FINAL REPORT Speech & Language Therapy Service Model ... · FINAL REPORT Speech & Language Therapy...

154
FINAL REPORT Speech & Language Therapy Service Model & Effectiveness Review: Stages II and III (5-18 year olds) Prepared for The Department of Education and Children’s Services and the Department of Health, South Australia Prepared by Deborah G H James, Leah Jeffries & Anthea Worley Centre for Allied Health Evidence University of South Australia North Terrace Adelaide SA 5000 Email: [email protected] Telephone: 08 8302 2769 Submission date of final report 12 th December 2008 This report should be referenced as follows: James, D.G.H., Jeffries, L. & Worley. A. (2008) Speech & Language Therapy Service Model & Effectiveness Review: Stages II and III (5-18 year olds).Centre for Allied Health Evidence, University of South Australia

Transcript of FINAL REPORT Speech & Language Therapy Service Model ... · FINAL REPORT Speech & Language Therapy...

FINAL REPORT

Speech & Language Therapy Service Model &

Effectiveness Review:

Stages II and III (5-18 year olds)

Prepared for

The Department of Education and Children’s Services and the

Department of Health, South Australia

Prepared by

Deborah G H James, Leah Jeffries & Anthea Worley

Centre for Allied Health Evidence

University of South Australia

North Terrace

Adelaide SA 5000

Email: [email protected]

Telephone: 08 8302 2769

Submission date of final report

12th December 2008

This report should be referenced as follows:

James, D.G.H., Jeffries, L. & Worley. A. (2008) Speech & Language Therapy Service Model &

Effectiveness Review: Stages II and III (5-18 year olds).Centre for Allied Health Evidence,

University of South Australia

2

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Table of Contents ................................................................................................................................... 2 Glossary of Outcome Measures ............................................................................................................. 3 Chapter 1: Introduction ...................................................................................................................... 4

Background ........................................................................................................................................ 4 Primary speech and language impairment in school children ....................................................... 4 Incidence and prevalence of primary speech and language difficulties ........................................ 5

Speech Pathology Services ................................................................................................................ 6 Research Questions ............................................................................................................................ 7

Chapter 2: Method ............................................................................................................................... 8 PECOT (Population, Exposure, Comparator, Outcomes, Time) ....................................................... 8

Search strategy ................................................................................................................................... 8 Search terms Stage II & III combined ........................................................................................... 8 Search limits ................................................................................................................................... 9 Search validation ............................................................................................................................ 9

Data extraction ................................................................................................................................... 9 Chapter 3: Results .............................................................................................................................. 10

Description of the studies ................................................................................................................. 10 Country of origin .......................................................................................................................... 10 Level of Evidence and quality of research ................................................................................... 10 The nature of the service delivery ................................................................................................ 11 Sample sizes ................................................................................................................................. 11 The age of participants ................................................................................................................. 11

The nature of the impairments ..................................................................................................... 12 Types of outcome measures ......................................................................................................... 12

Key Results ...................................................................................................................................... 12 Service delivery ........................................................................................................................... 13 Efficacy of the interventions ........................................................................................................ 17

Chapter 4: Discussion ........................................................................................................................ 22 Efficacy of Interventions .................................................................................................................. 22

Effectiveness and effects .............................................................................................................. 22 Efficiency of the interventions ..................................................................................................... 22

Service Delivery Constructs ............................................................................................................ 24 Efficacy of Direct models of service delivery ............................................................................. 24 Efficacy of Indirect models of service delivery ........................................................................... 24

Efficacy Of The Collaborative Models Of Service Delivery ....................................................... 25 Limitations of the review ................................................................................................................. 26

Chapter 5: Core learnings ................................................................................................................. 27 Efficacy of Interventions .................................................................................................................. 27

Effectiveness ................................................................................................................................ 27

Efficiency ..................................................................................................................................... 27 Models of Service delivery .............................................................................................................. 28

Effectiveness ................................................................................................................................ 28

Efficiency ..................................................................................................................................... 28 References ........................................................................................................................................... 29 Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................................ 35

Appendices .......................................................................................................................................... 36 Appendix 1. Summary of all the studies against extraction criteria. .............................................. 36 Appendix 2 Complete summary of each of article .......................................................................... 51

Appendix 3 Summary of the Articles in the review by Cirrin & Gillam (2008) ........................... 137

3

GLOSSARY OF OUTCOME MEASURES

Outcome measure: Acronym Measures:

American Speech Hearing Association National

Outcomes Measurement

ASHA NOM Functional communication

Analysis of Language Impaired Children‟s

Conversation

ALICC Conversational interaction

Analysis of the Language of Learning ALL Metalinguistics

Analysis of Variance ANOVA A statistical test to determine if group

differences are present

Assessment of Comprehension and Expression ACE Language composite of receptive and

expressive language that include

differencing and narrative propositions

Attention Level Rating Scale Attention level rating scale

British Picture Vocabulary Scale-II BPVS II Receptive vocabulary

Burt Word Reading Test Reading

The Bus Story Discourse

Child Behaviour Checklist CBCL Child behaviour (attention, engagement

delinquent behaviour)

Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-3 1. Subtests include sentence structure; word

structure; word association; recalling sentences

2. Note a UK version exists

CELF-3 Composite language assessing reception

and expression yielding total score,

receptive language score and expressive

language score

Childhood Autism Rating Scale CARS Autism

Children‟s Communication Checklist CCC Pragmatics

Correct Underlying Representations CUR Level of adult-like phoneme production

Developmental Sentence Scoring DSS Expressive syntax

Expressive Vocabulary Test EVT Expressive vocabulary

Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Revised GFTA-R Articulation

Lindamood Auditory Conceptualisation Test LAC Phonological awareness

Mean Length of Utterances MLU Syntax

Neale Analysis of Reading NARA Reading

Non-Word Decoding Test Reading

Non Word Repetition Test Phonological processing

Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised

3. Subtests include reading recognition and reading

comprehension; spelling; arithmetic

PIAT-R Reading, spelling and arithmetic

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised PPVT-R Vocabulary comprehension

Percentage Consonants Correct PCC Phonology

Percentage Phonemes Correct PPC Phonology

Percentage Vowels Correct PVC Phonology

Phonological Abilities Test PAT Phonological awareness

The Preschool Language Assessment Instrument PLAI Discourse

Ravens‟ Coloured Progressive Matrices RCPM Non-verbal cognition

Renfrew Action Picture Test RAPT Syntax

South Tyneside Assessment of Phonology STAP Phonology

Terminal Units T-units Syntax

Test of Language Development-2 Primary TOLD-P 2 Word discrimination

Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-2 TONI-2 Non-verbal skills

Test of Reception of Grammar TROG Receptive grammar

Token Test Receptive grammar

Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-III WISC-III Intelligence

Wechsler Objective Reading Dimension WORD Reading

Woodstock Language Proficiency Battery-R Reading

Note: This list does not include all outcome measures as some were not named or described

4

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION

BACKGROUND

The State Government of South Australia recognises the importance of optimal speech and language

outcomes, as measured by literacy and numeracy rates, for all children in South Australia and this

has been demonstrated in “South Australia’s Strategic Plan 2007”. In 2006, it instituted a review of

the state paediatric speech pathology services. The South Australian State-wide Interagency Speech

Pathology Group, representing the departments of Health, Education and Children‟s Services and

Disability has the delegated responsibility for conducting this project. As a part of this process, they

commissioned the Centre of Allied Health Evidence (CAHE) within the University of South

Australia, to conduct a systematic review of the literature to examine the evidence for speech and

language therapy services for children aged 0-18 years with speech, language and communication

needs and the service delivery constructs underpinning that intervention. They divided the review

into four stages. Stage I addressed children with primary speech and language impairment from birth

to less than five years and this review was concluded in December 2007. Stages II and III are the

focus of this report, addressing children with primary speech and language impairment, aged five to

eight years and nine to eighteen years. The final stage will address children, aged 0-18 years with

speech, language and communication needs secondary to disabilities such as cerebral palsy, autism,

intellectual disability, hearing impairment, behavioural or emotional difficulties or neurological

impairment.

PRIMARY SPEECH AND LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT IN SCHOOL CHILDREN

For the approximate 10% of children who begin school with a primary speech and/or language

impairment (PSLI), their difficulties begin manifesting in reading and writing in addition to speaking

and listening difficulties. The cause of PSLI is unknown and they are not the consequence of hearing

loss, intellectual disability, frank neurological problems or syndromes (Law et al. 2003). Rather,

these problems exist despite normal functioning in these other areas.

Speech impairment affects the phonological system, that is the production (pronunciation) and

processing of sounds. School-age speech production or phonological output difficulties may be overt

and obvious such as a young school-child who says I tan tee a do ditting in na buet meaning I can

see a dog sitting in the bucket. However, school-age speech production difficulties are typically

covert and insidious, possibly only manifesting as literacy problems (Gillon & Dodd 1993). The

typical pathway of speech impairment that persists into the school years is to manifest overtly first as

pronunciation difficulties in the years before school and in the early school years, then manifest

covertly as literacy problems without obvious pronunciation difficulties and then as academic, social

and vocational difficulties (Felsenfeld, Broen & McGuel 1992, 1994; Gillon & Dodd 1993;

Snowling, Adams, Bishop & Stothard 2001; Stothard, Snowling, Bishop, Chipchase & Kaplan

1998).

Phonological processing is different to and broader than phonological awareness (Gillon 2004).

Phonological processing difficulties mean that children may have problems with making sense of the

acoustic information in speech for both spoken and written language and manifests in a variety of

ways. Phonological awareness problems are one such way and may mean difficulty segmenting

words into sounds (sounding out words), a skill critical to literacy success (Gillon 2004). Other

phonological processing problems may be short-term memory problems or retrieving information

from long-term memory, manifesting in word finding problems. Difficulties with phonological

processing are thought to be causal component of primary speech and/or language impairment,

including specific language impairment and dyslexia (Gillon 2004; Stackhouse & Wells, 1997;

5

2001). What is important to note is that children can have poor phonological processing skills with

apparently normal speech.

Language impairment manifests in a variety of ways. It may affect one or many domains of

language, affecting the reception (comprehension) or expression of words (semantics/vocabulary),

sentences (syntax/grammar), discourse and pragmatics in oral and/or written form. Language

difficulties may be overt and obvious such as a young school-age child with syntax problems who

says “Me go toilet” for “Could I go to the toilet please?” More typically, however, school-age

language difficulties are covert, buried within discourse and pragmatics. Discourse problems result in

poor linkages between sentences resulting in conversations and written work (essays and

composition) that are disorganised and hard for listeners and readers to follow. Pragmatic problems

often mean that the children cannot match the language to the social context and this in turn, may

make them appear uncooperative, insolent or inattentive. The typical pathway of language

impairment that persists into the school years is to overtly manifest as grammar problems in the years

before school and in the early school years and then the pathway is similar to that for speech

difficulties, as language impairment manifests as literacy, academic, social and vocational difficulties

(Gillon & Dodd 1993; Nation, Clarke, Marshall & Durand 2004; Stothard et al. 1998). For both

groups the social difficulties may include the previously stated manifestations of uncooperativeness,

insolence and inattentiveness as well as social isolation and withdrawal, depression and offending

(Bercow 2008; Bryan 2004; Damico & Oller 1980; Snow & Powell 2004).

INCIDENCE AND PREVALENCE OF PRIMARY SPEECH AND LANGUAGE DIFFICULTIES

Primary speech and language impairment is common in childhood with prevalence estimates varying

from 1 to 25% (Blum-Harasty & Rosenthal 1992; Law et al. 2003). Recent studies conducted in

Australia of school age children estimated the prevalence as about 1-20% as displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 The prevalence of speech and language difficulties in Australia

Speech Language

Author Location Prevalence Author Location Prevalence

McKinnon et al.

(2007)

NSW primary school

children

1.1% James (1994) SA metropolitan children

aged 5;0 to 5;5 years

3-13%

Jessup et al.

(2008)

preparatory children

in Tasmania

8.7% Jessup et al.

(2008)

preparatory children in

Tasmania

18.2%

Harasty & Reed

(1994)

NSW primary school

children

18.6% Harasty &

Reed (1994)

NSW primary school children 16%

In South Australia, this means there are about 24,000 children with primary speech and language

impairment1

2 which translates into about 2 to 3 children in every class from reception to year 12

with primary speech language and communication needs. Primary speech and language impairment

is more prevalent than autism (about 0.7%) (Australian Advisory Board on Autism Spectrum

Disorders)3, and cerebral palsy (Access Economics 2008) and about the same as asthma (14-16%)

4

and childhood obesity (about 20%)5.

1 Calculated using a conservative prevalence figure of 10% based on the estimated 248, 573 South Australian school

children as at February 2008 accessed in 2 below 2 http://www.ngss.sa.edu.au/ Tues 9

th September, 2008

3 http://www.autismaus.com.au/aca/pdfs/PrevalenceReport.pdf

4 www.healthinsite.gov.au/topics/Asthma_Statistics

5 www.asso.org.au/freestyler/gui/files//factsheet_children_prevalence.pdf

6

SPEECH PATHOLOGY SERVICES

An array of models of service delivery and interventions exist for school-age children with primary

speech and language impairment. Models of service delivery are the ways agencies deliver their

speech pathology services from an organisational perspective and typically encompass the

organisational values, strategies and goals as well as inputs and intended outcomes (Greenhalgh,

Harvey & Walshe 2005). Models of service delivery include clinical (direct), consultancy (indirect),

collaborative and language based classroom models (Paul 2007). The clinical model involves a

speech pathologist working directly with a child or a group of children having determined the goals

and strategies of the management plan based on an assessment of the children. Within the

consultancy model, the speech pathologist works indirectly with children through another agent such

as teachers, parents and teacher assistants but often is still responsible for determining the

management plan. In the language based classroom model, the speech pathologist is also the teacher,

and again develops the management plan. The collaborative model may combine aspects from all

three models, but is differentiated from these approaches in that the development of the management

plan is usually a collaborative effort with all stakeholders, rather than it being only the speech

pathologist‟s responsibility. However, in practice, the divisions between these delivery models are

not this distinct, that is, different permutations of these models occur.

By contrast, interventions occur within models of service delivery and involve the techniques to

strengthen linguistic skills. An array of interventions techniques exist for all speech and language

domains (Paul 2007).

Speech pathology models of service delivery and interventions are ever evolving. Some of the

drivers of change are:

the gathering momentum within the profession about the application of the World Health

Organization‟s International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) (WHO

2001) to care (e.g. Issues 1 & 2 of the International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology6

2008 were devoted to this topic; Campbell & Skarakis-Doyle 2007). The ICF describes human

functioning at three levels. One level is body function and structures referring to physiological

systems and body parts. So in the context of this review that encompasses speech and language.

The second level is activity and refers to the ability to execute tasks and the third level,

participation, is the ability to participate, to be involved in life activities. So these two levels

encompass the ability to use speech and language to discharge daily activities of communicating.

the gathering momentum to add public health frameworks to models of service delivery (Bercow

2008; Snow 2008a,b,c)

the increasing press for evidence based practice and (Bernstein Ratner 2006; Dodd 2007; Fey

2006; Gillam & Gillam 2006; Kent 2006)

the extensive and growing database indicating children‟s oral language skills are both risk and

protective factors for later literacy, academic, social and vocational success (e.g. Bercow,

Felsenfeld et al. 1992, 1994; Law et al. 2003; Lewis & Freebairn 1992; Snowling et al. 2001;

Stothard et al. 1998).

Given the increasing recognition of the benefits of speech pathology services for children with

primary speech and/or language impairment, it is important to evaluate service delivery models and

interventions for school children (i.e. 5-18 years) in order to ensure access to optimal services in a

timely manner. It is also anticipated that both innovative and varying means of identification and

6 Speech Pathology Australia, the Australian professional body of speech pathologists, publishes this journal. All

members of this association receive this journal.

7

intervention in this age group need consideration because of the possibility that these children may

be undetected.

Although a previous systematic review (Law et al 2003) concluded that speech and language therapy

interventions for children with expressive phonological and expressive vocabulary difficulties had a

positive effect, the review only considered randomised controlled trials and not other experimental

studies. The majority of the studies located for this review were about children aged five years and

under so it was not clear from this review what the intervention outcomes were for school children.

Also, this review did not examine the constructs underpinning service delivery models. Since it is

widely recognised that in addition to knowing “what” works best, it is also important to know “how”

to go about performing what is considered to be best practice; this review aims to investigate the

efficacy of speech pathology services for school children and some of the constructs underpinning

service delivery models.

RESEARCH QUESTIONS

Stage II: What is the evidence underpinning speech and language therapy services for children aged

5-8 years with primary speech and/or language impairment/delay/disorder for improved speech and

communication outcomes?

Stage III: What is the evidence underpinning speech and language therapy services for children aged

9-18 years with primary speech and/or language impairment/delay/disorder for improved speech and

communication outcomes?

CHAPTER 2: METHOD

The following search parameters were determined in consultation with the sub-group of the

Statewide Interagency Speech Pathology Reference Group responsible for supervising this

review.

PECOT (POPULATION, EXPOSURE, COMPARATOR, OUTCOMES, TIME)

P Children (aged 5-8 years or 9-18 years) with primary speech and language problems

(e.g. non-verbal, unintelligible, dyspraxic/apraxic) that are not secondary to a

diagnosed co-morbid condition

E Speech and language therapy including: parent training/education/Hanen, consultative

therapy, teacher training, group therapy, communication partner, school-based

therapy

C Either usual/standard care, placebo or no treatment, or no comparator

O Primary: speech, language, communication skills (including verbal and non-verbal),

communication with others (such as peers, siblings, parents, teachers), articulation,

intelligibility, literacy, quality of life, educational attainment/outcomes, social

outcomes.

Secondary: parents‟ behaviour/responsiveness, attitudes of others (i.e. peers, parents,

teachers)

T Short term (immediate post intervention) and long term outcomes will be considered

if possible. Since speech and language difficulties can have a lasting impact it is

important to consider long term impact of interventions for children, their families

and other stakeholders involved in health and educational service delivery.

SEARCH STRATEGY

A literature search of peer-reviewed databases was conducted to identify all levels of

evidence-based research. Included were any studies that reported speech or communication

outcome data (or secondary parental outcome data) in relation to speech and/or language

intervention services for children, aged 5-18 years, with primary speech and/or language

impairment. Participants must have had a primary speech and/or language problem or at least

be at risk of such problems, with no diagnosis of any co-morbid conditions that impact on

their speech or communication abilities.

The search yielded a relevant systematic review (Cirrin and Gillam 2008), which was used as

a basis for developing a search strategy that aimed to identify:

(1) contemporary publications published after the search (1985-2006) conducted by

Cirrin and Gillam (2008) and

(2) additional publications (primarily speech focus) not included in the Cirrin and

Gillam (2008) review.

SEARCH TERMS STAGE II & III COMBINED

Term 1 AND Term 2 AND Term 3 NOT Term 4

Language OR

Speech OR

Phonology* OR

Intervention OR

Therapy OR

Treat* OR

Program*

School* OR

Child* OR

Student*

Autis* OR ADHD OR Inattent* OR Hyperactiv*

OR Stutt* OR Voice OR Deaf* OR Hearing OR

Intellectual OR Global OR Cerebral Palsy OR

Down* OR ESL OR second language

9

Databases

AMED A+ Education

Campbell Collaboration CINAHL

Education (SAGE) Education abstracts

ERIC Exceptional child Education Resources

Health Source: Nursing PsychARTICLES

Medline Psychology (SAGE)

PsychINFO ScienceDirect

Science Citation Index What Works Clearinghouse

Social Science Citation Index Linguistics and language behaviour

abstracts

SEARCH LIMITS

Publication type: outcome based research paper

Year of publication: 1995 onwards

Language: English

Age: 5-8 year olds (Stage II)

9-18 year olds (Stage III)

SEARCH VALIDATION

Three journals were manually searched using the search terms and limits listed above:

Advances in Speech-Language Pathology (now International Journal in Speech-Language

Pathology)

Australian Communication Quarterly (now ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech-Language

Pathology)

The New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy

Pearling also occurred to ensure relevant articles were not missed. Pearling involves scanning

the reference lists of the identified articles.

DATA EXTRACTION

Once the relevant publications were identified and retrieved, they were summarised using a

standardised data extraction form that included reference details, country of publication, level

of evidence (NHMRC 2000), study aims, population, service setting, service delivery model,

service provider, language domain targeted, intervention type and method, the quantum of

service provision (frequency/length/duration/total number of intervention sessions), study

outcomes, author‟s conclusion and reviewer‟s conclusion. The data were extracted by three

reviewers following reliability checks of 10% of the included papers.

Using meta-narrative analysis, the extracted information was synthesised to summarise the

current evidence for the efficacy of speech and language services for children aged 5-18

years with primary speech and/or language impairment as well as the constructs underpinning

service delivery models.

10

CHAPTER 3: RESULTS

The search located 97 potential articles; 82 from the peer-reviewed databases, 10 from the

manual search and five from pearling. Stage I articles were included if the number of children

five years and over could be ascertained from all participants in the study and there was more

than one participant. Two articles from Stage 1 met these requirements and were included.

The sub-group of the Statewide Interagency Speech Pathology Reference Group and the first

two authors scrutinised the 82 articles from the peer-reviewed databases and rejected those

that Cirrin and Gillam (2008) reviewed and those they judged outside the search parameters

based on the information in the title and abstract. The full texts of the remaining 45 articles

were retrieved along with nine of the ten articles located manually and all five from pearling.

Eleven more articles were rejected leaving a final pool of 50 articles, including the two from

Stage I.

DESCRIPTION OF THE STUDIES

The 50 papers comprised 48 studies of primary research and two systematic reviews. The

search for this review, coupled with the searches from the two systematic reviews means that

the literature from 1872 to 2008 was searched. This report captures the findings of 105

studies because the systematic reviews included 57 articles.

COUNTRY OF ORIGIN

All papers were published in the western world, as displayed in Table 2, with about a quarter

from Australia and New Zealand. None of the research was from South Australia.

TABLE 2 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY COUNTRY

Country Number of publications

Australia 8

Canada 2

Germany 1

New Zealand 4

The Netherlands 1

United Kingdom 17

United States of America 17

Total 50

LEVEL OF EVIDENCE AND QUALITY OF RESEARCH

Most papers (47/50) were quantitative in design and were Level III-2 and III-3 (NHMRC

2000), as displayed in Table 3. The two systematic reviews drew on different types of

evidence. Law, Garret & Nye (2003) only used the equivalent of Level II evidence (NHMRC

2000) whereas Cirrin and Gillam (2008) used the equivalents of Levels I to III-3 (NHMRC

2000). Law et al. (2003) located 36 randomised controlled trials (RCTs) published between

1872 to September 2002 and Cirrin and Gillam (2008) located 21 experimental studies within

their specified time frame of 1985 to 2006.

All articles except three appeared in peer-reviewed journals. Two articles appeared in the

Australian Communication Quarterly which was not a peer-reviewed publication. One article

appeared in ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing; whereby the authors

exercised the choice of not having their article peer reviewed.

11

TABLE 3FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY LEVEL OF EVIDENCE

NATURE OF EVIDENCE Number of publications

NHMRC level of evidence

I Systematic reviews 2

II Randomised control trial 2

III-1 Pseudo-randomised controlled trials 6

III-2 Comparative studies with a control group 10

III-3 Comparative studies with historical control 11

IV Case series, or post-test or pre-test/post-test. 10

Other forms of evidence

Case study of N=1 6

Qualitative case studies 2

Retrospective audit of clinical files 1

Follow up of medium N study 1

Qualitative case series 1

Note Two articles included more than one study

THE NATURE OF THE SERVICE DELIVERY

The 48 primary research papers addressed different aspects of service delivery. Most papers

(40) focussed solely on different interventions for various linguistic skills and, by default

addressed the clinical or direct model of service delivery whereby speech pathologists work

with children individually or in groups. An example of an intervention is core vocabulary for

phonology (Crosbie et al. 2005). Within these 40 papers, 35 reported short-term linguistic

gains, one reported the service quantum children received, one reported parent‟s perceptions

of their children‟s gains and three reported long-term gains.

Eight papers addressed models of service delivery and interventions together. Boyle et al‟s

(2007) paper compared direct and indirect interventions using a RCT. Three papers reported

indirect services whereby school assistants (Gardner 2006), parents (Dale et al. 1996) and

teachers were in-serviced about intervention techniques (Hay et al. 2007). Two papers

involved language classroom based models (Hadley et al. 2000; Richardson & Kleckan-Aker

2000). The final two papers reported on collaborative models that involved joint services

between health and education authorities (Roulstone et al. 2005; Wren et al. 2001).

SAMPLE SIZES

The 48 primary research studies encompassed 2,376 participants with sample sizes varying

from 1-234. Most studies had 10 or less participants (31/48). The most frequent sample size

was one participant (6 studies). As sample sizes were relatively small, this may affect the

degree to which some of the studies are able show significant effects.

THE AGE OF PARTICIPANTS

The age range of participants was 2-16 years. Consequently, this review cannot provide

evidence about intervention for children between the ages of 17 to 18 years. By contrast, it

may include children below the age of 5 years because it was not possible to determine the

proportion of participants across all studies that were younger than 5 years. As about a

quarter of the studies (14/48) included children who were less than 5 years, the proportion of

children younger than 5 years could be sizeable. The presence of and the unknown proportion

of under 5-year-olds is a potential confound within this review.

12

This same pattern was apparent in the 25 studies that Law et al. (2003) reviewed as 13 studies

addressed children younger than 5 years and six studies included children with a lower age

range of less than 5 years and an upper age range of 5 or 6 years. Only the remaining studies

exclusively included school age children. While Cirrin and Gillam reviewed (2008) studies

about schoolchildren, 19 of the 21 studies they reviewed were about children younger than 10

years.

Thus, the greatest number of participants were at the lower end of the range set for this

review. This bias towards children younger than the age of 10 years is another potential

confound within this review.

THE NATURE OF THE IMPAIRMENTS

All 48 primary research studies contained children with primary speech and language

impairments. Some studies included children with impairments outside this range (e.g.

Hadley et al. 2000) and such studies were only included if the participants with primary

speech and language impairments were isolated from the other participants. The two

systematic reviews only included participants with primary speech and language

impairments. Law et al. (2003) addressed children with primary speech and language

impairments whereas Cirrin and Gillam (2008) only included school-aged participants with

primary language difficulties.

TYPES OF OUTCOME MEASURES

Studies reporting the outcomes of interventions measured children‟s linguistic outcomes with

formal, standardised tests, criterion referenced tests, developmental scores and behavioural

observations. The measuring tools provided information about aspects of expressive,

metalinguistic7 and receptive language functioning in pragmatics, discourse, semantics,

syntax, and phonology, either separately or as composite scores. Generally, each study used

different outcome measures, limiting comparisons across studies. Interviews and surveys of

parents, teachers and assistants were used to measure children‟s linguistic functioning

indirectly. Secondary measures such as self esteem were taken.

Studies reporting the outcomes of service delivery models reported outcomes about the

identified stakeholders and organisational features. Stakeholders included children, parents,

teachers, assistants, speech pathologists and authorities (e.g. health and education).

Organisational features included referral processes, redeployment of staff time, meetings and

classroom practices. Measuring tools included interviews, questionnaires and surveys.

KEY RESULTS

The key results about service delivery are presented first, with the results about intervention

efficacy following. The results are summarised in Appendix 1. Appendix 2 contains a full

summary of each of the articles, while Appendix 3 is a summary of the articles that Cirrin

and Gillam reviewed. The articles Law et al. (2003) reviewed are freely available on the

internet. Chapter 4 discusses the efficacy of service delivery models.

7 Metalinguistics is the language of language (i.e. linguistics). Metalinguistic ability is the skills to reflect on

language and describe it. It encompasses skills such as phonological awareness (sounding out words, playing

with rhyme), defining words, parsing sentences into nouns and verbs etc and editing. Metalinguistic ability is a

critical skill for literacy acquisition (Paul 2007)

13

SERVICE DELIVERY

The following points about service delivery relate to the 48 primary research articles. The

two systematic reviews are not included.

PROVIDER/INTERVENTION AGENTS

Speech pathologists8 were the agent in 41 of the papers. This was clear in 33 papers and

assumed in another eight as the authors provided the intervention without detailing their

qualifications. Other agents included teachers, nurses and speech pathology assistants either

alone or in combination with a speech pathologist. In six studies, whilst the speech

pathologist was the provider, the service they provided was negotiated as a part of a team

within the school context. One study (Boyle et al. 2007) compared the service provided by

speech pathologists and speech pathology assistants trained by speech pathologists. There

were no studies of speech pathologists providing services in collaboration with other allied

health professionals such as physiotherapists or occupational therapists.

SERVICE SETTING

Schools were the main service setting (26/48) followed by clinics (community/medical/not

specified) (16) and then university clinics (6). In one study, the families had the choice of

intervention occurring at home, school or a clinic (Swanson et al. 2005). A school summer

camp was the setting in another study (Pokorni et al. 2004).

THERAPY GOALS

Interventions addressed many aspects of speech and language, as detailed in Table 4. Most

studies had a single intervention focus of either speech (24) or language (19) but six studies

addressed both speech and language.

TABLE 4 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY INTERVENTION GOALS

Domain Number of studies

Language

Pragmatics 5

Discourse 1

Syntax

Comprehension and expression

Expression only

3

2

Vocabulary

Vocabulary expansion

Word retrieval

1

2

Combination of at least 2+ of above

(excluding discourse)

5

Total Language 19

Phonology

1. Output (i.e. production/pronunciation) 15

2. Phonological awareness 6

3. Both 3

Total Phonology 24

Phonology + language; output and/or

processing

6

Total 49

Note: There is a mismatch because one study was categorised across two domains

8 The term speech pathologist is used throughout the document for the sake of consistency whilst being

cognisant that terms such as speech-language pathologist, speech-language therapist and speech therapist are

used by authors of the articles.

14

Language goals were relatively even across receptive and expressive pragmatics, syntax and

semantics but not discourse. Pragmatic goals included adapting language to the context.

Syntax goals included morphology, passives, relative clauses and complex sentences.

Semantic goals included improving word retrieval.

Phonology output goals occurred more frequently than phonological processing goals. Output

goals included error patterns (phonological processes), or individual sounds such as fricatives

(e.g. /v/). Phonological processing goals included phonological awareness (e.g. rhyming,

phoneme deletion) and enhancing processing speed.

INTERVENTION TYPE

There was an array of speech and language interventions across the studies, summarised in

Table 5. One trend was the use of published materials; virtual or real. The most frequently

occurring language intervention was teaching participants to adapt their communication to

the context; be they children or adults. Three interventions dominated phonology; cycles,

contrast interventions and teaching phonological awareness to affect output phonology.

Within phonological processing, interventions involved phonological awareness and

enhanced speech. No articles reported the use of augmentative or alternative communication

techniques in intervention9.

TABLE 5 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY INTERVENTION TYPE

Domain Type of intervention Study

Code*

N studies

Language Adapting communicative context

1. Children adapt their communication to the context

2. Significant adults alter their communication patterns with

children (e.g. using a system of levels of questions)

1, 2, 3, 4, 38

13, 20, 24,

25

9

Semantic and phonological elaboration 6 1

Mixed across word, sentence and discourse levels for

comprehension & expression

7 1

Discourse to effect discourse, syntax &/or semantics 43, 45 2

Metalinguistic 8 1

Script therapy + modelling + a published package Reading

Milestones

41 1

Computer software 10 1

Colour and shapes 16, 17, 18 3

Imitation vs conversational recast 23, 34 2

Phonology Psycholinguistic approach 35 1

Cycles

Cycles + interactive routines, play scripts

Cycles vs Parents and children together

Cycles + integrated phoneme awareness treatment

12

37

30

3

Traditional vs phonological 29 1

Integrated phoneme awareness treatment (Gillon) 14, 32 2

Contrast therapy

Minimal vs maximal contrasts

Minimal vs maximal contrasts vs naturalist speech

intelligibility

Contrast therapy vs core vocabulary

15

46

11

3

Traditional 19 1

Conventional vs phonological awareness training 42 1

Adapting the communicative context 20 1

9 This insight arose from an informal discussion with Catherine Olsson (19/09/2008)

15

Integral stimulation dynamic temporal cueing 44

Phonological

processing

Tabletop vs computer activities vs no Rx 48 1

Phonological awareness general 33 1

Enhanced speech

Enhanced speech vs not enhanced

Fast ForWord

Fast ForWord vs enhanced speech vs LiPs

40

31

36

3

Language &

phonology

LiPs + published syntax and semantic resources 21, 22 2

Not described (focus on service delivery model) 39, 47 2

Others Follow up/audit 6, 9, 27, 28,

25

5

Total studies 48

Key: *See Appendix 1 for code

NUMBER OF CHILDREN IN INTERVENTION SESSIONS

Intervention occurred on an individual basis in 36/48 (75%) studies, in groups 9/48 (20%)

studies and in combination in 3/48 (5%) studies.

DOSAGE

SESSION DURATION

Most sessions were 31-60 minutes (52%), 39% of them were 30 minutes or less and some

(9%) were over an hour. These details are summarised in Table 6.

TABLE 6 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION ACCORDING TO SESSION DURATION

Time quantum

(in minutes)

Language Phonology Both

Study code No Study code No Study code No

0-15 0 0 0 0

15 43 1 10, 31, 40 3 0 4

16-29 0 0 0 0

30 mins 8, 18 2 11, 15, 44, 46, 30 5 26 1 8

31-44 7 1 0 0 1

45 16 1 19, 32 2 0 3

46-59 23, 45 2 30, 37 2 29 1 5

60 1, 2, 5, 17b 4 33, 35, 36 3 0 7

61-74 0 0 0 0

75+ 4 1 14, 42 2 0 3

Subtotal 12 17 2 31

Not stated 3, 13, 17a, 34, 38, 41 6 12, 20, 21, 22 4 27, 39, 47 3 13

Not relevant 9, 24, 25 3 6 1 28 1 5

Totals 21 22 6 49

Key: See Appendix 1 for study code

In the studies about collaborative and indirect models of service delivery, the agent‟s time

was deployed in a number of ways. Speech pathologists engaged in (1) weekly planning

conferences with the classroom teacher and in the classroom with the teacher on a 0.5 basis

(Hadley et al. 2000), (2) spent about half of their time with children in direct service delivery

and the other half with parents, teachers and other project staff (Roulstone et al. 2005) or (3)

spent one day a week in liaison with other team members in addition to providing direct

services (Boyle et al. 2007). In another model, the agent, a language support teacher, spent

two hours per week with the classroom teacher, teaching the teacher interactive techniques

(Hay et al. 2007).

16

FREQUENCY OF SESSIONS

Most sessions occurred more often than weekly, varying from daily to fortnightly, as

displayed in Table 7. Most sessions (74%) occurred 2-15 times a week, 37% occurred weekly

and 7% occurred fortnightly. No study provided sessions more than a fortnight apart.

TABLE 7 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY FREQUENCY OF SESSIONS

Language Phonology Both Total

N Weeks Study code N Study code Weeks Study code N

2-3 times per day> 0 36, 44 2 0 2

Daily 43 1 31 1 0 2

3 times per week 1, 2, 7, 45 4 10, 32, 37, 40 4 0 8

2 times per week 8, 23, 24 3 11, 19, 29, 30, 35, 42, 46, 7 27* 1 10

Weekly 3, 4, 5, 12, 1,

18, 41

7 14, 15, 20, 21, 22, 24, 25,

33, 48

9 24, 25 2 16

Every two weeks 13 1 0 39# 1 2

Could not be

classified

38, 47 2 6, 7, 28 3 0 5

Totals 18 26 4 48

Key: *Varied from 2 times per week to < weekly; # weekly or fortnightly

NUMBER OF SESSIONS

The usual quantum was ten sessions, however this varied from three to 60 sessions in the 32

studies that detailed this information. Most studies provided 5 to 14 sessions as detailed in

Table 8.

