Enaluation of Peer Influence

download Enaluation of Peer Influence

of 22

Transcript of Enaluation of Peer Influence

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    1/22

    CHILD

    SnUDYJOURNAL Volkme

    33/No.

    4/2003

    CHILDREN'S

    EVALUATIONS

    OF

    PEER

    INFLUENCE:

    THE

    ROLE

    O RELATIONSHIP TYPE

      ND

    SOCIAL SITUATION

    Brea A. Burton, Glen E. Ray Sheila Mehta

    Auburn

    University

    Montgomery

    Second-, third-,

    fifth-, and sixth-grade children evaluated

    peers portrayed

    as

    either best

    friends or acquaintances

    engaged

    in

    ive different

    peer

    pressure sce-

    narios (e.g., modeling,

    positive

    reinforcement, negative reinforcement,

    expert,

    referent).

    Evaluations

    were assessed in terms of

    the

    target

    peer's behavior

    and

    affect,

    and

    the

    evaluator s

    behavior

    and

    affect.

    Results

    demonstrated that

    older

    children evaluated the

    responder

    (peer

    being

    influenced)

    as

    being more suscepti-

    ble to peer pressure

    (i.e.,

    cheating)

    than did

    younger

    children.

    Older

    children

    also reported

    higher incidences

    of cheating,

    being less upset

    evaluating

    the vari-

    ous

    peer

    pressure situations, and being more likely to be influenced themselves,

    than did younger children.

    Gender

    differences emerged

    with girls being

    more

    upset

    than

    boys evaluating

    the

    various

    peer pressure

    situations. Further, all

    girls

    and younger

    boys

    evaluated

    the responder as being more upset

    in he best friend

    relationship

    with the initiator

    than in

    the

    acquaintance

    relationship with the

    ini-

    tiator.

    All

    children

    evaluated

    a

    peer's

    influence to

    be strongest in

    the referent

    peer

    influence

    scenario.

    Findings

    are

    discussed

    in

    terms of extending previous

    research on negative peer influence and the need

    for

    consideration

    of

    context

    ef-

    fects.

    Research convincingly demonstrates that children's peer relationships are

    important

    to social,

    cognitive,

    and

    physical development (e.g.,

    Newcomb, Bu-

    kowski, Hartup, 1996). However,

    it has also

    become clear that

    not

    all peer

    in-

    teractions and influence is positive. For example, children influence

    each

    other to

    engage

    in

    drug abuse, vandalism,

    and various

    other negative

    activities. In

    fact,

    some researchers

    (e.g.,

    Brofenbrenner,

    1970)

    report peer

    influence to be

    largely

    negative, particularly

    in the absence

    of

    adult supervision

    (Steinburg,

    1986). Fur-

    thermore, theories of

    deviance

    and delinquency have placed considerable empha-

    sis on the negative influence

    of

    peers (e.g., Akers, 1977; Elliot, Huizinga, Age-

    ton, 1985; Miller, 1958).

    Although

    researchers (e.g.,

    Brofenbrenner,

    1970; Savin-

    Williams Bemdt,

    1990) have investigated

    various

    behaviors and

    situations

    that

    lead

    to

    negative

    outcomes, little

    research has

    addressed children's social-

    informational processing (e.g., evaluations)

    of peer

    pressure situations. The

    pur-

    pose of

    the

    present

    study was to investigate possible influences on children's

    evaluations

    of

    peer

    pressure

    situations.

    Of

    particular

    interest

    were influences

    of

    different types

    of

    peer relationships (e.g., best friends, acquaintances)

    and

    different

    Page 35

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    2/22

    Burton RVy Mebta

    Importance

    of Evaluations

    As

    described

    in

    Ray,

    Graham,

    and Cohen

    (2001), children s

    evaluations

    of

    peer

    social behavior

    is developmentally

    significant

    for

    both

    the

    evaluator

    and

    for

    the target

    peer(s)

    being

    evaluated.

    Implications

    for the

    evaluator are twofold.

    First,

    children

    are constantly

    observing

    and evaluating

    the

    behaviors

    of others on

    a daily

    basis and subsequently

    using this information

    when

    determining

    their ow n

    potential

    relationship

    choices. For

    example,

    a child

    seen initiating

    or engaging

    in

    peer conflict

    situations in

    the classroom or

    on the

    playground

    will likely be

    avoided

    by the

    evaluator while

    a child whose

    interactions

    are prosocial

    and rela-

    tively conflict-free

    will

    be

    sought out

    as

    a potential friend. As

    a second implica-

    tion for the evaluator,

    peers

    influence each

    other not

    only

    through

    direct ex-

    changes and

    reinforcement, but

    also

    through modeling

    of others

    engaged

    in

    pro-

    social

    and conflict situations,

    facilitating

    the

    observer s

    ability

    to interact

    in

    socially

    competent

    ways with

    peers.

    Implications

    for

    the

    target peer(s)

    being

    evaluated

    center

    on issues of

    reputa-

    tion or social

    history. Models

    of

    children s

    social-information

    processing

    (see

    Crick

    Dodge, 1994,

    for review)

    assume

    that

    children

    represent

    social

    informa-

    tion

    about other peers

    in a knowledge

    base and

    this in turn,

    influences current

    and

    subsequent

    interactions

    by the

    child.

    That

    is,

    how a child

    evaluates

    a particular

    social situation

    will determine,

    in part,

    how that

    child will

    respond. Therefore,

    whether

    the child

    is

    directly interacting

    with peers or is an

    observer of

    other

    chil-

    dren s

    interactions,

    social

    information

    is

    being encoded and

    stored to

    be used

    in

    future

    interactions

    involving

    those

    target peers.

    Thus,

    examining

    factors

    that

    in-

    fluence

    children s evaluations

    of

    social information

    will prove

    useful to researchers

    investigating

    causal

    linkages

    between

    social

    cognition

    and social behavior.

    Negative

    Influences

    ofPeers

    While

    childhood is

    a time where

    fundamental

    skills of

    social

    competency

    are

    learned,

    researchers (e.g.,

    Parker

    Asher,

    1987)

    report

    that

    children

    also

    learn

    roguish, inappropriate

    behaviors,

    such

    as stealing

    and

    vandalizing from their

    peers.

