The formula for Shannon entropy is given in (1): (1) 𝐻(𝑋) = −∑ 𝑝(𝑥+) log 𝑝 (𝑥+)/

+01 , where N is the number of distinct forms and p(x) is the probability of occurrence of a specific form.6

We bootstrap the data (i.e. sampling with replacement) for 100 iterations per session to obtain a range of measurements for each data point. The entropy is measured for the focal child as well as for the surrounding adults; then we divide the child’s entropy measure by that of the adults. The resulting entropy ratio indicates the relative development: a number below 1 means the child’s flexibility of verb form use is below that of surrounding adults in the same session, while a ratio fluctuating around 1 represents the child approaching adult level of flexibility.

We divide the data by grammatical aspect and Aktionsart. Then we compute the entropy of distinct lemmas used with the forms of the verb paradigm: i.e. distinct lemmas used with past vs. non-past forms for telics and duratives, respectively.

Finally, we fit a generalized additive model to the bootstrapped entropy ratios and conduct a segmented regression to estimate the points in development at which flexibility increases. Note that this computes a different increase from the one discussed in 4.1.; the mere addition of forms to the inventory does not necessarily mean an increase in the flexibility of use of those forms. 5. Results 5.1. Comparing the distribution of forms in each phase

The early production of children in Phase 1 contains only verbs of telic and durative Aktionsart. Figures 1a and b exemplify the distributions of grammatical markers used with telic and durative verbs of Child 1 in Phase 1. The raw numbers of uses of a particular form (reflected in the distribution of tokens, Figure 1a) show a greater imbalance in the distribution in the child’s production than in that

6 For the computation of entropy over time we used the Chao-Shen estimation of entropy, a measure that accounts for unseen types (Chao & Shen, 2003).

370

Page 9: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

of adults. However, looking at the distribution of verb form types (Figure 1b), the distribution of forms across the Aktionsarten is much closer to that of adults, with very similar distributions.

Figure 1a: Distribution of grammatical markers across Aktionsarten during phase 1, distribution of tokens

Figure 1b: Distribution of grammatical markers across Aktionsarten during phase 1, distribution of types

The Jensen-Shannon divergence (JSD) also shows a greater distance betweenthe distributions of tokens than types. Both decrease in the second phase. However,the decrease in the distance of the token distributions is smaller. This is not unexpected, since the use of forms depends on context and tends to vary more than the types in the inventory.

The average JSD between children and surrounding adults during phase 1 for the distribution of types is 0.129, and 0.551 for tokens. These numbers decrease to 0.053 and 0.156 for types and tokens, respectively, in phase 2. The comparison of distributions between each child’s own production during the two phases results in numbers very similar to those computed for the difference between the

371

Page 10: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

children’s production and surrounding adults: 0.12 for types and 0.513 for tokens.7 5.2. Development of diversity of form use over time

Figures 2 and 3 show the development of entropy ratios in the focal children

in relation to surrounding adults over time. The data of each recording session was bootstrapped 100 times and each point represents one such computation. The regression lines represent the fitted generalized additive model, while the orthogonal lines show the estimated change points of the regression, which indicate the onset of the increase in flexibility. The dotted line shows where this change takes place for past forms, while the dashed line represents the same for non-past forms.8

Figure 2: Entropy ratios for telic perfectives

Figure 2 shows the development of past and non-past morphology use with different lemmas (i.e. lexical items) for perfective telic verbs. An increase in the variety of lemmas indicates an increase of flexibility, since the child is not restricting his/her use to one specific stem+morpheme combination. Figure 2

7 Since Child 5’s first phase predates the recordings, their production and distributions were only computed for the comparison of phase 2 production of the child to a matched sample from adults during the same time. 8 Once more, Child 5 does not show this change from item-specificity to more flexible use, since his item-specific stage predates the recordings.