TABLE 8 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION ACCORDING TO THE TOTAL NUMBER OF SESSIONS PROVIDED

Frequency Language Phonology Both

Study Code No Study Code No Study Code No

1-4 13 (3 sessions) 1 0 0 1

5-9 17, 18 2 14, 15, 20, 32, 33, 42 6 0 7

10-14 3, 4, 5, 25 4 10, 30, 40 3 0 7

15-19 8, 34, 45 3 0 0 3

20 0 31, 48 2 0 2

25 43 1 0 0 1

30 1, 2 2 11, 35 2 0 4

35 7 1 0 0 1

40 16 1 0 0 1

45 0 44# 1 0 1

50 0 29 1 0 1

55 0 0 0 0

60 0 36, 37, 46 3 0 3

Could not be

classified

9, 23, 24, 38, 39, 41,

47

7 6, 12, 19*, 21, 22 5 26, 27, 28 3 16

22 23 3 48

Key: *Varied per child; # midpoint

DURATION OF SERVICE

The usual service duration was 5-9 weeks (20/40 studies), varying from 3 weeks to 6; 11

years (83 months) in the 40 studies that detailed this information. This information is

summarised in Table 9.

17

TABLE 9 FREQUENCY OF PUBLICATION BY INTERVENTION DURATION

Language Phonology Both Total N

Weeks Study code N Study code N Study code N

1-4 0 5, 31, 32, 36, 42 5 5

5-9 1, 2, 3, 4, 8, 13, 18, 34,

38, 43, 45

11 10, 14, 16, 20, 30,

33, 40, 44, 48

9 20

10-14 17, 25 2 21, 22 2 4

15-19 7 1 0 1

20 0 11, 29, 37 3 3

25 24 1 12 1 2

30 16 1 46 1 2

35 0 35# 1 1

> 1 year 0 41 (3 years) 1 27* 1 2

Not

classified

9, 39 2 6, 19, 23 3 26, 28 47 3 8

Totals 18 26 4 48

Key: *varied from 1-82 months; # 15 weeks of intervention spread over 9 months

EFFICACY OF THE INTERVENTIONS

The following points about efficacy of the interventions relate to all 50 studies; the 48

primary research articles and the two systematic reviews. Efficacy is described in terms of (1)

effectiveness, that is, whether the change occurred due to intervention, (2) effects, that is the

type and extent of change and (3) efficiency, that is, which intervention involved the least

amount of time.

EFFECTIVENESS AND EFFECTS

Whilst at least one positive outcome followed intervention in 49 of the 50 studies, it was only

clear that intervention caused that change in some studies.

OUTPUT PHONOLOGY

Nine of the 18 studies about output phonology had adequate controls to isolate the

intervention effects. In seven of those nine studies, intervention was effective (Crosbie et al.

2005; Forrest & Elbert 2001; Long & Gillon 2007; Moriarty & Gillon 2006; Smith et al.

1998; Strand et al. 2006; Williams 2000) but it made no difference beyond maturation in the

other two studies (Denne et al. 2005; Wren & Roulstone 2008). As these studies varied in the

levels of evidence from Level I to other, there is reasonable but limited evidence to support

output phonology intervention when:

1. Speech pathologists provided the intervention on a 1:1 basis. Individual intervention

occurred in the seven studies reporting effective intervention (Crosbie et al, 2005; Forrest

& Elbert 2001; Long & Gillon 2007; Moriarty & Gillon 2006; Smith et al. 1998; Strand et

al. 2006; Williams 2000). In one of the two studies reporting that intervention made no

difference, school assistants conducted two of the three weekly sessions after conducting

one jointly with the speech pathologist (Wren & Roulstone 2008) and in the other study,

intervention occurred on a 1:3 ratio (Denne et al. 2005). Collectively, these findings

imply that intervention provided by speech pathologists on a 1:1 basis is a critical success

factor for phonology output intervention.

2. The intervention dosage was (1) at least twice weekly in (2) sessions that were no longer

than 50 minutes (3) with a duration of 5 to 9 weeks but flexibility to extend. This dosage

quantum occurred in the seven studies reporting effective intervention. When one 1.5

hour session occurred weekly for 8 weeks, Denne et al. (2005) reported a no-intervention

effect. Collectively, these findings imply dosage factors are critical in the success of

phonology output intervention. Specifically, they imply that sessions need to be delivered

more frequently than weekly and for no longer than 50 minutes per session.

18

3. Intervention that integrates phonological awareness and word production work. This

occurred in five of the nine studies (Crosbie et al 2005; Forrest & Elbert 2001; Long &

Gillon 2007; Moriarty & Gillon 2006; Williams 2000). In particular, word production

work focuses on reducing homophony rather than on the correction of individual sounds.

Homophony is a common occurrence in speech impairment whereby children say many

different words identically, compromising listeners‟ comprehension of what it said.

Phonology gains may be sustained beyond the conclusion of intervention but the evidence is

mixed. Bernhardt and Major (2005) followed up twelve 7-year-olds who had received

intervention for output phonology when they were 3-4 years of age and found that all

children were intelligible and had normal language scores as measured by the PPVT and the

CELF. Ten of them had normal reading skills and seven had normal spelling scores. Dodd et

al. (2008) reported that 14 of their 19 participants had maintained the gains from contrast

intervention measured 14 weeks after the intervention had ceased. Long and Gillon (2007)

reported that a boy, aged 5 years maintained the changes in speech (and phonological

awareness) five months later. However, although Wren & Roulstone (2008) reported

phonology increased over the study period, they found no intervention effect as there were no

differences between the control and experimental groups at the conclusion of the intervention

period or three months later.

The effects of intervention were measured in a variety of ways and included consistency of

word production (Crosbie et al. 2005), percentage of phonemes correct (Crosbie et al. 2005;

Long & Gillon 2007; Moriarty & Gillon 2006), correct underlying representations (the level

of adult like production of phonemes) (Williams 2000), individual phoneme accuracy

(Forrest & Elbert 2001) and whole words (Strand et al. 2006). Whilst Smith et al. (1998) used

the South Tyneside Test of Phonology to measure phonology output, they did not describe the

outcome measure.

PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING

Seven of the 11 studies on phonological processing had adequate controls to isolate the

intervention effects. In four of those seven studies, intervention was effective but it made no

difference for three studies. As these studies varied in the levels of evidence from Level I to

other, there is reasonable but limited evidence to support phonological processing

intervention it its focus is phonological awareness. All four studies which aimed to develop

phonological awareness reported effectiveness (Cirrin & Gillam 2008; Hadley et al. 2000;

Munro & Atkinson 2003; Long & Gillon 2007). Further, phonological awareness intervention

was effective when provided individually (Long & Gillon 2007), in a group (Munro &

Atkinson 2003) or in a classroom (Hadley et al. 2000), either weekly or more frequently.

If the focus of intervention is to improve phonological processing through intense exposure to

slowed speech (acoustic properties manipulated), then intervention seems to make no

difference because the four studies investigating this impact reported no effectiveness (Cirrin

& Gillam 2008; Marler et al. 2001; Pokorni et al. 2004; Segers & Verhoeven 2004).

The evidence that phonological processing skills are sustained after intervention ceases is

mixed. The one study reporting on phonological awareness indicated gains five months post

intervention (Long & Gillon 2007), whereas the one study about phonological processing did

not show any gains four months post intervention (Segers & Verhoeven 2004).

19

PRAGMATICS

Three of the seven studies addressing pragmatics had adequate controls indicating that the

changes were attributable to intervention (Adams et al. 2006; Adams & Lloyd 2007; Cirrin &

Gillam 2008). As these studies varied in the levels of evidence from Level I to IV, there is

some limited evidence to support pragmatic intervention in the short term when intervention

is conducted by a speech pathologist on an individual basis. However, there was no evidence

to indicate if these gains were sustained over time.

The effects of the intervention measured, included whether children responded in a socially

appropriate way within conversations (Adams et al. 2006; Adams & Lloyd 2007).

Specifically, Adams and colleagues determined whether children were over- or under-

dominant in conversation and whether they could “read between the lines”.

DISCOURSE

One of the three studies addressing discourse had adequate controls indicating that the

changes were attributable to intervention (Cirrin & Gillam 2008). This systematic review

(Level I) study provides strong but limited evidence to support discourse intervention. There

was no evidence about whether gains in discourse were sustained over time.

SYNTAX

Seven of the 13 studies described in 11 articles had adequate controls to determine the

influence of syntax intervention (Cirrin & Gillam 2008; Ebbels & van der Lely 2001; Ebbels

2007 (Studies 1 & 2); Ebbels et al. 2007; Law et al. 2003; Nelson et al. 1996). In all of those

studies, intervention was reported as effective. The following results are highlighted when

considering the collective evidence.

1. The conclusions of the two systematic reviews were that there was only evidence to

support expressive syntax intervention not receptive syntax.

2. Ebbels and colleagues (Ebbels & van der Lely 2001; Ebbels 2007; Ebbels et al. 2007)

reported effective intervention for receptive and expressive syntax for children aged 11 to

16 years. In all studies, a speech pathologist provided the intervention. Reported increases

in children‟s comprehension and use of syntactic construction included questions (wh-

and comparative), passives, dative case (the cow is giving the pig the sheep) and past

tense markers.

As these studies varied in the levels of evidence from Level I to IV, there is reasonable but

limited evidence to support syntax receptive and expressive intervention when intervention is

conducted by a speech pathologist on an individual basis. Further, the studies by Ebbels and

colleagues imply that the intervention is predicated on in-depth language analysis to yield a

highly individualised care package indicate and that it needs to occur in a specialised school

for children with specific language impairment.

There was no evidence about whether syntax gains were sustained.

SEMANTICS

Six of the 10 semantics studies had adequate controls to determine the influence of

intervention. In five of these six studies, intervention was effective (Bayne & Moran 2005;

Cirrin & Gillam 2008; Hadley et al 2000; Law et al. 2003; Steigler & Hoffman 2001). The

following results are highlighted for considering the collective evidence.

1. Expansion of expressive semantics intervention was supported in all three studies

examining this (Cirrin & Gillam 2008; Hadley et al. 2000; Law et al. 2003)

2. Word retrieval was enhanced in the two studies examining this skill (Bayne & Moran

2005; Steigler & Hoffman 2001)

20

3. Support for receptive vocabulary intervention was mixed with support in a no-

intervention control group study (Hadley et al. 2000) but not in an RCT (Boyle et al.

2007).

As these studies varied in the levels of evidence from Level I to IV, there is reasonable but

limited evidence to support semantic intervention when delivered by a speech pathologist

directly or in a collaborative situation (Hadley et al. 2000). As Boyle et al. (2007) reported no

changes to receptive vocabulary on the PPVT, this finding suggests that the program they

developed may not have been effective for enhancing receptive vocabulary (see pg 26 for the

conditions).

There was no evidence that semantics gains were sustained.

COMPOSITE LANGUAGE OUTCOMES

One of the five studies expressing outcomes in composite scores (combined score of more

than one language domain) had adequate controls to determine the influence of intervention

(Boyle et al. 2007). This level II study reported effectiveness for expressive language but not

receptive language with changes sustained over 12 months.

EFFICIENCY

OUTPUT PHONOLOGY

Five of the 19 studies about output phonology compared interventions. Core vocabulary

intervention resulted in more speech changes for 10 4- to 6-year-olds with inconsistent

speech impairment than consistent speech impairment. However, contrast therapy resulted in

more speech changes for eight 4- to 6-year-olds with consistent speech impairment than

inconsistent speech impairment (Crosbie et al. 2005). Minimal and maximal contrast

interventions were equally effective for all 19 3- to 6-year-olds with phonological disorder

(Dodd et al. 2008). Phonological therapy was more efficient than traditional therapy for 36

children with multiple articulation errors, aged 3;0 to 5;10 years (Klein 1996). Conventional

therapy combined with phonological awareness training altered output for nine of the 18 5- to

8-year-olds with persistent specific phonological disorder (Smith et al. 1998).

The final study has significant implication for schoolchildren. Kirk and Gillon (2007)

concluded that children with speech impairment who received intervention that integrated

phonological awareness with production work as 4-year-olds had better long-term literacy

outcomes as 8-year-olds than their peers whose intervention only focussed on sound

production and excluded phonological awareness work.

PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING

Three of the 12 studies about phonological processing compared interventions. They showed

that children did not receive a phonological processing advantage from working with

proprietary software using enhanced (slowed) speech over normal speech, vocabulary work

or phonological awareness work (Marler et al. 2001; Pokorni et al. 2004; Segers &

Verhoeven 2004). More specifically, the proprietary software Fast ForWord did not deliver

an advantage.

However, with respect to phonological awareness, Pokorni et al. (2004) reported that the

LiPS and Earobics programmes improved phonological awareness whereas Fast ForWord

did not. Also, Earobics, seemed most effective for enhancing phoneme segmentation whereas

LiPs, seemed most effective for enhancing phoneme blending and segmentation.

21

SYNTAX

Three of the 10 primary syntax research studies compared interventions. Conversational

recasts10

resulted in quicker gains in syntax than imitation across 14 children with language

impairment, aged 4-6 years and seven typically developing control children, aged 2-4 years

(Gillum et al. 2003; Nelson et al. 1996). By contrast, syntactic-semantic therapy and semantic

therapy11

appear equally effective in fostering receptive and expressive syntax for children

with specific language impairment, aged 11 to 16 years but superior to the usual package of

care with respect to syntax (Ebbels et al. 2007).

PRAGMATICS, DISCOURSE, SEMANTICS AND COMPOSITE LANGUAGE

None of the studies about pragmatics, discourse, semantics and composite language

compared different interventions so there is no evidence about efficiency from this review.

10

Conversational recasts occur when an agent repeats children‟s immature or erroneous sentences in a more

correct way to model to the child accurate production. Usually there is not expectation for the child to respond

however extensive studies of typically developing children indicate they do this spontaneously whilst acquiring

language. 11

See the descriptions of these interventions in the full description of the articles authored by Ebbels and

colleagues in Appendix 2

CHAPTER 4: DISCUSSION

The studies located for this review were heterogeneous with regards to many of the variables

under consideration such as service delivery model, the intervention and intervention goals.

This limits the generalities that can be distilled from them. For example, whilst output

phonology was the focus of many articles, it was rare that the same intervention was used and

if the same intervention was used, the goals were not, limiting comparisons. So whilst some

generalities are determined, practitioners will need to consider this heterogeneity when

applying the findings of this review to their local situation.

EFFICACY OF INTERVENTIONS

Overall, intervention was efficacious for children aged 5 to 16 years with primary speech and

or language impairment indicated by higher and lower levels of evidence.

EFFECTIVENESS AND EFFECTS

The higher level evidence (systematic reviews and RCTS) (Boyle et al. 2007; Ebbels et al.

2007; Cirrin & Gillam 2008; Law et al. 2003) indicates that speech and language intervention

was responsible for the intended changes in children‟s speech and language.

The lower level evidence (non-randomised control studies through to single subject case

series studies) implied that intervention was associated with improvements in children‟s

outcomes ranging from increased sound accuracy, phonological awareness, discourse,

receptive and expressive semantics, word retrieval and receptive and expressive syntax.

On balance, the evidence seems to support output phonology and expressive vocabulary

intervention but is mixed for phonological processing, receptive vocabulary and receptive

and expressive syntax intervention. The evidence for discourse and pragmatics intervention is

small in amount but positive.

EFFICIENCY OF THE INTERVENTIONS

Given some phonology and syntax interventions seemed more efficient than others, aspects

of the relevant studies are further considered.

Whilst Pokorni et al. (2004) reported that the proprietary packages of Earobics and LiPs

resulted in differential effects for phonological awareness the opportunity costs of their

program are noteworthy. The children appeared to achieve small gains for considerable

expenditure of time, as each child received 60 hours of intervention over 20 days. Whilst this

study is not directly comparable with other studies about phonological awareness, these

findings of minimal gains for 60 hours of intervention contrast with Gillon and colleagues‟

findings (Gillon & Dodd 1995; 1998; Kirk & Gillon 2007; Long & Gillon 2007; Gillon &

Moriarty 2006) who achieved significant effects in phonological awareness with packages of

6 to 27 hours.

The theme common to the studies that Gillon and colleagues (Gillon & Dodd 1995; 1998;

Kirk & Gillon 2007; Long & Gillon 2007; Gillon & Moriarty 2006) conducted is that

production and phonological awareness work were integrated and this may have been the key

to success in phonological awareness and production. This notion that children need a

multidimensional input that includes information about the motoric (movement) and

phonological information is well supported in the theoretical phonology literature (e.g.

Stackhouse & Wells 1997; 2001) and other intervention literature (e.g. Baker 2000 and see

23

Stage 1 of this review). However, it is noteworthy that Denne et al. (2005) did not achieve the

same outcomes as Gillon and colleagues did when using the program that Gillon and

colleagues developed. However, Denne et al. (2005) implemented this intervention with at

least three variations that may account for these outcome differences. They conducted all

sessions with in groups of three children rather than in individual and group sessions.

Sessions were weekly rather than 2 to 3 times per week. And finally, sessions ran for 1.5

hours rather than no longer than 50 minutes. These three differences in service delivery may

be the critical features for successful phonological intervention.

Dodd et al. (2008) found no differences when using minimal or maximal pairs in

phonological intervention. This may have significant implications for planning time for

children‟s intervention. Minimal pairs require much greater planning time than maximal pairs

because minimal pairs demand a greater level of customising the intervention words than

maximal pairs. For example, if a child substitutes „t‟ for „s‟ in words (sat, sack, seal, said as

tat, tack and teal respectively), the minimal pair approach dictates that the only rhyming

words that can be used are those staring with „t‟, requiring the speech pathologist to select a

custom group of words for that child. This also means that this package of words could not be

used again with another child who erred on the same words but substituted „s‟ with „ch‟ and

said chat chack and cheal instead. By contrast, the maximal pair approach requires a broader

group of rhyming words to be used. So for the child who substitutes „t‟ for „s‟, the

intervention words could include chat, mat bat and cat in addition to tat (and so on for all the

other intervention words).This means that this same group of words can be used for the child

in the second example, thereby saving considerable planning time.

The evidence that Ebbels and colleagues (Ebbels 2007; Ebbels & van der Lely 2001; Ebbels

et al. 2007) presented about positive outcomes for receptive syntax is at variance with the

conclusions of the two systematic reviews that receptive language was resistant to

intervention (Law et al. 2003; Cirrin & Gillam 2008). Whilst the studies within the reviews

are not comparable with those of Ebbels and colleagues, the fact that Ebbels and colleagues

achieved positive outcomes may indicate that their technique was effective in promoting

receptive syntax. The three steps in their technique involved; segmenting sentences into

grammatical components (such as subject and verbs), linking each component with a specific

shape of a particular colour then manipulating the coloured shapes, and therefore the

grammar components to develop their comprehension of grammar. Whilst their findings need

replication to increase confidence in them, their findings may suggest that the use of coloured

shapes as codes in syntax intervention is a key success factor.

Given this success in receptive syntax intervention, it is interesting to note that Gillon and

colleagues (Gillon & Dodd 1995, 1998; Kirk & Gillon 2007; Long & Gillon 2007; Gillon &

Moriarty 2006) and Denne et al. (2005) used a similar technique and steps to advance

children‟s phonology. Specifically, the children were engaged in tasks to segment words into

sounds, link each sound to different coloured blocks (shapes) and then manipulate the

coloured blocks and therefore the sounds of the words to enhance phonology. Gillon and

colleagues reported associated changes in phonological awareness and output phonology in

the short term (Gillon & Dodd 1995, 1998; Long & Gillon 2007; Gillon & Moriarty 2006)

and literacy in the long term (Kirk & Gillon 2007). Denne et al. (2005) using similar

techniques and steps reported improved outcomes for children‟s phonological awareness but

not for output phonology. Thus taking the findings of the Gillon and colleagues and Ebbels

and colleagues together may indicate that representing components of linguistic structures

24

with coloured shapes and then manipulating those shapes may be a critical feature of

successful intervention in phonology and language.

SERVICE DELIVERY CONSTRUCTS

Given the studies that focus on interventions has now been considered, taken together,

generalities can be distilled from these studies about interventions that relate to the efficacy

of models of service delivery.

EFFICACY OF DIRECT MODELS OF SERVICE DELIVERY

Direct, clinical services for speech and language appear to be effective. They were the most

frequent type of service occurring in 39 of the 48 studies. The repeated findings of

improvements over a broad range of outcomes in phonology and language imply that direct

services are effective, especially from the 33 studies that had sufficient controls to determine

the influence of intervention.

Direct clinical services provided on an individual or group basis appear effective. This seems

clearer for individual services than group services, merely because individual services

occurred more frequently (29 studies) than group services (seven studies) with another three

studies combining the two. However, the RCT that Boyle et al. (2007) conducted is

particularly noteworthy for this point about the effectiveness of direct group or individual

intervention because they compared it for language intervention. They found that the children

in both groups made significantly more gains than the control children who received usual

care, but there were no differences between the two experimental groups. This suggests that

both types of intervention are equally successful, when the conditions they developed are met

(these are detailed on the next page).

Group intervention may be equally as effective as individual intervention for phonology

(Law et al. 2003). Law et al. (2003) concluded this within their Level I study for school age

children receiving phonology intervention. However, this one study was published in the

1960s using an intervention that is rarely used now, precluding comparisons to contemporary

publications.

There were 33 direct service delivery model studies that included adequate controls to

determine the effect of the direct intervention. Common features from the successful

interventions are:

provided by a speech pathologist

usually on an individual basis

time-limited and intense with a dosage of two to three 30-minute sessions per week

for about 5 to 9 weeks but with the flexibility to extend if needed

usually addressing either speech or language rather than both

integrating production and phonological awareness work to achieve positive outcomes

for phonology.

EFFICACY OF INDIRECT MODELS OF SERVICE DELIVERY

Indirect services occurred in four of the studies. Only one study had adequate controls to

determine the intervention effect. This study also compared indirect services with direct

services. Boyle et al. (2007) suggested that direct and indirect language services were equally

effective because all the experimental groups made significant gains over the children in the

control group who received usual care, and there were no significant differences in the gains

made between the experimental groups, as measured by the CELFUK

. Whilst this is only one

25

study, it was a well-constructed RCT and it may be worth noting the program features and

some of the secondary outcomes. Boyle et al. (2007) noted that their findings about the

children‟s outcomes and the outcomes about cost can only be generalised to other

educational/health systems where the resources would equate to those described in this study

which included:

a programme explicitly negotiated with and supported by senior management and

with management providing dedicated resources in addition to staff

a well planned program whose implementation was preceded by a long lead time for

planning and developing the service, including predicting and marshalling all

resources

allocated liaison time of one day per week for each speech pathologist

a caseload of 9 children for each speech pathologist or speech pathology assistant

intensive therapy provision (3 times per week for 30-40 minutes for 15 weeks)

a detailed intervention manual, developed from an extensive literature review

clearly articulated policy and procedures

assistants that were provided with extensive training and support. Prior to the

commencement of the programme, the assistants were trained using a credentialed

training package and were supported throughout the project.

transporting children from their home school to the programme schools to receive

their intervention..

In addition to measuring language outcomes about impairment status, activity and

participation information was procured through parent and teacher questionnaires and focus

groups. Teachers reported that children‟s self-esteem increased (Boyle et al. 2007, p56)

indicated by statements such as, „The child was much happier and began to participate in the

class discussion‟; „It helped the child with conversation and this gave her more confidence

with reading and writing‟. Parents also reported improvements in self-confidence, social

interaction, behaviour and literacy (Boyle et al. 2007, p. 61). This implies that this type of

package may also address the impairment, activity and participation levels of the ICF (WHO

2001).

EFFICACY OF THE COLLABORATIVE MODELS OF SERVICE DELIVERY

Four of the studies were described as collaborative services (Gardner 2006; Hadley et al.

2000; Roulstone et al. 2005; Wren et al. 2001). Whilst all papers reported improved outcomes

for children and other stakeholders, only one study had adequate controls to determine the

influence of interventions (Hadley et al. 2000).

However, given one of the aims of this review was to determine service delivery constructs

these four studies are discussed further because they were judged as providing useful

information.

These four studies involved schools and shared many of the following service delivery

characteristic constructs with two of them receiving explicit support for these projects by

senior levels of management or the relevant authority(ies) (Roulstone et al. 2005; Wren et al.

2001). In these later two studies, local service agreements were developed collaboratively

and grounded in philosophies, policies and procedures relevant to the local stakeholders but

with the imprimatur of senior management. These philosophies typically had children and

their communication difficulties at their core but recognised that to maximise children‟s

speech, language and communication outcomes the focus of intervention had to be broader

26

than the child (Roulstone et al. 2005; Wren et al. 2001). Resources (human and other) were

predicted from a system perspective, negotiated, planned, and procured prior to

implementation. Thus, the service delivery procedures were not centrally prescribed but

developed locally to suit the local context within guidelines. Implementation was supported

by dedicated, inclusive joint management structures that enabled team members to be

accountable to their project and professional managers. Management practices supported

collaboration rather than leaving it to chance, by allocating times for collaboration between

teachers and speech pathologists. Staff development was an integral part of all four projects

to ensure that team members developed a shared language as well as enhanced understanding

of the roles of other team professionals to facilitate collaboration. These practices meant that

accessibility of speech pathologists in schools was increased and their time was deployed in

varying ways. One way was being in the classroom for half a week working with the teacher

and children (Hadley et al. 2000). Another way was to spend about half the allocated time

working with children individually and the other half with parents and teachers (Roulstone et

al. 2005).

In addition to measuring the children‟s outcomes, Roulstone et al. (2005) and Wren et al

(2001) reported on the outcomes from the perspectives of other stakeholders including

parents, teachers and speech pathologists. Parents reported that they were pleased with the

outcomes for their children. Teachers and speech pathologist‟s self- reported an increased

knowledge about other professionals and that speech pathologist‟s were considered as part of

the school team. For organisations that provide peripatetic speech pathology services,

projects such as these may provide guidelines for team building and may have staffing

implications.

LIMITATIONS OF THE REVIEW

One limitation of this review is that the studies within it may not reflect community clinical

practice with respect to the time expended on planning and evaluating them. These studies

were probably developed by academics in academic environments that may well be different

to community services in a number of ways including access to resources such as journals

and time for project development and evaluation. Whilst it is impossible to quantify this time,

it is reasonable to assume it is considerable. This is so because all the studies are published

and, this in turn, presupposes a number of planning and evaluations processes. Aside from the

preparation time to write grant and ethics applications, considerable time was probably

expended developing the intervention using processes which may include literature reviews,

peer review, negotiations with stakeholders, trials and developing instruction manuals. These

steps were explicitly acknowledged by Boyle et al. (2007) and Roulstone and colleagues

(2001; 2005) in developing their service delivery models and are implied in most of the

studies. Some or all of these processes may be critical to the success of the interventions and

therefore may be constructs for consideration in developing services.

27

CHAPTER 5: CORE LEARNINGS

EFFICACY OF INTERVENTIONS

EFFECTIVENESS

1 Speech pathology seems effective for children, aged 5 years and above with primary

speech and/or language impairment to advance their linguistic skills. There is some

evidence for secondary gains in literacy and self-esteem.

2 Output phonology, phonological awareness and expressive vocabulary intervention is

supported.

3 Phonology interventions that combine phonological awareness with production work

appear to positively affect speech and literacy.

4 Phonological awareness interventions that codes sounds with coloured blocks that can be

manipulated seem effective.

5 Gains in phonology from intervention appear to be sustained in the short and long term.

There appear to be secondary gains for literacy.

6 Intervention that aims to enhance phonological processing intervention with the use of

enhanced (slowed) speech is not supported. More particularly, there was no support for

using the proprietary software programme, Fast ForWord.

7 The evidence is mixed for receptive vocabulary and receptive and expressive syntax

intervention. However, recent positive evidence about intervention for receptive and

expressive syntax is at variance with the earlier consensus that it seemed not to be

beneficial.

8 Syntax interventions that code grammatical constituents with coloured shapes that can be

manipulated seem effective.

9 The evidence for discourse and pragmatics intervention is small in amount but positive.

10 The applicability of many of the findings is limited because within each of the domains

of language the numbers of studies were too small to adequately cover all the relevant

variables.

EFFICIENCY

1 Phonology output intervention that combines speech production work and phonological

awareness seems more efficient than speech production work alone.

2 Children with different forms of speech (phonological) impairment appear to respond

differentially to different interventions.

3 The phonological interventions of minimal and maximal contrast therapy may be equally

efficient (1 study).

4 Customised intervention for phonological awareness intervention seems more efficient

than the commercial packages of LiPS™ and Earobics.

5 Within expressive syntax, conversational recasts seem a more efficient intervention than

imitation.

6 Syntactic-semantic therapy and semantic therapy for receptive and expressive syntax

may be equally effective.

7 There was no efficiency data about pragmatics, discourse, semantics or composite

language interventions.

8 Representing linguistic components (sounds and syntax components) with coloured

shapes such as blocks that can be manipulated may be a key feature of successful

phonology and syntax interventions.

28

MODELS OF SERVICE DELIVERY

EFFECTIVENESS

1. Direct intervention seems effective. More particularly, time-limited intensive direct

intervention with a dosage of two to three 30-minute sessions per week, for about 5 to 9

weeks, results in significant changes to speech and language.

2. Direct therapy should ideally be administered by a qualified speech pathologist,

particularly for phonology.

3. Direct and indirect group or individual intervention for children with language

impairment seems effective when meeting a prescribed set of conditions (see page 26).

4. The constructs of indirect and collaborative models on service delivery appear to be:

a programme explicitly negotiated with and supported by senior management and

with management providing dedicated resources in addition to staff that is

underpinned by a negotiated philosophy

This philosophy appears to subsume many principles of the International

Classification of Functioning

Deployment of resources such that

a. Speech pathologists provide time-limited intensive therapy provision (e.g. 3

times per week for 30-40 minutes for 3 to 15 weeks). In one study, this co-

occurred with a case load of 9 children (at any one block of time)

b. There is allocated liaison time for teachers and speech pathologists and other

staff involved in service delivery

c. Staff are educated to meet the agency requirements and are supported with

work manuals

d. Speech pathologists are recognised as a key team member when planning

speech pathology services and they are involved in service level planning,

irrespective of rank within the service.

EFFICIENCY

1 For language

Group or individual direct therapy may be equally effective when a range of

conditions are met (see page 26). Clinical decision making can take into account

clinician judgement and experience, and client preference when making a decision

about using group or individual direct therapy.

2 For phonology

There appears to be clear evidence to support individual direct intervention. As there

is little information about other models of service delivery for phonology,

comparisons between direct and other service delivery models cannot be made.

29

REFERENCES

Access Economics Pty. Ltd. (2008). The economic impact of cerebral palsy in Australia in

2007. Australia: Access Economics Pty. Ltd.

Adams, C., Baxendale, J., Lloyd, J., & Aldred, C. (2005). Pragmatic Language Impairment:

Case Studies of Social and Pragmatic Language Therapy. Child Language Teaching

and Therapy, 21(3), 227-250.

Adams, C., & Lloyd, J. (2007). The effects of speech and language therapy intervention on

children with pragmatic language impairments in mainstream school. British Journal

of Special Education, 34(4), 226-233.

Adams, C., Lloyd, J., Aldred, C., & Baxendale, J. (2006). Exploring the effects of

communication intervention for developmental pragmatic language impairments: A

signal-generation study. International Journal of Language & Communication

Disorders, 41(1), 41-65.

Anger, N., & Sullivan, L. (1995). Problem solving with parents and children. Australian

Communication Quarterly Summer, 28-31.

Baker, E. (2000). Changing nail to snail: A treatment efficacy study of phonological

impairment in children. Unpublished PhD, Sydney: University of Sydney.

Bayne, G., & Moran, C. (2005). The effect of single word semantic-phonological

intervention on developmental word finding difficulties at single words and discourse

levels. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy, 60, 31-44.

Bercow, J. (2008). The Bercow report: A review of services for children and young people

(0-19) with speech, language and communication needs. Nottingham: DCSF.

www.dcsf.gov.uk/bercowreview.

Bernhardt, B., & Major, E. (2005). Speech, language and literacy skills 3 years later: A

follow-up study of early phonological and metaphonological intervention.

International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40(1), 1-27.

Bernstein Ratner, N. (2006). Evidence-based practice: An examination of its ramifications for

the practice of speech-language pathology Language, Speech & Hearing Services in

Schools, 37(4), 257-267.

Boyle, J., McCartney, E., Forbes, J., & O'Hare, A. (2007). A randomised controlled trial and

economic evaluation of direct versus indirect and individual versus group modes of

speech and language therapy for children with primary language impairment. Health

Technology Assessment, 11(25:iii-iv, xi-xii), 1-139.

Blum-Harasty, J. A., & Rosenthal, J. B. M. (1992). The prevalence of communication

disorders in children: A summary and critical review. Australian Journal of Human

Communication Disorders, 20, 63-80.

Bryan, K. (2004). Preliminary study of the prevalence of speech and language difficulties in

young offenders International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,

39(3), 391-400.

Campbell, W. N., & Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2007). School-aged children with SLI: The ICF as a

framework for collaborative service delivery Journal of Communication Disorders,

40, 513-535.

Cirrin, F. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2008). Language intervention practices for school-age

children with spoken language disorders: A systematic review. Languag , Speech, and

Hearing Services in Schools, 39(Supplement), S110-S137.

Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Knox, E., & Simkin, Z. (2002). Different school placements

following language unit attendance: which factors affect language outcome?

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(2), 185-195.

30

Cox, S. (2003). Using computer technology to teach phonological awareness skills

ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing, 5(3), 111-113.

Crosbie, S., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (2005). Intervention for children with severe speech

disorder: A comparison of two approaches. International Journal of Language &

Communication Disorders 40(4), 467 - 491.

Culatta, B., Setzer, L. A., & Horn, D. (2005). Meaning-based intervention for a child with

speech and language disorders. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(4), 388-401.

Dale, P. S., & Crain-Thoreson, C. (1996). Parent-child book reading as an intervention

technique for young children with language delays. Topics in Early Childhood

Special Education, 16(2), 213-235.

Damico, J. S., & Oller, J. W. J. (1980). Pragmatics versus morphological/syntactic criteria for

language referrals. Languag , Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 11, 85-94.

Denne, M., Langdown, N., Pring, T., & Roy, P. (2005). Treating children with expressive

phonological disorders: does phonological awareness therapy work in the clinic?

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(4), 493-504

Dodd, B. (2007). Evidence-based practice and speech-language pathology: Strengths,

weaknesses, opportunities and threats. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 59, 118-

129.

Dodd, B., Crosbie, S., McIntosh, B., Holm, A., Harvey, C. Liddy, M., Fontyne, K., Pinchin,

B., & Rigby, H. (2008). The impact of selecting different contrasts in phonological

therapy. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(5) 334-345.

Ebbels, S. (2007). Teaching grammar to school age children with specific language

impairment using Shape Coding SLI: A randomized control trial. Child Language

Teaching & Therapy, 23(1), 56-93.

Ebbels, S., & van der Lely, H.K.J. (2001). Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for

children with severe persistent SLI. International Journal of Language &

Communication Disorders, 36 Suppl:345-50.