    Findings suggest

    that

    many deviant

    activities need

    the context

    of

    a peer

    group

    for their

    initiation as well

    as

    their maintenance. That

    is,

    children

    usually

    do

    not behave

    mischievously

    on their own.

    Many

    of the pranks

    and

    disobedient

    be-

    haviors

    children

    engage in are

    committed

    in

    parties of two

    or

    more

    (Sutherland

    Cressey,

    1970).

    Kandel

    (1978) reported

    that children who

    are friends

    consistently have

    similar

    attitudes

    and engage

    in

    similar behaviors.

    For

    example,

    if one s friend

    uses

    drugs,

    they will be

    more likely

    to engage in

    illegal drug use

    themselves.

    While

    drug use

    may be

    one kind

    of

    negative

    behavior

    friends

    facilitate

    in

    each

    other,

    there are

    numerous

    others

    such

    as alcohol, cigarette

    smoking,

    premarital

    sex, academic

    Page

    236

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    3/22

    CHILD TUDYJOURNAL/IVolume 33/No. 4/2003

    argue that

    friends

    influence is a

    major factor in

    adolescence s alcohol

    use,

    drug

    use, and many other

    delinquent behaviors. Further, research demonstrates clear

    evidence of the influence

    of

    intimate friends and friendship

    groups on sexual

    be-

    havior,

    the tendency towards

    risky

    driving

    (e.g.,

    Millstein,

    Petersen, Nightingale,

    1993), as

    well

    as the use

    of

    cigarettes,

    alcohol and marijuana (e.g., Fisher

    Bauman, 1988; Kandel,

    1978).

    Peer influence

    is

    a common

    source

    for the involvement in negaive activities

    for children

    (Berndt,

    1996; Kandel, 1978).

    Interestingly, Duck

    (1996)

    demon-

    strated that children

    do

    acknowledge the negative qualides

    of

    their friends. For

    example,

    children

    report that their friends sometimes annoy them and boss them

    around.

    These

    behaviors are often the source of conflicts between friends

    (Hartup,

    1992).

    Researchers

    (e.g.,

    Berndt Savin-Williams,

    1993; Youniss, 1980)

    have

    also

    demonstrated

    that

    friends

    often

    engage

    in

    rivalry

    and

    competion

    As

    children grow

    into

    adolescence they begin noticing more of their peers personal-

    ity and negative characteristics.

    As

    intimacy

    and

    companionship

    increases, chil-

    dren develop

    an increasingly sophisticated view of their peer s

    personalities,

    moods, wishes,

    desires, motivations, and

    intentions.

    In turn, children

    use

    this

    information to manipulate each other

    (Duck,

    1996). While friends can become

    close, best

    friends have

    more

    intimate knowledge

    about each other (Berndt

    Keefe, 1994; Kandel, 1978;

    Cohen,

    1983)

    and,

    thus, potentially more power with

    which to manipulate each other.

    Prior

    research

    (Duck,

    1996;

    Hartup,

    1992) has

    concluded

    that

    simple

    peer

    pressure

    is

    not

    the

    primary

    means

    by

    which

    friends

    influence each other. That is, depending on the gender and the type

    of relation-

    ship involved, a boy or girl

    may

    use different peer

    pressure strategies (e.g., positive

    reinforcement,

    expert power).

    Sherif and Sherif (1964) believe

    there

    is

    a mis-

    guided

    assumption that peer pressure is always direct and overt

    and suggest,

    how-

    ever, that

    peer

    influence

    often

    operates in a

    much softer

    fashion.

    Indeed,

    research

    demonstrates

    that more socially skillful children learn to use gentler sorts

    of pres-

    sures

    in

    close

    relaionships to increase their desired outcome (Hartup,

    1992).

    Tvpes

    of

    PeerInfluence

    Berndt (1996)

    suggested that

    understanding peer

    influence

    could be enhanced

    by

    linking

    children s friendship research with social-psychological

    theories of in-

    terpersonal

    influence

    such as French and Raven s 1959 classic work on

    social

    power.

    To better understand

    the processes involved in negative

    peer

    influence,

    researchers need

    to investigate the ways (how)

    in which

    peers manipulate each

    other. Towards this end, the current

    study had children

    evaluate

    five different

    types of peer

    influence modeled after

    French and

    Raven s

    (1959)

    typology of

    so-

    cial power.

    The

    first type

    of

    influence

    is

    reinforcement

    or

    reward. Reward refers

    to

    any-

    thing

    that

    promotes a behavior being repeated in

    the

    future.

    Among friends,

    re-

    Pa e237

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    4/22

    Bturton

    Ray

    Mebta

    encourage

    peers to start

    drinking (Graham

    et al., 1991)

    or to become

    involved in

    other

    negative behaviors.

    Whether

    a child's

    own behavior

    was reinforced or a

    child witnessed

    another

    child's behavior

    being

    reinforced

    (modeling),

    it becomes

    highly

    influential in

    changing behavior.

    More

    directly,

    friends

    sometimes offer

    rewards

    to others. For example,

    a friend

    might say, Lets

    go to

    the

    movies.

    I'll

    pay

    for the gas.

    Another

    common

    reinforcement used

    among children

    is com-

    panionship.

    A friend

    might say,

    Let's go to

    the mal first

    and

    then

    we can

    go eat

    like

    you

    want to.

    Reinforcement

    is equally seen

    in

    antisocial

    situations. A

    friend

    might

    say,

    If you smoke

    with us,

    we

    can l

    be

    friends

    (Berndt

    Savin-

    WiDiams, 1993).

    Another

    mechanism

    utilized by peers

    to

    influence

    each

    other

    is

    negative

    rein-

    forcement

    (Kopfstein,

    1972). Negative

    reinforcement

    is increasing

    the likelihood

    of

    a

    behavioral occurrence

    because

    the behavior

    keeps

    something

    negative

    from

    occurring.

    Thus,

    we engage

    in a behavior

    to

    keep negative

    consequences from

    happening.

    For example,

    a

    friend

    might

    say,

    If you do

    not

    sneak

    out

    with us,

    you

    can no

    longer

    be

    part

    of our club.

    Studies (e.g.,

    Berndt Savin-Wiliams,

    1993; Cohen,

    1983)

    have revealed

    similar findings

    concluding that by associating

    with

    deviant

    peers, an

    individual

    may

    themselves

    engage in deviant

    activities

    be-

    cause their peers have

    facilitated

    and reinforced

    beliefs

    in them

    that

    delinquent

    activity

    is not wrong.