372

Page 11: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

demonstrates that the predicted facilitation for the acquisition of past forms does not hold for Russian, since the two lines are temporally close together. Were the prediction of the Aspect Hypothesis outlined above to hold, we would expect to see a delay in the increase of flexibility.

Similarly, Figure 3 shows that the development of flexibility of use of non-pastforms with duratives (which correspond to activities and states in the Vendlerian classification used in the Aspect Hypothesis) is not quicker than that of past forms. This shows that the predictions of the Aspect Hypothesis do not hold here either.

Figure 3: Entropy ratios for durative imperfectives 6. Discussion

Our results show that Russian children do not pass through an intermediaryphase of prototype categorization during their development of verb morphology. Instead, they use morphology across grammatical aspect and Aktionsart early on. This suggests that the predicted facilitation of certain combinations of grammatical aspect, Aktionsart, and tense morphology does not hold for any of the combinations. Instead of building up prototypes, children are sensitive to the distributions in input.

A main take-away from this study is that it is essential to consider token andtype distributions as two different pieces of information. Most of the prior work on this subject focused on the counts of categories in language use. That, however, only reflects a part of language use, which highly depends on context and each speaker’s expressive needs. A look at the distribution of types, on the other hand,

373

Page 12: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

gives insight into the development of the inventory. Statements about learnability or cognitive preference of forms are hardly justifiable when they are based on the distribution of tokens uttered in a particular time window.

The method we propose in this paper allows us to obtain a systematic comparison to adult production with a high temporal resolution in the same context of speech. Furthermore, entropy evaluates both the amount of types as well as the distribution of tokens across these types. References Agrell, Sigurd, 1908. Aspektänderung und Aktionsartbildung beim polnischen Zeitwort.

Lund: Ohlsson. Aksu-Koç, Ayhan A. (1998). The role of input vs. universal predispositions in the

emergence of tense-aspect morphology: evidence from Turkish. First Language, 18, 255-280.

Antinucci, Francesco, & Miller, Ruth (1976). How children talk about what happened. Journal of Child Language, 3, 167-189.

Bar-Shalom, Eva (2002). Tense and aspect in early child Russian. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 10(4), 321-337.

Bertinetto, PierMarco, Freiberger, Eva Maria, Lenci, Alessandro, Noccetti, Sabrina, & Maddalena Agonigi (2015). The acquisition of tense and aspect in a morphology-sensitive framework: Data from Italian and Austrian-German children. Linguistics 53(5), 1113–1168.

Bickel, Balthasar, (1996). Aspect, mood, and time in Belhare: studies in the semantics –pragmatics interface of a Himalayan language. Zürich: ASAS-Verlag.

Bickel, Balthasar, (1997). Aspectual scope and the difference between logical and semantic representation. Lingua 102, 115-131.

Bloom, Lois, Lifter, Karin, & Hafitz, Jeremie (1980). Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language, 56, 386-412.

Breu, Walter, 1985. Handlungsgrenzen als Grundlage der Verbklassifikation. In: Lehfeldt, W. [ed.], Slavistische Linguistik 1984, pp. 9-34. München.

Bronckart, Jean-Paul, & Sinclair, Hermine J. (1973). Time, tense and aspect. Cognition: International Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 107-130.

Chao Anne, Shen Tsung-Jen (2003). Nonparametric estimation of Shannon's diversity index when there are unseen species in sample. Environmental and Ecological Statistics, 10, 429-443.

Clark, Eve V. (1996). Early verbs, event-types, and inflections. Children’s Language, 9, 61–73.

Comrie, Bernard (1976). Aspect. London: Cambridge University Press. Gagarina, Natalia (2000). The acquisition of aspectuality by Russian children: the early

stages. ZAS-Papers in Linguistics, 15, 232-246. Harner, Lorraine (1981). Children talk about the time and aspect of actions. Child

Development, 52, 498-506. Johnson, Bonnie W., & Fey, Marc E. (2006). Interaction of lexical and grammatical aspect

in toddlers’ language. Journal of Child Language, 33(02), 419-435. Li, Ping (1990). Aspect and aktionsart in child Mandarin (Doctoral Thesis). Li, Ping (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect in a self-organizing

feature-map model. In Proceedings of the 22nd annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

374

Page 13: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

Li, Ping, & Bowerman, Melissa (1998). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect in chinese. Journal of Child Language, 54, 311-350.