Ebbels, S., van der Lely, H. K. J., & Dockrell, J. E. (2007). Intervention for verb argument

structure in children with persistent SLI: A randomized control trial. Journal of

Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(5), 1330-1349.

Felsenfeld, S., Broen, P.A., & McGue, M. (1992). A 28-year follow-up of adults with a

history of moderate phonological disorder: Linguistic and personality results. Journal

of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1114-1125.

Felsenfeld, S., Broen, P. A., & McGue, M. (1994). A 28-year follow-up of adults with a

history of moderate phonological disorder: Educational and occupational results.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1341-1353.

Fey, M. E. (2006). Commentary on "Making Evidence-Based Decisions About Child

Language Intervention in Schools" by Gillam and Gillam. Language, Speech &

Hearing Services in Schools, 37(4), 316-319.

Forrest, K., & Elbert, M. (2001). Treatment for phonologically disordered children with

variable substitution patterns. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(1), 41-45.

Gardner, H. (2006). Training others in the art of therapy for speech sound disorders: An

interactional approach Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22 (1), 27-46.

Gillam, S. L., & Gillam, R. B. (2006). Making evidence-based decisions about child language

intervention in schools. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 37(4), 304-

315.

Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (1993). The phonological, syntactic and semantic skills of children

with specific reading disability. Australian Journal of Human Communication

Disorders, 21 (1), 86-102.

31

Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (1995). The effects of training phonological, semantic and syntactic

processing skills in spoken language on reading ability. Language, Speech, and

Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 58-68.

Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (1998). A developmental case study of a child with severe reading

disability: Response to specific intervention programmes. New Zealand Journal of

Speech-Language Therapy, 52, 9-21.

Gillon, G. T. (2004). Phonological awareness: From research to practice. New York:

Guilford Press.

Gillum, H., Camarata, S., Nelson, K. E., & Camarata, M. N. (2003). A comparison of

naturalistic and analog treatment effects in children with expressive language disorder

and poor preintervention imitation skills. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions,

5(3), 171-178.

Hadley, P. A., Simmerman, A., Long, M., & Luna, M. (2000). Facilitating language

development for inner-city children: Experimental evaluation of a collaborative,

classroom-based intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

31(2).

Harasty, J., & Reed, V. A. (1994). The prevalence of speech and language impairment in two

Sydney metropolitan schools: A summary and critical review. Australian Journal of

Human Communication Disorders, 22(1-23), 63-80.

Hay. I., Elias, G., F ielding-Barnsley, R., Homel, R., & Freiberg, K. (2007). Language delays,

reading delays, and learning difficulties: Interactive elements requiring

multidimensional programming. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(5), 400-409.

Jacoby, G. P., Lee, L., Kummer, A. W., Levin, L., & Creaghead, N. A. (2002). The number

of individual treatment units necessary to facilitate functional communication

improvements in the speech and language of young children. American Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology, 11(4), 370-380.

James, D. G. H. (1994). The efficacy of a language screening battery for Australian five-year-

old children. Australian Journal of Human Communication Disorders, 22(2), 31-46.

Jessup, B., Ward, E., Cahill, L., & Keating, D. (2008). Prevalence of speech and/or language

impairment in preparatory students in northern Tasmania. International Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology.10 (5) 364-377

Keilmann, A., Braun, L., & Napiontek, U. (2004). Emotional satisfaction of parents and

speech-language therapists with outcome of training intervention in children with

speech and language disorders. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 56(1), 51-61.

Kent, R. D. (2006). Evidence-based practice in communication disorders: Progress not

perfection. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 37(4), 268-270.

Kirk, C., & Gillon, G. T. (2007). Longitudinal effects of phonological awareness intervention

on morphological awareness in children with speech impairment. Language Speech

and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 342-352.

Klein, E. S. (1996). Phonological/traditional approaches to articulation therapy: a

retrospective group comparison. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

27(4), 3140323.

Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2003). Speech and language therapy interventions for

children with primary speech and language delay or disorder. Cochrane Database of

Systematic Reviews, Issue 3 Art. No.: CD004110. DOI: 004110.001002/14651858.

CD14004110.

Lewis, B. A. & Freebairn, L. (1992). Residual effects of preschool phonology disorders in

grade-school, adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,

35, 819-831.

32

Long, K., & Gillon, G. T. (2007). Enhancing early literacy acquisition in a child with speech

and language impairment: A case study. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language

Therapy, 62(4-19).

Marler, J. A., Champlin, C. A., & Gillam, R. B. (2001). Backward and simultaneous masking

measured in children with language-learning impairments who received intervention

with Fast ForWord or Laureate Learning Systems software. American Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology, 10(3), 258-268

McKinnon, D., McLeod, S., & Reilly, S. (2007). The prevalence of stuttering, voice and

speech-sound disorders in primary school students in Australia. Language, Speech &

Hearing Services in Schools, 38(1), 5-15.

Moriarty, B. C., & Gillon, G. T. (2006). Phonological awareness intervention for children

with childhood apraxia of speech. International Journal of Language and

Communication Disorders, 41, 713-734.

Munro, N., & Atkinson, K. (2003). School-aged group therapy within community health

services. ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing, 5(2), 72-75.

Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairment

in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language

impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 199-211.

National Health and Medical Research Council. (2000). How to use the evidence: Assessment

and application of scientific evidence. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.

Nelson, K. E., Camarata, S. M., Welsh, J., Butkovsky, L., & Camarata, M. (1996). Effects of

imitative and conversational recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in

children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal children.

Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39(4), 850-859.

Pascoe, M., Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (2005). Phonological therapy within a

psycholinguistic framework: Promoting change in a child with persisting speech

difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(2),

189-220.

Paul, R. (2007). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence. (3rd

ed.). St Louis,

Missouri: Mosby Elsevier.

Pawson, R., Greenhalgh, T., Harvey, G., & Walshe, K. (2005). Realist review-A new method

of systematic review designed for complex policy interventions. The Journal of

Health Service Research Policy, 10 (Supp 13), S1:21- 34.

Pokorni, J. L., Worthington, C. K., & Jamison, P. J. (2004). Phonological awareness

intervention: Comparison of Fast ForWord, Earobics, and LiPS. The Journal of

Educational Research, 97(3), 147-157.

Ray, J. (2002). Treating phonological disorders in a multilingual child: A case study.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(3), 305-315.

Richardson, K., & Klecan-Aker, J. S. (2000). Teaching pragmatics to language-learning

disabled children: a treatment outcome study. Child Language Teaching & Therapy,

16(1), 23-42.

Roulstone, S., Owen, R., & French, L. (2005). Speech and language therapy and the Knowles

Edge standards fund project: An evaluation of the service provided to a cluster of

primary schools. British Journal of Special Education, 32(2), 78-85.

Scientific Learning Corporation. (1998). Fast ForWord. Berkeley, CA: Scientific Learning

Corporation.

Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2004). Computer-supported phonological awareness

intervention for kindergarten children with specific language impairment Language,

Speech & Hearing Services in the Schools, 35(3), 229-239.

33

Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2005). Reconceptualizing treatment goals from language impairment to

functional limitations: A case study. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(4), 353-363.

Smith, J., Downs, M., & Mogford-Bevan, K. (1998). Can phonological awareness training

facilitate minimal pair therapy? International Journal of Language & Communication

Disorders, 33(supplement), 463-468.

Snow, P. (2008a, May). Child abuse and neglect: Is speech-language pathology missing in

action? Paper presented at the Joint conference between New Zealand Speech-

Language Therapists Association and Speech Pathology Australia, Auckland, New

Zealand.

Snow, P. (2008b, May). Oral language competence in childhood and adolescence: The

missing link in mental health promotion? Paper presented at the Joint conference

between New Zealand Speech-Language Therapists Association and Speech

Pathology Australia, Auckland, New Zealand.

Snow, P. (2008c, May). Strengthening schools as mental health promotion settings: The role

of speech-language pathologist as language and literacy advocates. Paper presented

at the Joint conference between New Zealand Speech-Language Therapists

Association and Speech Pathology Australia, Auckland, New Zealand.

Snow, P. C., & Powell, M. B. (2004). Developmental language disorders and adolescent risk:

A public health advocacy role for speech pathologists? Advances in Speech-Language

Pathology, 6(4), 221-229.

Snowling, M. J., Adams, J. W., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2001). Educational

attainment of school leavers with a preschool history of speech-language

impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36,

173-183.

Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (1997). Children's speech and literacy difficulties. London:

Whurr.

Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (Eds.). (2001). Children's speech and literacy difficulties 2:

Identification and intervention. London: Whurr.

Stiegler, L. N., & Hoffman, P. R. (2001). Discourse-based intervention for word finding in

children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 277-303.

Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998).

Language impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech,

Language and Hearing Research, 41, 407-418.

Strand, E. A., Stoeckel, R., & Baas, B. (2006). Treatment of severe childhood apraxia of

speech: a treatment efficacy study. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology,

14(4), 297-307.

Swanson, L. A., Fey, M. E., Mills, C. E., & Hood, L. S. (2005). Use of narrative-based

language intervention with children who have specific language impairment.

American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology, 14(2), 131-143.

Ward, S. (1999). An Investigation into the Effectiveness of an Early Intervention Method for

Delayed Language Development in Young Children. International Journal of

Language & Communication Disorders, 34(3), 243-264.

Williams, A. L. (2000). Multiple oppositions: Case studies of variables in phonological

intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9(4), 289-299.

World Health Organization. (2001). International classification of functioning, disability and

health. Geneva: WHO.

Wren, Y., & Roulstone, S. (2008). A comparison between computer and tabletop delivery of

phonology therapy. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,10(5) 346-

363.

34

Wren, Y., Roulstone, S., Parkhouse, J., & Hall, B. (2001). A model for a mainstream school-

based speech and language therapy service. Child Language Teaching & Therapy,

17(2), 107-126.

35

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The Department of Education and Children‟s Services and Department of Health, SA for

funding this project.

Cathy Olsson, Jenny Moody, Ellie Chronis, Linda Nimmo, Craig Mattiske and other

members of the Statewide Interagency Speech Pathology Reference Group, SA for their

guidance.

Stephanie Grant, Libby McMahon and Lynn Williams for their preparedness to answer

questions about their organisations and work.

Saravana Kumar, Cathy Olsson and the Reference Groupd for their helpful comments on

earlier drafts.

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

APPENDICES

APPENDIX 1. SUMMARY OF ALL THE STUDIES AGAINST EXTRACTION CRITERIA.

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

1. Adams

et al 2005

(Level IV-

case

studies)

School 2 boys

8;1 & 9;9

years with

pragmatic

language

impairment

SP pragmatics

&social

interaction

ICF-. I, A & P

Adapting

communicat-

ive context

Direct

1:1 with

child &

input for

other

interlocut-

ors

60 mins 3 times

per week

32 8 weeks Pragmatics but

not clear that Rx

was the change

agent

2. Adams

et al

(2006)

These two studies report on the same cohort of children. See the next row for the relevant details

3. Adams

& Lloyd

2007

(Level III-

3 (ABA)

School 6 boys

5;11- 9;9 years

with

pragmatic

language

impairment

SP Pragmatics-

conversation,

inference &

narrative

ICF-. I, A & P

Adapting

communicat-

ive context

Direct

1:1 with

child &

input for

other

interlocut-

ors

? 1 per

week

8 group

+ 3 home

visits

8 weeks Pragmatics &

probably due to

Rx

4. Anger

& Sullivan

(1995)

Level –

Other

Qualita-

tive

Case

series

Clinic N=9, 2 girls &

7 boys

9;10 - 13;5

years with

social

communicativ

e deficits

Carers N=8

Children

SPs &

nurse

Parents

group

child thera-

pist, nurse

& psych-

ologist

Pragmatics

ICF-A & P

Adapting

communicat-

ive context

Cognitive Rx

Direct

group Children

1.5 hours

Carers

?

? 1 per

week

Children

7

Carers

6

Children

7 weeks

Carers

5 weeks

Children use

more appropriate

social strategies

Parents

1. sensitivity to

their children‟s

communicative

needs

2. recognition of

their facilitative

role in their

children‟s

development

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

5. Bayne

& Moran

2005

Level III-3

(ABA)

Clinic N= 3, 2 girls

& 1 boy, 7;1;

7;7 & 8;2

years with

language

impairment

Clinician Word retrieval

ICF-I

Combined

semantic &

phonological

elaboration

Direct

1:1 1 hour 1 per

week

10 3 weeks retrieval for

trained words

only at word level

with no transfer to

discourse

6.

Bernhardt

& Major

2005

Level-

Other

Case study

clinic N= 12

Average age 7

years with

phonological

impairment

SP This study is a follow-up study of these children who had had phonology therapy three years earlier

when they were aged 3;3 and 4;11 years that was non-linear in approach. They had 3 45-minute

sessions per week for 16 weeks. Most of them had had some community based therapy in the

intervening years

Phonology; intelligible; Oral

language WNL

on CELF &

PPVT-R;

Literacy PIAT-R

reading 10/12

Wnl spelling 7/12

wnl

7. Boyle et

al (2007)

Level II

RCT

Schools N= 152

children with

primary

language

impairment,

aged 6-11

years

SP or SP

assistant

Language

comprehension

& expression at

word, sentence

and discourse

levels

ICF-Impairment

Eclectic

evidenced-

based

direct 1:1 vs

direct group vs

indirect 1:1 vs

indirect group.

Groups

and

individual

30-40 min 3x per

week

Mean 38

sessions

20 hours

over 15

weeks

No differences

between therapy

modes.

Significant gains

in expressive

language but not

in receptive

language. Indirect

group therapy was

most cost

effective

8.

Campbell

& Vidler

(1996)

Level III-2

School N= 16 Year 1

& 2 school

children (age

not stated)

with language

impairment

SP Metalinguistics

in phonology,

semantics &

syntax

Metalinguistic

Direct

Groups of

4

30 minutes 2 per

week

18 8 weeks Experimental

scores > control

group scores on

Analysis of the

Language of

Learning but not

statistically

analysed

9. Conti- School N=106 11- Not The outcomes of children with specific language impairment who continued attending a language Education

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Ramsden

et al

(2002)

Level III-2

year-olds with

SLI; 54 who

attend a

language unit

and 54 in

mainstream

school

specified unit from the age of 8 years were compared with those who left the unit at the age of 8 years and

went into mainstream school to determine the effect of schooling on outcomes.

placement at 8

years appears not

to change 11-year

outcomes Note

mainstream

children needed &

had additional

assistance to the

classroom.

10. Cox

(2003)

Level III-2

School N =32; 20

boys & 12

girls 6;3 -10;3

years with

phonological

awareness

difficulties

Final (4th)

year

honours

SPs

Phonological

awareness

Computer

software

Direct

Small

group

15 minutes 3 per week 12 5.5 weeks No gains in

phonological

awareness

11.

Crosbie et

al (2005)

Level III-2

School

&

home

N= 18; 11

boys & 7 girls;

4; 8-6;5 years,

with

consistent or

inconsistent

speech

impairment.

SP Phonology

ICF- I

Contrast Rx vs

core

vocabulary

Direct

1:1 30 minutes 2 per

week

32 Two 8-9

week

blocks

with 4-

week break

between

20-22

weeks

Rx altered speech.

Core vocabulary

best for

inconsistent

speech

impairment

Contrast Rx best

for consistent

speech

impairment

12. Culatta

et al

(2005)

Level-

other

Univers

ity

clinic

N=1 A boy,

5;9 years with

speech &

language

disorder

University

clinician

Phonology

ICF- I

Modified

cycles +

interactive

routines, play

scripts & story

enactments

Direct

1:1 Not clearly stated.

It appears that Rx occurred weekly for 6 months.

There was not indication of the length of each

session

Rx caused

changes in speech

& language.

However few

controls

13. Dale & Univer- N=33 mother- Not stated Discourse Teaching Groups of Three sessions that occurred over about 6 to 8 Parents learn

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Crain-

Thoreson

(1996)

Level III-1

sity

clinic

child dyads,

Children ;24

boys & 9 girls

aged 3 - 6

years with

language

delays

CF- I, A P

parents an

interactive,

responsive

style of

communicat-

ion with

children

indirect

mothers week language

facilitation

techniques from

brief input & this

children‟s

language.

Teaching them in

book-reading

seems useful

14. Denne

et al

(2005)

Level III-3

Comm

unity

clinics

N= 20

children, 5 to

7 years with

expressive

phonological

problems

SPs Expressive

phonology

ICF- I

Gillon

Phonological

Awareness

Training

Programme

Direct

Groups of

3

1.5 hours 1 per

week

8 8 weeks Rx phonological

awareness skills

but not speech or

literacy

15. Dodd

et al

(2008)

Level III-1

Schools

&

univers

ity

clinic

N= 19, 3;11 to

6;5 years, with

phonological

disorder

SPs Phonology

ICF-

Impairment

Phonological

contrasting

minimal pairs

with minimal

contrasts and

those with

maximal pair

contrast

Direct

1:1 30-minute

weekly

1 per

week

6 6 weeks Significantly

speech &

maintained for

14/19 children at

8-10 weeks post

Rx. No

differences

between contrasts

16.Ebbels

& van der

Lely

(2001)

Level III-3

Special

-ist SLI

school

N= 4 children

with SLI (3

boys&1 girl)

11;8-12;9

years.

SP Syntax-wh-

questions &

passives;

comp‟n & exp

ICF- I

meta-syntactic

Rx -visual

coding ( based

on colourful

semantics)

Direct

1:1 ? ≈45

minutes

+ 30

minutes

training

sessions

1 per

week

30 +13

training

sessions

30 weeks ¾ children made

significant in

comprehension &

4/4 in at least one

aspect of

expression taught

17a.

Ebbels

Special

ist SLI

S1; N= 3

children with

SP S1; Syntax-

dative (C & E)

Colour

shaping

1:1 ? ? ? One school

term

S1 ; 2/3 made

changes

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

(2007)

Studies 1

(S1) &

2(S2)

Level III-3

school SLI (3 boys &

1 girl) 11-12

years.

S2; 2 of the

above

ICF- I S2; C &

E of comparison

questions

ICF- I

Direct

S1 ; 2/2 made

changes

17b.

Ebbels

(2007)

Study 3

Level IV

Special

ist SLI

school

N=9 children

with SLI aged

9-11 year

Not stated Syntax-past

tense markers

ICF- I

Colour

shaping

Direct

Group 1 hour 1 per

week

? the

autumn

school

term

6/9 made

significant

changes

18. Ebbels

et al

(2007)

Level II

RCT

Special

ist SLI

school

N=27 children

with SLI 10

girls & 17

boys) aged

11;0 to 16;1

years

SP Syntax-verb

argument

structure

ICF- I

Syntactic vs

semantic Rx

Direct

1:1 30 minutes 1 per

week

9 9 weeks

(4.5 hours)

Both treatments

changed some

aspect of verb

argument

structure with

neither being

more efficient

than the other.

19. Forrest

& Elbert

(2001)

Level III-3

Clinic N= 4 boys

with speech

impairment,

aged 4;11,

5;1,5;1 & 5;3

years

Student SP Phonology;

fricatives

ICF-impairment

Traditional

Direct

1:1 45 minutes 2 x per

week

varied so

criterion

of ~68%

generali-

zation

reached

All children

learned the target

sound but

generalization

was limited

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

20

Gardner

(2006)

Level

other

Qualita-

tive

School N=6 (1) 2

learning

support

assistants

(LSAs) (2) 2

SP students

(3) 2 children,

aged 5 & 6

years, with

speech

impairment

learning

support

assistants

& SP

students

under SP

supervis-

ion

Phonology

ICF-impairment

Adapting

communicat-

ive context

based on

conversation

analysis

Indirect+

direct

Collaborative

with school

support staff

1:1 with

each child

alternated

with LSA

& student

SP

not

specified

1 per

week

6 6 weeks 1 LSAs & SP

students use of

target behaviours

2. children

21. Gillon

& Dodd

(1995)

Level III-2

School N= 10

children with

language

disability, 10 -

12 years,

SP 1. Phonological

processing,

2 semantics &

syntax

1. Based on

Lindamood

2. Published

resources for

vocabulary &

syntax

Direct

Groups of

5 children

2 hours weekly ? 2x 6 week

blocks

Rx for

phonological

processing,

semantics &

syntax led to

oral & written

language scores

22. Gillon

& Dodd

(1998)

Level-

other; case

study lon-

gitudinal

School N= 1 child

with language

disability,

tracked for 4

years from 8;9

to 12 years,

SP Phonology

ICF-impairment

1. Based on

Lindamood

2. Published

resources for

vocabulary &

syntax

Direct

Groups of

5 children

2 hours weekly ? 2x 6 week

blocks

Rx for

phonological

processing led to

reading & Rx in

semantics &

syntax led to

language scores

23. Gillum

et al

(2003)

Level IV

Clinic N= 4 aged

4;3, 4;4, 6;5 &

6;8 years with

expressive

SP Syntax (relative

clauses;

questions etc)

ICF-impairment

Imitation vs

conversational

recast

Direct

1:1 50 minutes Twice

weekly

Not

specified

Not

specified All 4 children

& made quicker

progress with

recasts than

imitation

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

language

24. Hadley

et al

(2000)

Level III-1

N= 86

children, aged

5;0- to 6;9

years; 46

experimental

children with

various

language

needs (5/46

had speech-

language

impairments)

& 40 controls

SPs &

teachers

Vocabulary &

phonological

awareness

ICF- I, A, P

Adapting

communicat-

ive context to

use

naturalistic

language

techniques

Collaborative

classroom

based

Direct+

indirect

In

classroom

1 hour

planning

meetings

between

teacher &

SLP

+

SP in

classroom

for 2.5

days

+

25 minutes

phonologi-

cal

awareness

work

Weekly

weekly

weekly

? 6 months Experimental

group made

significant in

receptive &

expressive

vocabulary &

phonological

awareness over

control group.

The children with

specific language

impairment also

made gains but

these were not

statistically tested

25. Hay et

al (2007)

Level III-2

School N =116 Grade

1 children,

aged 5;9 years

(mean) with

language

difficulties

Teachers

in

collaborat-

ion with a

language

support

teacher

(LST)

Vocabulary,

syntax &

discourse

(dialogue)

ICF- I, A & P

Adapting

communicat-

ive context to

use

incorporate

Blank et al‟s

levels of

questioning

Collaborative

Indirect

Indirect

through

teacher

training

LST spent

2 hours per

week with

teacher

+

small

group

session

with

children

(time not

stated)

weekly 1 school

term -12

weeks

Experimental

group

significantly

outperformed the

control group on

the end of year

scores.

Number of

children at risk for

acquiring literacy.

Not clear that Rx

was change agent

26. Jacoby

(2002)

Level-

Other

Clinic

in

Childre

n‟s

Medica

N= 234

children, aged

3;0- 6 ;6 years

with low

functional

SP articulation/intel

ligibility,

receptive &

expressive

spoken language

Not relevant &

not described

Not

relevant &

not

described

30 minutes weekly Not

stated

Not stated (1) More service

tends to lead to

better outcomes.

(2) Younger

children need less

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Retrospect

ive audit

of case

files

l

Centre

communicat-

ion scores &

had Rx

Mixed group

ICF- not

possible to

classify

Rx than older

ones.

(3)20-hours

seemed critical

amount

27.

Keilmann

et al

(2004)

Level –

other

Qualita-

tive; ques-

tionnaires

Hospi-

tal

clinic?

N= 169

children (104

boys & 65

girls) with

speech &

language

disorders,

aged 2;8 to

9;11 (no

breakdown).

SPs Speech &

language

ICF-not possible

to classify

Not stated Not stated Not stated Varied

form 2+

times per

week to

<weekly

Not

stated

Range of

Rx

duration 1-

82 months

Parents perceive

speech &

language Rx as

creating in their

children. Parents

perceived that

higher frequency

of Rx was

associated with

higher rate of

progress.

28. Kirk &

Gillon

(2007)

Level-

Other

Follow-up

study two

studies

levels III-2

& III-3

School N= 41

children, aged

8 years;

17 with a

history of

moderate to

severe speech

impairment &

24 normal

children

In an earlier study (Gillon, 2005) two groups of children with moderate to severe speech impairment had received

two types of Rx in preschool; one that included phonological awareness work in addition to speech production

work & the other with only sound production work. Each child had 2 to 3 blocks of Rx. Each block of Rx typically

lasted between 4–6 weeks with two 45-min Rx sessions per week; one group session with 2 or 3 other study

children & one individual session (ie 8 to 27 hours on Rx) .The children were followed up three years later for

reading & language outcomes

students under the supervision of qualified SLPs

Better literacy

outcomes when

Rx is

phonological

awareness +

speech production

than sound

production alone.

Rx with

phonological

awareness seems

to promote

morphological

understanding

needed for

spelling

29. Klein

(1996)

Univers

ity

clinic

N= 36; Two

Rx groups 19

traditional,

2 students

supervised

by SPs

Articulation &

phonology

Traditional vs

phonological

1:1 50 minute 2-3 50

minute

weekly

50 hours or

child was

dismissed

Phonological Rx

speech &

quicker change

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Level III-3 aged 4;5 years

(mean) with 6

5;0+ years 17

phonological

aged 4;5 years

(mean) with 3

5;0+ years

ICF;

Impairment

Direct before this

with

normal

speech

than traditional

Rx> More

children in

Phonological Rx

met the dismissal

criteria than

traditional Rx

30. Long

& Gillon

(2007)

Level

Other

Univer-

sity

clinic

N=1 boy, 4;10

years, with

speech &

language

impairment

SP Stage 1; speech

production &

phonological

awareness

ICF:

Impairment

Stage 2;

Measurement of

literacy at

school

ICF: A & P

Integrated

phoneme

awareness &

speech

intelligibility

framework.

Use of Cycles

approach

Direct

1:1 50 minutes 2 per

week

10 5 weeks Rx phono-

logical awareness

skills from

delayed to normal

Spelling &

engagement in

literacy

instruction normal

for his first 4

weeks at school

31. Marler

et al

(2001)

Level IV

School N=7 boys,

6;10 - 9;3; 4

with language-

learning

impairment &

3 normal

SP Phonological

processing

ICF level-

impairment

Fast ForWord

Direct

1:1 15 minutes Daily 20 4 weeks processing for

all boys, with &

without language

impairment

showing no Fast

ForWord

advantage

32.

Moriarty

& Gillon,

(2006)

Level III-3

School N= 3 children,

aged 7;3, 6;3

& 6;10 years

with

childhood

apraxia of

speech

SP Phonology

ICF level-

impairment

Integrated

phonological

awareness Rx

Direct

1:1 45 min 3 times

per week

9 3 weeks Phonological

awareness ,

speech production

& non-word

reading for all

children

consequent to

phonological

awareness Rx

33. Munro Comm N= 20 school SP Phonological Phonological Group 1 hour weekly 6 6 weeks Significant in

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

&

Atkinson

(2003)

Level IV

Within-

group

compari-

son

unity-

health

clinic

children with

phonological

awareness

difficulties

from

kindergarten

(ie 1st year of

school- Year3.

awareness

ICF Impairment

awareness

Direct

phonological

awareness &

spelling but not in

control goal of

visual motor

integration

supports group

work but does it

offset opportunity

cost of missing

school ?

34. Nelson

et al

(1996)

Level III-2

N= 14

children; 7

with SLI in

experimental

group, aged,

4;7 to 6;7

years & 7

typically

developing

control aged

2;2 to 4;2

Clinician

without

explicit

statement

that they

were SLPs

Syntax

including

gerunds , copula

or auxiliary

verbs, past

tense, articles,

3rd person

ICF I, A, P

Conversation-

al recast vs

imitation

Direct

1:1 Not stated 2 per

week

Average

18.8

9-10 weeks Children with &

without SLI learnt

more stage III-V+

syntax structures

in conversational

recasting than in

imitation & learnt

them more

quickly. Progress

rates for children

with & without

SLI similar &

learn absent

structures

35. Pascoe

et al

(2005)

Level-

Other

School N= 1 a girl

with speech

difficulties,

aged 6;5 years

SP Phonology

ICF , I, A, P

Psycho-

linguistic

Direct

1:1 1 hour 2 times

per week

3 Stages

each for

10 hours

ie 30

sessions

15 weeks

of Rx

spread

over 9

months

Speech

production in

single words &

connected speech

. The effect on

spelling is unclear

36.

Pokorni et

al (2004)

School

sum-

mer

N= 54

children, aged

7;5-9;0 years

3 SPs co-

ordinated

& group

Phonological

awareness

Fast ForWord

vs Earobics vs

for LiPS

Groups but

worked on

computer

1 hour

+ 2 hours

of breaks

3 times

were day

60 20 days/4

weeks

LiPS &Earobics >

effective than Fast

ForWord for

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Level III-1

Pseudo-

RCT

camp with language

impairment,

randomly

allocated to 3

groups

leaders

implement

-ted it were

SPs, spec

education

or regulars

teachers -

all trained

in

techniques

ICF impairment

Direct

on own phonological

awareness. No

changes in oral or

written language

for any programs

The opportunity

cost of 20 days of

Rx for small

changes needs

consideration

37. Ray

(2002)

Level-

Other

Single

case study

Clinic N=1 tri-lingal

5-year- old

boy with

phonological

impairment &

normal

language

Speech

pathologist

fluent in

all three

languages

Phonology

ICF-Impairment

Cycles vs

Parents &

children

together

(PACT)

Direct

1:1 3 x 45-60

weekly

sessions

over

3 times

per week

About 60 5 months Rx of cycles &

PACT conducted

in English seemed

to consonant

accuracy,

intelligibility &

occurrence of

phonological

processes in 3

languages Rx Not

clear change

agent

38.

Richard-

son &

Klecan-

Aker

(2000)

Level IV

school N=20 aged 6;5

to 9;8 years

with

pragmatic

problems

SP Pragmatics

ICF Impairment

General

instruction

Conducted in

classroom

with teacher

Indirect

Not stated

but

probably

all done

within

class

?30

minutes

Not

stated

Not

stated

6 weeks pragmatics in

conversation,

internal responses

& object

descriptions. Not

clear Rx was the

change agent

39.

Roulstone

et al

(2005)

A

cluster

of 7

schools

N= 103

children aged

4 to 11 years

with speech,

SP

only after

extensive

negotiate-

speech,

language or

pragmatics

ICF I, A & P

No discussion

of Rx type as

focus was

service

Group or

individual

as

negotiated

Not stated Weekly

or fort-

nightly

58% of

the time

with

child &

6 hours (1) Significant

for children with

speech or

language probs

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Level IV

language or

pragmatic

problems

ions to

determine

the amount

of input

delivery

Holistic &

collaborative

Direct+

indirect

on a case-

by-case

basis

42% with

parents,

teachers

& others

but not for

children with

pragmatic probs.

(2). Parents,

teachers & SPs

satisfied & other

stakeholders

supportive of

project.

40. Segers

&

Verhoeven

(2004)

Level III-2

Special

schools

N= 24 , aged

4;10-6;11

years with

specific

language

impairment

Experi-

menter (no

qualificatio

n stated)

phonological

awareness

ICF:

Impairment

Computed

assisted vs

computer

assisted with

enhanced

speech

Direct

Groups of

3 at the

computer

but worked

individual-

lly on the

computer

15min 2-3 per

week

10-15 5 weeks phonological

awareness +

enhanced speech

no advantage over

control Rx

41.

Skarakis-

Doyle

(2005)

Level-

Other

School N= 1 boy, 10

years, with

persistent &

severe

receptive &

expressive

language

impairment.

SP but

goals

developed

collabora-

tively with

teacher

Language

ICF; I, A& P

ICF used to

frame goals

Reading

Milestone

program +

script Rx &

modelling

Direct

Individual

& group

3 hours 1;1

1 hour

groups

Weekly Not

stated

3 years 1.Standard

language test

scores stable

2. Comprehension

of (i) anaphoric

pronouns &

answering factual

questions

accurately

(3) Expression

of topic initiations

& conversation

maintaining

devises & length

of utterance

4, Adults reported

contributions to

conversations -

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

initiating &

adding

information

5. Language for

daily living: in

taking phone

message t

accurately;

placing restaurant

orders & banking

42. Smith

et al

(1998)

Level III-2

School N=18 children

with persistent

phonological

disorder, aged

5;8 - 8;2 years

Sp Phonology

ICF:

Impairment

Conventional

speech Rx

vs

Phonological

awareness

training

Direct

Not stated 1.25 hours 2 x per

week

8 4 weeks (1) Significant

speech . (2)

additional

phonological

awareness work

adds values over

maturation or

conventional Rx

(3) 3 response

patterns to Rx

43.

Stiegler &

Hoffman

(2001)

Level III-3

School N= 3 boys,

aged 9;2, 9;10

& 9;6 years

with language-

learning

disorders &

word finding

problems

Clinician

without

explicit

statement

that they

were an

SPs

Expressive

vocabulary-

word finding

ICF: I, A & P

Discourse

based

contextual Rx

Direct

1:1 15 minutes 5

sessions

for each

of the

five

program

stages

weeks.

25 5 weeks Incidence of word

finding problems

for each child.

Rx may not be

change agent

44. Strand

et al

(2006)

Level III-3

X-baseline

N= 4 non

verbal

children aged

5;5 – 6;1 with

childhood

apraxia of

SP Phonology

ICF;

Impairment

Rx based on

motor

learning-

Integral

stimulation,

dynamic &

1:1 30 mins Twice

daily

38-50 6 weeks 1. Significant

speech for ¾

children

suggesting that

frequent Rx using

motor learning

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

across

behaviours

replicated

4 x

speech who

had had

previous Rx

for 2 -4 years

temporal

cueing

(DTTC)

Direct

principles was

effective for some

children with

CAS

45.

Swanson

et al

(2005)

Level IV

Home,

school

or

clinic

as

preferre

d by

family

N= 10

children with

specific

language

impairment,

aged 6;11 to

8;9 years.

SP Language-

discourse

(narrative),

syntax

semantics

ICF-

impairment

Hybrid

approach

using

narrative-

based

language Rx

Direct

1:1 50 minutes 3 per

week

18 6 weeks

(15 hours)

2. Narrative

quality

3. for 8/ 10

children

4. Number of

different words

for 1/10 children

Grammar -No

change; Cognitive

processing- no

change in

recalling

sentences or non

word repetition

Changes may not

be due to Rx

46.

Williams

(2000)

Level IV

clinic N= 10

children (8

boys & 3

girls) with

moderate to

severe

phonological

impairment,

aged 4;0 to 6;5

years

Clinician

without

explicit

statement

that they

were SPs

Phonology

ICF: I, A & P

multiple

oppositions vs.

minimal pairs

vs. naturalistic

speech

intelligibility

Direct

Individual 30 minute3 2

sessions

per week.

Rx continued until

criterion reached which

was on average 3.5

university semesters

(15 weeks of which 10

weeks was given over

to Rx) with mean

number of 60.3

sessions

5. (1) phoneme

representation

Group means

from 37- t 85%

(2). Children with

mod -severe

phonological

impairment may

require a package

of different Rxs

47. Wren

et al

(2001)

School N=28 children

with speech &

language

SP Phonology &

language

Rx per se not

described.

Not stated Quantum of service provision not described Outcomes in all

four areas of need

met (1) children‟s

Key: Studies addressing the same participants are indicated by en emboldened border; C= comprehension, E = expression. ICF= International classification of functioning; I=

Impairment; A =activity; P = participation; PLI pragmatic language impairment; SLI=specific language impairment; Rx = intervention or therapy SP Speech pathologist; ? used

when a point is implied but not explicit; = improved/increased decreased;

Reference Service delivery model Outcomes

Setting Pars Provider focus & ICF

level

Rx type/

Service

delivery

Group/

Individual

Session

length

Frequenc

y of

sessions

number

of

sessions

Duration

of Rx

Level IV impairment

with 23

available for

follow up

ICF: I, A & P Service

delivery

model that

placed SLT

within

educational

context of 4

needs (1)

children‟s

communicat-

ion, self

esteem &

curriculum

access; (2)

classes (3)

education &

Rx staff (4)

school.

communication

skills, self-esteem

& curriculum

access , (2)

teacher‟s

understanding of

communication

difficulties . (3)

therapists more

aware of

education issues

(4) procedures for

referral & liaison

established.