    It

    is

    as if peers

    become

    desensitized

    to

    the deviant behavior

    altogether

    due to

    their friends'

    reinforcement of negative

    behavior.

    A

    third

    way

    friends

    influence

    each

    other

    is

    through

    modeling.

    It

    involves the

    imitating

    of one

    person's behavior

    to another

    person's behavior

    as

    a

    consequence

    of direct or symbolic

    observation.

    Bandura

    (1977) demonstrated that

    children will

    actualy

    be

    influenced

    to

    engage

    in

    particular behaviors simply

    by observing

    an-

    other

    child's

    behavior

    and the consequences

    that

    follow.

    That

    is, children are

    likely to

    engage

    in

    behaviors

    they

    see

    being

    reinforced

    (vicarious reinforcement)

    in

    others

    and are

    likely not to engage

    in behaviors they

    see not

    being

    reinforced

    or

    being punished (vicarious

    punishment)

    in others.

    Interestingly,

    research

    has dem-

    onstrated that

    the modeling

    and

    reinforcement

    required to produce

    antisocial

    be-

    haviors

    do not come

    from

    the family unit, but from

    an individual's

    peers (Eliot

    et

    al., 1985).

    Further,

    a child may

    model

    anbther's

    behavior

    because

    they

    admire and

    want

    to

    be like

    them (Berndt,

    1996). This

    fourth

    type

    of social power

    is

    caled

    referent

    power. O'Connor

    (1969)

    studied

    withdrawn

    children

    by

    showing

    them a series of

    videos

    of

    socialy

    competent peers interacting.

    After

    viewing

    the videos,

    with-

    drawn children

    became

    more sociable

    in their interactions

    with

    other children.

    Previous

    studies

    (e.g., Epstein,

    1983; Sherif Sherif, 1964)

    likewise

    concluded

    that

    children often try

    to become

    friends

    with

    other

    children they perceive

    as

    popular,

    athletic,

    or outstanding in

    some

    way. After

    these friendships

    are formed,

    Page

    238

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    5/22

    CHILD STUDYJOURNALI

    Volume

    33/No.

    4/2003

    may

    occur

    through

    passive

    observations

    of

    one

    another

    in

    conversations.

    A child

    may say, I think athletes are cool and in

    return others may begin to share

    similar

    views.

    The

    present

    study

    also investigated

    the

    influence

    of

    expert

    social

    power.

    Here, it

    is

    believed

    that

    competent

    individuals generaly exert

    greater influence

    than

    those not competent.

    It is the power a person

    with special knowledge uses

    to influence others.

    While

    one

    friend may be

    better

    at soccer,

    another

    may be

    better

    in their scholastic

    abilites. When

    a

    decision

    concerning a friend's area

    of

    expertise appears,

    their ideas

    may be more highly

    valued and in turn,

    more influ-

    ential

    than

    the advice of

    a

    non-expert

    friend.

    An

    analysis

    of

    the how peer influence

    occurs

    must

    also

    include

    the

    type

    of

    relationship that exists

    between those

    involved.

    That is,

    the orientaion of

    the

    source

    (e.g.,

    best

    friend, friend, acquaintance, enemy),

    rather than

    the

    content

    of

    the behavior,

    has been shown to be

    influential

    in changing

    behavior (McDavid,

    1959)

    and influential in evaluating behavior (see Ray, et

    al., 2001 for

    review).

    It

    appears that peer influence

    has different

    effects

    in

    different

    dyadic

    relationships.

    While there is significant variability

    across

    different types

    of relationships, best

    friend relationships may have a unique

    impact on

    development apart from other

    relationships

    in

    the

    child's

    broader social network. For

    example, if influence de-

    rives

    from

    the

    need to please

    or be like

    a close friend,

    one

    could infer that

    best or

    inimate friends would

    be more influential

    than

    general

    friendship

    groups. Several

    studies

    have

    revealed

    such

    evidence

    (Berndt

    Keefe,

    1994;

    Cohen,

    1983;

    Kandel,

    1978, Morgan

    Grube,

    1991; Mounts Steinberg,

    1995).

    Influence in a best

    friend relationship can be

    a mutual process. Each

    child

    influences

    his or her

    friends

    and is, in turn, influenced by them. A close, supported, collaborative

    rela-

    tionship with a

    best

    friend provides

    a degree

    of

    intimacy and mutual

    engagement

    that is not

    found

    in any

    other

    relationships. Hallinan

    (1983)

    also

    found a friend's

    influence may

    be strongest based

    on

    mutual

    trust

    and shared goals. As a result, a

    best

    friend may

    have

    the most

    influence

    on a child's

    behavior and

    attitudes

    (Mor-

    gan

    Grube, 1991;

    Mounts

    Steinberg,

    1995; Sullivan, 1953).

    The Present Study

    In

    this

    study we examined

    children's evaluations of

    peers as

    a function of re-

    lationship

    type

    (best friends, acquaintances) and peer pressure

    scenario

    type (e.g.,

    modeling, reinforcement).

    One

    peer, named

    Patwas

    the

    peer

    being

    influenced

    and

    was

    also

    labeled the responder The other

    peer, named

    Chris

    was always at-

    tempting to influence Pat and was

    thus

    also labeled the initiator.

    Given previous

    research

    (e.g.,

    Berndt,

    1996)

    demonstrating

    that children's

    susceptibility to peer

    influence increases

    with age,

    we

    hypothesized

    that

    older

    children (grades

    5-6),

    would

    evaluate

    the

    responder

    as

    being

    more

    likely

    to

    com-

    ply,

    and

    would also

    evaluate

    the emotional impact on the responder to

    be less

    PageW3

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    6/22

    Bunton,

    RVy Mehta

    predicted that

    the closer

    the relationship

    between

    the two

    peers (best

    friends, neu-

    tral

    acquaintances)

    the more influential,

    they

    would

    be

    on

    each other.

    Thus,

    we

    predicted

    that children

    would evaluate

    best friends

    as being more

    influential

    than

    acquaintances

    in terms

    of

    the

    responder's

    cheating behavior.