Li, Ping, & Shirai, Yasuhiro (2000). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Lieven, Elena V., Pine, Julian M., & Baldwin, G. Douglas (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24, 187–219.

Lin, Jianhua (1991). Divergence measures based on the shannon entropy. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory, 37(1), 145–151.

Pine, Julian M., & Lieven, Elena V. (1997). Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 123–138.

R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org

Shannon, Claude E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27 (3), 379–423.

Shirai, Yasuhiro (1998). The emergence of tense-aspect morphology in japanese: universal predisposition? First Language, 18 (54), 281–309.

Shirai, Yasuhiro, & Anderson, R. W. (1995). The acquisition of tense-aspect morphology: a prototype account. Language, 71, 743-762.

Shirai, Yasuhiro., Slobin, D. I., & Weist, R. M. (1998). The acquisition of tense-aspect morphology (Vol. 18). Alpha Academic.

Smith, Carlota S., (1983). A theory of aspectual choice. Language 59, 479-501. Stephany, Ursula (1985). Aspekt, Tempus und Modalität: Zur Entwicklung der

Verbalgrammatik in der neugriechischen Kindersprache. Tübingen, Germany: Gunther Narr.

Stoll, Sabine (1998). The role of aktionsart in the acquisition of russian aspect. First Language, 18, 351-378.

Stoll, Sabine (2001). The acquisition of Russian aspect. University of California, Berkeley, Faculty of Arts. (Doctoral Thesis)

Stoll, Sabine, & Gries, Stefan Th. (2009). How to measure development in corpora? an association-strength approach to characterizing development in corpora. Journal of Child Language, 36, 1075-1090.

Stoll, Sabine, & Meyer, Roland (2008). Audio-visional longitudinal corpus on the acquisition of Russian by 5 children.

Timberlake, Alan, (1985). The temporal schemata of Russian predicates. In: Flier, M. S. and R. D. Brecht [eds.], Issues in Russian Morphosyntax. Columbus, Ohio: Slavica Publishers, 35-57.

Timberlake, Alan (2004). A reference grammar of Russian. Cambridge University Press. Tomasello, Michael (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tomasello, Michael (2000). First steps toward a usage-based theory of language

acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1/2), 61–82. Tomasello, Michael (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language

acquisition. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press. Vendler, Zeno, (1967). Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca, NY.: Cornell University. Weist, Richard M., Wysocka, Hanna, Witkowska-Stadnik, Katarzyna, Buczowska, Ewa,

& Konieczna, Emilia (1984). The defective tense hypothesis: on the emergence of tense and aspect in child Polish. Journal of Child Language, 11, 347-374.

375

Page 14: How Do Children Become Flexible in Their Use of ... · preference for the combination of (1) past tense marking and verbs with an inherent endpoint and of (2) non-past and verbs without

Proceedings of the 44th annualBoston University Conference on Language Development

edited by Megan M. Brown and Alexandra Kohut

Cascadilla Press Somerville, MA 2020

Copyright information

Proceedings of the 44th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development© 2020 Cascadilla Press. All rights reserved

Copyright notices are located at the bottom of the first page of each paper.Reprints for course packs can be authorized by Cascadilla Press.

ISSN 1080-692XISBN 978-1-57473-057-9 (2 volume set, paperback)

Ordering information

To order a copy of the proceedings or to place a standing order, contact:

Cascadilla Press, P.O. Box 440355, Somerville, MA 02144, USAphone: 1-617-776-2370, [email protected], www.cascadilla.com