48. Wren

&

Roulstone

(2008)

Level III-1

School N= 33

children, aged

4-8 years with

phonological

impairment

Researcher

/SLP with

assistant/

volunteer

providing

follow-up

sessions

Phonology

output

ICF-

Impairment

Tabletop vs

computer vs

no Rx using

phonological

awareness

work

Direct

1:1 30-minute

+

1/ week

with SLP

+ 2/week

with

assistant/

volunteer

24 8 weeks 6. All groups

made significant

progress with no

between group

differences;

control or

treatment groups

7. There were

no significant

differences in

progress between

either of the

treatment groups

at the end of

treatment or at 3

months follow up.

51

APPENDIX 2 COMPLETE SUMMARY OF EACH OF ARTICLE

1.Adams, C., Baxendale, J., Lloyd, J., & Aldred, C. (2005). Pragmatic Language Impairment: Case Studies of Social and

Pragmatic Language Therapy. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21(3), 227-250.

Summary of intervention program: Pragmatic

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Two case studies

Level IV

Aim of study:

To develop an evidence base for intervention for children with pragmatic language impairment

Determine if the outcomes affected social interaction and language pragmatics skills reflected

in changes to conversational participation or if the intervention showed more general effects

by improving language processing skills as well as pragmatic ability.

Population:

Two boys, aged 9;9 and 8;1 years, with confirmed pragmatic language impairment but without

autism according to The Children‟s Communication Checklist (Bishop, 1998) and The

Autism Diagnostic Interview (Lord et al., 1994) respectively. Both had normal cognition

(Raven‟s Progressive Matrices (1976)

The older child was within normal limits for (1) receptive and expressive vocabulary (The

British Picture Vocabulary Scales (Dunn et al., 1997) and the Expressive The Naming subtest

of the Assessment of Comprehension and Expression) (Adams et al., 2001) (2) receptive

syntax (The Test for Comprehension of Grammar (TROG) (Bishop, 1983) whereas the

younger boy was not

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

Specialist speech and language therapist

Domain of language:

pragmatics and social interaction

ICF-impairment, activity and participation

Type of intervention:

The three components of the intervention framework were

1. optimizing social interaction and social cognition so children could better adapt their

communication to interlocutors and social situations.

2. optimize adaptation of the interlocutor to communication with the child.

3. elements of language pragmatics work are included to provide the building blocks of

successful social interactions.

This was realised in two ways:

1. direct work with the child on formal pragmatic skills

2. training interlocutors who live and interact with the children to adapt their communication

for the child

Intervention emphasised verbal scaffolding for children in the home and school.

Note Syntax or semantics were not the intervention focus

Method of

intervention:

These principles were used to develop a child specific program that incorporated the individual‟s

strengths and weaknesses that centred around the three key activities of (1) communication

adaptation through isolating the tasks children could manage and adapting the communicative and

academic contexts accordingly (2) Social cognition and flexibility by making explicit the meaning

of emotions and some conventions and (3) Language pragmatics therapy on specific skills such as

exchange structure, turn-taking and topic management,

The aim for both children was to reduce verbosity and to improve coherence and quantity of

information offered in conversation

Quantum of service

provision:

3 x 60 min / week over 8 weeks.

52

Outcomes evaluation

Measures of children language

Child 1 Child2

Pre test Post

test

Pre test Post

test

ACE-inferential comprehension %ile 99 99 1 5

ACE-narrative propositions %ile 84 98 9 16

CELF-R sentence recall %ile 99 99 1 25

CELF-R forming sentences %ile 99 99 25 98

ALICC-Conversational Dominance ;

ALICC-Loquacity

ALICC-Responsiveness;

ALICC-Response problems

ALICC-Pragmatic problems Key ALICC Index changes determined through visual analysis and the post therapy indices( and indications)

are relative to the pre therapy indices; improvement regression no change

Parent and teachers perceptions of the child’s skills

Teachers & parents reported their own skills had increased and that the children‟s

communication had improved.

Author’s conclusion: Intervention resulted in change for both children; for child A in the targeted pragmatic skills and

for Child B in syntax, sentence recall and inference and propositional content of narrative but not

within the targeted pragmatic skills. Noted that addressing both the within-child and environmental

factors may be critical.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The pre-test post-test design does not have adequate controls to show clearly that the changes were

due to intervention. For example there was not a control for maturation. There were no fidelity

measures.

53

2. Adams, C., Lloyd, J., Aldred, C., & Baxendale, J. (2006). Exploring the effects of communication intervention for

developmental pragmatic language impairments: A signal-generation study. International Journal of Language & Communication

Disorders, 41(1), 41-65.

Summary of intervention program: Pragmatic language

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

This study reports on the same children as Adams & Lloyd (2007) (See next study). The only

additional points that are included in this summary a are the different points that emerged in the

description of this study

Level III-3

Aim of study:

This study is a signal generation study to determine the presence/absence and magnitude of a

treatment effect for children with pragmatic language impairment. This study has five aims

1. Is there a signal that targeted speech and language therapy brings about change in

language and pragmatic skills of children with PLI?

2. How is this signal detected?

3. What is the magnitude of the signal?

4. What insights into the condition of PLI does studying intervention permit?

5. What are the implications for future studies?

Population: No additional information to report as This study reports on the same children as the previous

study described above by Adams & Lloyd (2007) Service setting:

Description of

intervention

Quantum of service

provision:

In this description, the authors stated that each session lasted for about 1 hour with teacher/teacher

assistants liaison time added to this. In summary each child received about 24 hours of specialist

contact time plus practitioner time for training and administration.

Outcomes evaluation

Author’s conclusion:

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

54

3. Adams, C., & Lloyd, J. (2007). The effects of speech and language therapy intervention on children with pragmatic language

impairments in mainstream school. British Journal of Special Education, 34(4), 226-233.

Summary of intervention program: Pragmatic language (conversation, inference and

narrative)

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Serial case study using ABA reversal design. A1 and A2 phases utilised four and three evenly

spaced repeated assessments respectively. The B phase was intervention.

Level III-3

Aim of study:

Three aims

1. To develop measures for assessing pragmatic behaviour for school-aged children

2. To measure the size and nature of effects of an intensive speech and language therapy

programme for children with pragmatic language impairment delivered in mainstream schools

3. To explore parents‟ and teachers‟ perspectives on intervention generalisation.

Population:

Six boys with pragmatic language impairment, aged 5;11 to 9;9 years (mean 7;10 years) who met

the inclusion criteria of:

a communication impairment as measured by the Children‟s Communication Checklist;

no frank autism as indicated by the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI)

a judgement of pragmatic language impairment by two experienced SLTs.

Group was heterogeneous with respect to :

pragmatic skills-varying from subtle pragmatic problems to Asperger syndrome and

behavioural problems warranting additional school support.

expressive and receptive vocabulary and syntax scores- varying from normal to very low

receptive vocabulary test scores

Service setting:

Mainstream school

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model 1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

Senior SLT.

Domain of language:

Pragmatics- conversation, inference and narrative (deliberate exclusion of explicit teaching of

semantics and syntax

ICF-impairment, activity and participation

Type of intervention: No name for this

Method of

intervention:

Each child received an individually designed programme to meet their needs that incorporated

three common intervention aspects

1. The adaptation of language environment and classroom demands to suit the child. This

involved training of school personnel and parents

2. The children‟s social understanding developed by teaching concepts such as emotional

language, the vocabulary of social situations and insight into others‟ emotions

3. Teaching pragmatics skills such as exchange structure, turn-taking, topic management,

conversational skills, building sequences, cohesion and coherence in narrative and

discourse.

Individual goals derived from assessment data and in consultation with parents and teachers

with two common aims; to reduce verbosity and to improve coherence and quantity of

information offered in conversation and narrative.

Therapy techniques included modelling and individual practice, role-play, practising specific

pragmatic skills in conversations; meta-pragmatic therapy; promoting self-monitoring and

coping strategies; and rule-flouting exercises.

Quantum of service

provision:

20 sessions, 3 x week over 8 weeks (no indication of the time allocated to each session)

55

Outcomes evaluation Analysis of Language Impaired Children‟s Conversation (ALICC).

Discourse participation improved for all children meaning they dominated the

conversation less than at the pre-test.

Conversational dominance improved for one children, decreased for 2 and remained

stable for 3 children

Loquacity increased in three children and fell in one meaning they either increased or

decreased the proportion of unsolicited contributions than at the pre-test.

Assessment of Comprehension and Expression (ACE 6–11)

no significant group differences between the pre and post test scores for inferential

comprehension or narrative propositions with individual scores either decreasing,

increasing or staying stable

Clinical Evaluations of Language Fundamentals Test (CELF).

significant differences between the pre and post test scores for sentence recall subtest (Z =

-2.04, p = 0.04) and formulated sentences subtest (Z= -2.06, p=0.04).

Parent perceptions;

understanding of their child‟s difficulties had increased;

changed their approach to the child‟s communication;

their children benefitted from the therapy at school and the children were using useful

strategies at home.

Noted children had

o improved listening and comprehension skills

o volunteered more information

o better recall skills

o used more relevant language in conversation.

Teaching and support staff perceptions

felt involved in the intervention;

had gained skills

changed their approach to the child‟s communication;

used new skills in the general school environment.

They noted that the children had improved

o spontaneity in class discussions

o written work

o group listening skills

o ability to follow instructions in class

o concentration on tasks & increased independence to carry out tasks

o increased flexibility and willingness to experiment

Author’s conclusion:

The measured changes in pragmatics were associated with intensive speech and language

therapy with classroom support.

The conversation results from ALICC were mixed; few changes in inference and narrative

propositions from the ACE and the CELF sentence recall and formulated sentences subtests

changed despite these skills not being targeted directly.

Noted that the study was not an efficacy or effectiveness study

Emphasised the importance of collaboration between SLTs, caregivers and teachers for

intervention for children with pragmatic language impairment

Note that the optimal package of school-delivered speech and language therapy care for

children with language impairments is unknown and they caution policy makers against

making definitive decisions about this in the absence of such information.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The changes may be due to the intervention. However as it was not clear that the pre and post

baseline measures were stable, it is not clear whether the changes are due to intervention. Further

there were no fidelity measures. However, data analysis was performed blind to the data collection

sequence. It is not stated how long the sessions were. Two additional theoretical issues exist.

Firstly, it seems assumed that good pragmatics is based on equal sharing of conversational turns

this is not necessarily so. However equality of conversation turns seemed to be the benchmark for

judgement but this may not be appropriate for the context. Secondly, the standardised measures of

language changes are based on subtests rather than complete tests.

56

4. Anger, N., & Sullivan, L. (1995). Problem solving with parents and children. Australian Communication Quarterly Summer,

28-31.

Summary of intervention program: Cognitive therapy /Language impairment

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Case series

Level Other (Qualitative)

Aim of study:

Cognitive therapy for children with internalizing and externalizing behaviours involves verbalizing

instructions. Parents have an integral role to play in such therapy because of their influence in

shaping children‟s linguistic capacities and hence their internalized language and thought. The aim

of this intervention study was to bring about the supportive involvement of parents in a language-

based problem-solving group for children with social communication deficits.

Population:

N=9

2 girls and 7 boys, aged 9;10 to13;5 years, with normal IQ and judged by the SLP as

requiring assistance for social communicative deficits or poor functioning.

language skills on standardised tests scores were <-1 standard deviation below the mean

for 6 of the 9 children

8/9 rated by their parents has having behavioral problems at a clinical level a significant

clinical level.

Parents

Five mothers, one father, one custodial grandfather and 20-year old brother of one of the

children

Service setting:

Clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Group sessions (child group and parent group)

Provider(s):

Children

Two speech language pathologists and a clinical nurse

Parents group

A child therapist, a clinical nurse and a psychologist

Domain of language:

Pragmatics

ICF-activity and participation

Type of intervention:

Cognitive therapy

Method of

intervention:

Children‟s Group

Taught problem-solving techniques that were language-based and self reflective

Applied to everyday situations using 4-stage scripts of (1) identifying problems, ( 2)

allocating responsibility for the problem, (3) how to make considered choices to avoid

reactive or passive response (4) reflect on the outcome in order to plan for the future.

Encouraged to learn behavioural rules for containment and friendship to enhance the

group processes.

Activities were reinforced with role plays revolving around the session topics.

Parents Group

Over 5 concurrent sessions, parents engaged with information about the aims for their

children and their progress as well as learning techniques of child behaviour management.

A variety of techniques were used including cognitive therapeutic ones

57

Quantum of service

provision:

7x 1.5 hour group sessions with the children

5 concurrent group sessions with the parents with a follow up session six weeks later to assess

progress

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Pre, post and 6 week post group assessments conducted using

An informal rating scale/test that parents and children completed

o Children ratings indicated more use of appropriate social strategies

o Parent ratings indicated increased

sensitivity to their children‟s communicative problems although this

seems to have resulted in them identifying more problems in their

children

recognition of the efforts their children made to improve

recognition of their role in facilitating their children‟s development

Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) ratings

At the post test 2/9 carers noted improvements on the social dimension; 3/9 saw no

difference and 2/9 rated a higher level of social problems.

Direct observation of increased appropriate social behaviours as well as strategies

Author’s conclusion:

Efficacy of programme supported

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The reporting of data precluded supporting the claim that this program was efficacious.

58

5. Bayne, G., & Moran, C. (2005). The effect of single word semantic-phonological intervention on developmental word finding

difficulties at single words and discourse levels. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy, 60, 31-44.

Summary of intervention program: semantic-phonological intervention for expressive

vocabulary

Country: New Zealand

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Within subject multiple baseline across behaviours (ABA)

Level III-3

Children named experimental and control words in all phases

Aim of study:

Observing that programs that have semantic elaboration and others with phonological components

have decreased word finding problems, these authors proposed that a combined program may have

cumulative effects. The aim of this study was to examine the effectiveness of a single-word

semantic-phonological intervention in remediating word finding difficulties at the single word level

and determine whether this would transfer to the discourse level.

Population:

N= 3, 2 girls and 1 boy, aged .7; 1; 7;7 & 8;2 years

Ethnic backgrounds: New Zealand European; New Zealand Maori; and Samoan.

All spoke English, one also spoke Maori and another Samoan

Each participant met the following criteria:

Normal non-verbal test of intelligence (Test of Non-verbal Intelligence 3rd

edition)

At least 1 SD below the mean on an expressive language score (CELF-3; CELF-P)

Performed below criterion on either speed or accuracy in a rapid automatic naming task

(CELF-3)

At least 1 SD below the mean on word finding at the single –word level (TWF-2)

At least 1 SD below the mean on productivity or word finding behavior measures in word

finding at the discourse level (TWFD)

no diagnosed emotional, psychological, behavioural or neurological conditions

One participant had a mild-moderate speech disorder as measured by the Goldman Fristoe Test of

Articulation and the other two had speech production skills appropriate to their age and culture.

Service setting:

? school (not clearly specified)

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

Clinician (no further details)

Domain of language:

Expressive vocabulary

Type of intervention:

Semantics and phonology

Method of

intervention:

Semantic and phonological tasks were used to teach 11 new words in each of 8 of the 10 sessions.

Sessions 5 and 10 were review sessions. Within both children encouraged to complete tasks

independently and to use visual cues. The clinician also modelled the response and assisted

participant to imitate that model and other prompts were given or key features made more salient

Semantic tasks: providing a definition or attributes of target, stating its function, naming its

category, providing non-identical exemplars, describing similarities and differences between

target and other words and customary location of target using colour photographs and laptop

Phonological tasks: identification of initial phoneme, phoneme or syllable segmentation and

generation of words that rhymed with target using colour blocks and clapping.

Control words were also presented for naming so improvement due to word exposure rather

than intervention technique could be controlled

.

59

Quantum of service

provision:

10 x 1 hour sessions over 3 weeks

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Visual analysis of the percentage of naming errors indicated that in

Single word context

o decreased errors for trained words for all children

o Little change for control words for all children

Discourse context

o decreased errors for trained words for 1 child and little change for other two

o decreased errors for control words for 2 children and little change for 1 child

Percentage gain scores from pre-test to post-test

Single word context

o Gain scores for trained words were 4-8 times greater than for control words for

all children

o 2/3 had positive gains scores for control words

Discourse context

o Similar gains between control and trained words for all children

o The number of T-units used increased for 2 children but decrease for one

o Word finding behaviours decreased for 2 children but increased for one

Author’s conclusion:

Findings suggest that single-word semantic-phonological intervention is effective for remediating

word finding at the single word level but not at the discourse level. The participants had diverse

cultural and language backgrounds and speech and language profiles that were being managed via

varying methods. Despite these differences all children showed similar gains in single-word

production following intervention.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Outcomes demonstrate improvement in word-finding at the single word level, but were mixed at

the discourse level. There was no control group, and a small sample. It is not clear; whether the

children were having any other concurrent therapy, the setting for the intervention or who the

service provider was.

60

6. Bernhardt, B., & Major, E. (2005). Speech, language and literacy skills 3 years later: A follow-up study of early phonological

and metaphonological intervention. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40(1), 1-27.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological therapy

Country of

publication:

Canada

Publication type &

level of evidence:

descriptive, case study category

Level Other (The original study was Level III-3)

Aim To report on the long term outcomes of preschool children who had phonological therapy

Population:

12 children with moderate to severe phonological impairments aged between 3;3 and 4;11 years

of age at the commencement of the programme, followed up 3 years later at mean age 7;2 years.

The children had normal hearing and oral-motor function at the time of testing.

Service setting: Community, clinic-based individual therapy SLPs who received 2 days of training to learn the

analysis and treatment procedures

Service delivery

model:

Children participated with „significant others‟

Service delivery model One-on-one sessions

Provider(s): community speech-language pathologist

Domain of language: phonology

Type of intervention: non-linear phonological analyses and treatment approaches that emphasized the componential

structure of speech sounds, syllables and words

Method of intervention:

First 12 weeks addressed phonological goals, divided equally between syllable

structure and segments (phonemes) and features

Final 4 weeks also addressed metaphonological goals in rhyming, alliteration and/or

segmentation

Goals for each child were based on individual needs as determined by the analyses, in

accordance with three basic principles (strengths address phonological categories and

target non-default structure and features).

Quantum of service

provision:

45 min individual treatment sessions three times a week over 16 weeks

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

At 3 year follow up:

Phonology skills

o All children were

intelligible in conversation

in normal limits on the Word Discrimination subtest of the TOLD-2P

o 7/12 had average or above average GFTA-R scores

o Total meta-phonology scores ranged from 18 to ceiling of 60

Language skills

o Vocabulary

All children within normal limits on PPVT-R

o CELF

Sentence Structure subtest 12/12 within normal limits

Word Structure and Word Association 12/12 within normal limits

Recalling Sentences; 12/12 within normal limits

Auditory memory

o 11/12 children within normal limits on the WISC-III (Wechsler 1989) digit span

subtest.

All children scored within or above normal limits on the three subtests of the TONI-2

Reading PIAT-R

o Comprehensions and recognition 10/12 within normal limits; on the arithmetic

subtest, 9/12 within normal limits

o Spelling, 7/12 c within normal limits.

Only one preschool study variable was significantly correlated with later reading and

spelling skills: post-intervention metaphonology

61

Author’s conclusion:

Most children performed within normal limits on a number of speech, language and literacy

tasks, in spite of their early history of severe phonological impairments and delays in language

production and metaphonology. Early phonological and metaphonological intervention can

promote normalization of speech development, and normal acquisition of literacy skills for

children with severe phonological impairments. Not all children will demonstrate short-term

normalization, but the level and prevalence of risk can be reduced.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The phonological intervention appeared to lead to improved outcomes for children with

phonological impairments, both in the short term, and at 3 years follow-up. Although only a

small case series, it may give some indication that ongoing intensive therapy may not be

required for this population.

62

7. Boyle, J., McCartney, E., Forbes, J., & O'Hare, A. (2007). A randomised controlled trial and economic evaluation of direct

versus indirect and individual versus group modes of speech and language therapy for children with primary language

impairment. Health Technology Assessment, 11(25:iii-iv, xi-xii), 1-139.

Summary of intervention program: Language comprehension and expression at word,

sentence and discourse levels using either direct individual; direct group; indirect individual and

indirect group intervention

Country: Scotland

Publication type &

level of evidence:

RCT with allocation described. Children were assessed at pre-intervention and post intervention

and then 12 months later

Level II

Aim of study:

Determine the relative effectiveness of four modes of therapy for children with primary language

impairment and determine their cost effectiveness. The four modes are direct individual; direct

group; indirect individual and indirect group. Direct means a speech language therapist works with

the children whereas indirect means an speech language therapist assistant works with the children

under the guidance on an speech language therapist

Population:

152 Scottish children with primary language impairment, aged 6-11 years, with a

standard score of <-1.25 SDs on the CELF-3 UK on either the expressive or receptive

scales or combined,

non-verbal cognitive skills within normal limits on the WASI

hearing reported as normal

no moderate to severe articulation or phonology disorder

normal fluency.

Children (N=130) were randomly allocated to one of four intervention modes as described above

and the remaining 22 were the control group receiving the usual standard of care in the community.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Group and individual

Provider(s):

Speech language therapists and speech language therapist assistants

Domain of language:

Comprehension and expression at word, sentence and discourse levels

ICF-Impairment

Type of intervention:

Intervention especially designed for this project to ensure flexibility and replicability. A therapy

manual was developed.

Method of

intervention:

A therapy manual was developed that provided the principles and steps of intervention. The

intervention principles were

1. the formation of a strong therapeutic alliance between the SLT/A and child, focused on the

alleviation of communication problems

2. encouragement of child self-reflection and self-monitoring

3. repeated exemplification and practice of language features in a motivating context.

The steps and content were

Comprehension monitoring: designed to help children to identify speaker and listener aspects

of successful comprehension, and to seek clarification when they did not understand.

Vocabulary development: comprehending, learning and using words relating to concepts

relevant in schools, and teaching children self-cueing vocabulary items. Reflecting on

phonological and semantic aspects of target words were used and using memory and rehearsal

techniques.

Grammar: teaching age-appropriate understanding and use of grammar. Grammar markers

were taught in salient contexts and activities highlighting the relationships between syntactic

structures.

Narrative therapy: teaching comprehension and use of narrative in activities

63

Quantum of service

provision:

Each child had 20 hours of intervention- 3 x 30-40 min / week over 15 weeks.

Each speech language therapist and speech language therapist assistant saw 9-11 children per

week for the above time and, in addition, had time for planning and liaising with one another

that varied from 0.5-1.5 days per week per staff member

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

No significant differences existed for CELF-3UK

receptive, expressive and total scores or the

BPVS II for direct, indirect group or individual any of the types of therapy for T2 (all F values

were < 1.01, all p values >0.364) and T3 (all F values were < 3, all p values >0.086).

Children made significant gains for expressive language but not receptive language

Indirect therapy was the least costly

Author’s conclusion: 1. SLTAs can deliver services as effectively SLTs within primary schools to children with

primary language impairment who do not to require the specialist skills of speech language

therapists (i.e. speech is excluded).

2. They note their findings about the children‟s outcomes and the outcomes about cost can only

be generalised to other educational/health systems whereby the resources would equate to

those described in this study (i.e. therapy 3 times per week, well supported staff, a well

detailed therapy manual)

3. They also note that this study raised many issues that include the need for:

i. research into effective interventions for receptive language problems

ii. investigation of the efficacy of the relationship between dose and treatment effect in both

expressive and receptive language.

iii. Research into models of integrative service delivery, cluster models of delivery via

integrated community schools, and the involvement of class teachers, classroom assistants

and parents/carers.

iv. Research that identifies the characteristics of children who are most likely to succeed with

indirect intervention approaches,

v. evaluating alternative methods of working with those who may benefit from different

modes.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This study indicated that a speech pathology program is effective for a proportion of children with

primary language problems when it has the following characteristics

well resourced (for example, SLTs had one day per week allocated to liaison with the

SLTAs and had a caseload of 9 children)

integrated within a school,

is well planned

explicitly negotiated and supported by senior management

provides intensive therapy (3 times per week)

has a detailed manual

provides support for SLTAs.

However, it does not seem to be effective for children with receptive language problems. There is

little systematic evidence on whether the children were more effective communicators after the

completion of the program as the outcome measures only measured impairment not activity or

participation. There are some indications from the qualitative information that this was so but the

extent of this is not defined.

64

8. Campbell, M. & Vidler, K. (1996). Oral language literacy project. Australian Communication Quarterly Autumn, 25-27.

Summary of intervention program: Metalinguistics in phonology, semantics and syntax

Reference:

(Study 3 only because it addresses children 5 years and older with proven language impairment.

The other two did not)

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Study 3 - multiple baseline cohort study with two control groups and two experimental groups;

each with four children. Two groups assessed at the initial assessment to the end of the intervention

and the other two were assessed from the beginning of intervention to the end of the maintenance

Level III-2

Aim of study:

Establish the efficacy of a suite of speech and language services offered by the Queensland

Department of Education by examining three school based programs.

Population:

16 year 1 and 2 children (age not stated) with proven language impairment described as having a

24 to 36 month delay on the test Analysis of the Language of Learning.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

4 groups of 4 children

Provider(s):

Speech pathologists

Domain of language:

Metalinguistics in phonology, semantics and syntax

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention:

Method of

intervention:

Activities included Lindamood techniques and tasks that included sound-letter, word awareness

rhyming activities, work book ones, phonology-semantics-syntax absurdities, sound sequencing

and sentence formulation

Quantum of service

provision:

2 x week for 30 minutes each over 8 weeks

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Teacher knowledge of speech pathologists

o Not reported

Teacher observations of changes in children

o Children were more eager to engage in pre-literacy tasks

Analysis of the Language of Learning

o Year 1 Change in experimental groups was 16.75 compared with 2 control group

scores of 7.75 and 5.5

o Year 2 Change in experimental groups was 16.62 compared with control group scores

of 2.5 and -9.25

Author’s conclusion:

The program led to changes.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The program may have led to changes but the small group sizes may mean that these results are not

significant. Comparison groups were uneven in size and the results were not examined statistically

65

9. Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Knox, E., & Simkin, Z. (2002). Different school placements following language unit

attendance: which factors affect language outcome? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(2), 185-

195.

Summary of intervention program: Impact of attending school for specific language

impairment

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative study of two groups

Level III-2

Aim of study:

This study aims to ascertain the benefits children with SLI being enrolled in a language unit

beyond the age of 8 years.

Population:

The outcomes of children with specific language impairment who continued attending a language

unit from the age of 8 years were compared with those who left the unit at the age of 8 years and

went into mainstream school. The children were matched on non-verbal cognition, language

comprehension of syntax and language expression on a picture naming. Up until the age of 8 all

children attended units.

A total of 242 children attending language units were assessed at the ages of 7-, 8- and 11-years.

This study relates to the assessment at 11 years. Of the 222 children available for the 8-year-old

assessment, 62 children went to mainstream school. At the 11 year old assessments, 53 of these 62

children were available. They were matched with children still attending units.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Language unit with no other details provided

Provider(s):

Not specified

Domain of language:

Not specified

Type of intervention:

Not specified

Method of

intervention:

Not specified

Quantum of service

provision:

Not specified

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Children‟s educational placement

o that by 11 years-of-age, 43 of the 46 attending mainstream school at the age of 8

years were still doing so compared with 16 of the 46 who attended a language unit at

the age of 8 years. However there appeared to be differences in the extra assistance

the 11 year olds were receiving

language Scores

o children were tested against nine outcomes of language (past tense markers, EVT,

BPVS, TROG, reading single words). No group differences occurred at 11 years

Socio-cognitive variable

o subtests of the WISC-III, CCC, CARS and behavioural questionnaire indicated no

group differences at 11 years

Differences emerged for language skills when teacher opinion was factored in A MANOVA

showed main effect for teacher opinion for the comprehension variables

> 50% of the teachers were unhappy about some aspect of the child‟s placement. For some this

was a personal concern about being under-skilled or under resourced for teaching these

children.

66

Author’s conclusion:

It seems that education placement at 8 years does not change outcomes at 11 years. However, they

argued that the significant findings that were captured when teacher‟s opinions were factored in

indicate the need to capture this information when engaged in decision making.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Some important clinical points arise

The two groups may have been different on other areas that were not tested such as

discourse or meta-linguistics. IF the ICF framework had been used, this too may have

captured possible differences.

Teacher skills may influence outcomes

67

10. Cox, S. (2003). Using computer technology to teach phonological awareness skills ACQuiring Knowledge in Speech,

Language and Hearing, 5(3), 111-113.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology (Phonological software with or without adult

support v mathematical software with or without adult support.)

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative study

Level III-2

Aim of study:

To explore the use of computer technology to teach phonological awareness in children and to

evaluate the role of adult support while children were using computer technology.

Population:

32 children; 20 males and 12 females aged between 6;3 and 10;3 years

Each identified by their teacher has having difficulty with phonological awareness

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Small group (though on individual computers)

Provider(s):

Research 4th

year undergraduate honours student in speech pathology

Domain of language:

Phonology

Type of intervention:

Phonological software with or without adult support vs mathematical software with or without

adult support.

Method of

intervention:

Randomly assigned to one of four groups:

Phonological awareness software with high support of low support

Mathematical software with high support low support or low support (control)

High support: received support from researcher; keeping child focused and attending,

providing positive reinforcement and monitoring and adjusting the level of complexity of

tasks.

Low support: only assistance with computer related and technical problems.

Quantum of service

provision:

3 x 15 min sessions/ week over 5 ½ weeks (total 12 sessions; 3 hours)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

No statistical difference in phonological awareness scores between phonological awareness

software and mathematical software

No statistical difference in phonological awareness scores between high and low support

No interaction effect

Author’s conclusion:

The use of phonological awareness software alone used over an intensive period does not

significantly improve children‟s phonological awareness skills irrespective of the level of adult

support when the ratio is one adult to four children.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The use of phonological awareness software did not enhance phonological awareness skills nor did

mathematical software, with or without support. The small N and short timeframe may have

adversely affected results. Mathematical software participants (controls) also made unexpected

progress and therefore it could be questioned whether this was a true control.

68

11. Crosbie, S., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (2005). Intervention for children with severe speech disorder: A comparison of two

approaches. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 40(4), 467 - 491.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology (contrast therapy compared with core

vocabulary therapy).

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative study with ABAC study- Treatment 1 (B) was implemented after the baseline

period followed by a 4-week withdrawal period, followed by treatment 2 (C). Children were

allocated to different treatments in referral order

(level III-1)

Aim of study:

To evaluate the effect of two different types of therapy on speech accuracy and consistency of

word production of children with consistent and inconsistent speech disorder.

Population:

N= 18 (11 boys & 7 girls) 4;08 - 6;05 years, with a mean age of 6;02 years, who had:

Severity: standard score of 3 on PCC derived from Phonology Assessment of DEAP

[standard score mean of 10, normal range of 7–13]).

Subgroup classification: an inconsistent speech disorder or a consistent speech disorder.

Oromotor structure and skills: no structural problems on oral examination; normal range

on oromotor assessment of the DEAP (isolated movements of lips &tongue, sequenced

volitional movements and diadochokinetic skill (mean of three standard scores above 6).

Normal receptive language on Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals - Preschool

Normal non-verbal skills on the Visual-Motor Integration Assessment

Normal hearing as shown by the child‟s last hearing test.

Language background: monolingual speaker of English.

Ten children had an inconsistent speech disorder (7 boys and 3 girls) and 8 had consistent speech

disorder (4 boys and 4 girls). The two groups were comparable for age and severity of speech

impairment. (Inter-rater reliability for classification established)

Service setting:

School and home

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Individual therapy

Provider(s):

Two experienced paediatric speech language pathologists

Domain of language:

Phonology

Type of intervention:

Contrast therapy compared with core vocabulary therapy.

69

Method of

intervention:

Parents were asked to complete daily practise activities at home during the treatment blocks.

Phonological contrast therapy (targeting error patterns):

nondevelopmental patterns treated before developmental; consistency and frequency of

the use of the error pattern; effect on intelligibility of successful remediation; and

stimulability of the speech sounds required.

four stages: auditory discrimination; production in single words then in phrases (set and

spontaneous) then in sentences within conversation and movement between stages

occurred when 90% accuracy-training criterion was achieved.

When one error pattern had progressed to phrase stage another error pattern was targeted.

A minimal pair approach (sometimes with multiple oppositions) was used starting with

auditory discrimination, and then producing the minimal pairs (imitated then

spontaneously).

Core vocabulary therapy (targeting consistency of word production):

The child, parents and teacher selected a list of 50 intervention words with 10 randomly

selected each week.

the child‟s best production of each target word was achieved by teaching the word sound-

by-sound, using cues such as syllable segmentation, imitation and cued articulation

The child was required to say those ten words the same way throughout the week.

The parents and teacher practised the words daily with the child and reinforced the

selected way of saying the word.

If word production deviated from the best production, the clinician attended to this.

Any words they produced consistently were removed from the list of 50 words and new

ones added.

Quantum of service

provision:

16 30-minute sessions in each 8–9-week treatment block i.e. two sessions per week for 16-18

weeks with a 4 week break in between.

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Consistency of word production (4 findings)

1. Therapy positively affected speech

2. Core vocabulary resulted in greater change to consistency than phonological contrast

therapy

3. The consistency of the children with inconsistent speech increased most through core

vocabulary therapy and

4. The consistency of children with consistent speech disorder changed more when they

received phonological contrast therapy

o because there was a significant therapy effect (F(1,17)=5.62, p<0.05) ; a

significant group effect (F(1,17)=5.77, p<0.05) and significant interaction effect

(F(1,17)=13.79, p<0.005).

At follow up 8 weeks post therapy the consistency scores deteriorated

Speech accuracy (PCC)(4 findings)

Therapy positively affected speech (F(1,17)=4.52, p<0.05)

Phonological contrast therapy was more effective in changing the PCC than core

vocabulary therapy

no group effect (F(1,17)=0.98, p=0.34).

Phonological contrast therapy was most effective in changing the PCC of children with a

consistent speech disorder whereas, the PCC of children with inconsistent speech disorder

increased when they received core vocabulary therapy indicated by the significant

interaction between the type of therapy and subgroup of speech disorder (F(1,17)=18.75,

p<0.001).

All children maintained the accuracy gains made during therapy indicated at follow-up

testing at 8 weeks

At follow up there were no significant differences between children with inconsistent or consistent

speech disorder) on either the PCC measure (F(1,17)53.08, p50.99) or inconsistency score

(F(1,17)51.05, p50.32).)

Author’s conclusion:

The results indicate that different parts of the speech-processing chain respond differently to

therapy targeting different processing skills. A phonological planning deficit can be targeted

effectively using a whole word approach. A cognitive– linguistic deficit responds best to a

phonological contrast approach. Clinically, it is essential to differentially diagnose consistent from

inconsistent phonological disorders.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Phonological contrast therapy appears more effective for children with consistent speech disorder,

whilst core vocabulary therapy appears more effective for inconsistent speech disorders in children.

It was not clear whether there was consistency in the level of parental involvement in the home

sessions amongst the children.

70

12. Culatta, B., Setzer, L. A., & Horn, D. (2005). Meaning-based intervention for a child with speech and language disorders.