    Similarly,

    it

    was

    pre-

    dicted

    that the

    evaluator would

    report being

    more

    likely

    to comply

    when

    evaluat-

    ing the

    best

    friend

    relationship

    than

    when evaluating

    the acquaintance

    relation-

    ship.

    A great

    deal of

    research

    documents

    the importance

    of gender

    in children's

    social development.

    Researchers

    have

    noted sex differences

    in friendship

    network

    size

    (Ray Cohen,

    1992),

    their

    activities

    (Huston,

    1985),

    their

    linguistic

    styles

    (Maltz Borker,

    1983), and

    their

    play

    style preferences

    (Maccoby,

    1988,

    1990).

    Relevant

    to the current

    study, Matthys,

    Cohen-Kettenis

    and

    Berkhout,

    (1993)

    reported that

    girls'

    evaluations

    of

    peers

    were

    more

    context

    dependent

    (embedded

    in background

    information, family,

    and

    kinship) than

    were boys' evaluations.

    Therefore,

    we

    predicted

    that

    relationship

    type

    would have a greater

    impact on

    girls' evaluations.

    That

    is, girls

    were

    hypothesized

    to

    make finer

    distinctions

    among relationship

    types than

    were boys.

    Due

    to the gender differences

    in

    boys'

    and girls'

    peer relationships,

    we hypothesized

    that

    girls'

    relationships

    would

    be

    more influential in terms

    of compliance

    than would boys'

    relationships

    (Billy

    Udry,

    1985;

    Davies Kandel,

    1981).

    Predictions

    were also made concerning

    the

    five peer influence scenarios

    (i.e.,

    modeling,

    positive reinforcement,

    negative

    reinforcement,

    referent,

    expert).

    Spe-

    cifically,

    it

    was

    predicted

    that younger

    children would

    be more influenced

    in terms

    of evaluations

    of compliance

    by either

    positive

    or

    negative

    reinforcement

    and

    older children

    would

    be more influenced

    by

    referent,

    expert, or

    modeling, because

    as

    we age, friends

    and status become

    more important

    (Berndt,

    1979).

    Lastly, Berndt

    (1996)

    speculated

    that referent

    power may

    be

    the primary

    means

    through

    which friends

    influence each

    other.

    Thus, we predicted

    that

    chil-

    dren

    would

    evaluate

    a best

    friend as

    being

    more

    influential

    in terms of

    compli-

    ance,

    in the modeling

    and

    referent influence

    scenario

    than

    would an acquaintance.

    Method

    Paffidpants

    Participants

    were 78 girls

    and

    82 boys

    from second,

    third, fifth,

    and sixth

    grades

    N=1 60) of

    a public elementary

    school

    in Montgomery,

    Alabama.

    All

    par-

    ticipating

    children

    returned

    written parental

    consent

    and

    also

    gave their

    own writ-

    ten

    assent

    prior

    to participating

    in the

    study. Although

    ethnicity of

    participants

    was

    not

    a

    variable

    of

    interest in

    the

    current

    study,

    the

    racial

    composition

    of

    the

    sample was

    45

    African American

    and 55 European

    American.

    Participants

    Page 4

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    7/22

    ClHLDSTUDYJOURNAL/IVolume 33/No. 4/2003

    Design

    In

    addition to

    Grade

    and Gender, Relationship

    Type

    (Best

    Friend,

    Acquaint-

    ance) was a between-participant yariable.

    The

    study

    also

    included Influence

    Type

    (Modeling,

    Referent, Negative Reinforcement, Positive Reinforcement,

    Expert)

    as

    a within participants variable. Thus,

    the

    general

    design was

    a 2

    (Grade: young,

    old) x 2 (Gender of child) x 2 (Relationship Type: best

    friend,

    acquaintance) x

    5

    (Influence Type: Modeling, Positive Reinforcement, Negative Reinforcement,

    Referent, and Expert) mixed-factorial design.

    Measures

    Each

    child

    listened to a

    total of

    six

    audiorecorded

    scenarios:

    one for

    a par-

    ticular

    level of

    Relationship Type (e.g., Best Friend, Acquaintance) and five deliv-

    ering

    the

    different

    Peer

    Influence

    Scenarios

    (e.g.,

    Modeling,

    Positive Reinforce-

    ment, Negative Reinforcement, Referent, Expert).

    Relationsbo T)pe. Two

    Relationship Type Vignettes

    (e.g., Best

    Friend, Ac-

    quaintance) were audiorecorded, following

    Ray

    and

    Cohen

    (1999;

    2000;

    see

    Ap-

    pendix A). The Best Friend

    vignette focuses

    on trust and intimacy and

    behavioral

    reciprocities such as sharing and cooperation that distinguish

    best

    friends from

    other

    types of social

    relationships (Hartup,

    1983). The

    Acquaintance vignette

    reflects

    that

    the

    two

    children

    are unacquainted and

    have not formed a judgment

    of

    each other. Thus, they neither liked

    nor disliked

    one another.

    In

    addition

    to the

    audiorecorded vignettes,

    a

    drawing to

    represent

    each

    Rela-

    tionship Type was

    constructed (Ray

    Cohen,

    1999; 2000). All drawings

    included

    two

    same-gender,

    same-race

    children and were matched to the gender and race

    of

    the participants. There were

    8

    drawings in total: two male and two female for

    African Americans,

    and two male and

    two

    female for European

    Americans.

    For

    Best

    Friends,

    the

    drawings were of two

    boys/girls

    (named

    Chris and

    Lee) standing

    close

    together,

    facing one

    another and

    smiling.

    For Acquaintances the

    drawings

    were of Chris

    and Lee

    standing

    beside each other

    with neutral facial

    expressions

    and

    facing

    forward.

    PeerInfluence

    Scenarios.

    Five audiorecorded

    Peer

    Influence Type

    Scenarios (i.e.,

    Positive and Negative

    Reinforcement, Modeling,

    Expert, and

    Referent) were con-

    structed (see Appendix B).

    In

    the Positive

    Reinforcement

    scenarios,

    the re-

    sponder

    (Pat)

    is offered some

    type of

    incentive to cheat from the initiator peer

    (Chris).

    In

    the Negative

    Reinforcement

    scenario, Pat is encouraged

    with

    a nega-

    tive consequence from

    Chris

    to engage in cheating.