Topics in Language Disorders, 25(4), 388-401.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology and language using Modified Cycles

incorporated language activities

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Single case study

Level -Other

Aim of study:

To detail a 9-month section of a child‟s 5½-year speech pathology programme, from the time he

was 5;9 to 6;6 years

Population:

A boy, with a proven speech and language disorder, receiving therapy from the age of 4;2 to 9;9

years. At the age of 5;9 years, his performance IQ was in the 70s and non-verbal IQ was in the mid

40s. He had endured middle ear infections and in testing, he responded to speech in the sound field

at 10 dB and the in the headphones at 20dB. His play was described but is not detailed here. His

phonology was described qualitatively, indicating his phonemic inventory was almost complete for

initial consonants but no intervocalic and final consonants. Phonological processes occurring were

final consonant deletion, unstressed syllable deletion, cluster reduction, gliding and vocalization.

His output syntax was predominantly at the 2-word stage with some 3-word utterances and his

type-token ration was 0.25.

Service setting:

University speech-language clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

One to one with parental involvement

Provider(s):

University clinicians

Domain of language:

Phonology (increase intelligibility by decreasing final consonant deletion)

Language ( expressive grammar and output lexicon)

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention:

Phonology; Modified Cycles developing goals using principles of maximal contrasts and

phonological knowledge (Gierut)

Language; Not described as article focussed on the phonology aspect of the intervention

Method of

intervention:

Phonology The cycles approach utilised interactive routines, play scripts and story enactments.

Themes and target words were introduced through the use of books with a repetitive story or using

structured play routine that highlighted target words. This was followed by reciprocal exchanges in

which the clinician modelled target words, and then created opportunities for the boy to use them

during the activities. Contexts were systematically manipulated from simple, predictable routines

to more complex, less structured scripted play and story enactments. During the activities, the

clinician assumed a dominant character role at first and then exchanged roles with the boy,

providing him more opportunities to take verbal turns and to allow the clinician to model target

productions, guide the play, and illustrate response option.

Quantum of service

provision:

The frequency and time dedicated to therapy was not explicit. It appears that therapy occurred

weekly for 6 months given:

The article stated it was covering a period of 9 months

There was a 3-month summer break during this time ( pg 397)

Two indirect references to weekly therapy on pages 391 and 392

71

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Outcomes at the end of the 9 month section of therapy

Engagement in play and participation; Not reported here

Phonology; Percent of specific consonants correct in final position. The target phonemes were

/p/ 82%, /k/ 58%, /m/ 80%, /s/ and “sh” 12%. Generalization was claimed for final /z/ 1%

final „ch” 38% and final “j” 25%

Language; New two word forms, increased lexicon items. more 3 word utterances as well as

coding plurals with final /s/ an d/z/

Outcomes at conclusion of therapy when boy was aged 9;9 years

( Note; Results were described with no details of how measurements were ascertained)

Phonology o Output phonology: Intelligible to strangers but /r/ and /l/ errors persisted.

o Phonological awareness Reported difficulty with phonological awareness tasks as

well as spelling and reading CVC words.

Language (No measures were provided)

o Syntax; some morphemes errors in conversational speech. Used simple and complex

sentences

o Lexicon broadened with reduction in use of nonspecific markers and overused

phrases

o Higher level language problems skills persisted The apparent manifestation was his

tendency to attract attention by bothering the children around him when he did not

understand the language or the content matter

Academic skills o Reading and math abilities at first-grade level

o in his resource classroom, he required scaffolding to complete assignments and to

stay on task.

Social skills o Teacher described him as a "loner" in the playground. Accessing his peer group

appeared difficult but he did not seem to be bullied or teased.

Author’s conclusion:

o Significant progress in the target areas over the 9-month period and asserted that the meaning

based intervention used was responsible for this change.

o However, given his persisting problems at the age of 9 years, they indicated that other goals of

addressing higher level language and phonology needs may have been beneficial.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The study design does not include empirical measurements so it is not possible to determine if the

asserted changes did occur and further it is not possible to isolate the change agent. There were no

fidelity measures.

72

13. Dale, P. S., & Crain-Thoreson, C. (1996). Parent-child book reading as an intervention technique for young children with

language delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 16(2), 213-235.

Summary of intervention program: Teaching parents an interactive, responsive style of

communication with children and comparing teaching an interactive book reading program

with a conversational language program

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pseudo-randomised controlled trial as an experimental study (No description of allocation)

Level III-1

Aim of study:

To examine the value of parent education as a method of facilitating the language of children with

language delay. In particular, the study examined the value of teaching joint book reading as a way

of facilitating oral language development. The specific research questions were to :

1. compare the effect of instructing mothers of children with language delays in effective joint

book-reading techniques to those instructed in language facilitation techniques using

conversation by examining changes to their own language and their children‟s.

2. determine whether Whitehurst's Dialogic Reading Training Program changes parents'

language use during book-reading activities with their children

3. determine if this positively affects children's expressive language

4. determine whether there are predicable individual differences in children‟s response to this

training

Population:

Thirty-three mother-child dyads, accumulated over 2 years

Children

o 24 boys and 9 girls aged between 3 and 6 years (no breakdown to know how many

were over 5 year of age)

o mild-to-moderate language delays determined by PPVT-R and MLU from two

language samples,

Service setting:

Appeared to be conducted in a University speech pathology clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Groups of mothers (size of groups not indicated)

Provider(s):

Not stated

Domain of language:

Discourse

ICF: Impairment, activity and participation

Type of intervention:

Teaching parents an interactive, responsive style of communication with children

Method of

intervention:

Whitehurst's Dialogic Reading Training Program (Whitehurst et al., 1988).

In two sessions, parents viewed a videotaped presentation of seven ways of facilitating language

development through reading, followed by a group discussion. In session 1, 5 were addressed: (1)

Ask „what‟ questions, (2) follow the child's answers with questions, (3) repeat what the child says,

(4) help the child as needed, (5) praise and encourage the child, (6) shadow the child's interests,

and (7) have fun and in the second (l) Ask open-ended questions, and (2) expand what the child

says. A parent handout of these was provided. Books were selected for parents to take home that

told a story with vivid pictures and minimal text.

Conversational Language Training

In two sessions, parents viewed commercial videotape presentations seven effective ways of

facilitating language development through conversation prepared for teachers, teaching assistants,

and parents of children in early childhood special education followed by a group discussion. In

session 1 four were addressed (1) Show your interest, (2) use information talk, (3) limit closed

questions, and (4) use indirect correction and the second session the reaming two were addresses

(1) Use information talk, (2) use expansions, and (3) ask open-ended questions. Relevant selected

toys were provided for the parents to take home.

73

Quantum of service

provision:

Three sessions that occurred over about 6 to 8 week

Session 1; pre test and one instructional unit asked to use the targeted conversational style at

home for 3 to 4 weeks

Session 2 ; second instructional unit asked to use the targeted conversational style at home for

3 to 4 weeks

Session 3 post test

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Dialogue Measures (coding of language samples)

Parents behaviour

3/8 behaviours were used more by Dialogic Reading Training Program groups than

Conversational Language Training group. (wh-questions, imitation and open-ended

questions) There was no change in the total number of parent utterances between the pre

and post tests meaning these results were not a function of increased talking by parents

For behaviours predicted to increase irrespective of which program was used, there

seemed to be context effects. Parents taught the Dialogic Reading Training Program used

significantly more wh-questions and expansions than the other group but this change was

only apparent in the one of the post test activities: book activity and not in the

conversation task. Similarly, parents taught the Conversational Language Training used

significantly more expansions in the post test task of book reading. These results

combined indicated that parents learning of these skills were constrained to the context in

which they learnt them

Children‟s Behaviours

Overall engagement of the children was rated as high

No overall program effect apparent

One of the 9 coded behaviours to show a differential effect was verbal responses to adult

questions, which increased more in the Dialogic Reading Training Program group,

F(1,30), 12.61, p < .01. This effect may consequent to the increased rate of wh-questions

from parents in the DRTP group.

Child Language Measures

Program effects

o significant for number of different words used, with more increase observed in the

Dialogic Reading Training Program group.

o For MLU, there was no significant difference between the change scores (post test

score- pre-test score) between the programmes MLU increased in both groups from

pretest to post-test, but significantly only for the Dialogic Reading Training Program

group during the book-reading, t(15) = 2.48, p < .05.

post test context effects

o significant effect for number of different words (greatest in play)

o verbal questions about story/topic (decrease in play but not in book reading)

o More changes were apparent in the toy play context than book reading

Interaction effects

o Total number of utterances increased in book-reading for the Dialogic Reading

Training Program group, but decreased for the CLTP group, whereas there was a

modest increase for both groups in the play episode.

o For number of different words used, the post test context effects effect was largely

due to the Dialogic Reading Training Program group.

Predictors of change

Increases in parental asking yes/no and wh-questions coupled with increasing children‟s

time for responding were associated with increased child engagement , MLU and

vocabulary

Author’s conclusion:

Parents of young children with language delays can learn language facilitation techniques from

relatively brief and simple instructions. As there were significant differences between the groups,

this indicated that joint book reading does have a unique contribution to make to language

development in that parents language changes and this in turn seems to facilitate children‟s

language. As this was only demonstrated by increases in the children‟s language in the toy play

context, this implies that the book reading context is a useful context to teach these skills for both

children and the adults.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above but the conclusion may be better restricted to mother than parents because only mothers

were involved and there are demonstrated differences between the ways mothers and father

communicate with their .

74

14. Denne, M., Langdown, N., Pring, T., & Roy, P. (2005). Treating children with expressive phonological disorders: does

phonological awareness therapy work in the clinic? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 40(4), 493-

504

Summary of intervention program: Expressive phonology (Gillon Phonological Awareness

Training Programme)

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative study children randomly assigned to treatment or to no treatment pre and post testing

conducted by different clinicians. Groups were equivalent on selection criteria

Level III-1

Aim of study:

To examine the effectiveness of phonological awareness therapy under conditions more similar to

those prevailing in many speech and language therapy clinics.

Population:

Twenty children, aged 5 to 7 years with proven speech and language difficulties that were

predominantly expressive phonological nature.

All received a speech and language therapy assessment, and direct or indirect intervention,

before being referred to the study.

All the children were monolingual English speakers

Inclusion criteria:

o > 25th percentile on the Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices

o > 10th percentile on the British Picture Vocabulary Test

o > 10th percentile on the sentence structure subtest of the CELF

o < 10th

percentile on at least one subtest of the Phonological Abilities Test

o at least one phonological process as assessed on the South Tyneside Assessment of

Phonology (STAP).

Service setting:

Community clinics

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Small groups (3 children)

Provider(s):

Speech and language therapists

Domain of language:

Expressive phonology

ICF- Impairment

Type of intervention:

Gillon Phonological Awareness Training Programme

Method of

intervention:

Therapy developed phoneme awareness and grapheme/phoneme knowledge rather than correct

speech production.

Tasks included segmenting and blending sounds, identify the number of syllables in word,

rhyming identification and production tasks, phoneme manipulation tasks, developing

grapheme to phoneme correspondences.

corrective feedback was given when errors occurred in speech production,.

Quantum of service

provision:

1.5 hour x weekly sessions over 8 weeks (2 hours of therapy) [used a treatment regime that was

more consistent with the resources available in UK clinics.

75

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Phonological awareness (assessed with Phonological awareness test & Non-Word Decoding Test )

All children improved their phonological awareness scores over time (F(1,17)=42.72,

p<0.001) and the treated group improved significantly more than the untreated

group(significant interaction of groups by time (F(1,17)=10.78, p<0.01).

Literacy skills (Wechsler Objective Reading Dimension (WORD) & the Non-Word Decoding

Test)

All children improved in reading and spelling on the WORD (reading (F(1,17)=24.30,

p<0.01) and spelling (F(1,17)=26.410, p<0.01)). There was no group effect.

On non non-word reading the treated group made most progress (significant interaction

effect F(1,17)511.2, p,0.05).

Speech production (the STAP).

Improved significantly in all children (F(1,17) 42.295, p<0.001) with no group effect.

Author’s conclusion:

Phonological awareness improved but no effect was seen on literacy or speech production. It

appears clinicians who want to exploit the benefits that phonological awareness therapy offers,

must offer more therapy. Clearly we require more research on the minimum duration of therapy

that is required and, given the variation in the progress of children seen in this study, on the

identification of those children who may need more or less therapy to benefit.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Results showed significant improvement on phonological awareness in the treated group compared

with the control. However, similar effects were not seen for literacy or speech. The limited amount

of therapy may have impacted on this. However, the study was trying to replicate resource

availability within these clinics. The sample size was small, less than suggested by Gillon to obtain

80% power.

76

15. Dodd, B., Crosbie, S., McIntosh, B., Holm, A., Harvey, C. Liddy, M., Fontyne, K., Pinchin, B., & Rigby, H. (2008). The

impact of selecting different contrasts in phonological therapy. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(5) 334-

345.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological contrast therapy (minimal pairs vs

non-minimal pairs)

Country: Australia

Publication type & level of

evidence:

Randomised independent group trial

3 assessments of children; baseline, end of treatment & 8-10 weeks after

treatment

the first four authors jointly selected the targets and prepared the materials for

clinicians to ensure consistent decision making, intervention planning and

materials

Discussion at handover to each clinician by one of the first four authors, included

provision of information regarding the intervention process (to ensure consistency

of feedback given, activities used and session structure)

no direct observation of sessions

Level III-1

Aim of study:

Compare outcomes based on two approaches to goal selection for contrast therapy;

traditional minimal pairs and non-minimal approach

Population:

N= 19 children, 11 boys and 8 girls, 3;11 to 6;5 years, with proven moderate to severe

phonological disorder receiving speech-language services from the authors prior to the

study commencing.

Heterogeneous group for previous intervention but no child received intervention for

four months prior to study.

Inclusion criteria: (1) Severity: standard score of 3–5 on the PCC measure on DEAP)

(2) Consistent error patterns (3) No oromotor structure and skills problems (4) Normal

Receptive Language measured by PPVT-3 (5) Monolingual speaker of English

Service setting:

Education Queensland (Australian state government service provider) and the University of

Queensland

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

Eight (community and/or academic) SLPs all with 12 + years of experience

Domain of language: Phonology

ICF- Impairment

Type of intervention: Contrast therapy

77

Method of intervention:

Phonological contrast intervention

new target contrasts were discussed by telephone or e-mail, clinicians received new

materials and two case discussion meetings involving all therapists were held during

the course of the study

Targets were selected for each child based on clinical judgement of impact on

intelligibility of successful remediation and sound-stimulability assessment

non-developmental patterns were targeted before developmental patterns

Developmental error patterns were generally addressed in the following order:

stopping, cluster reduction (s þ consonant clusters before plosive þ glide clusters),

fronting, gliding, voicing.

All target sounds were stimulable

4 steps of intervention for each error pattern (1) auditory discrimination, (2) single

words, (3) phrases and (4) sentences within conversation

Games involved child producing word pairs

Non-verbal and verbal feedback was provided

90% accuracy-training criterion was required to move from word to phrase to sentence

A new error pattern was introduced when an error pattern moved into the phrase stage

Minimal or near-minimal contrasts:

Word pairs differed by one sound in voice, place or manner; contrasting singleton

sounds belonged to the same major sound class (obstruents or sonorants)

Word pairs contrasted the child‟s target and error sound

For clusters, the error form was always compared with the target

A range of clusters could be targeted for any child within their phonological pattern

Non-minimal contrasts:

target sound was paired with a word that differed by one sound that differed to the

target sound voice, place and manner

For clusters, the error form was never compared with the target

Word pairs differed in terms of obstruent-sonorant or marked-unmarked sound classes

Quantum of service

provision:

12 x 30-minute weekly individual therapy sessions (total 6 hours)

Outcomes evaluation (by

disorder subtype if

possible):

Therapy improved all children‟s speech accuracy as reflected by:

o a significant difference in pre and post PCC (t (df 18)=9.25, p<.001), PPC (t

(df 18)=8.89, p<.001), and the number of error patterns suppressed during

therapy (t (df 18)=8.23, p<.001).

No difference between the groups:

o between pre-and post PVC (t (df 18)=1.47, p=1.60)

o across the three speech measures, nor on any of the individual measures

(PCC, PVC or PPC) indicated by multivariate analysis of variance

o in number of error patterns suppressed during therapy

o in the number of contrasts targeted in the minimal contrast group (mean=3.9,

SD 1.5) as compared to the group exposed to non-minimal contrasts

(mean=3.0, SD 1.1) indicated by one-way ANOVA

o in terms of increased speech repertoire of singletons or clusters indicated by

one-way analysis of variance

At 8–10 weeks post-intervention, paired-samples t-test indicated that 14 of the 19

tested:

o continued to improve between the end of therapy and the maintenance

assessment,

o no differences between the two groups

Author’s conclusion:

Children‟s speech improved significantly from therapy in accuracy and number of

error patterns suppressed and these gains were maintained for of 14 of the 19 at 8-10

weeks post intervention

No group differences in the progress made by children receiving minimally or non-

minimally paired intervention stimuli during therapy or in maintenance.

These gains were achieved with only 6 hours of therapy apportioned over 12 weeks

Setting maximal contrast goals rather than minimal contrast goals may be more cost

effective as the latter take longer to prepare than the former

Reviewer’s conclusion:

Both types of phonological contrast therapy (minimal contrasts and non-minimal contrasts)

appeared to be equally effective in improving children‟s speech accuracy, however there

78

was no control group with which to compare this outcome

Questions arising:

Assumption that previous therapy does not influence later gains so this may have had

an impact

No information on attendance rate and what parents may have done at home

79

16. Ebbels, S., & van der Lely, H.K.J. (2001). Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for children with severe persistent SLI.

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36 Suppl:345-50.

Summary of intervention program: syntax for older children with specific language

impairment

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Time-series design with established baseline pre-therapy measures.

Level III-3

Aim of study:

To evaluate meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for addressing a core deficit in some

children with „grammatical-SLI‟ in syntactic „movement‟, i.e. forming dependent relations between

lexical items which are needed for grammatical interpretation or production of sentences

Population:

N= 4 children with severe persistent SLI (3 boys and 1 girl) aged 11; 8-12; 9 years. Language

scores on the CELF-R UK 3 standard deviations below the mean; British Picture Vocabulary

Scales (BPVS-II) (Dunn et al. 1997) between from -2 to -3.3 standard deviations; TROG ranged

between –1.65 and more than -2.3 standard deviations

Visual perceptual skills (Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (Gardener 1988)) within the normal range

Service setting:

Specialist school in the UK for their language needs

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model 1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

SLT.

Domain of language:

Syntax-wh questions (which, who, what) and passives

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention:

meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding (drawing on principles from colourful semantics)

Method of

intervention:

shape coding system to make syntactic structures concrete. Phrases are determined and then

associated with a shape. The shapes are moved

comprehension therapy taught the children to recognise and use syntactic cues necessary for

correct interpretation of „wh‟ questions and passives. They were taught to identify gaps in

object questions and passives, to link these with the moved constituent (or „wh‟ word) and

assign the correct thematic roles.

Expressive therapy taught rules about, „do‟ support and movement of tense and aspect

inflections in „wh‟ question

Quantum of service

provision:

Each participant received 13 half-hour sessions training on the coding system.

2 blocks of 10 week therapy totalling 7-8 hours.

An additional block on „wh‟ as it required more teaching.

Then a period of no therapy followed.

Re-testing took place after each block of therapy and at follow-up (after ten weeks without

therapy) to establish maintenance of new skills.

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Proportion corrects from all testing

1. comprehension and expression of passives

¾ made significant progress

Variable across participants with reference to comprehension and expression

2. „wh‟ therapy

Comprehension

2/4 made significant progress on one of the four aspects taught. For both is was „which‟ in

object position

Expression

All made some significant progress on one of the three aspects taught

80

Author’s conclusion:

Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding was effective in improving the performance of at least

three out of four participants. The aspect of grammar that it seemed to assist was comprehension of

grammatical structures which involve „movement‟ involving dependent structural knowledge

between sentence constituents.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding was effective in bringing about some change in

comprehension. This is significant given the resistance to change that has been reported. The

observation that change seemed selective to just some of the aspects implies that different aspects

of grammar may need different interventions and adds to the consensus of the need for

individualised planning that targets the deficit. This is an important study is that it is one of the few

that focus on children, older than 8 years.

81

17. Ebbels, S. (2007). Teaching grammar to school age children with specific language impairment using Shape Coding SLI: A

randomized control trial. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 23(1), 56-93.

Summary of intervention program: syntax for older children with specific language

impairment

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Studies 1 & 2 Single case design study ABA design

Level III-3

Study 3 Pre-post test with no comparison

Level IV

Aim of study:

To evaluate an approach to teaching grammar to school-aged children with specific language

impairment (specific language impairment using s shapes, colours and arrows to make the

grammatical rules of English explicit.

Population:

Study 1

N= 3 children with SLI and were 11- to 12-years old at initial testing and 12 to 14 at the time when

they received therapy targeted at the dative construction. All had severe receptive and expressive

difficulties (> 2 SDs below the mean on the CELF, TROG and BPVS) but normal visual perceptual

skills (Test of Visual Perceptual Skills (Gardner, 1988)) .Note these children are three of the same

children reported by Ebbels and van der Lely (2001).

Study 2

N=2 2 of the above 3 children

Study 3

N=9 children with SLI aged 9-11 years (no other details provided)

Service setting: Specialist school l in the UK for their language needs

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Studies 1 & 2 1:1 intervention; Study 3 in class

Provider(s): Studies 1 & 2 speech language therapist; Study 3; not clear.

Domain of language:

Syntax-Study 1 comprehension and expression of dative; Study 2 comprehension and expression of

dative Comparative questions; Study 3 past tense marker;

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention: Shape Coding

Method of

intervention:

The shape coding system was using in all three studies with the following steps common to them

all

Step 1 Taught the system first i.e. to associate different grammatical constituents with different

shapes and codes and then create sentence templates with the shapes and codes

Step 2 For each of the targeted syntax structures named above, there was focus on the relevant

shapes for the target and developing the specific template

Step 3 This template was contrasted with another simular structure (e.g. the two forms of the

dative; one with a prepositional phrase and the other without (She gave the ball to him vs She

gave him the ball) with the templates

Step 4 as above but without templates and encouraging the child to self evaluate their response

prior to receiving feedback

Quantum of service

provision:

Studies 1 & 2 1:1 intervention for one term (frequency not stated);

Study 3 in class 1 hour per week for the autumn school term

82

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

For studies 1 & 2 Baselines were established the year before this intervention began with 4

measurements one week apart followed by another 4 measurement one term apart Study 1 (dative)

2/3 children made good progress shifting from pre therapy baseline means of 2 and 4 to 100 each.

The third child made no change.

Study 2 (comparative questions)

The two children made good progress shifting from pre therapy baseline means of 35% and 2% to

83% and 100% respectively.

Study 3 (past tense marker) (pretest-post test)

As a group there was no differences between pre and post test scores but if the outliers were

removed there was. Removing the one child with limited verb usage resulted in a significant group

difference (t(7)-4.46, p 0.001, d= 3.70) or the two children who received additional paired therapy

and using their final scores (t(8)-4.46, p 0.001,d = 5.88).

Author’s conclusion:

Shape coding intervention is effective for some older children with SLI. However the individual

variation suggests that it does not suit all children and their individual needs warrant further

consideration in practice and research.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Shape coding intervention may be effective for some older children with SLI. However, some

threats exist These include

No blind assessment or analysis of the data

No controls for the third study

83

18. Ebbels, S., van der Lely, H. K. J., & Dockrell, J. E. (2007). Intervention for verb argument structure in children with persistent

SLI: A randomized control trial. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(5), 1330-1349.

Summary of intervention program: syntax for older children with specific language

impairment

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

RCT (allocation with random numbers) with blind assessment

Level II

Aim of study:

To investigate theories of verb argument structure, two different treatments addressing this were

compared in children with specific language impairment. Specifically the aims were to determine;

1. the effectiveness and efficiency of semantic-syntactic therapy relative to semantic therapy

2. whether these interventions address linking (verb) arguments to the correct syntactic positions

and using obligatory and optional (verb) arguments- two areas that pose particular difficulty

for children with specific language impairment

Population:

27 pupils, aged 11;0 to 16;1 years (mean 13;4 years) with specific language impairment (10 girls

and 17 boys). All pupils attended a specialist school in the UK for their language needs. Children

were included if they had:

Intelligible spontaneous speech

No hearing loss

no frank autism as indicated by the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI)

no neurological dysfunction

no structural abnormalities

performance on the full score of the CELF-3 - 1 SD below the mean

Children were randomly allocated to one of 3 therapy groups

Service setting:

Specialist school l in the UK for their language needs

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model 1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

SLT.

Domain of language:

Syntax-verb argument structure

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention: Semantic therapy (drawing on principles from colourful semantics) versus semantic-syntactic

therapy

Method of

intervention:

All children received their usual care package which included specialist teaching and therapy at

school as well as the therapy associated with the study.

Syntactic-semantic therapy

shape coding system to make syntactic structures concrete

therapy focussed on linking particular syntactic structure with the two general verb meaning -

change of location and change of state

Sequence of sessions; 1 & 2- change of location verbs, 3-6 change of state verbs;7-9

alternating verbs

Activities for both verb types involved sentence making with and without the template with

therapist and child switching roles from acting to observing each other

Semantic therapy

developed written definitions of verbs by joint brainstorming the meanings of one or two verbs

each session with the therapist using trigger activities to add absent meanings

verb presentation sequence as above

child actively involved in tasks to reflect on the meanings

compared and contrasted verb meanings

control group

focussed on developing skills of inferencing by looking for clues to meaning

filled in missing information from stories with missing lines

84

Quantum of service

provision:

9 consecutive weekly 30 minute (4.5 hours of therapy)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Mean normalized gain score in overall argument structure score (proportion correct) from first to

second assessment.

Main effect of group F (2,24) = 6.12, p=007 with post hoc testing indicating no difference

between Semantic and syntactic-semantic therapy but significant differences between these

two groups and the control group

There were significant gains to both the targeted and control verbs indicating generalisation to

untrained verbs. At follow up this was only significant for the semantic condition not the

syntactic-semantic condition

Secondary measures

Significant gains existed for both experimental treatment groups for linking but not for the use

of obligatory and optional arguments.

No changes occurred in morphology, indicating specificity of treatment to verbs .

Author’s conclusion:

Both forms of experimental treatment were effective in treating some aspect of verb argument

structure with neither being more efficient than the other. Further, they have a specific and

intended effect on verbs

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Therapy that focuses on semantics or syntax as described here can positively affect the correct use

of verbs within targeted sentences both at the end of the therapy period and 3 months follow-up.

However, it is not clear whether this makes a difference to well being in that there is no

information about whether it generalises to the activity and participation levels.

This is an important study is that it is one of the few that focus on older children, that is older than

8 years.

85

19. Forrest, K., & Elbert, M. (2001). Treatment for phonologically disordered children with variable substitution patterns.

Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(1), 41-45.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology using the traditional approach

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

A multiple baseline across behaviours with /r/ as the control sound for all subjects

Level III-3

Aim of study:

The presence of variable substitutions for one sound seems to compromise successful intervention.

In the current report, the results are presented of a successful treatment procedure used with

children with variable error patterns who did not evidence any inventory expansion as a result of

prior treatment.

Population:

Four boys aged 4;11, 5;1,5;1 and 5;3 years with proven speech sound disorder and proven normal

language, hearing and of the oral-mechanism

Speech; Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation) indicated multiple articulation errors which

ranged from below the 1st percentile to the 15th percentile.

Language within normal limits CELF-Preschool, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised

Service setting:

University clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

University clinic

Provider(s):

Graduate student-clinician

Domain of language:

Phonology (fricatives that were missing from the inventory

ICF-impairment

Type of intervention:

Traditional

Method of

intervention:

Treatment began at the word level with imitation with continuous reinforcement, correction and

modeling. Once 90% criterion was reached, reinforcement frequency was decreased. Once

criterion reached spontaneous productions in response to pictures was required by their pictorial

representations, were targeted. Generalization probes were conducted on alternate weeks.

Quantum of service

provision:

2 x 45 minute sessions per week.

The number of sessions was variable and treatment was continued until the child reached a level of

~68% generalisation

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

All children learned the target sound in word-final position in all treatment words and generalized

this knowledge to the probe words that contained the treatment sound in word-final position.

Although three subjects evidenced some generalization to other word positions, only subject 3 met

the generalization criterion for treatment termination. Correct production of 72% of the words on

the generalisation probe was noted for this subject.

Author’s conclusion:

Results of the current research demonstrate that treatment that starts with easier positions within a

word may help provide the foundation for learning the omitted sound.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

No data was provided to verify the results so it is not clear that intervention was responsible for the

change

86

20. Gardner, H. (2006). Training others in the art of therapy for speech sound disorders: An interactional approach Child

Language Teaching and Therapy, 22 (1), 27-46.

Summary of intervention program: phonology using education of teachers assistants

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Qualitative

Level- Other

Aim of study:

The aim of this study was to describe the outcomes of a pilot study to evaluate of an interactional

training programme for speech and language therapists to use with school support assistants,

students and parents to enhance their implementation of therapy for children with speech sound

difficulties.

Population:

Six participants were involved;

two learning support assistants working within a mainstream school with a language resource

base, to which children were referred.

two speech and language therapy students in the second year of a preregistration course.

two children aged 5 and 6 years, with severe persisting speech sound disorders who were

receiving individual and group therapy at school.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Collaborative therapy sessions whereby speech pathologists trained school support staff to enhance

their implementation of therapy for children with speech sound difficulties

Provider(s):

learning support assistants and SLT students under the supervision of SLTs

Domain of language:

Phonology

ICF-impairment

Type of intervention:

This training was designed to complement any type of intervention for speech sounds

Method of

intervention:

Learning support assistants and SLT students were taught the “Talking about Speech” programme

which had been developed from conversational analysis theory as well as empirical data. The aim

of this program is for participants to develop their interactional discourse skills that have been

shown to form a unique part of an SLT‟s professional knowledge, and are critical to intervention

success for children with speech sound disorders. The programme covered listening tasks,

modeling, imitation and eliciting spontaneous “tries” of the target sound from the child. This study

focused on teaching adults to modify their response to the children so that the adults y provided

children with more information about the target sound, such as phonetic details. Methods of cueing

and prompting and how to manipulate the scale of support were explained.

Quantum of service

provision:

1 session per week for 6 weeks(Length of sessions not specified)

Each child worked with one LSA (from their class) and with one student, consecutively each

week, for the duration of the training programme.

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

The qualitative analysis of the data indicated that

learning support assistants and SLT students demonstrated increased use of target behaviours

children improved

The learning support assistants and SLT students reported that they

felt more positive about their role in therapy

had more understanding about the therapeutic process.

used the techniques outside the therapeutic setting in the classroom and in conversation with

the child

Author’s conclusion:

1. This pilot research showed that this highly specific training resulted in positive and

measurable changes in terms of targeted interactional outcomes for both staff and the children.

However, further research is required to determine the benefits for children.

2. The fact that the learning support assistants and SLT students reported that they used the

techniques outside the therapeutic context is an important development in settings where

designated one-to-one time can be rare.

3. Whether this classroom-based input results in better generalization of speech skill for the child

is another future investigation.

87

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As the author indicated, the focus of this study was on training the trainers rather than speech

outcomes for children. Thus the children‟s speech outcomes are less clear, especially as they were

measured part way through the children‟s therapy program rather than at its completion.

However in the context of this review, this study raises important questions about the deployment

of speech pathology resources; Should they be used to train trainers or in teaching the children?

The assistants‟ perceptions that they were using the techniques with in activity and participation

levels (ICF) is noteworthy but it needs to be noted that this is self report rather than established

empirically.

88

21. Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (1995). The effects of training phonological, semantic and syntactic processing skills in spoken

language on reading ability. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 58-68.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pseudo-randomised controlled trials .Alternating treatment design (ABACA) with random

allocation of the 10 students to each type of therapy. Allocation method not described

Level III-2

Aim of study:

Evaluate the efficacy of a program designed to remediate the spoken language deficits of students

with a specific reading disability.

Population:

Ten students, aged 10 to 12 years, with proven language and reading disability over a two year

period prior to the trial, on the CELF and NARA respectively. All students passed the cognition

test (TONI) and receptive vocabulary (PPVT).Children randomly allocated to two groups with no

group differences existed at the pre-intervention assessment.

Children attended school through out the program and received not other reading support during

the program

Service setting: School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Group therapy (2 x 5 children)

Each group received both types of intervention (see below) in counter-balanced order

Group

Provider(s):

SLP

Domain of language:

Phonological processing, semantics and syntax

ICF- Impairment

Type of intervention:

Phonological sub skills training vs Semantic and syntactic skills

Note; Group 1 Phonological training followed by semantic /syntactic training and Group 2

semantic /syntactic training followed by phonological training

Method of

intervention:

Phonological sub skills.

Phonological sub skills training was based on the Auditory Discrimination program

(Lindamood & Lindamood, 1975) focussing on segmenting, manipulating and blending speech

sounds in syllables.

Semantic and syntactic skills

Using published resources, syntax enhancing activities were completing, formulating and

deconstructing complex and simple sentences. Semantic and syntactic enhancing activities

were thematic activities that had integrated vocabulary building activities to expand and

creating sentences with the new vocabulary items

Quantum of service

provision:

2 hours per week x 6 weeks for each program with a 6-week break between programs

89

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Overall outcomes

Reading scores (NARA)

Significant improvement effect (ANOVA F (2, 18) = 18.35 p<.001). Post hoc testing

indicated intervention accounted for improved reading scores (t(9) = 6 .43 p<.001)

Specific outcomes

Lindamood Auditory conceptualisation test

After treatment 1, group 1 (phonology first) significantly outperformed group 2

(syntax/semantics first) (t(9) = 4.15 p<.01) but after treatment 2, group 2 significantly

improved (t(9) = 4.8 p<.01). By the end of both treatment phases 7/10 subjects received

100% on this test

Spelling real words, Spelling non words and Spoonerisms

After treatment 1, group 1 (phonology first) significantly outperformed group 2

(syntax/semantics first) on each of these tasks respectively (t(9) = 2,34 p<.01); (t(9) =

7.02, p<.01) (t(9) = 3.21, p<.05) but by the end of treatment 2, group 2 had improved on

all these tasks

Formulated sentences task

After treatment 1, group 2 (syntax/semantics first) significantly outperformed group 1

(phonology first) (t(9) = 2.8, p<.01) but after treatment 2, group 1 had improved. By the

end of both treatment phases all subjects were within the normal range on this task.

Effects of training program on reading achievement

The phonology training had significantly more impact on reading accuracy than

comprehension but both programs had an equally significant impact on reading

comprehension. Qualitative analysis indicated that four children made greater

comprehension gains with phonological intervention than semantic/syntactic intervention.

There was no order effect.

Author’s conclusion:

Intervention addressing the underlying phonologic, semantic and syntactic deficits improved

reading accuracy and comprehension. Phonological training enhances reading accuracy more than

syntax/semantics training. Reading comprehension is differentially affected by phonological

training or syntax/semantics training depending on the pre-test phonology skill. For the children

with the most severe phonology problems, comprehension improved most when therapy focus was

phonology but for the others with higher phonology scores, comprehension improved most when

therapy focus was syntax/semantics training.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above but noting that these conclusions need to be mitigated in light of small groups.

90

22. Gillon, G, & Dodd, B. (1998). A developmental case study of a child with severe reading disability: Response to specific

intervention programmes. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy, 52, 9-21.