    In

    the Modeling

    scenario,

    Pat

    observes Chris cheating and doing very well. In the

    Expert

    scenario, Pat

    wit-

    nessed Chris, who is good at schoolwork and tests,

    cheating. And

    in the Referent

    scenario,

    Pat

    desperately

    wanted

    to be like Chris (popular,

    lots

    of

    friends). It is

    important

    to

    note

    that

    the

    two children

    depicted

    in

    the Relationship Type Vi-

    gnettes

    are

    the same

    two

    children depicted in each

    of

    the

    Peer

    Influence Scenarios.

    Pa ge241

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    8/22

    Burton Ray, Mebta

    response (e.g., Is Pat

    going

    to cheat like Chris?) and affective state (e.g., How

    up-

    set

    does Pat feel?). To

    assess the influence

    of these evaluations

    on the evaluator

    (participating child),

    questions pertaining to the evaluator's affective

    state (e.g.,

    How upset would

    you be

    watching

    this happen?)

    the

    evaluator's

    behavior

    re-

    sponse

    (e.g.,

    If you were Pat, would you cheat?)

    and the frequency of

    such

    behav-

    iors (e.g., How

    often does this sort

    of thing happen?) also were assessed.

    Chil-

    dren's

    xesponses

    were

    assessed using a Likert-type

    scale

    ranging

    from 1

    to

    6.

    Procedure

    Each child was individually

    interviewed

    in a

    quiet

    area

    outside his

    or her

    classroom in a 20-25 minute

    session.

    Each child listened to a total

    of six

    audiore-

    corded scenarios:

    one for a

    particular

    level

    of Relationship Type

    (Best Friends

    or

    Acquaintances)

    and

    five

    delivering

    the

    Peer Influence

    Scenarios

    (Modeling,

    Posi-

    tive Reinforcement, Negative

    Reinforcement, Expert,

    Referent).

    Each child was

    also

    presented

    with a picture

    corresponding

    to the particular level

    of Relationship

    Type. First, a particular

    Relationship Type scenario

    and

    corresponding picture

    was

    presented. The

    particular Relationship Type p icture

    remained in

    full

    view for

    the entire interview.

    After

    administration of the first peer

    influence type

    scenario,

    children filled out

    the corresponding questionnaire.

    Children

    filled

    out

    each ques-

    tionnaire immediately following

    each

    Peer

    Influence

    Type

    scenario. After

    com-

    pleting the questionnaire, the child

    was

    presented

    with the next

    peer influence

    scenario. Presentation of the peer

    influence

    scenarios

    was counterbalanced

    to

    control

    for possible

    carryover

    and sequencing

    effects.

    Children

    were instructed

    at

    the

    beginning

    of each scenario, Instead

    of that happening, let's

    pretend

    that

    this

    is what happened.

    Upon

    completion

    of

    all questionnaires, the

    child was de-

    briefed

    by

    being

    reminded

    that

    the scenarios

    are

    not real and

    did

    not actually

    hap-

    pen. The child was then

    asked if he

    or

    she

    had any questions

    and

    then

    returned

    to

    the classroom.

    Results

    A series

    of 2 (Grade: young, old)

    x 2

    (Gender) x 2 (Relationship

    Type:

    best

    friend,

    acquaintance) x 5

    (Influence

    Type: modeling,

    positive reinforcement, nega-

    tive reinforcement,

    referent, expert) mixed factorial

    ANOVAs

    were

    conducted

    on

    variables assessing

    the

    responder's

    behavioral

    response (compliance), affective

    state

    of

    the

    responder, as well as the evaluator's affective

    state, and the evaluator's

    behavior response. Follow-up tests to statistically

    significant

    interactions

    were

    conducted as

    tests

    for

    simple effects

    followed by Newman-Keuls

    post hoc tests

    to

    determine

    sources

    of

    differences

    where appropriate.

    Page

    242

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    9/22

    CHILD STUDYJOURNAL/l/olume 33/No. 4/2003

    Responder

    Compliance

    Analysis on children's evaluations

    of

    responder

    compliance revealed a Grade

    main effect, F

    1,

    152)

    =

    17.31,

    p

    <

    .001. As

    hypothesized, older children

    M =

    3.92,

    SD

    =

    1.11)

    reported

    the

    responder

    as

    being

    more

    likely

    to

    comply,

    than

    did

    younger children

    M=

    3.11,

    SD =

    1.35).

    Analysis

    also

    revealed an

    Influence Type

    main

    effect,

    F

    (4,

    608)

    =

    6.88 p <

    .001. As predicted, children evaluated

    the

    responder as being

    more likely to

    cheat

    in the

    referent peer

    influence scenario

    M

    = 3.99, SD

    = 1.92) compared to

    all

    other peer influence

    scenarios,

    which did not

    differ

    from each other

    Ms and SDs

    =

    3.24

    (1.92),

    3.59

    (1.89),

    3.16

    (1.92),

    3.53

    (1.92), for modeling, positive rein-

    forcement,

    negative

    reinforcement,

    and

    expert,

    respectively).

    ResponderAffeat

    Analysis

    investigating children's evaluation of

    how

    upset the responder was

    revealed a

    significant

    Relationship

    Type

    x Gender

    interaction,

    F 1, 151) = 5.91,p

    < .05.

    As

    seen in

    Figure

    1, girls evaluated the responder

    as

    being more upset

    in

    the best

    friend

    relationship

    M

    =

    3.62,

    SD = 1.35) compared to the

    acquaintance

    relationship

    M = 2.93,

    SD

    = 1.32).

    No relationship differences

    emerge for

    boys

     Ms and SDs

    =

    3.10

    (1.49),

    3.37 (1.25), for

    best friends and acquaintances,

    respec-

    tively).

    3.75

    Best Friends

    -3.50 j cquaintances

    3.25

    3.00

    Pa

    243

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    10/22

    Burton,

    Ray, Mehta

    Analysis

    also revealed

    a significant

    Relationship

    Type

    x Grade

    interaction

    F

     1, 151)

    = 4.45, p

    .05. As

    seen

    in

    Figure

    2,

    for

    best

    friends, younger

    children

    evaluated

    the responder

    as being more

    upset

    M

    =

    3.92, SD

    = 1.34)

    than did

    older

    children

    M

    =

    2.70,

    SD

    =

    1.16).

    No

    developmental differences

    emerge

    for

    ac-

    quaintances Ms

    and

    SDs= 3.34

    (1.36),

    2.99 (1.22),

    for best

    friends

    and

    acquaint-

    ances,

    respectively).