Summary of intervention program:

Country: Australia (Queensland)

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Longitudinal case study.(One of the children described by Gillon & Dodd, 1995)

Level Other

Aim of study:

A post hoc study of a child who participated in a longitudinal group study designed to improve

reading decoding and comprehension through facilitating phonological, semantic and syntactic

skills. This case study was conducted to tease out this child‟s different response pattern to the

group.

Population:

A boy, aged 8;9 years, with proven higher level language problems and a reading impairment.

Service setting:

school

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Group intervention

Provider(s):

Speech pathologist.

Domain of language:

speech production and phonological awareness

ICF - Impairment

Type of intervention: Phonological processing skill development and semantic and syntactic training

Method of

intervention:

Phonological processing skill

Two step coding activity based from the work of the Lindamoods. Children learnt to represent

the sounds with the coloured blocks and then read simple and complex syllables

semantic and syntactic training

syntax; activities from published activity books decomposing and formulating sentences

semantics thematic approach to develop vocabulary and sentence structure

Quantum of service

provision:

Two blocks of 12 hours of intervention; each was conducted for 2 hours per week for 6 consecutive

weeks separated by six weeks

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Increased reading ability was only associated with the phonological processing work and not the

syntactic/semantic work, whereas the latter work was associated with an increase in language

skills. However, a severe reading (decoding) problem persisted.

Author’s conclusion:

That the work targeting his phonological processing skills resulted in increased phonological

processing skills and reading decoding. By contrast, the syntactic/semantic work seemed to yield

improved language skills but did not seem to affect reading decoding. However, a severe reading

(decoding) problem that persisted could plausibly link to his poor phonological processing skills..

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above

91

23. Gillum, H., Camarata, S., Nelson, K. E., & Camarata, M. N. (2003). A comparison of naturalistic and analog treatment effects

in children with expressive language disorder and poor preintervention imitation skills. Journal of Positive Behavior

Interventions, 5(3), 171-178.

Summary of intervention program: Expressive language (imitation treatment v conversation

recast treatment)

Country: US

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Case series

Level IV

Aim of study:

As one predictor of the success of different intervention techniques appears to be pre-intervention

imitation skills, the aim of this study was to compare the treatment effects of naturalistic

(conversation recast) and analog imitation treatment in children with expressive language disorder

and poor imitation skills( i.e. imitation skill was kept constant).

Population:

N= 4 children (1 girl; 3 boys), aged 4;3 – 6;8 years (mean 5;5 years) with low imitation skills being

at least 1.5 SD below the mean on the sentence imitation subtest of the TOLD-2:P and with

expressive language disorder defined as :

at least 1.5 SD below the mean on one or more expressive subtests of the TOLD-2:P

significantly low mean length of utterance derived from language sample gathered with the

mother

Passing an audiometric screening prior to the onset of testing and training

Normal non-verbal cognitions as measured by the Leiter International Performance Scale

no reported history of frank neurological trauma or impairment or emotional disturbance

Met DSM-IV description for expressive language disorder

Service setting:

Clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

1:1

Provider(s):

Licensed speech-language pathologists

Graduate students enrolled in a speech-language pathology degree program under supervision

of a licensed SLP.

Domain of language:

Expressive language

Type of intervention:

Analog (imitation treatment) v naturalistic (conversation recast treatment)

92

Method of

intervention:

Intervention targets were syntax structures that were totally absent from language sampling

analysis and for each child the targets were randomly allocated to one of the training

techniques. Treatment scoring was completed by SLPs, graduate students or undergraduates

with specialised training.

Analog treatment

Step 1; Child imitated the target following a clinician model and prompt. Model and prompt

were paired with pictures or objects with verbal and/or token reinforcers following correct

responses and continues until 90% accuracy achieved

Step 2. child was prompted to label pictures or objects without clinician modelling.

Throughout sessions, free play time occurred to determine if child was generalising.

Naturalistic training

Clinician structured the setting to obligate the use of the targets

The play activities included naturalistic interaction between the child and clinician and

included open-ended statements to encourage verbalization

No imitative prompts or tangible reinforcers were delivered

If the child omitted the target, the SP delivered a „growth recast‟ that included the target

by recast the semantic information from the child‟s utterances.

The training context was constructed to elicit attempts and to support production.

The context for relative clause training included several items that were similar in many

dimensions but different with regard to minor details.

While playing the child would comment on the activities of which the clinician would

then use as a platform for recast.

For grammatical morphemes and complex sentences the recast retains the semantic base

of the child‟s core utterance while providing a model of the target form.

Inter-observer agreement for target selection was 92%

Inter-observer agreement for target production during treatment of absent targets was 82%

Inter-observer agreement for identifying clinician presentations was 99%

Quantum of service

provision:

2x 50 minute session/week (number of weeks of therapy not specified)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Growth curves (of the number of stimuli the SP used) were used to determine treatment efficacy.

The fewer they used the better the treatment.

All 4 participants used the targets treated with naturalistic training with fewer stimuli than with

imitation.

Participant A, aged 6;5 years reached criterion for production of the passive after 67

presentations in naturalistic training vs 245 presentations of relative clauses in imitation

training

Participant B, aged 4;5 years reached criterion for production of irregular past tense after 35

presentations in naturalistic training vs 245 presentations of regular third person singular in

imitation training.

Participant C, aged 4;4 years reached criterion for production of wh- non-infinitive after about

the same number of presentations in naturalistic as the infinitive with different subjects in

imitation training. However, reached a second criterion with natural training more quickly than

for imitation training

Participant D, aged 6;8 years reached criterion for s production of the inverted wh-questions

after 3 presentations in naturalistic training vs 150 presentations for relative clauses with

different subjects in imitation training

4/4 demonstrated an initial advantage for naturalistic treatment

3/4 maintained a consistent difference across treatments

Author’s conclusion:

For these participants, each of whom showed poor pre-intervention imitation skills, naturalistic

treatment was a more efficient therapeutic procedure than analog treatment. It is important that

clinicians appreciate that a child‟s lack of proficiency in imitation is likely to limit effectiveness of

imitation-based procedures for that child.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Naturalistic treatment appeared to be more effective than analog treatment for children with ELD

and poor imitation skills.

The amount of therapy received (over how many weeks) and whether each child received

the same amount of therapy was not specified.

There was no control used in the study.

Level of parental involvement was not discussed.

The results were limited in their detail and tended to be rather general.

93

24. Hadley, P. A., Simmerman, A., Long, M., & Luna, M. (2000). Facilitating language development for inner-city children:

Experimental evaluation of a collaborative, classroom-based intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

31(2).

Summary of intervention program: Collaborative classroom based for Vocabulary and

phonological awareness

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pseudo randomised controlled trial (Group design; pre-test-post test)

One school selected and then four out of 8 kindergarten (reception) classrooms where

teachers volunteered were randomly selected (no details of how this was done).

Level III-I

Aim of study:

To determine the effectiveness of a collaborative service delivery model teaming a SLP

with regular classroom teachers to facilitate English language development for inner city

children with limited language skills.

The research question was to determine if the experimental children show greater

receptive and expressive vocabulary gains and phonological awareness and letter-sound

awareness gains than the control children

Population:

The school selected was one where 83% lived below the official poverty line. The participants

were 86 children aged 5;- to 6;9 years; 46 experimental children in two classrooms and 40 controls

in two classes

35 had limited English proficiency

5 children had speech-language impairments

76% were kindergarteners and 24% were Grade 1

Service setting:

School within the classroom

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

(individual/group etc):

Planning meetings lasted an hour

Provider(s):

Teachers and SLPs

Domain of language:

Vocabulary and phonological awareness

ICF-Impairment, activity and participation

Type of intervention:

Collaborative classroom based

Method of

intervention:

Experimental Groups

Three components

1. Professional education

a. this was woven into curriculum planning

2. Joint curriculum planning

a. classroom teachers had primary responsibility for curricula goals and SLPs had

primary responsibility for using the curriculum to meet the students‟ communication

goals

b. goals

i. vocabulary ( nouns, verbs, adjectives and prepositions)

ii. Phonological awareness including sound letter matching

3. Use of naturalistic language facilitation techniques

a. Goals were embedded into classroom activities

i. SLP led weekly 25-minute Phonological awareness groups

b. SLP was in the classroom for 2.5 days each week and with the teacher shared direct

instruction on vocabulary and sound letter association tasks

Control groups

1. Standard curriculum

2. Paraprofessional attended for 2.5 days per week e

94

Quantum of service

provision:

Duration 6 months

Weekly planning meetings between the teacher and SLP for an hour. Some of these were

attended by the University researcher

SLP in each of the two classrooms for 2.5 days per week each

SLP conducted several groups for 25-minutes of phonological awareness work so all

children attended these each week within the time in the classroom

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Vocabulary (receptive and expressive)

No pre-test group differences on vocabulary scores but significant post-test differences as

measured by the PPVT (F(I, 85) = 11.06 p=.001)and the EVT (F(I, 85) = 11..63 p=.001).

Separate scores for native and non native English speaker, children with limited English

proficiency and children with speech and language impairment indicated all groups made

gains ( but not tested).

Group pre-test scores were below normal limits and group post test scores were within normal

limits.

For children with speech and language impairments, their post test PPVT scores were within

1SD of the mean and for the EVT within 1.5 SDs

Phonological awareness

No pre-test group differences on rhyme, beginning sound awareness, letter sound association

syllable deletion and phoneme deletion raw scores but significant post test differences for

beginning sound awareness (F(I, 85) = 4.23 p=.04), letter sound association (F(I, 85) = 13.48

p=.000) but not for rhyme, syllable deletion and phoneme deletion scores

Author’s conclusion:

Classroom based, collaboration between regular classroom teachers and speech pathologists holds

promise as an effective means of facilitating the vocabulary and some phonological awareness

skills of a range of children with language needs

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Gains over maturation appear to be made. The significant features of this program is that the staff

were given time to plan and for the SLP to spend all her time in 2 classrooms and this may be a key

feature to the success of this collaboration.

95

25. Hay. I., Elias, G., Fielding-Barnsley, R., Homel, R., & Freiberg, K. (2007). Language delays, reading delays, and learning

difficulties: Interactive elements requiring multidimensional programming. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(5), 400-409.

Summary of intervention program: Collaborative for vocabulary, syntax and discourse

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative study of two groups of Grade 1 children; one control and one experimental.

Level III-2

Aim of study:

Hypothesizing that if teachers enhance their dialogic classroom skills, then children‟s oral and

written language skills will improve, the aim of this study was to investigate this relationship.

Teachers were taught to use the Blank, Rose and Berlin levels of questioning and children‟s

reading scores were expected to improve as a consequence.

Population:

N == 116 Grade 1 children, aged 5;9 years (mean) at the beginning of the study, with language

difficulties from a school in a low SES area in Queensland. 40% had at least a 12 month delay with

receptive vocabulary (PPVT) and 20% had expressive naming problems using the Hundred Picture

Naming tests (Not stated whether these groups are mutually exclusive or not)

Note; It is not clear how many children had primary language difficulties not clear how many had

low English proficiency as a consequence of English being their second language

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Collaborative

Provider(s):

Teachers in collaboration with a language support teacher

Domain of language:

Vocabulary, syntax and discourse (dialogue)

ICF- activity and participation

Type of intervention:

The language support teacher in-serviced classroom teachers over the academic year, spending

about 2 hours per week with each teacher during Term 1 (12 weeks) then reducing the frequency of

visits over the latter part of the year (time commitment not indicated). Also she

worked with the teachers to

o use more developmentally appropriate language curriculum for each child in the

classroom

o foster the children‟s social language, communication skills and comprehension of

instructional language

conducted small group session with children for activities based on Blank‟s theory

Method of

intervention:

Teachers were responsible for interpreting and for incorporating the theory for into the classroom

activities.

Lessons early in the year focused on the lower levels and progressed through the year to the higher

levels

Quantum of service

provision:

Duration: academic year

Initially weekly support of teachers (2 hours /teacher/week for 12 weeks (i.e. 24 hours per

teacher)

Additional support for the teachers for the remainder of the year but quantum not defined

Note Not clear if other activities described above are additional to or incorporated within

the 2 hour allocation per teacher

96

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Experimental group significantly outperformed the control group on the end of year scores

Reading scores (Burt Word Reading test) F(1,114)=5.59 p = 016

The number of children reading below their age level as measured on Neale Analysis of

reading decreased in the experimental group 2 (1, N=116)=11.45 p<.001 (

The number of children judged as at risk for language skills according to the PLAI decreased

significantly in the experimental group 2 (1, N=57) =15.96, p<.001 (Note the number in the

control group was not given)

Author’s conclusion:

Concluded that

the addition of Blank et al‟s. language development strategies facilitated reading development

and that this was consequent to multi-dimensional intervention rather than uni-dimensional

intervention that focused on phonological and word decoding.

o Reading instruction requires attention on the specifics of literacy AND oral language

Noted limitations including the lack of controls for all variables in this program

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

It is not clear what contributed to the experimental groups increased end-of -year reading

performance because of the

lack of experimental controls,

no fidelity checks

the comparability of the two groups prior to intervention was not established

What is clear is that groups differences existed at the end of the year

97

26. Jacoby, G. P., Lee, L., Kummer, A. W., Levin, L., & Creaghead, N. A. (2002). The Number of Individual Treatment Units

Necessary to Facilitate Functional Communication Improvements in the Speech and Language of Young Children. American

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(4), 370-380.

Summary of intervention program: Articulation/intelligibility, receptive and expressive spoken

language

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Retrospective review of client records

Level Other

Aim of study:

The aim was to determine the quantum of service required for children to attain gains in functional

communication,

Population:

234 subjects, aged 3 to 6 ;6 years, selected from an audit of the speech pathology records of 527

children who had received speech pathology services who

received intervention for speech and/or language over a specified 2-year period

received a low functional communication score on the ASHA National Outcomes

Measurement system (<7) in one or more areas of articulation/intelligibility, receptive and

expressive spoken language

rated on the same scale at discharge

Half the children had associated factors such as hearing loss and autism

Service setting:

a speech pathology department in a Children‟s Medical Centre

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

probably 1:1 but not explicitly stated

Provider(s):

SLPs

Domain of language:

articulation/intelligibility, receptive and expressive spoken language

ICF-not possible to classify

Type of intervention:

Not relevant and not described

Method of

intervention:

Not relevant and not described

the number of billing units (15 minutes) were counted for each child between first rating and

discharge

Quantum of service

provision:

Children generally received 30 minutes of intervention per week

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Younger children required fewer units than older child

Children with higher functional scores at pre-test required fewer units than children with lower

scores but the number of units was not related to initial rating for any diagnostic group

The presence of more than one area of difficulty did not relate to the number of required units

Generally 20 hours or more of intervention was required to improve by at least 1 functional

communication measure level and mostly they increased by 2 levels

Author’s conclusion:

Children with aarticulation/intelligibility and expressive spoken language problems who receive

services from a medical centre targeting children with complex medical issues generally make

better outcomes with more therapy than less. However, the younger children are the less therapy

they seem to need. Children with receptive language problems made few gains irrespective of time

spent when therapy.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

It seems that within this hospital setting, a package of 20 hours of therapy administered weekly had

the greatest impact on outcomes for children with articulation/intelligibility and expressive spoken

language problems. There is some evidence that 18 hours is the critical number as 31% of children

had no gains after 15 hours of therapy but 42% had an increase of 1 level after 18 hours of therapy

and 23% had 2 to 3 increases of levels after 55 hours of therapy.

98

27. Keilmann, A., Braun, L., & Napiontek, U. (2004). Emotional satisfaction of parents and speech-language therapists with

outcome of training intervention in children with speech and language disorders. :Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 56(1), 51-61.

Summary of intervention program: Outcomes of therapy for speech and language disorders for

as perceived by parents

Country: Germany

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Qualitative research using questionnaires administered to parents and speech-language therapists.

Questionnaires comprised forced choice questions

Level- Other

Aim of study:

Recognizing that parents have a crucial role in the success of therapy for children with speech and

language impairment, the aim of this study was to measure parents satisfaction with speech

pathology intervention outcomes

Population:

169 children (104 boys and 65 girls) with speech and language disorders, aged 2;8 to 9;11 ( no

breakdown).

Service setting:

Not clearly stated but hospital implied

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual with 53% attending therapy alone and 33% accompanied by a parent

Provider(s):

Speech-language therapists

Domain of language:

Speech and language

ICF-not possible to classify

Type of intervention:

Not described

Method of

intervention:

Not described

Quantum of service

provision:

Frequency of intervention varied from < once per week for 7%, weekly for 76% and twice weekly

for 15% and more frequently for 2%. Range of intervention duration 1-82 months (mean of 19.3)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Parents

On a 4-point scale, the percentages of parents choosing the highest, positive rating for items:

Our child likes to attend sessions-80%

Our child has made good progress in speech and language-56%

The speech-language therapists handles the child appropriately-88%

The speech-language therapists knows the job-77%

The time spent is worthwhile-74%

The SLT has informed us sufficiently about the speech and language development of

our child-74%

The SLP has informed us sufficiently about the therapy -71%

Parents attending sessions did not respond differently to those who did not

80 % of parents whose child attended weekly intervention recognised good progress

compared with 93% who attended intervention 2+ times per week

Parental satisfaction was associated with the number of sessions; the more sessions the

higher the satisfaction

Speech-language therapists indicated

Caseload, on average, 70% speech and language problems

frequency with which they saw children as suboptimal

that 90% of the children liked attending

86% of parents kept appointments

60% of parents judged the severity of their children‟s problems correctly

Home practice done 59%

Indicated that parents accepted 64% of their recommendations on education, play and TV

The more experienced they are the more likely they treat the children without parent

present and spend less time with the parents

99

Author’s conclusion:

Parents do not perceive speech and language therapy as harming children, rather most expressed

happiness with it and attributed improvement in children to the therapy. Parents perceived that

higher frequency of therapy was associated with higher rate of progress.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above. However for this review it is not clear how these results relate to the children five years

and older

100

28. Kirk, C., & Gillon, G. T. (2007). Longitudinal effects of phonological awareness intervention on morphological awareness in

children with speech impairment. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 342-352.

Summary of intervention program: intervention for speech sound disorder with or without

components addressing phonological processing

Country: New Zealand

Publication type &

level of evidence:

The study described follows up children who had received therapy in their preschool years. The

therapy was described in a previous article (Gillon, 2005)

Level- Other

Aim of study:

The aim of this study was to examine relationship between morphological and reading skills by

examining the literacy outcomes of children, aged 8 years, with a history of speech impairment

who had received preschool intervention

Population:

41 children, aged 8 years

Group 1 (Experimental 1)

8 children in their 3rd

or 4th

year of schooling, aged 8;5 years (mean) with a history of

moderate to severe speech impairment. . At ages 3 or 4 years, they received intervention

addressing speech production as well as phoneme awareness and phoneme-grapheme

correspondences

Group 2 (Experimental 2)

9 children in their 3rd

or 4th

year of schooling, aged 8;6 years (mean) with a history of

moderate to severe speech impairment. At ages 3 or 4 years, they received intervention

addressing speech production that did not explicitly address phoneme awareness and

phoneme-grapheme Groups 1 and 2 were matched for intervention dosage and speech and

language status.

Group 3 (Control)

24 children, aged 8;7 years (mean) in their 4th

year of schooling rated by their teachers as

achieving grade appropriate levels in the language curriculum

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

The therapy was described in a previous article (Gillon, 2005) which is summarised below

Service delivery model

Clinic-based individual and group therapy

Provider(s):

Group sessions: The researcher, or a qualified SLP trained by the researcher

Individual sessions: A senior speech-language therapy student under the supervision of a

qualified SLP who was trained on program content

Domain of language:

Speech

depending on the

availability of children and therapists) followed the same

model: two

Type of intervention:

See below

Method of

intervention:

For Group 1 above

Two or three blocks of therapy between the ages of 3 and 5 years

Each block of therapy typically lasted between 4–6 weeks with two 45-minute therapy

sessions per week; one group session with 2 or 3 other study children and one individual

session .

Children‟s primary caregiver usually present for therapy but not specifically trained to

implement the therapy procedures at home

Therapy focused on speech intelligibility (Cycles), phonological awareness (phoneme level);

letter-name and letter-sound knowledge

For Group 2 above:

Received a similar quantum of speech intervention as group 1 above but the details of how the

therapy was conducted were not described

101

Quantum of service

provision:

See above description but note there was no therapy provided as a part of this study

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

The effect of intervention type on reading several years later

Non-word decoding

A significant group effect F(2,38) = 7,12 p=.002, f= .52 with post hoc testing indicating

that the group who received phonological awareness intervention significantly

outperformed

the group who did not receive phonological awareness intervention (and)

the typically developing control children

Word recognition

A significant group effect F(2,38) = 6.15 p=.005, f= .49 with post hoc testing indicating

that the typically developing control children group significantly outperformed the group

who did not receive phonological intervention. However the considerable individual

variation prompted inspection of the individual scores of the children who received

phonological awareness work, indicating that 6 / 8 children were 1 to 4 years above their

chronological age but the other two were more than a year below their age

The effect of intervention type on morphological skills several years later

Spelling dictation

A significant group effect F(2,38) = 5.36 p=.01, f= .47 with post hoc testing indicating no

difference between the group who received phonological awareness intervention and the

typically developing group but both these groups significantly outperformed the group

who did not receive phonological awareness

Morphological spelling rules

A significant group effect F(2,38) = 10.85 p=<.001, f= .60 with post hoc testing indicating

no difference between the group who received phonological awareness intervention and

the typically developing group but both these groups significantly outperformed the group

who did not receive phonological awareness

Author’s conclusion:

Children with speech impairment seem to have better literacy outcomes when their preschool

intervention includes phonological awareness work in addition to speech production work than

focussing on sound production alone. It also seems that intervention that includes phonological

awareness promotes understanding of morphology that in turn, can be used in spelling words.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above but being mindful of the authors comments about the limitations of this study with

respect to sample size and generalisability of the findings

102

29. Klein, E. S. (1996). Phonological/traditional approaches to articulation therapy: a retrospective group comparison. Language,

Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27(4), 3140323.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology: traditional and phonological

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Retrospective group comparative design

Level III-3

Aim of study:

To compare the efficacy of traditional and phonological approaches to the treatment of children

with multiple articulation disorders

Population:

The retrospective audit of files of children who attended a clinic within a 10 year span for therapy

for multiple articulation disorders was conducted to locate children who had:

reduced intelligibility at first session determined by perceptual judgment

a severity rating of 16.5on Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scaled (Fudala, 1970)

no co-morbid disorder

no speech therapy intervention before first admission

received 50 hours of therapy or were dismissed with normal speech

attended 75% of all sessions

received either traditional or phonological program of intervention.

Two SLPs independently checked the files of the selected children for the above and determination

of the type of intervention. There were 36 subjects in two groups with no between group

differences in average age or severity of speech when therapy commenced. The age range was 3;0

to 5;10 years

Group 1 traditional therapy

19 children (5 girls and 14 boys) with average age of 4;5 years with 6 aged 5;0+ years at the

first therapy session

Group 2 (Phonological therapy)

17 children (5 girls and 12 boys) with average age of 4;5 years with 3 aged 5;0+ years at the

first therapy session

Service setting:

University clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

students under the supervision of qualified SLPs

Domain of language:

Articulation and phonology

ICF; Impairment

Type of intervention:

Traditional

direct instruction on how to articulate a sound using

motor exercises and/or sensori-motor training

speech sound discrimination training

modelling and requests for imitation

production started in isolation moving to nonsense syllables then higher levels

Only one or two sounds were addresses at any time in any child

Phonological

procedures were a variation of minimal pairs or imagery

no instruction on how to articulate a sound

no need for direct imitation

base unit was a phonological rule

semantic identification and rule production activities began with nonsense syllables followed

by single words and higher level once a 100% criterion was met

Rules were taught in the context of its contrast

no value judgements were made regarding whether sound was correct or not

only one simplification rule was expected to change in a given word at one time

procedures were a variation of minimal pairs or imagery s

Method of

intervention:

103

Quantum of service

provision:

2-3 50 minute weekly sessions until about 50 hours of therapy was reached or child was dismissed

with normal speech

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Final Speech severity rating

traditional group significantly higher (worse) than phonological group (t(34) =

18.57,p<.001)

total number of therapy sessions

no difference

total months of therapy

traditional group significantly more months (mean 22 months) than phonological group

(Mean 14 months) (t(34) = 3.32,p<.005)

Dismissal rates

All children (17/17) in phonological therapy group were dismissed from therapy with

normal speech by the last session whereas 2/19 children from the traditional therapy group

were dismissed

Author’s conclusion:

The outcomes matched expectations in that the outcomes from phonological therapy were superior

to those for traditional therapy. Children in phonological therapy reached a higher level of speech

in a shorter number of session that their peers enrolled in traditional therapy

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Concur with the above notwithstanding the appropriate limitations to the study that the author

made

104

30. Long, K., & Gillon, G. T. (2007). Enhancing early literacy acquisition in a child with speech and language impairment: A

case study. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy, 62(4-19).

Summary of intervention program: Integrated phoneme awareness and speech intelligibility

framework

Country: New Zealand

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Single case design with repeated assessments in the pre and post assessment phases of speech

production, phonological awareness letter and sound knowledge and linguistics concepts. Analysis

used the celeration line and two standard deviation method.

Level - Other

Aim of study:

Two aims

1. To determine if the benefit that children with speech impairment gain from early intervention

to facilitate phonological awareness also occurs for children who have both speech and

language impairments

2. To determine if phonological awareness skills that are stimulated in the preschool years are

associated with positive interactions with classroom literacy instruction.

Population:

A boy, aged 4;10 years, with proven speech and language impairments who had some intervention

just prior to starting formal schooling. He was tested on the PIPA, the CELF. PROPH and for oral

motor skills (WNL) and hearing (WNL)

Service setting:

University speech and language clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model 1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

Speech pathologist.

Domain of language:

speech production and phonological awareness

ICF: Impairment

Type of intervention: Integrated phoneme awareness and speech intelligibility framework developed by Gillon &

Moriarty (2005).

Method of

intervention:

Phase 1. Occurred prior to school. Each session focussed on concurrent and integrated

development of

phonological awareness;- phoneme isolation, detection and categorization

development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge

stimulation of speech intelligibility at word level using the Cycles approach

Phase 2 Occurred when he started school Responsiveness to early classroom literacy instruction

Direct classroom observations of responsiveness to literacy instruction

Descriptive analysis of prompted and unprompted writing attempts

Quantum of service

provision:

Phase 1: 2 50-minute sessions /week 5 weeks just prior to starting formal school with mother

observing

Phase 2 Observations over the first 2months at school

105

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Phase 1

1. phonological awareness

o Significant increase for trained and untrained phoneme identity

2. letter and sound knowledge

o Significant increase for trained and untrained letters

3. speech production

o Significant increase in PCC from 36% to 84%

4. linguistics concepts

o Control measure and no change as predicted

Phase 2 Responsiveness to early classroom literacy instruction

Direct classroom observations of responsiveness to literacy instruction

o within normal limits

Descriptive analysis of prompted and unprompted writing attempts

o Within normal limits

Post test assessment at 5 months

1. phonological awareness and letter and sound knowledge were within normal limits on PIPA

2. speech production

Rating for PCC changed from moderate-severe to mild

3. linguistics concepts

no change

Author’s conclusion:

The Integrated phoneme awareness and speech intelligibility framework was effective for this boy,

changing the status of his phonological awareness skills from delayed to age-appropriate by the

time he commenced school. The authors note that his early responses to literacy instruction were

age appropriate for his level of engagement and spelling for the four weeks measurement occurred

when he started formal schooling.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The evidence for improved phonological awareness skills and speech production is clear given the

use of the research design and the check on fidelity. It is noteworthy that progress was achieved

with 8 hours and 20 minutes of 1:1 intervention provided twice weekly for 5 weeks.

106

31. Marler, J. A., Champlin, C. A., & Gillam, R. B. (2001). Backward and simultaneous masking measured in children with

language-learning impairments who received intervention with Fast ForWord or Laureate Learning Systems software. American

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(3), 258-268

Summary of intervention program: Phonological processing using Fast ForWord

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Small N cohort study

Level IV

Aim of study:

One of the proposed causes of language-learning impairment is poor auditory processing,

particularly temporal processing of sounds. A custom made computer-assisted language

intervention program Fast ForWord has been devised to redress this difficulty and thereby resolve

the consequent language problems. The aim of this pilot study was to compare changes in auditory

temporal processing for children who received Fast ForWord training and training with computer-

assisted language learning not designed to address auditory temporal processing.

Population:

N= 7 boys, aged 6;10 - 9;3 years with proven normal hearing, no physical, emotional or motor

problems or episodes of otitis media in the last year. According to parental report. They were two

groups

Experimental group: 4 boys with a language-learning impairment; 2 boys received the

FFW training and the other 2 boys received the other computer-assisted language learning

Control group: 3 typically developing boys to control for maturational changes

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

speech-language pathologist

Domain of language:

Phonological processing

ICF level-impairment

Type of intervention:

Computer-assisted language intervention for both all four children with language-learning

impairment; 2 using Fast ForWord and 2 using commercial packages. All protocols were the same

except for the software packages

Method of

intervention:

Fast ForWord

This package consists of computer games where the acoustic and temporal properties of the

speech has been modified

Commercial computer packages

This package consists of computer games but without modified speech

Quantum of service

provision:

4 week program of daily 15 minutes session i.e. 5 hours

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Signal thresholds in backward and simultaneous masking conditions were measured pre,

during and post intervention.

signal thresholds were higher in the simultaneous condition than in the masked condition for

all children

thresholds for all boys, with and without decreased language-learning impairment ( i.e.

improved) over each of the four testing points

The boys with LLI did not display the typical signal threshold differential between backward

and simultaneous masking

The pattern of results indicated an auditory memory effect in addition to temporal processing

difficulties

Author’s conclusion:

Overall results did not support Fast ForWord developer‟s claims that the altered speech of their

program resulted in improved temporal processing. Children in both treatment groups showed

improved processing early in their programs, suggesting that the intensive format of FFW may not

be contributing to positive results. A practice effect is also present given the signal thresholds

decreased for the typically developing children who did not undergo the intervention

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This study does not seem to provide evidence for the claimed program specific effect Fast

ForWord

107

32. Moriarty, B. C., & Gillon, G. T. (2006). Phonological awareness intervention for children with childhood apraxia of speech

International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 713-734.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological awareness for childhood apraxia of speech

Country: New Zealand

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Multiple single-subject design with repeated measures

Level III-3

Aim of study:

Noting that childhood apraxia of speech is a multilevel disorder that affects phoneme

representation, motor planning and motor programming and that intervention for these children is

usually motor based, the aim of this study examined the effectiveness of phonological awareness

intervention for children with childhood apraxia of speech, that is, focusing on the representational

deficit.

Population:

Three children, aged 7;3, 6;3 and 6;10 years with CAS who had confirmed oro-motor sequencing

problems and difficulties with multisyllabic word production.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

Speech language therapists

Domain of language:

Phonology

ICF- Impairment

Type of intervention:

phonological awareness

Method of

intervention:

All sessions included 4 phonological awareness tasks,

o identifying phonemes in isolation

o indentifying initial and final phoneme s in words

o phonemes segmentation and phoneme blending

o phoneme manipulation with letter (grapheme) blocks

These tasks were usually part of game which also required the children to say the words

containing the target sounds. When erroneous target sound production occurred, the SLT

encouraged the child to identify the error a word.

No specific instructions on how to articulate sounds were given

Quantum of service

provision:

3x 45 min weekly sessions for 3 weeks, i.e. 9 sessions for 7 hours

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

The celeration line and 2 standard deviation method analysis revealed:

speech production expressed as percentage of phonemes correct

o 2/3 children made significant gains on target words;

o 1/3 made significant gains on control words

phonological awareness

o phoneme segmentation

all children made significant gains on trained target

2/3 significant gains on control targets

o Manipulation

2/3 significant gains on trained and control items

Non-word reading and letter sound knowledge

o all children made significant gains on trained target

o 2/3 significant gains on control targets

108

Author’s conclusion:

This investigation revealed that an integrated phonological awareness intervention can

facilitate speech production, phonological awareness, linking letters and sounds and decoding

for some children with CAS as 2/3 children made significant progress on target and control

targets across these areas.

The study supports notion that CAS is a multilevel impairment

Supports that short periods on intense therapy over a 3-week time frame achieves significant

change for some children.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This study seems robust in design and method (e.g. fidelity check) that allow one to agree with the

cautious conclusions that the authors make given the acknowledged limitations. These include no

follow up and that intervention was not limited to representational work. However, it does provide

evidence that an intervention that has as its primary focus phonological awareness work has the

capacity to bring about change in children‟s production. The child who did not make progress does

not fit the usual criteria for CAS as his non-verbal skills were below normal limits.

109

33. Munro, N., & Atkinson, K. (2003). School-aged group therapy within community health services. ACQuiring Knowledge in

Speech, Language and Hearing, 5(2), 72-75.

Summary of intervention program: phonological awareness development for children with

phonological awareness problems

Country: Australia

Publication type &

level of evidence:

A cross sectional within group study

Level IV

Aim of study:

To relieve increasing client demand in a government funded community clinic, a different model of

service delivery was trialled with group therapy rather than individual therapy. The aim of this

study was to evaluate the trial by examining the outcomes for children with language impairment.

Population:

20 school children with phonological awareness difficulties; 8 kindergarteners (i.e. the first

year of school/reception), 7 Year 1s, 3 Year 2s and 2 Year 3s (Ages not provided).

All children ≤ 1SD below the mean on the Sutherland Phonological Awareness test (Neilson,

1995)

Service setting:

Community-health clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Group, centre based

Provider(s):

Speech language therapist

Domain of language:

Phonological awareness

Type of intervention:

No specific name

Method of

intervention:

To develop phonological awareness and knowledge the structure of oral language, games that

develop skills of rhyme detection and production, phoneme segmentation and phoneme

manipulation in structures of increasing difficulty. Reading and spelling of real and non-words was

also targeted.

Quantum of service

provision:

six sessions of 1 hour weekly group therapy

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

phonological awareness

Significant increases in the group scores (SPAT) (t=-7.43, df=19, p<.05)

Spelling

Significant increases in the group scores (10 words from NARA) (t=-3.59, df=19, p<.05).

control variable of visual motor integration

No significant change in the

Parent satisfaction

90% of parents (18 /20) responded & reported that the group intervention useful

Author’s conclusion:

The results suggest a significant trend between improvements in phonological awareness skills and

spelling using group therapy in a community health setting and that parents were satisfied with

this. This trial improved speech pathology services for school-aged children community health

clinic based services.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The results suggest support for centre-based group services for school-aged children in a

community health service. However one of the outcome measures was also used to determine

eligibility into the programme so this may mean the children were taught to pass the test.

The opportunity cost of seeing children outside the school environment needs consideration.

110

34. Nelson, K. E., Camarata, S. M., Welsh, J., Butkovsky, L., & Camarata, M. (1996). Effects of imitative and conversational

recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal

children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39(4), 850-859.

Summary of intervention program: Syntax –recasts vs imitation

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Small N comparative design

Level III-2

Aim of study:

The purpose of the study was to compare the relative effectiveness of imitative treatment and

conversational recast treatment in children with language impairment and in a group of children

with normal language skills.

Population:

N= 14 across two groups

Experimental Group-7 children aged, 4;7 to 6;7 years (5 were 5 years +) with specific language

impairment with:

Normal cognition (Leiter International Performance Scale (Arthur, 1952))

no known sensory, motor, or psychosocial deficits determined by case file review, parent

interview and direct observation of the subjects

normal hearing from audiometric screening

MLU (Miller, 1981; Miller & Chapman, 1981) from pre-intervention naturalistic samples of

mother-child conversation was 11.25 standard deviations below the mean

a delay of at least 1.5 SD on the sentence imitation subtest of the TOLD2-Por the elaborated

sentences subtest of the Carrow Elicited Language Inventory (Carrow, 1974).

Control group-7 language normal children aged 2;2 to 4;2 with

normal ranges for MLU

no evidence of sensory, motor, or psychosocial deficits determined from parent interview and

direct observation,

passing an audiometric screening

Groups were matched on MLU derived from naturalistic conversational samples, on profiles of

already-mastered syntactic structures, and on raw scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

Revised (PPVT-R, Dunn & Dunn, 1982) and on intervention targets. By design, the groups

differed in age [no group overlap; t(12) = 6.16, p < .0001], but not for PPVT-R raw scores [t(12) =

.93, p < .30] or MLU [t(12): .51, p > .60].

Service setting:

Clinic- probably a university one

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

Clinician without explicit statement that they were SLPs

Domain of language:

Syntax - gerunds , copula or auxiliary verbs, past tense, articles ,3rd person singular, possessives

relative clause , passive ,complex negative, complex questions, wh-complement, simple infinitives,

complex infinitives and coordinate conjunctions.

ICF: Impairment, activity/participation

Type of intervention:

See below

111

Method of

intervention:

Each child had six intervention targets; 3 absent from their repertories and 3 partially mastered.

The targets were assigned randomly to each of three conditions: control, imitative treatment, and

conversational recast treatment. The control condition included no direct treatment of the target but

did include monitoring that was identical to that for the treated targets. Thus, there was one absent

and one partially mastered target assigned to conversational recast treatment, to imitative

treatment, and to the control condition. The two control structures received no intervention,

whereas the treated targets were trained using the treatment procedures described above.

Conversational recast procedure

children engaged in conversation with clinician who adjusted the patterning of the target

presentations to the ongoing discourse while targeting specific grammatical goals.

the clinician provided targets that were growth recasts, i.e., replies to an immediately prior

child utterance that retained the basic meaning but recast it into a new sentence containing the

targeted structure.

Growth recasts were intended to maintain conversation flow whilst allowing the child to

process the target within appropriate verbal and social contexts inherent in conversation.

No explicit prompts for production or requests for imitation or verbal reinforcements of the

child's target productions were included

Imitative treatment

Teaching included prompting, modelling, requested imitation, and reinforcement. Prompts

included pictures and toys selected to elicit targets; models included clinician production of

the target; and reinforcers included verbal praise and tokens that could be traded for small toys

or stickers after the training session.

Six training steps

1. Level 1- presentation of an appropriate prompt, a model, and a direct request for

imitation.

2. Level 2- fading of the request for imitation so only the prompt and model were

presented.

3. Level 3 - fading of the model so that only the prompt was delivered

4. Level 4- presentation of a set of untrained prompts.

5. Level 5- fading of the reinforcer.

criterion to move levels was 90% in at least 20 trials per session.

Quantum of service

provision:

2 sessions per week for an average of 18.8 sessions (9-10 weeks). The duration of each session was

not described. Sessions were continued along with ongoing analyses of target presentations until

comparable numbers of target trials had been achieved for all four intervention targets.

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Absent targets

More targets from treated group acquired than from control group (2 (1) = 13.67, p < .001.

targets taught in the conversational recasting acquired more quickly than in imitation for both

groups (F (1,12) = 9.17, p < .01).

More targets taught in conversational recasting (77%) generalised to the home environment

than the imitative targets (31%) (2 (1) = 5.52, p < .02.) for both groups

Targets Partially Mastered Before Intervention

generalized spontaneous use of syntactic structures occurred on average 23.9 times per child

with no difference between conditions

conversational recasting led to a significantly higher mean number of spontaneous

productions, averaging 55.1 generalized spontaneous uses.

the total generalized spontaneous uses of these targets averaged 138.9 for the SLI children and

60.2 for the LN children with ANOVA indicating no group effect but only a treatment effect F

(2,24) = 3.92, p < .04

Author’s conclusion:

Children with SLI and typically developing language can learn stages III to V+ syntactic structures

(Brown) in response to conversational recasting and learning is faster in conversational recasting

than in imitation. Both groups of children were similar in the rates of progress and can learn

structures that are absent from their repertoires.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This study suggests that conversational recasting is the treatment of choice for facilitating the

development of Brown's Stages III to V syntactic structures. However, these findings are weakened

by the limitations that the authors acknowledged of small group sizes and variability in scores as

well as the lack of maturational controls. There was no accounting for the observation that some of

the control goals, the ones not treated also improved

112

35.,Pascoe, M., Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (2005). Phonological therapy within a psycholinguistic framework: Promoting

change in a child with persisting speech difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40(2), 189-

220.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological therapy within a psycholinguistic framework

Country: England

Publication type &

level of evidence:

A single subject research design was used with pre- and post-intervention assessment

Level Other

Aim of study:

The aim of the research was to determine if psycholinguistically-based intervention could result in

(a) specific and (b) generalized improvements in the speech production of a child with severe and

persisting speech sound difficulties.

Population:

A girl, aged 6;5 years with persistent speech sound difficulties who:

Is highly unintelligible to unfamiliar listeners

Is in her second year of formal schooling

has normal hearing

comes from a monolingual English home

lives with both parents and one older sibling

attends a mainstream school with special educational assistance

has normal cognition IQ (WASI, Wechsler, 1999) verbal = 83, performance = 78; full

scale=78.

in the „weaker ability group‟ for all her subjects.

described as popular and enjoys school

Communication skills and intervention history

receptive and expressive language delays present and confirmed from two years of age

Intervention

From 2;3 to 4;2 years- 4 blocks of therapy; 2 x 6 weekly sessions; 1 blocks of 10

weekly sessions and the remaining unspecified

At 4 years one visit

First two years at school 2-3 visits per term with learning support assistant carrying

out the program on a daily basis

At the commencement of the research project, an extensive evaluation of her receptive and

expressive speech skills was performed, indicating deficits in her psycholinguistic processing-

input, retrieval and motor planning and her output phonology reflected in a PCC of 22%.

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

An extensive assessment of macro and micro skills with the micro skills facilitating intervention

planning

Provider(s):

speech-language therapists

Domain of language:

Phonology

ICF impairment, participation and activity

Type of intervention:

Psycholinguistic

Method of

intervention:

Intervention aimed for the child to use the CVC frame in conversation through three phases :

– Phase I: therapy on a specific set of single words

– Phase II: therapy on a wider range of single words

– Phase III: therapy on connected speech

In each phase spelling was explicitly included.

Quantum of service

provision:

2 x 1 hour session each week for 10 hours for each phase (i.e. 30 hours of intervention) over 9

month from time child was aged 6;8 years to 7;5 years

Implies each phase was five weeks and therapy was 15 weeks spread over 9 months

Final follow up assessment was conducted seven months later when child was aged 8;2 years

113

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Micro Evaluation of PCC

Single words

o Significant overall intervention effect (F(2, 44)=38.310, p,.001)

o Phase 1 significant increases for treated and untreated words

o Phase 2 significant increases for treated and untreated words

o Phase 3 significant decrease for treated and untreated words

o At long-term follow-up following 7 months of no intervention, significant gains were

made

Connected speech

o Significant overall intervention effect [F(2, 44)=108.477, p,.001].

o Phase 1 no significant increases for treated and untreated words

o Phase 2 no significant increases for treated and untreated items

o Phase 3 significant increases for treated and untreated items

o At long-term follow-up after 7 months of no intervention, significant gains were

made

Note change in connected speech only occurred when this level was targeted

Spelling speech

o Significant overall intervention effect [F(2,44)=14.083, p,.001].

o Phase 1 no significant increases for treated and untreated words

o Phase 2 no significant increases for r treated untreated items

o Phase 3 no significant increases for treated items but significant increases for

untreated items

o At long-term follow-up following 7 months of no intervention, significant gains were

made and maintained for untreated items

Auditory discrimination

o Some changes were noted

Macro evaluation

PCC significant increases from first to last assessment (t(99)524.662, p,.001) derived from

Phonological Assessment of Child Speech and Edinburgh Articulation Test

word final phonetic inventory expanded

Incidence of final consonant deletion decreased, the specific process addressed

Auditory discrimination improve real words but not for non-words

Author’s conclusion:

Intervention was effective in improving speech production at the single word and connected speech

but was not responsible for the significant changes in spelling scores as this may well have been

influenced by the school curriculum.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

As above.

Noteworthy that therapy provided within the research project seemed to culminated in greater

change to her speech than all the other therapy she had had since she was two-years-old

Suggests that an intervention program with a long lead time in its planning and a short period

of implementation brings about rapid changes to children‟s output speech

114

36. Pokorni, J. L., Worthington, C. K., & Jamison, P. J. (2004). Phonological awareness intervention: Comparison of Fast

ForWord, Earobics, and LiPS. The Journal of Educational Research, 97(3), 147-157.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological awareness for children with language

impairments

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pseudo-randomised controlled trial

Level III-1

Aim of study:

Noting the increased attention on enhancing children‟s‟ phonological awareness skills to facilitate oral

and written language skills, the aim of this study was to compare the effectiveness of three commercial

intervention programs that aim to do this - Fast ForWord, Earobics and Lindamood Phonemic

Sequencing program (LiPS)- in children aged 7;5 to 9;0 years with oral and written language problems.

Specific aims were to determine if children achieve gain phonological awareness, language and reading

related skills and whether one program or more result in greater gains for these skills more than others.

Population:

N= 54 children, aged 7;5 to 9;0 years, who had received school based speech and language services,

were reading more than 1 year below their grade level, spoke English as their main language, had

normal hearing and performed ≥ 1 SD below the mean on at least one of three subtests from the CELF-

3. Children randomly allocated to either the Fast ForWord (20), Earobics (16) or LiPS (18) groups.

Service setting:

School (summer camp especially established implement this program)

Description of

intervention

Service delivery

model

Group sessions with of 4 children in the LiPS group and 5 children in Fast ForWord or Earobics.

Provider(s):

Three speech pathologists trained and experienced with the particular programme for their allocated

group(s) coordinated the training of the group leaders who implemented the program

Group leaders were either speech pathologists, special education teachers or teacher carried out the

sessions the direction of the speech pathologists after training. There were 2 days of training for

Fast ForWord and Earobics or 3 days for LiPS

Domain of language:

Phonological awareness

Type of intervention:

See below

Method of

intervention:

All children attended a 20-day summer camp that ran for 5 hours per day with 3 hours assigned to

intervention and the remaining 2 hours for breaks. Group leaders conducted the respective program with

the children according to the program directions and, when necessary, redirected the children‟s attention

to the task. The same group leader supervised the children through the breaks. No reading activities

were done during these breaks.

Fast ForWord

Each child assigned a computer to do the program and did 3 of the assigned tasks foe about 20

minutes each (i.e. 9 per day)

Earobics

Each child assigned a computer to do the program

Students played each of the 5 games three times during each hour

Lindamood Phonemic Sequencing program

Group leaders conducted the program according to the program directions but excluded the CD-

Rom exercises

Quantum of service

provision:

3 hours per day from a 5 hour program run for 20 consecutive week days

115

Outcomes

evaluation (by

disorder subtype if

possible):

Effectiveness of interventions

Phonemic awareness measure (Phonological Awareness Test )

o Significant group x time effect F 92,51)=3.69 p<.01 with post hoc testing indicating that

this was associated with increases in blending on LiPS

Language measures (CELF-3)

o No significant group x time effect

Reading measures (Woodstock language proficiency battery –R)

o No significant group x time effect

Effects of individual interventions(i.e. did each intervention bring about change)

Phonemic awareness measure

o significant increases for Earobics F (1,15)=6.06 p<.05 & LiPs (1,17)=11.83 p<.01

For Earobics, differences in phoneme segmentation.

For LiPs, differences in phoneme blending and segmentation

Language measures (CELF-3)

o No significant increases for any of the interventions

Reading measures

o No significant increases for any of the interventions

Author’s

conclusion:

LiPS and Earobics were more effective than Fast ForWord for improving phonological awareness. None

of the programs were associated with changes in oral or written language.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

These three programs appear to bring about little changes in the output measures when conducted for

three hours per day over 20 days, with the only changes noted in phonological awareness. However, it is

not clear whether maturation may account for these changes as there was no control for this. Also three

groups were different sizes so this may influence outcomes. Also, as no time was allocated for the

newly acquired phonological awareness skills to generalise to language skills, the notion that there is no

transfer needs to be treated cautiously.

It is interesting to note that the program associated with the greatest change (LiPS) involved human

interaction. This study suggests that using computer packages for phonological awareness intervention

may have limited value.

116

37. Ray, J. (2002). Treating phonological disorders in a multilingual child: A case study. American Journal of Speech-Language

Pathology, 11(3), 305-315.

Summary of intervention program: Cognitive linguistic therapy for a trilingual child with

phonological disorder

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Case study with pre and post testing and monthly reassessments over the 5-month course of

therapy to check generalisation to untrained words and to conversational speech through

conversational analysis.

Level-Other

Aim of study:

Working with children from a non-English speaking background raises questions of practice

when the speech pathologist is monolingual. This study aimed to

better understand the phonology of a trilingual child

explore the efficacy of using a cognitive linguistic approach

Population:

A 5 year old boy learning Hindi and Gujarati since birth and English from the age of 4 years

when he commenced preschool.

Parental report indicated

Difficulty with the articulation of sounds from the age of two and inconsistent

productions of speech sounds across words in all languages

Unremarkable birth history -

Assessment indicated normal cognitive skills; no dyspraxia; normal oral motor functioning,

hearing, sound discrimination and syntax semantics or pragmatics from language sample analysis

Service setting:

Clinic

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

Speech pathologist fluent in all three languages

Domain of language:

Phonology-suppression of phonological processes by specifically targeting the sounds /r, l, f, v /

and clusters.

ICF Impairment

Type of intervention:

Cognitive linguistic using concepts from Cycles (Hodson) and PACT (Bowen & Cupples, 1998)

Method of

intervention:

Therapy focussed on English as requested by the child‟s parents

Perception

o Distinguish between word pairs through auditory perception

o Auditory bombardment

Production

o Production practice

Quantum of service

provision:

3 x 45-60 weekly sessions over 5 consecutive month

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

From the first assessment (T1) to the last assessment (T3):

Phonological processes decreased from 88% to 16%

Speech intelligibility rating increased from 2 to 3

PCC increased from 70 to 95%

GFTA scores increased from10th

to 44th

percentile

Author’s conclusion:

The use of the cognitive linguistic approach conducted in English was effective in increasing

consonant accuracy, intelligibility and reducing the occurrence of phonological patterns in three

languages.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Whilst the phonology system clearly changed, it is not clear that therapy in general and the

cognitive linguistic approach in particular was responsible for this changed as there were no

controls for maturation or other events that may have triggered change, such as starting school.

117

38. Richardson, K., & Klecan-Aker, J. S. (2000). Teaching pragmatics to language-learning disabled children: a treatment

outcome study. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 16(1), 23-42.

Summary of intervention program: Pragmatics

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pre-post test

Level IV

Aim of study:

To measure the effects of a pragmatic treatment programme.

Population:

N=20 in two groups of subjects, aged 6;5 to 9;8 years, who had to have a full scale IQ scores of

85

Tested on CELF with variable results ranging from 72-93

Service setting:

private school for children with learning needs

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Classroom based

Provider(s):

SLPs

Domain of language:

pragmatics- in three areas (1) conversation,(2)internal responses and (3), qualitative and

quantitative descriptions of objects

Type of intervention:

Method of

intervention:

Each area of conversation, internal responses, qualitative and quantitative descriptions of objects

was addressed for 10 minutes in each session using visual stimuli such as blackboard and posters.

Each component involved a description by the teacher following activities whereby children

practiced the new skills and then reflected on them.

Quantum of service

provision:

6 weeks but the length of each session nor the number of sessions each week was stated (implied

each session was 30 minutes)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Criterion referenced test. For both groups, the post -test scores were higher than the pre-test scores

for most items on the test.

Author’s conclusion:

The authors concluded that all five areas improved over and consequent to the program

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

In the absence of controls for maturation and no validity and reliability measure for the tool to

measures outcomes and no discussion whether results were clinically and statistically significant, it

is not clear that the children made progress and even of they did, that the agent of change cannot be

isolated in this study.

118

39. Roulstone, S., Owen, R., & French, L. (2005). Speech and language therapy and the Knowles Edge standards fund project:

An evaluation of the service provided to a cluster of primary schools. British Journal of Special Education, 32(2), 78-85.

Summary of intervention program:Holistic & collaborative for speech and language

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pre-test post test using quantitative and qualitative methods

Level IV (Quantitative component)

Aim of study:

As the (UK) local education authorities (LEAs) had difficulty securing speech and language

therapy services in schools, their Department for Education and Employment established a

funding mechanism in 2000, the Standards Fund, to encourage “collaborative working and

strategic cooperation” between the National Health Service and the LEAs. Consequently, 25 pilot

projects were supported and this paper reports on the outcomes of one of those projects.

A model of service delivery was developed using a systems analysis approach which assumes that

children‟s speech, language and communication outcomes are maximised when the model of

service has the child at its heart but that its focus is broader than the child. The five components of

the system were; (1)the children, (2) their parents, (3) the education and therapy staff, (4) the

schools and classrooms and (5) the strategic structures in the health and education services.

Population:

Over 18 months, 177 of the 185 children, aged 4 to 11 years, who were assessed, received therapy.

Of those, 103 had a standard assessment performed at baseline and 6months later and they formed

4 groups according to diagnosis (See results section).

Service setting:

Schools (& in the cluster)

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model see above; holistic & collaborative such that

children received

o individual or group therapy that was supported by teaching staff and/or parents;

o guided intervention for the teachers with monitoring progress and review

o follow up where progress was monitored after a period of therapy

Parents were involved in giving a case history, negotiating goals and ways of helping as well

as invited to join school based sessions and implement specific or general activities at home

Education and therapy staff were involved in activities that enhanced liaison between them by

focusing of developing their knowledge bases of speech therapy and curriculum respectively

and collaborative practice through dedicated staff development days as well as allocated time

for the teacher and speech language therapists to meet at the times of assessment and review.

Schools and classroom

o Video recordings of class time focussing on the target children which the Speech

language therapists and teacher viewed together to identify positive and negative

aspects of communication and interaction and from this developed strategies for

intervention

Health and education structures

o Management at a strategic level was central to the project with project funding

providing 2 full time speech language therapists for 9 schools. Funding came from (1)

the LEA, (2) the health authority and (3) the schools. Representatives from these

three bodies formed a project team and met each term with the project SLTs to agree

to goals and solve problems.

Provider(s):

Speech language therapists only after extensive negotiations to determine the amount of input.

Domain of language:

Speech and language as negotiated

Type of intervention: As negotiated and see above

Method of

intervention:

119

Quantum of service

provision:

SLTs spent on average about 6 hours per child; with 58% of the time in direct contact with the

child and 42% with parents, teachers and other staff. Individual sessions were weekly or fortnightly

e

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Children

Receptive & expressive

language (N=45)

Expressive

language (N=12)

Speech

(N=37)

Pragmatics

(N=9)

CELF-Ling concepts CS & SS NS NS NS

RAPT grammar SS Not used SS NS

RAPT information SS Not used SS NS

CELF- recalling

sentences

CS NS NS NS

Speech % correct CS & SS CS & SS CS & SS NS

KEY: CS =clinically significant; SS = statistically significant; NS= not significant

The following information was generated from semi-structured interviews with parents, children,

head teachers and therapists; questionnaires, discussions and audit of case records

Parents

Trends from parent interviews indicated they were generally pleased

preferred the school based service to other services they had received

Teachers and Therapists

Increases in satisfaction with their knowledge of speech and language difficulties and the

curriculum respectively

School and classroom

Changes in organisation issues ; explicit referral procedures and a list of resources, created

additional non-contact time for teachers and SLTs to meet

Therapists were seen as a part of the school team

Health fund and education structures

Funding for continuation of the project was received

Meetings each term between relevant staff

Author’s conclusion:

Whilst the changes in the children cannot be confidently assigned to the program there was an

overall sense that the project was successful. This project had unique features (relative to other

projects) and one was that it focussed on a cluster of school rather than a whole trust of LEA.

Subsequent to the project, this model has been rolled out to another two clusters of schools.

Factors thought to contribute to the project‟s success included

o joint management that enabled the participants to be accountable to their project and

professional managers

o increased accessibility of therapists within schools

o negotiation of therapy input at all levels

o needs driven approaches to therapy delivery allowing flexibility in service delivery

o management practices that support collaboration rather than leaving it to chance such

as allocated times for collaboration between teachers and therapists.

Part of the success of this project may have been that its approach to service delivery was

broader than a consultative model.

The outcomes of the broader Standards fund project indicate there is little information about

how speech and language interventions are organised and how they different types of service

delivery compare

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This report on this project raises many relevant questions to the current review about service

delivery and highlights the need to examine models of service delivery in conjunctions with

different interventions

120

40. Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2004). Computer-supported phonological awareness intervention for kindergarten children with

specific language impairment Language, Speech & Hearing Services in the Schools, 35(3), 229-239.

Summary of intervention program: Computer-supported phonological awareness

intervention

Country of publication: The Netherlands

Publication type (include

level of evidence):

Comparative study (level III-2)

Three matched groups of children with SLI groups pre tested once and post tested twice

Aim To determine whether the phonological awareness skill of children with Specific Language

Impairment can be enhanced through computer intervention and whether speech

enhancement (by slowing etc) has any added learning benefit.

Population:

N= 36 children with specific language impairment aged 4;10-6;11 years (all but one child

was 5 years +) who were judged by an interdisciplinary team of clinical linguists and

school psychologists to have

a significant deficit in production and/or comprehension of language

no cognitive, sensorimotor, neurological or psychiatric disorders

no general lack of exposure to language

no significant hearing loss

However no details of testing were given.

Service setting:

Five different classrooms in two special schools (small group intervention although

children worked independently from each other).

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Groups of 3 children each working on separate computers with an experimenter

supervising ensuring children were engaged in the task experimenter clinician in the room

Provider(s):

Not stated

Domain of language:

Phonological awareness

ICF: Impairment

Type of intervention:

Method of intervention:

Experimental intervention:

A custom made educational software programme focusing on rhyming and synthesis

skills to enhance emergent and beginning literacy skills

Children progress through a series of games ordered from least to greatest difficulty.

Each game has three sets of five exercises and children progress once they have met

criterion

Intervention involved groups of 3 children (one from each experimental group)

Experimental group 1 received the above programme

Experimental group 2 received the above program with enhanced speech (slowed

down by 150% and the fast transitional elements in the speech were enhanced up to

20dB, with the amount of speech manipulation decreasing as the intervention

proceeded

Control group intervention:

Used the vocabulary acquisition part of the same computer software which involved

listening to a story on the computer and answering questions by pointing to the screen

Audio and visual feedback

Quantum of service

provision:

2-3 x 15-minute computer sessions per week for 5 weeks (3.5 hours)

Experimental group 2: 4 sessions with maximum speech manipulation, 4 sessions with

no delay but enhancement to 20dB, 3 sessions with enhancement up 10dB, and 3

sessions with normal speech

Outcomes evaluation (by

disorder subtype if

possible):

phonological awareness skills

At first post testing

o experimental group 1(normal speech) made significant and most progress

o no difference between experimental group 2 (enhanced speech) and the

control group

At second post test, 18 weeks after the intervention, no group effect s.

121

Author’s conclusion:

Children with SLI who had not begun formal literacy education benefited from a short,

intensive computer assisted phonological awareness intervention with speech that was not

manipulated. Computer assisted phonological awareness intervention that incorporated

manipulated speech did not produce any additional effect over vocabulary work or

phonological awareness work

Reviewer’s conclusion:

A short computer assisted phonological awareness programme appeared to be beneficial

for children with SLI, however the addition of speech manipulation may have actually

decreased its effectiveness as these children were no different to the control group. It is

also possible that the control treatment may have improved children‟s phonological skills.

122

41. Skarakis-Doyle, E. (2005). Reconceptualizing treatment goals from language impairment to functional limitations: A case

study. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(4), 353-363.

Summary of intervention program: Discourse

Country: Canada

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Case study

Level-Other

Aim of study:

To describe intervention that focuses on addressing the functional limitations rather than on the

impairment itself.

Population:

A boy, aged 10 years, with persistent and severe receptive and expressive language impairment.

This child had had ongoing, intensive therapy for the previous 5 years, 3 x 30 minutes session each

week f that had focused on the level of impairment.

Service setting:

Private special education school

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual and group

Provider(s):

Speech-language pathologist but goals were developed in collaboration with the classroom teacher

Domain of language:

ICF activity and participation framework used to determine goals of :

Comprehension of language in context, such as reading

Production of language that was contextually appropriate to the task (e.g., answering the

phone, carrying out messages)

Type of intervention:

Semantic to develop meaning of

anaphoric referencing

temporal and causal connectors

different sentence types

new vocabulary items

Method of

intervention:

Extensive assessment and analysis of his language skills relative to the context in which he was

operating prior to establishing the program of intervention:

Comprehension

The program, Reading Milestones, was used to develop meaning (he decoded age

appropriately but could not extract the meaning)

Social use of language

Use of script therapy and modelling

Quantum of service

provision:

3 hours of individual intervention per week plus an hour of communication group work with other

students in his class.

Service was provided over three years but not clear if the above dosage remained the same

throughout that time span

123

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Discursive report of outcomes

Standardised language scores (not specified)

Remained stable

Comprehension

Showed some gains in use of anaphoric pronouns into his daily reading activities. This

contrasted to other results from treatment on wh-question understanding

answering factual content questions increased in accuracy from 56% to 83% after 6

treatment sessions

Language in use outcomes

a 12-fold increase in topic initiations

more than doubled conversation maintaining comments compared to baseline

utterance length increased a little and attempted complex sentences

His teachers and mother reporting a notable increase in spoken contributions to

conversations, noting he increasingly initiated conversations and contributed new

information that extended the conversation beyond a single turn. This was particularly so

with peers.

language for daily living activities;

o take a phone message mostly correctly and give it to the appropriate individual,

o successfully place his order at fast food restaurants on class field trips

o carry out simple banking transactions with class lunch program funds.

At the completion of treatment, he was still learning to use polite forms

Author’s conclusion:

It is possible to ameliorate the impact of language impairment on activity and participation.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

It is not clear whether the therapy was responsible for the changes because of the discursive

reporting of results. The importance of this article is demonstrating the importance of monitoring

progress and changing strategy when one strategy does not appear to be working. It also

demonstrates how to apply the ICF to paediatric practice.

124

42. Smith, J., Downs, M., & Mogford-Bevan, K. (1998). Can phonological awareness training facilitate minimal pair therapy?

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 33(supplement), 463-468.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological awareness training vs Conventional speech

therapy for children with persistent phonological disorder

Country: England

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Comparative Group study (Assessed blind to group assignment). Each group received two types of

treatment in counterbalanced order and were assessed at four points, once prior to intervention and

then after each block and follow-up

Level III-2

Aim of study:

To determine if phonological awareness therapy facilitates conventional therapy for children with

persistent speech impairment

Population:

N=18 children with persistent specific phonological disorder aged 5;8 to 8;2 years who had

completed at least 10 sessions of group speech therapy without success (no indication of how

this was measured)

no therapy for three months

performed normally on (≥10th

percentile) on receptive language (the TROG) expressive

language (Renfrew Bus Story &British Picture Vocabulary test) on and non-verbal cognition

(Ravens‟ Coloured Matrices) testing.

Divided into two groups with each group having therapy sequentially

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Not stated if service delivery was 1:1 or in groups

Provider(s):

Speech-language therapists different from the person who conducted the assessments

Domain of language:

Expressive phonology

ICF; Impairment

Type of intervention:

Conventional speech therapy

Aim: to introduce a single new phonological contrast into the child‟s speech

An eclectic approach including articulation therapy, minimal pair work and generalisation

tasks

vs

Phonological awareness training

Aim to increase child‟s awareness of phonological structure of spoken words

Tasks included (1)rhyming and (2) phoneme awareness- judging word length for the number

of sounds; counting and segmenting syllables from words, identifying phonemes in words and

deleting them

Method of

intervention:

As above

Quantum of service

provision:

(2 x 1.25 hours per week for 4 weeks) x 2 ( i.e. 12 hours of therapy in total)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Phonology (South Tyneside Test of phonology but scoring system not described).

Speech significantly improved for both groups when comparing the first and fourth group

assessment scores

Neither group‟s speech skills significantly increased after the first phase of therapy but did

after the second, irrespective of the therapy they had (conventional speech therapy or

Phonological awareness training

Three subgroups of response patterns

o 50% of the children improved in speech and phonological awareness scores

o 22% only improved phonological awareness scores

o 28% only improved on phonological awareness tasks of rhyming and no others nor

made any progress in productive speech

125

Author’s conclusion:

The addition of phonological awareness training added significant value over and above that

attributable to maturation or conventional therapy.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

It appears that the about half of the children need a therapy package that incorporates conventional

components as well as phonological awareness training. However, others need a more specialised

package and this may only become apparent once therapy has commenced hence underscoring the

need to closely monitor children‟s progress in therapy.

126

43. Stiegler, L. N., & Hoffman, P. R. (2001). Discourse-based intervention for word finding in children. Journal of

Communication Disorders, 34, 277-303.

Summary of intervention program: Discourse based intervention for word finding problems

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Within-subject multiple baseline across task design

Level III-3

Aim of study:

Cognizant that intervention for word finding problems is generally conducted in discrete,

decontexualised, drill type activities with little evidence that these skills generalize to discourse,

the aim was to determine if intervention for word finding difficulties is more effective conducted

within discourse. Thus the intervention was designed to facilitate effective on-line word processing

in children.

Population:

Three boys, aged 9;2, 9;10 and 9;6 years children with proven language-learning disorders who

had :

no co-morbid disorder

normal hearing

no history of a fluency disorder

normal receptive vocabulary as measured by the PPVT

one standard deviation below the mean on the Test of Word-finding in discourse

Confirmed clinical occurrences of word finding problems

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

Clinician without explicit statement that they were SLPs

Domain of language:

Expressive vocabulary-word finding

ICF: Impairment, Activity and participation

Type of intervention:

Discourse based contextual intervention

Method of

intervention:

At the point of a word-finding problem in three types of discourse (picture-elicited narratives,

story-retelling narratives and conversations about familiar topics), the clinicians provided

immediate appropriate feedback aimed to

o make the student aware that communication breakdown had occurred

o systematically assist the child through the word finding process

o confirm the appropriateness of the target word when it was produced.

Giving feedback followed a pre-determined response protocol and included requests for

associative information, requests for clarification and comments to confirm appropriate word

choices.

Intervention did not aim to directly cue word production

Quantum of service

provision:

5x15 minute sessions for each of the five program stages (i.e. 6 hours 15 minutes per child) over 5

weeks. The interval between each session was not reported

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

The mean ratios and their standard deviations of total overt word finding behaviours to total words

derived collectively from three types of discourse, picture-elicited narratives, story-retelling and

conversations decreased from pre-to post-baseline for all three children

subject 1 -12.3 (6.4) to 9.3 (4.5)

subject 2- 8.9 (4.9) to 7.0 (5.0)

subject 3 7.3 (5.0) to 5.2 (3.4)

Author’s conclusion: That discourse based intervention reduces word finding problems

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The data presented was variable without baselines being established that it was not clear that

the intervention was the agent of change.

The hypothesis of whether discourse based intervention for word finding problems is more

effective than intervention using discrete tasks could not be answered by the design used by

these researchers.

127

44. Strand, E. A., Stoeckel, R., & Baas, B. (2006). Treatment of severe childhood apraxia of speech: a treatment efficacy study.

Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 14(4), 297-307.

Summary of intervention program: a customised motor speech intervention for childhood

apraxia of speech

Country: USA

Publication

type (include

level of

evidence):

Single subject, multiple baseline across behaviours and replicated across the four children

Level IV

Aim of study:

Determine the efficacy of integral stimulation dynamic temporal and tactile cueing (DTTC)

Population:

4 nonverbal children aged 5;7, 5;8, 5;5 and 6;1 years. All had confirmed childhood apraxia of

speech and 2 had co-occurring dysarthria. All had had speech pathology services varying from 2

to 4 years

Service

setting:

Not stated-University clinic?

Description of intervention

Service

delivery model

(individual

/group etc):

1:1 intervention

Provider(s):

The first two authors; qualifications not stated

Domain of

language:

Phonology output

Type of

intervention:

Integral stimulation dynamic temporal and tactile cueing (DTTC) which draws on principles of

motor learning (mass and distributed practice, amount of practice, how practice is organised in the

session and the type and frequency of feedback) and methods developed for adult acquired

apraxia. Aims to enable the child to assemble and retrieve the motor plans with reducing temporal,

tactile and visual cueing. Integral to the approach is a hierarchy of temporal delay but the temporal

relationship is changed on a trial by trial basis

Method of

intervention:

Two phases (1) teaching articulatory configurations (2) to transition the movement smoothly

through to the syllable shape using techniques such as phonetic placement, tactile and prosodic

cueing. Each of the following steps are followed

Stimuli based on syllable shape vowel context and phonetic complexity

Practice A small stimulus chosen to achieve for mass practice with each item (utterance)

practiced 15-30 times per block with 2-3 blocks per session.

Feedback is specific to movement but its specificity and frequency was gradually reduced with

increases in latency time between stimulus and response.

Frequency/

intensity/

duration of

service

provision:

Twice daily 30-minute therapy 5 days per week for 6 weeks

128

Outcomes

evaluation (by

disorder subtype

if possible):

Three of the four children achieved accurate movement gestures for pre-determined

utterances such as hello, no way, why up, out and my dad over the 6 week period. The other

child would not engage in the practice. Examination of the graphs showed that increased

accuracy of movement only occurred after therapy of the target began. Authors stated that

the children had continued to progress after the completion of this project bur still continued

with speech pathology at their clinics.

Author’s

conclusion:

DTTC treatment that involves frequent treatment, the principles of motor learning and an

approach that utilises practice of speech movement gestures was efficacious for 3 of the 4

children. The key factors were

Frequent treatment realised as twice daily 30 minutes sessions for 6 weeks + twice daily 5

minute sessions of home practice where parents were taught how to do the practiced

Type and frequency of feedback that focused on movements not sounds

Noted that the additional evidence that this intervention was efficacious was that the

previous intervention had not been successful for speaking.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

A treatment regime that carefully details the child‟s speech impairment linguistically coupled

with a principles of motor learning used in a pre-determined, systematic way seems to bring

about change for some children with CAS

129

46.Swanson, L. A., Fey, M. E., Mills, C. E., & Hood, L. S. (2005). Use of narrative-based language intervention with children

who have specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology, 14(2), 131-143.

Summary of intervention program: narrative-based language intervention for children with

specific language impairment.

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Pre-post testing

Level IV

Aim of study:

To determine the feasibility of using narrative-based language intervention by trialling a program

designed to do this children with SLI

Population:

N= 10 children with specific language impairment, aged 6;11 to 8;9 years. Children came from

homes where English was the main language and performed ≤ -1 SD below the mean on TOLD-

P:3. Children had proven normal hearing, oral motor skills and non-verbal cognition and no frank

neurological problems or social emotional disorders

Service setting:

Home, school or clinic as parents requested

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model Individual

Provider(s):

Speech language pathologists

Domain of language:

Language-discourse (narrative), syntax and semantics but using discourse as the vehicle to teach all

language levels.