    4.00

    1

    3.757

    3.50 -

    3.25-

    3.00

    -

    2.75 -

    2.50

    Fie

    s

    D

    Acsquatan s

    2-3

    5-6

    grade

    level

    Figure2. Responder

    affect Relationship

    Type x

    Grade interaction.

     

    Page

    244

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    11/22

    CHILD

    STUDYJOURNAL/Volume

    33/No.

    4/2003

    EvaluatorAffect

    Analysis

    investigating

    how

    upset

    the evaluator

    would

    be

    evaluating

    one

    peer

    manipulating

    another

    revealed

    a

    significant

    Grade

    effect, F

    1,

    152)

    12.07,

    p

    <

    .01.

    Younger

    children s

    evaluations

    M

    =

    4.78,

    SD =

    1.41)

    revealed

    that

    they

    were

    more

    upset

    evaluating

    the

    scenarios

    than

    were

    older

    children

    M

    =

    3.92,

    SD

    1.74).

    Analysis

    also revealed

    a

    significant

    Gender

    x

    Influence

    Type

    interaction, F

    1,

    608) 3.01,

    p

    .05. As

    seen

    in Figure

    3,

    girls

    reported

    being

    more

    upset

    than

    boys

    in

    the positive

    reinforcement

    and

    negative reinforcement

    peer

    influence

    sce-

    narios.

    No other

    gender

    differences

    emerge

    among

    the

    different

    peer

    influence

    scenarios.

    Girls reported

    being

    more upset

    evaluating

    the positive

    reinforcement

    and

    negative

    reinforcement

    peer influence

    scenarios,

    which

    did

    not

    differ from

    each other, but

    were higher

    than

    the modeling

    peer

    influence scenario.

    No

    other

    differences

    emerged

    for

    girls

    WMs and

    SDs =

    4.14 (2.02),

    4.65

    (1.74),

    4.64 (1.64),

    4.52

    (1.80),

    4.37

    (1.83),

    for

    modeling,

    positive

    reinforcement,

    negative

    reinforce-

    ment,

    referent,

    and

    expert,

    respectively).

    No

    differences

    in evaluator

    affect

    emerged

    across

    the

    different peer

    influence

    scenarios

    for

    boys Ms

    and SDs

    4.34

    (1.92),

    4.21

    (1.94),

    4.21

    (1.99),

    4.11 (2.01), 4.32

    (1.92), for

    modeling,

    positive

    rein-

    forcement,

    negative

    reinforcement,

    referent,

    and

    expert,

    respectively).

    4.75

    4.60-

    4.45-

     

    4.30-

    4 15

    4.00-

    Pa ge 245

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    12/22

    2Burton

    Raj,

    Mebta

    How

    Realisfic

    Analysis

    investigating

    how

    often

    children

    experienced

    incidences

    of cheating

    revealed

    a statisically

    significant

    effect

    for Grade,

    F,

    (1,

    152)

    13.91,

    p <

    .001.

    Older

    children

    (M

    4.19,

    SD =

    1.27)

    reported that

    cheating incidences

    happened

    more

    often

    than did

    younger

    children

    (M 3.43,

    SD

    1.33).

    Discx.ssion

    Peer

    relationships

    are

    vital

    to the

    social, cognitive,

    and physical

    development

    of

    children

    (e.g.,

    Newcomb,

    Bukowski,

    Hartup,

    1996).

    Much

    work

    has

    been

    done

    investigating

    special

    relationships

    of children

    and

    the influence

    they

    have

    on

    one

    another

    (see

    Berndt,

    1989;

    Newcomb

    Bagwell,

    1995

    for

    meta-analytic

    re-

    view).

    Until

    recently,

    it

    was

    assumed

    that

    a peer's

    influence

    was

    always

    positive,

    especially

    in

    children's

    friendships.

    However,

    it

    has become

    increasingly

    obvious

    that

    some

    peer

    influence

    is

    largely

    negative

    (Duck,

    1996;

    Savin-Williams

    Berndt,

    1990).

    Researchers

    have

    found

    that

    friends

    do

    encourage

    friends

    to engage

    in

    undesirable

    and antisocial

    acts

    (e.g., fighting,

    smoking,

    drugs).

    Interestingly,

    little

    work

    has

    been

    directed

    toward

    children's

    social-informational

    processing

    of peer

    pressure

    situations.

    Thus,

    the

    present

    study

    investigated

    children's

    evaluation

    of

    negative

    peer

    influence

    as

    a

    function

    of different

    types

    of

    peer relationships

    (e.g.,

    best friends,

    acquaintances)

    and different

    types

    of

    peer

    pressure

    (e.g.,

    modeling,

    positive reinforcement,

    referent).

    The predicion

    that

    older

    children

    (Grades

    5-6)

    would

    evaluate

    the

    responder

    as being

    more

    likely

    to

    cheat

    than

    would

    younger

    children

    (Grades

    2-3)

    is

    sup-

    ported,

    and

    both

    replicates

    and

    extends

    earlier

    work

    on

    children's

    susceptibility

    to

    peer

    influence.

    Researchers

    (e.g.,

    Berndt,

    1996;

    Steinberg

    Silverberg,

    1986)

    demonstrate

    that conformity

    to peer

    influence

    increases

    from middle

    childhood

    to

    middle

    adolescence

    (about

    age 15)

    and

    then decreases

    in late adolescence.

    The

    current

    study

    extends

    these

    findings

    in

    two important

    ways.

    First,

    the

    present

    study

    is

    among

    the

    first

    to

    use children

    as

    young

    as

    second

    grade

    in

    a

    sample

    in-

    vestigating

    peer

    influences.

    Second,

    previous

    research

    uses

    hypothetical

    situations

    that directly

    involve

    the

    participant

    (e.g.,

    Will

    you

    cheat?).

    The

    current

    research

    is

    one

    step

    removed,

    where

    participants

    are evaluators

    of the

    interactions

    of

    other

    peers.

    Given

    that

    similar

    findings emerge

    when

    children

    evaluate

    themselves

    or

    when

    they

    witness

    the

    interactions

    of others,

    it

    appears

    that

    children

    may

    base

    their

    perceptions

    and

    evaluations

    of

    others

    and

    what

    others

    will do,

    n part,

    on

    how

    they

    themselves

    would

    act in similar

    situations

    (Ray

    Cohen,

    2000;

    Ray et

    al.,

    2001).