ICF- impairment

Type of intervention: Hybrid approach using narrative-based language intervention

Method of

intervention:

Goals

Overall: Increase frequency of complex grammatical forms found in stories

Intermediate

o Grammar- post-modification of nouns (relative clauses, prepositional phrases and

appositives); subordinating & coordinating conjunctions and verb phrase elaboration.

o Story-goals setting, endings, problem-resolution pairs

Goal attack strategy- horizontal/cycling

Narrative-based language intervention

modelling targets forms at least 15 times per session in spontaneous activities

recasting

Warm up activity

o The child retold the story from the previous session and the speech-language

pathologists made NO corrections

Story retell imitation task

o (1) Theme explanation of theme, (2) a story reading by speech-language pathologist

using large inflectional modulations , (3) re-read story one component at a time (4)

the child retold it on a component-by-component basis whilst speech-language

pathologist placed corresponding coloured pictures out.

o If the target was not produced, the speech-language pathologists modelled the

sentence or recast the child‟s sentence

task whereby child imitated sentences containing the target form to provide child with

intensive exposure to and production practice of target forms

Story generation task whereby

o with scaffolding the child told a story with the speech-language pathologists having a

script as to prompt the child fro each of the story grammar components.

o The story was then drawn

o Child retold story

Repeated retellings: the child took the pictures and a copy of the story home to retell at home

130

Quantum of service

provision:

3 50 min sessions per week for six weeks (i.e. 15 hours)

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Primary measures

These were determined by using re-sampling techniques and randomisation tests and

comparing the gains scores to those of a previous longitudinal study of children with specific

language impairment on these same measures.

9. Narrative quality

8/10 children made clinically significant gains

10. Number of different words

1/10 children made clinically significant gains

Secondary measures

Grammar

1. Developmental Sentence Scoring-No change

Cognitive processing

1. Recalling sentences sub-test of CELF -3 - No change

2. Non word repetition (Dollaghan & Campbell) - No change

Author’s conclusion:

This feasibility study indicated that this approach to intervention with children with specific

language impairment had merit and was worth developing further. Whilst the children made

statistically and clinically significant gains in narrative quality over the six-week period but not on

other measures-primary or secondary, it was not conclusive that the intervention was the change

agent. This feasibility study indicated that Grade 2 children accepted this approach and aspects of

the intervention that need modification.

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

This study provides some evidence that narrative-based language intervention is successful in

enhancing children‟s discourse. For speech-language pathologists looking for a well-justified study

about narrative-based language intervention, this article provides excellent information. Whilst the

authors have been appropriately cautious in attributing the outcomes to the intervention, the study

offers more scope for attribution that pre-post studies because of their methodical use of data from

a previous study. The method they used to refer to the data from a previous but related study

suggests that the changes in narrative quality the children achieved are greater than chance. This

study also needs to be noted because it is the realisation of more contemporary views of holistic

and functional intervention and demonstrates to clinicians how this form of intervention can be

achieved.

131

47. Williams, A. L. (2000). Multiple oppositions: Case studies of variables in phonological intervention. American Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology, 9(4), 289-299.

Summary of intervention program: Phonology (Multiple Oppositions vs. minimal pairs vs.

naturalistic speech intelligibility)

Country: USA

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Longitudinal case studies

Level IV

Aim of study:

The aims of this study are

to describe the course of intervention for children with moderate to severe phonological

impairment to tease out variables that affect phonological change such as different

interventions (e.g. multiple oppositions vs minimal pairs etc) and structures for implementing

interventions goals

to determine if these children require a package of interventions that begins with multiple

oppositions, then progresses to single oppositions and finally to conversation based

intervention

Population:

Ten children (8 boys and 3 girls) with moderate to severe phonological impairment, aged 4;0 to 6;5

years at the beginning of the longitudinal study (3 children were 5+ years) who (had):

excluded at least 6 sounds across three manner categories

normal hearing

normal cognition

normal receptive vocabulary as measured by the PPVT

absence of organic or motor disorders

English as their first language

Service setting:

A clinical practice

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Individual

Provider(s):

Clinician without explicit statement that they were SLPs

Domain of language:

Phonology

ICF: Impairment but steps to ensure that criterion in conversation (i.e. activity and participation)

were achieved

Type of intervention:

A package of intervention that begins with multiple oppositions and incorporated minimal pairs,

traditional and naturalistic speech intelligibility interventions as needed.

Method of

intervention:

Intervention package

Goals were based on phonemic factors and learnability theory

four steps; (1) imitation, (2) imitation + spontaneous production, (3) spontaneous production

and (4) conversation with pre-established criterion for progressions between steps.

Multiple opposition contrast

Addresses homonymy by intervention that focuses on the multiple collapses of adult contrasts

Simultaneously contrasts several target sounds within the group of sound the child collapses

(e.g. /s, k, t/ and „ch‟ realised as /t/ so all are contrasted)

Therapy is unique to each child‟s sound system

Minimal pair approach

Addresses homonymy by addressing one contrast at a time

Natural speech intelligibility

o Intervention of targeted sounds in naturalistic activities designed to provide frequent

opportunities for that sound to occur.

o SLPs recast the child‟s error production without imitative prompts or direct motor training.

The recast is immediately contingent on the child‟s errored production

132

Quantum of service

provision:

2x30 minute sessions per week.

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Correct underlying representations (the level of adult-like production of phonemes)

Group means changed from 37% (range 12 – 60) to 85% (range 69-99)

Intervention quantum

Number of sessions-Mean 60.3 sessions (i.e. 30 hours); range 26- 105 with an average of

3.4 semesters

The more severe the impairment the more therapy that was required

Treated sounds

Moderate impairment - training on mid and later developing alveolar and palatal

fricatives, affricates, stops and liquids

Severe to profound impairment - training on early, mid and later developing sounds

involving all points and manners of articulation

Intervention types

All children had multiple oppositions intervention

6/10 required minimal pairs in addition

5/10 needed naturalistic speech intelligibility intervention

1/10 children needed traditional therapy.

8/10 children needed more than one type of intervention

Goal structuring

Horizontal goal attach was used with all children

4/10 cycling was used for

1 child needed vertical structuring

Author’s conclusion:

Children with moderate to severe phonological impairment may require a package of different

interventions to resolve their impairment rather than one intervention type. The key success factors

of therapy include consideration of (1) the models and structures of intervention (a package that

combines different interventions and how goals are targeted (cycling/vertical/horizontal) and (2)

the processes to select treatment (child factors in how they use the sounds vs. the characteristics of

the sounds)

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

The severity of these children‟s phonological impairment coupled with the evidence that children

with this level of severity do not change their speech patterns without therapy add weight to the

notion that this package of intervention was the agent of change for these children. If so, then the

notion that these children benefited from an intervention package that comprised different

interventions has significant service delivery implications. The indicator for changing types of

intervention was the children not reaching the predetermined criteria to move to each stage.

133

48. Wren, Y., Roulstone, S., Parkhouse, J., & Hall, B. (2001). A model for a mainstream school-based speech and language

therapy service. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 17(2), 107-126.

Summary of intervention program: A model for a mainstream school-based speech and

language therapy service

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

A clinical study using pre and post measurement but with baselines. The children were assessed

three times- once at time 1; their initial assessment once at time 2, at the beginning of the project, a

year later than the initial assessment with no therapy and once at time 3 after the project

intervention. The children acted as their own controls in that their progress between times 1 and 2

(control period) was compared with their progress between times 2 and 3 (intervention period).

Level IV

Aim of study:

The aims of this 2-year pilot study were to describe the process of setting up and evaluating a

mainstream school-based and -funded speech and language therapy service. The context for this

project was of growing concerns and reports (in the UK) about the provision of speech and

language therapy services for mainstream children with communication difficulties. Agreement

existed that provision of services was an educational need and that support should be made

available from the Department for Education and Employment enabling local education authorities

(LEAs) to create enhanced SLT services in partnership with the NHS.

A model of service delivery was developed emphasizing that consider SLT provision occur within

a broad educational context with four needs; those of the children (speech, language and

communication, self esteem and curriculum access), the classes, the education and therapy staff

and the school.

Population:

28 of the 98 children referred to the service who had waited at least 1 year between their initial

assessment and beginning therapy and were also those causing greatest concern to the school staff.

At the end of the 2-year period 23/29 children were available for follow up. Children were grouped

according to affected language domain. However there age was not described.

Service setting:

A cluster of 7 preschools and schools were staffed by two part time (EFT =1) speech language

therapist. The SLTs worked in the schools

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model

Not stated

Provider(s):

Speech language therapist

Domain of language:

Phonology and language (grammar?)

Type of intervention:

As negotiated but not described

Method of

intervention:

Time was allocated about equally between the four components

Children

Therapy not described

Teachers and assistants

attended staff development activities for one day and five twilight sessions

liaison over setting Individual Education plans was also expected to achieve staff development

goals

Speech language therapists

Attended training on curriculum

School

Dedicated management structure-heads of all schools, the local speech language therapist

manager, an educational psychologist, a community paediatrician and a local NHS trust

manager

134

Quantum of service

provision:

Not stated

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Child-based outcomes

Language Contrasting progress in the intervention period with that in control period

o Expressive -Bus Story (Renfrew, 1995)-

expressive language therapy group

4/9 improved; 4 lost ground and no results for 9th

child.

receptive/expressive language therapy group

2/4 improved in expressive language ; 2/4 fell further behind

mixed group

2/6 improved in expressive language ;2 children maintained the

level of delay

o Receptive TROG and the Token Test

Missing data so no report

Phonology An informal naming test

o 9/11 improve

o Children with phonology and language problems whose therapy focused on

phonology skills alone showed less speech progress

self-esteem and access to the curriculum

o Trends suggested both improved

o Early in the project teaching staff considered therapists‟ comments and suggestions as

being relevant to the delivery of the English but this seemed to shift to the broader

curriculum over the project

Class based outcomes

outcomes of teacher questionnaire indicated that staff development activities appeared to

increase teachers understanding of the impact of communication impairments in the classroom

setting, as well developing strategies to assist

Teacher-therapist based outcomes

results from questionnaires indicated teachers and therapists had increased understanding of

each other‟s role

Teachers and assistants participating in the development program reported greater confidence

in identifying children with speech and language difficulties compared with staff who had not

yet attended. Also some teachers indicated they had gaps in their knowledge consequent to

becoming more aware of speech and language issues in schools.

Therapists‟ attendances at courses improved their knowledge and understanding of education

initiatives

School-based outcomes

An agreed referral procedure allowing for prioritizing children

Therapists increasing knowledge of school work schemes and the national curriculum resulted

in increasing use of curriculum as a tool for intervention

A resource base containing useful materials for teachers in the classrooms

Author’s conclusion:

This program in which children‟s needs were considered holistically in the context of their

educational environment resulted in all four outcomes being met to some degree as:

children‟s communication skills (phonology more than language), self-esteem and access to

the curriculum improved,

teacher‟s understanding of communication difficulties had increased.

therapists were more aware of education issues.

procedures for referral and liaison had been established.

The pilot study identified areas for focus in future such as more objective measurements and the

involvement of parents. The key success factors were the philosophy, the dedicated management

structure and process and the frequency of contact of the SLTs within the schools to develop

relationships between teaching staff and therapists

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

A speech and language therapy model that that is holistic and situated within the children‟s

educational environment seemed to result in changes in many the a priori areas. In addition to the

above key success factors, are the initial collaboration between all participants to create a service

delivery that suits their needs, explicit time for all participants to plan individual sessions towards

common aims, the expectation that all stakeholders will learn about each other‟s roles, the explicit

collaboration between the employers and managers, the flexibility to plan programs for children to

suit that child‟s needs within principles of practices rather than specific procedures.

135

Wren, Y., & Roulstone, S. (2008). A comparison between computer and tabletop delivery of phonology therapy. International

Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,10(5) 346-363.

Summary of intervention program: Phonological therapy (tabletop vs computer)

Country: UK

Publication type &

level of evidence:

Controlled trial (level III-1)

Aim of study: Compare two modes of phonology intervention delivery: tabletop versus computer

Population:

N=33 children aged 4-8 years with phonological impairment (+/- additional phonetic

disorder) except those in their first term of first year of school with (1) English first language,

(2) normal hearing, (3) no structural or oral motor difficulties and (3) no direct speech and

language therapy for previous 3 months

Service setting:

School

Description of

intervention

Service delivery model 1:1 intervention at school

Provider(s): Researcher/SLP with assistant/volunteer providing follow-up sessions

Domain of language: Phonology output

Type of intervention: Phonological awareness (on phoneme detection and blending, minimal pair discrimination

and rhyme awareness)

Method of

intervention:

Tabletop/computer therapy:

Program developed around eight interactive games to develop aspects of the speech

processing. The activities were: phoneme detection, phoneme blending, minimal pair

discrimination and rhyme awareness. Emphasis was on providing the child with

opportunities to hear and use phonological contrasts. Children encouraged to develop

their metaphonological skills by reflecting on the sounds that they heard.

Therapy programme

o First 3 therapy sessions: 1-2 related phonemes selected as initial targets were

used in each of the activities

o 4th

week: either new target phonemes or a new word position for those

phonemes already being targeted were introduced

o 6th

week: where child was making good progress a third target phoneme or

word position was introduced

o Final week: all targets were revisited

The number and length of each therapy session was based on local SLP provision so

therapy was consistent with standard care under the NHS

Each session carried out jointly with SLP and assistant/volunteer at the school whereby

assistant observed and participated in each activity and then repeated the activity with

the child on two other occasions each week

Therapist checked with the assistant how the sessions had progressed in the previous

week

Tabletop therapy used:

printed pictures and table games such as tiddlywinks, snap, pairs and lotto

Puppets to support therapy in pretend activities such as a shopping game

Computer therapy:

All activities were delivered using the specially commissioned software „Phoneme

Factory‟

The only exception was where advice on tongue placement was given as a tabletop task

for both modes of delivery to help elicit target sounds

Control group:

Received no therapy

136

Quantum of service

provision:

Tabletop/computer therapy:

One 30-minute therapy session per week for 8 weeks provided by a SLP (& assistant

present)

2 x 30 minute sessions over 8 weeks with assistant/volunteer

Outcomes evaluation

(by disorder subtype

if possible):

Note: children were matched into triads according to the centile rating from the sounds in

Words subtest of the GFTA. Within each triad, each child was then randomly assigned to one

of the 3 treatment conditions.

Children attended 18-24/24 (75-100%) sessions

Children in all three groups made significant progress over study period as indicated by a

series of Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests comparing T1 and T2 PCC scores

No differences between the groups indicated by analysis of covariance

Similarly, there were no statistically significant differences between the two therapy

groups in GFTA and PCC scores from the end of treatment to follow up 3 months later

Analysis of covariance revealed no significant difference between scores at the end of

treatment for targeted categories adjusted for scores at baseline between the three

therapy conditions, suggesting that therapy condition did not significantly affect the

extent of change of targeted consonants

Similarly there was no significant difference from the end of treatment to three months

follow up between the conditions for targeted consonants

There was considerable individual variation however within each group

A stepwise logistic regression analysis between improvers and non-improvers (according

to GFTA) revealed that phoneme stimulability (p=0.032) was the only predictor of

improvement, and the following were not: speech processing measures, type of errors,

TROG and RCPM centiles at eligibility assessment or ALRS score

Author’s conclusion:

Although all groups made significant progress there were no significant differences between

the control group receiving no therapy and the two treatment groups (tabletop and computer

therapy). There were no significant differences in progress between either of the treatment

groups at the end of treatment or at 3 months follow up. One reason that may account for this

lack of difference was the quantum of therapy. This was limited in this study to fit with the

local service provision but may not have need enough to effect change, perhaps explaining

the lack of significance. Another reason may have been the heterogeneity of the group and

whether the therapy was suitable for all the children. However, the challenge is to find

therapy approaches which are effective but which are also practical and relevant given

current service limitations. They noted that the study does not adequately answer the study

questions

Reviewer’s

conclusion:

Therapy (via either tabletop or computer) was not found to be more effective than no therapy,

perhaps due to the limited amount of therapy provided. There were no significant differences

in progress between either of the treatment groups at the end of treatment or at 3 months

follow up. The study raises the questions about what brings about change in speech output;

spontaneous maturation, schooling or therapy. It also raises the point about accommodating

the heterogeneity of this group of children . It also raises the importance methodologically of

a control group because otherwise the conclusion may have been different.

137

APPENDIX 3 SUMMARY OF THE ARTICLES IN THE REVIEW BY CIRRIN & GILLAM (2008)

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Beilinson &

Olswang (2003)

Single-

subject

3 children aged

5;6 -6;3 with

„social

communication

deficits‟

identified by SLP

and teachers

based on

observation and

comparison with

normal peers;

also, scores on

normed language

tests (range from

-2SD to

„average‟ on

Peabody

Vocabulary Test,

Test of Language

Development)

University lab

school

Group therapy 2nd

year

master‟s

student in

SLP and

classroom

teacher

Social

communicat

ion deficits?

Direct instruction: in

the use of high-risk

entry behaviours

(general and specific

statements) and the use

of props (desirable

toy)to gain entry

Direct instruction

included the use of

Myer-Johnson symbols

of steps in an entry

sequence (i.e. watch

your friend, get a toy

like your friend is

using, do the same

thing as your friend,

and tell an idea).

Teachers were

instructed to prompt

students to use an entry

sequence in the

classroom.

Effect sizes for

participants between

baseline and treatment

and treatment and

withdrawal.

3/3 increased frequency

of high-risk entry

behaviours d from 2.2

to 4.5 (large)

3/3 showed increase in

prop use d from 2.5 to

10.2 (large)

3/3 showed increase in

frequency of

cooperative play d from

1.5 to 13.2 (large).

138

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Bishop, Adams

& Rosen (2006)

Non-

randomised

comparison

36 aged 8-13

with receptive

language

disability

Schools Individual

therapy

School staff Grammar:

sentence

interpretatio

-n (slow

speech v

modified

speech)

Children moved or

activated objects on

computer screen to

match spoken sentences

that increased in

syntactic complexity

Slow speech condition:

1.2s delay between the

end of one phrase

within a sentence and

the start of the next

Modified speech

condition: same

algorithm for

modifying rate and

amplification of

frequencies as used in

Fast ForWord-

Language.

Untrained: did not

receive computerized

training, other

interventions unknown

20 x15 min sessions on

consecutive school days

No difference on any

measure (TROG-2;

ERRNI and a variety of

speech and non-speech

auditory processing

measures) between

three groups

Effect sizes in slow

speech: TROG-2 .77;

ERRNI comprehension

-.17; and ERRNI MLU

-.84

Effect sizes in modified

speech: TROG-2 .37;

ERRNI comp -.03; and

ERRNI MLU -.44

Effect sizes in

untrained: TROG-2 .53;

ERRNI comp -.74; and

ERRNI MLU -.68

139

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Blischak, Shah,

Lombardino &

Chiarella

(2004)

Multiple

baseline

single

subject

design

3 children aged

5;6-7;6 with

severe speech

impairment with

concomitant

language

disorder

? ? SLP Phonology Treatment targets:

phoneme-grapheme

correspondence,

phoneme segmentation,

phoneme manipulation,

pseudo word spelling.

Therapy methods:

Phoneme-grapheme

instruction and

phonemic awareness

instruction

Both methods increased

the encoding skills of 2

of 3 participants and

generalized these skills

to untrained pseudo-and

real words.

Effect sizes (PND for

either treatment over

baseline).

Phoneme manipulation

moderate PND 85% of

probes in instruction

phase were above

highest baseline level

Encoding: large PND

98% of probes in

instruction phase were

above the highest

baseline level

140

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Cohen, Hodson,

O‟Hare, Boyle,

Durrani,

McCartney et al

(2005)

RCT 60 children aged

6-10 years with

mixed (receptive/

expressive)

language

impairment

School Group and

individual

therapy?

Computer

plus school

clinicians

Receptive-

expressive

language

FFW-L: discrimination

of tones, phonemes,

syllables and words;

memory for commands;

comprehension of

grammatical

morphology and

complex sentences.

Other computer games:

listening, spelling,

phonological

awareness, reading,

writing, vocabulary,

problems solving,

narration, syntax, and

morphology

Control: school therapy

services only

Similar gains on the

clinical evaluation of

language fundamentals

3rd

edition for all groups

at 9 week and 6 month

f/up periods.

Computer intervention

plus school therapy was

no more effective than

school therapy alone.

Effect sizes at 9 weeks:

d=-.09 (FFW-v control)

no effect; d=.27 (FFW v

computer) – small

Effect sizes at 6

months: d=.05 (FFW v

control) no effect; d=-

0.27 (FFW v computer)

small

141

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Connell &

Stone 1992

Split plot

factorial

32 SLI (-1 SD on

either MLU or

normed language

test), 24 age

matched and 20

language-

matched

controls, aged

5;0 -6;11.

Schools and

clinics

Individual

therapy?

Investigators? Morpheme

(modeling

and

imitation)

Participants taught set

of Investigator-

designed invented

morphemes by

computerized language

program

Modeling: Participants

heard morpheme being

used in meaningful way

but was not asked to

repeat or use the

morpheme in any way

Imitation: same as

modeling but

participants instructed

to give a direction to a

computer cartoon that

contains the target

morpheme.

4 computerised

language sessions over

2 weeks.

Modeling alone did not

significantly increase

morpheme use;

imitation increased

morpheme production.

Both modeling and

imitation appeared

equally effective for

increasing performance

on comprehensive

probes.

The treatment strategy

administered first

overrode any

differential effect within

the counter-balanced

design)

Effect sizes in modeling

condition for: a)

production probes .78

(moderate); and

comprehension probes

1.32 (large)

Effect sizes in imitation

condition for:

production probes .50

(moderate); and

comprehension probes

1.30 (large)

142

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Crowe (2003) Non-

randomised

controlled

trial

12 participants

aged 8-11 years,

classified as

having language

learning

disabilities

(scores of at least

-1SD on normed

test of language,

defined as „poor

readers‟, scoring

below 50th

percentile on

scored test of

reading

achievement

School Group therapy Investigator Oral

language

performance

(traditional

reading

decoding

strategies v

communica-

tive reading

strategies)

Investigator designed

traditional reading

decoding strategies or

meaning-based

communicative reading

strategies (CRS) to aid

oral language

performance

T1 Traditional

decoding feedback i.e.

reads passage, sounds

out words, answers

questions and retells

what was read.

T2 CRS: interactive

conversational style i.e.

reads small portions of

text, asking and

answering questions

during and after

reading, commenting,

summarizing, reacting

to story events and

retelling.

12 x1hr sessions for 6

weeks

Control: no treatment

143

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Gillam, Loeb,

Hoffman,

Bohman,

Champlin,

Thibodeau et al

(in press)

RCT 216 children

aged 6-9 years

with specific

language

impairment

School Individual

therapy?

Computer or

SLP

Language

impairment

FFW-L: discrimination

of tones, phonemes,

syllables, and words;

memory for commands;

comprehension of

grammatical

morphology and

complex sentences.

Computer assisted

language intervention

(CALI): discrimination

of tones, phonemes,

syllables and words;

memory for commands;

comprehension of

grammatical

morphology and

complex sentences

Individual Language

Intervention (ILI):

vocabulary,

grammatical

morphology, complex

syntax, narration,

phonological awareness

Academic enrichment

(AE): listening,

mathematical

calculation, non-verbal

problem solving,

science, geography

All groups improved

significantly on a global

language test, a test of

backward masking, and

a test of language

comprehension.

FFW-L and CALI

groups made

significantly more

progress on

phonological awareness

test than other two

groups.

Effect sizes on the

CASL at 9 weeks:

d=.56 for CALI

(moderate); d=.71 for

FFW-L (moderate);

d=.79 for ILI

(moderate); d=.61 for

AE (moderate)

Effect sizes on the

CASL at 6 months: d

=1.02 for CALI (large);

d =.93 for FFW-L

(large); d = -1.33 for

ILI (large): and d = -

1.34 for AE (large)

144

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Gilliam,

Crofford, Gale

& Hoffman

(2001)

Multiple-

baseline

single-

subject

design

4 children aged

6-8 years with

SLI

? Individual

therapy?

Research

assistants

? FFW-L: discrimination

of tones, phonemes,

syllables and words;

memory for commands;

comprehension of

grammatical

morphology and

complex sentences

Laureate Learning

software (LLS):

comprehension and

memory of words,

grammatical

morphemes, sentences

and stories.

All children made

clinically significant

gains on the Oral and

Written Language

scales.

2/2 LLS and 1/2 FFW-L

made clinically

significant gains on

MLU computed form

language samples.

Effect sizes (PND for

MLU in f/up v

baseline): LLS –

immediate 100%; LLS-

delayed 100%; FFW –

immediate 0%; and

FFW – delayed 100%.

145

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Gillon (2002)

[follow-up

Gillon 2000]

Non-

randomised

(matched)

assignment

to treatment

and control

groups

compared

with cohort

of typically

achieving

children

20 of the original

23 SLI and all of

the normal

controls. Aged

5;6-7;6 with

speech and

language

disorder

Clinic and

school

? SLP and

graduate

students in

SLP

Phonology Experimental targets:

identification if

phonological

similarities, phoneme

manipulation, sounds

identification, phoneme

segmentation,

graphemephoneme

correspondence,

phoneme production

Traditional control

targets: phoneme

production in isolation,

syllables, words and

phrases.

1. Experimental

intervention: Gillon

phonological awareness

training

2. Traditional control:

Van Riper speech

therapy

3. Minimal intervention

4. Classroom

consultation: normal

comparison

Tested 11 months later

on phoneme awareness,

reading and spelling.

Treatment led to

sustained growth in

phoneme awareness and

word recognition.

The majority were

reading at or above age-

level expectations and

improved on nonword

spelling.

Effect sizes for word

recognition: d=2.42

(large)(SLI pre-to f/up);

d = 1.52 (large)

(control, pre-to f/up); d

=0.64 (moderate) (SLI v

control f/up)

146

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Gillon (2006) Non-

randomised

(matched)

assignment

to treatment

and control

groups

compared

with cohort

of typically

achieving

children

91 children aged

5;6 – 7;6 with

speech and

language

disorders

Clinic and

school

? SLP and

graduate

students in

SLP

Phonology Experimental targets:

identification if

phonological

similarities, phoneme

manipulation, sounds

identification, phoneme

segmentation,

graphemephoneme

correspondence,

phoneme production

Traditional control

targets: phoneme

production in isolation,

syllables, words and

phrases.

1. Experimental

intervention: Gillon

phonological awareness

training

2. Traditional control:

Van Riper speech

therapy

3. Minimal intervention

4. Classroom

consultation: normal

comparison

Group 1 greater

improvement on tests of

phonological awareness

and reading than Group

2 and 3.

At end of study,

phonological awareness

of Group 1 similar to

normal controls.

Effect sizes Group 1 v

Group 2: phoneme

awareness 2.58 (large);

rhyming 0.48; reading

accuracy 0.59; reading

comprehension 0.67

(moderate)

Effect sizes Group 1 v

Group 3: phoneme

awareness 1.77 (large);

rhyming 0.67

(moderate); reading

accuracy 0.34; reading

comprehension 0.61.

147

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Masterton &

Perry (1999)

Non-

randomised

comparison

12 children

between 9-14

years

(mean=11;11)

with SLI. 6

treatment; 6

control.

Pull-out

school setting

Small group

therapy

Investigator Analogical

reasoning

(mediated v

bridging

learning)

Investigator designed

mediated learning and

bridging strategies to

aid analogical

reasoning via a set of

verbal analogies from

five categories

Mediated learning:

included direct

instruction in

component processes of

analogical thinking (8

sessions in 2 weeks)

Bridging: included

exercises designed to

help participants

incorporate the

component processes of

analogical thinking into

everyday activities (8

sessions in 2 weeks)

148

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

McGregor &

Leonard (1989)

Non-

randomised

comparison

4 aged 9;1-10;5

diagnosed with

SLI (more than –

SD on at least

two normed tests

of language and

word finding)

School Individual

therapy?

Investigator? Word

finding

(word

finding

focus v

narrative

and syntax)

2 participated in 6

weeks of language

therapy with word-

finding focus, other 2

focused on narrative

and syntax (controls)

Investigator designed

elaboration and

retrieval strategies to

aid word retrieval via

asset of 120 nouns

concrete nouns

represented by pictures

Elaboration: clinician

presents picture/words

that rhyme with the

target word (phonemic)

and picture/words that

are similar to and

different than the target

word (semantic).

Retrieval strategies:

clinician presents

retrieved cues related to

target words

12 x 1 hour sessions

with both elaboration

and retrieval strategies

Naming and recall

tasks, treatment group

improved while control

group made no or small

gains

Performance on

maintenance tasks was

sometime poorer for

both groups post-test

Effect sizes not

computed.

Merzenich,

Jenkins,

Johnston,

Schreiner,

Miller & Tallal

(1996)

Non-

randomised

(matched)

comparison

22 children aged

5;2-10;0 with

mixed (receptive

and expressive)

language

impairment

? Individual

therapy?

Computer

games

Temporal

processing

Phonology

Experimental group

received FFW-L

games: circus sequence

and phoneme

identification

Experimental group

improved on the Tallal

Repetition Test (a

measure of auditory

temporal processing)

149

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Seger &

Voerhoeven

(2005)

Non-

randomised

(matched)

comparison

36 children aged

4;6-6;11 in

kindergarten

with SLI.

School Individual? Computer

games

Phonology Group 1: 10 rhyming

and sound synthesis

computer games with

normal speech

Group 2: 10 rhyming

and sound synthesis

computer games with

modified speech

(slower and amplified

formant transitions)

Control group:

vocabulary computer

games

14 x 15 mins over 5

weeks

Group 1 had positive

treatment results when

the phonological

awareness task results

were combined into

difference z scores.

These were no longer

significant 18 weeks

after intervention.

Effect size (group 1 v

control) d=0.29 (small)

Tallal, Miller,

Bedi, Byma,

Wang,

Nagarajan et al

(1996)

Non-

randomised

(matched)

comparison

22 children aged

5;6-10;0 with

mixed (receptive

and expressive)

language

impairments

? Individual

therapy?

Computer

games and

trained

clinician

Language

comprehen-

dsion

Treatment group: early

version of FFW-L

games (block

commander, phonic

match, phonic word,

and language

comprehension with

modified speech

stimuli)

Control: computer

versions of the same

tasks without modified

speech stimuli

Experimental group

showed significantly

larger improvements in

speech discrimination,

language processing

and grammatical

comprehension

measures

150

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Throneburg,

Calvert, Sturm,

Paramboukas &

Paul (2000)

Non-

randomised

controlled

trial

32 children in

grades K-3, 13

eligible for

speech services,

19 eligible for

language

services (criteria

for placement

was a score of -1

SD or greater on

two normed tests

of language or

one normed test

of articulation)

School Group and

individual

therapy

SLP,

classroom

teachers,

graduate

students

Vocabulary

skills

Collaborative: SLP and

classroom teacher

collaboratively planned

and implemented

activities to target

curriculum vocabulary

words in the classroom

(large group vocabulary

instruction and hands-

on activities for topic

units with embedded

vocabulary words)

Class-room based:

teacher and SLP

independently planned

and implemented

vocabulary activities

similar to above; SLP

provided independent

classroom lesson

Traditional pull-out:

SLP provided

vocabulary instruction

students in traditional

pull-out sessions (50

min /week), using same

materials as above.

Collaborative students

had higher scores on

curricular vocabulary

tests than other two

groups.

All three groups were

effective for teaching

vocabulary

Effect sizes

collaborative: d =2.5

(large); class-room

based: d=3.5 (large);

traditional pull-out:

d=1.2 (large)

151

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Van Kleeck,

Gillam &

McFadden

(1998)

Non-

randomised

comparison

24 with speech

and/or language

disorder pre-

school mean age

= 48 months,

pre-kindergarter

mean age = 60

months

Pre-school

and pre-

kindergarten

Group therapy? SLP and

graduate

students in

speech-

language

pathology

Phonology

(rhyming

and

phoneme

awareness)

Children received

rhyming instruction

during the fall semester

and phoneme

awareness instruction in

spring semester

Rhyming: rhyme

identification, rhyme

judgment and rhyme

generation

Phoneme awareness:

matching and

identifying initial

sounds, generating

words, phoneme

blending and phoneme

segmentation

Control group of older

children who had

attended the same

classroom

Both treatment groups

made significant

improvement in

rhyming and phoneme

awareness.

Gains in rhyming fell

below the lower

boundary of the 95%CI

of control group,

suggesting that the

development of

rhyming was not

dependent on treatment

Gains in phoneme

awareness were above

the upper limits of the

control group‟s CI

suggesting that training

contributed to

improvements in

phonemic awareness

The treatment groups

performed better on the

phonological awareness

tasks compare to control

group

Effect sizes: d

(preschool v control) =

1.58 (large) and d (pre-

K v control) = 1.76

(large)

152

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Weismer &

Hesketh (1993)

Non-

randomised

comparison

16 children (8

SLI and 8 normal

language), aged

5;1-6;7

kindergarten

Kindergarten Group therapy ? Investigator? Novel word

acquisition

(Prosodic

and gestural

cues)

Meaning of a set of 9

investigator designed

novel words

representing either

object labels or

locatives

Investigators varied the

presentation of target

words in 3

experimental treatments

Rate: stimulus

sentences with target

words presented at 3

rates (slow, medium,

fast)

Stress: stimulus

sentences with target

words presented with

and without emphatic

stress

Visual: stimulus

sentences with target

words presented

verbally or verbally

with an accompanying

iconic gesture

For both groups

acquisition of novel

words was affected by

alterations of speaking

rate and by the use of

gestures, but not by

stress manipulation

Slower rate helped SLI

performance as did the

addition of gesture

Effect sizes for SLI

students for

comprehension probes:

Slow-fast rate 1.1

(large)

Emphatic-neutral stress

0.12 (no effect)

Gesture-no gesture for

0.57 (moderate)

Effect sizes for SLI

students for production

probes:

Slow-fast rate 1.1

(large)

Emphatic-neutral stress

0.74 (moderate)

Gesture –no gesture

0.33 (small)

153

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Weismer &

Murray-Branch

1989

Single

subject

design

(alternating

treatment

design)

4 children aged

5;5-6;11 with

specific language

impairment

Public school,

university

clinic

Individual

therapy?

„Investigator‟ Metalinguis

-tics

(modeling

+/- evoked

production)

Each participant had

different grammatical

target

Modeling involved

focused models of

target form provided by

clinician; i.e. story-

telling, art construction

activities, puppet play

Modeling + evoked

production (MEP)

involved focused

models of target form

with opportunities to

produce form a receive

feedback (structured to

evoke spontaneous

productions of target)

No marked difference in

outcomes (no of correct

productions and total

attempts at target form)

between modeling and

MEP for any participant

Effect sizes (% of non-

overlapping data for

either treatment over

baseline) ranged from

0.20 (ineffective) to

1.00 (very high) for the

4 participants

154

Author/Year

of study

Study

Type

Participants Setting Service

delivery

model

Service

provider

Language

domain/

therapy

type

Method of

intervention

Summary of

outcomes

Wing (1990) Non-

randomised

comparison

10 children age

range 71-85

months mean=77

months (first

grade) identified

with severe

language

impairment

School Group therapy SLP Word

finding

(Semantic

and

phonologic-

al)

Investigator designed

semantic or

phonological strategies

to aid word retrieval via

set of vocabulary items

similar to but not

duplicating items on the

Test of word Finding

Semantic treatment:

activities to improve

elaboration and

organization of

semantic storage

Phonological treatment:

activities for

phonological

segmentation and

activities for auditory

imagery

30x25 mins over 2.5

months