    Why

    do older

    children

    evaluate

    peers

    as

    being more

    susceptible

    to peer

    influ-

    ence

    compared

    to

    younger

    children?

    As children

    age,

    they begin

    spending

    more

    Page246

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    13/22

    CHILD

    STUDYJOURNAL/Volume

    33/No 4/2003

    and

    replace it with dependency

    on

    their

    peers.

    Therefore, the

    older the

    child, the

    larger

    the measure

    of emotional

    autonomy

    in

    their

    family relationships.

    Also,

    past

    research (e.g.,

    Brown, Clasen,

    Eicher., 1986;

    1Hartup,

    1983) has

    shown that with

    age

    there

    is

    a

    steady increase toward

    misconduct and

    older children perceive these

    negative

    behaviors as normative. This is

    consistent

    with our finding

    that older

    children (fifth/sixth grades)

    report that

    being

    influenced

    to cheat

    by their

    peers is

    a more common

    occurrence

    than

    younger children (second/third

    grades).

    Per-

    haps,

    older children

    are more apt

    to

    behave mischievously

    because they

    view

    anti-

    social behaviors

    differently than do younger children

    and they also rely more

    heavily on

    their

    friends

    than

    their

    parents for

    direction.

    Contrary

    to past

    research

    (e.g.,

    Bemdt, 1996),

    the

    prediction

    that the

    closer

    the relationship between the two

    peers being evaluated (best friends vs.

    acquaint-

    ances)

    the

    more

    influential

    one

    child

    would

    be

    on

    the

    other

    is

    not

    supported.

    In

    light of this

    relationship type

    non-finding, it is important

    to note that

    children

    do

    evaluate

    best

    friends differently

    than acquaintances

    in that

    all

    children

    evaluated

    liking

    between best friends

    higher

    than liking

    between

    acquaintances.

    Also,

    the

    prediction that girls

    would evaluate best

    friends as being more

    influential than

    boys is

    not

    supported.

    Thus, girl best friends

    were no more influential

    than

    boy

    best friends.

    In the

    present

    study, only

    positive

    (e.g., best

    friends)

    and neutral

    (acquaintances)

    are used. Had

    a

    broader

    range

    of relationships been

    used (e.g.,

    best friends,

    acquaintances, enemies), perhaps

    relationship

    effects

    would have

    emerged.

    For

    example,

    Ray

    and

    Cohen

    (1999; 2000)

    demonstrate

    that

    children's

    evaluations of

    best friends

    and

    acquaintances

    are

    quite

    similar

    and

    always

    more

    positive than are the evaluations

    of enemies.

    In

    the present

    study,

    with

    reference

    to

    peer

    influence, children are evaluating

    two positive relationships and

    no

    differ-

    ences emerge.

    Further,

    researchers

    (e.g.,

    Berndt, 1996)

    have

    demonstrated

    three areas where

    susceptibility

    to

    friends' influence

    is greatest.

    The

    first

    is

    a child's

    position in the

    peer

    group.

    Children of

    lower

    social

    status

    are

    likely to

    be more

    influenced than

    higher status children. While

    sociometric status of

    participants

    is

    not

    measured

    in

    the

    current

    study,

    it

    is

    a safe assumption

    to

    consider

    that

    the

    majority

    of

    children

    participating are

    not from unpopular status groups

    (see Ray, Cohen, Secrist,

    1995). Second,

    children whose personal relationships

    (parents,

    other peers)

    are

    less satisfying

    are more susceptible

    to friends'

    influence.

    This again,

    would sug-

    gest that unpopular

    and marginal

    status children would

    be more influenced

    than

    popular and

    average

    status

    children.

    Lastly,

    children

    appear to

    be

    more suscepti-

    ble to friends'

    influence

    when the behavior/situation

    is not particularly

    important

    to their sense of self.

    While

    the

    current

    study

    does not measure

    characteristics of

    reputational

    salience, it

    appears that making

    good

    grades

    and

    being a good student

    are important

    to

    a sense of self

    for

    elementary school age

    children

    and

    are, thus,

    behaviors

    that are less susceptible

    to the influence of

    peers.

    Pagel24

    7

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    14/22

    Butnton Ray

    Mehta

    supported.

    However,

    children

    do evaluate

    referent

    power as being

    the

    most

    influ-

    ential

    type

    of social power.

    As

    stated

    in the literature

    review,

    referent

    power

    emerges

    when

    children

    admire and

    want to

    be

    like another child.

    Researchers

    (Epstein,

    1983;

    Sherif

    Sherif,

    1964)

    have

    found

    that

    children

    often

    try

    to

    be-

    come

    friends

    with

    other

    children

    whom

    they

    perceive

    as

    popular,

    athletic,

    or

    skilled

    in some

    other

    area. Berndt

    (1996)

    further stated

    that after

    these

    friend-

    ships

    are formed

    they

    continue

    to use their

    friends

    as referent

    others to

    guide

    and

    direct

    their own

    behavior.

    Analysis

    investigating

    children's

    evaluation

    of

    the responder's

    affect reveals

    that

    within best

    friend

    relationships,

    girls

    as well as

    younger

    children

    report the

    responder

    to be

    more upset

    than

    do boys and

    older

    children.

    This

    supports

    pre-

    vious

    research

    (Ray

    Cohen,

    2000)

    demonstrating

    that girls

    and younger children

    are

    more

    likely

    to report

    the

    peers

    they evaluate, particularly

    peers

    that

    have been

    wronged

    (i.e., aggressed

    against), as being

    more

    emotionally affected

    (more

    upset)

    than do boys

    and older children.

    Analysis

    investigating

    how upset the participating

    children

    would be

    evaluat-

    ing a

    child trying

    to get

    another

    child

    to

    cheat

    reveals

    younger

    children

    as being

    more

    upset

    than

    older

    children.

    As children

    get

    older incidences of

    cheating be-

    come

    much

    more

    prevalent

    as the

    current

    study shows,

    and

    perhaps has

    less of

    a

    negative

    effect

    on

    a child. That

    is, as children

    get

    older and

    they see cheating

    on

    a

    more routine

    basis,

    perhaps,

    they become

    desensitized

    to

    it.

    Also, older

    children

    are

    more willing to

    engage

    in such

    a negative

    behavior,

    as

    revealed

    in this

    study.

    Thus the

    older

    the

    child,

    the

    less they

    are going

    to

    be

    upset about

    an incidence

    that

    occurs

    everyday

    around

    them and

    that

    they

    themselves

    are

    more likely to

    engage in.

    Limitations

    of the

    current

    study

    include

    children

    being presented

    with

    hypo-

    thetical scenarios,

    which

    may

    have minimized

    the

    reality of

    the experiment

    for the

    evaluators.

    Children's

    responses

    to

    hypothetical

    situations

    may not coincide

    with

    how

    they

    will

    evaluate

    actual

    peer

    influence

    situations.

    Thus, it will

    be important

    for

    future

    research

    to begin investigating

    children's

    own real world

    peer

    influence

    situations

    or experimental

    analog

    situations

    with

    actual

    peers.

    In addidon,

    chil-

    dren

    evaluate

    each

    scenario

    using a

    forced choice

    questionnaire.

    Future

    research

    allowing

    children

    to

    respond

    more freely

    and

    independently

    to situations

    involving

    peer

    influence

    may

    provide

    a

    more complete

    understanding

    of

    children's

    percep-

    tions

    of influence.

    The

    present study

    focuses

    only

    on

    dyadic

    (one-on-one)

    inter-

    actions between

    children.

    Future

    research

    into

    children's

    influence

    on

    each other

    will need

    to consider the

    effect

    ofmultiple

    friends

    and

    their collective

    influence.

    Further,

    in

    addition

    to

    cheating

    situations

    there

    are

    various

    other types

    of negative

    situations

    where the

    susceptibility

    to peer

    pressure

    is

    more

    likely

    (e.g.,

    not

    impor-

    tant

    to

    identity).

    Lastly,

    while the

    present study only

    includes

    positive and

    neutral

    Pa ge248

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    15/22

    CHILD

    STUDYJOURNALI/Volume

    33/No.

    4/2003

    Page

    249

    themselves

    would

    be more

    likely

    to

    cheat

    than

    would

    younger

    children.

    These

    findings

    coupled

    with

    earlier

    research

    documenting

    that

    older

    children

    are

    more

    suscepible

    to

    the influences

    of

    their

    peers,

    begin

    to shed

    light

    on

    how

    children

    actually

    go

    about

    the business

    of

    making evaluations

    of

    others.

    It

    appears

    that

    how

    children

    evaluate

    the

    interactions

    of

    their

    peers

    is, in

    part,

    determined

    by

    how

    they

    themselves

    would

    behave

    in similar

    situations

    (see

    Ray

    et al.,

    2001,

    for

    re-

    view).

    For

    example,

    older

    children

    evaluate

    the

    responder

    and also

    themselves

    as

    being more

    likely

    to comply

    than

    do

    younger

    children.

    A

    similar

    pattern

    emerges

    with

    regard

    to

    the

    responder's

    affect

    and

    the

    evaluator's

    affect.

    Perhaps,

    the de-

    velopmental

    differences

    evidenced

    in

    the

    current

    study are

    the result

    of

    an

    ability

    of

    older

    children

    to

    put

    themselves

    in the

    situation ,

    while

    younger

    children

    re-

    spond

    to

    the

    situation(s)

    in

    a

    concrete

    way.

    Clearly,

    how

    children

    evaluate

    the

    interactions

    of

    their

    peers has

    important

    developmental consequences,

    not

    only

    for

    the evaluator,

    but

    for

    the

    children

    being

    evaluated

    as

    well.

    Peer pressure

    situa-

    tions

    are

    daily

    occurrences

    and

    children

    obviously

    witness

    peers

    attempting

    to

    influence

    each

    other.

    As

    such,

    the

    current

    study is

    a

    beginning

    in

    understanding

    how

    children

    evaluate

    these

    peer

    pressure

    situations

    between

    others

    and

    the

    fac-

    tors

    that

    influence

    these

    evaluations.

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    16/22

    Page

    250

    Burton

    Ry, &

    Mebta

    APPENDIX

    A

    Relationship

    Type

    Vignettes

    Best

    Fnendr

    Let's

    pretend

    that you're

    new

    to this

    school

    and

    don't

    know

    the

    kids

    in

    the

    story

    you're

    about

    to

    hear.

    The story

    is

    about

    Chris

    and

    Pat.

    Chris

    and

    Pat

    are

    best

    friends.

    They

    go

    to

    the same

    school

    and

    are

    in

    the

    same

    class.

    Chris

    lives

    down

    the

    street

    from

    Pat

    so

    they

    get

    to

    spend

    a

    lot

    of

    time

    together

    playing

    games

    and

    having

    fun.

    They

    sat

    together

    and

    shared

    their

    lunch

    today.

    Chris

    and

    Pat

    tell

    each

    other

    theirsecrets

    and

    take

    up

    for each

    other.

    Chris

    and Pat

    have

    been

    best

    friends

    for

    a

    long

    time.

    Acquaintances

    Let's

    pretend

    that

    you're

    new

    to this

    school

    and don't

    know

    the

    kids

    in

    the

    story

    you're

    about to

    hear.

    The

    story

    is

    about

    Chris

    and

    Pat.

    Chris

    and

    Pat

    are

    in

    the

    same

    class

    together.

    Chris

    and

    Pat know

    each

    other

    but

    don't

    sit

    by each

    other

    in

    class

    or

    at

    lunch

    time.

    It's

    not

    that they

    don't

    like each

    other.

    They

    just

    don't

    know

    each

    other

    well

    enough

    to know

    whether

    they

    like

    each

    other

    or

    not

    Chris

    and

    Pat

    are not

    enemies,

    but they

    are

    not

    really

    friends

    either.

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    17/22

    CHILD

    STUDYJOURNAL Volume

    33/No.

    4/2003

    APPENDIX

    B

    Influence

    Type

    Situations

    Modeling.

    Let's pretend that

    one

    day a

    class

    was

    about

    to

    take a

    very

    impor-

    tant

    test.

    Chris

    and Pat

    sit

    by

    each

    other

    in class.

    The

    teacher

    passes

    out

    the

    test

    to everyone

    and

    says Go

    ahead

    and

    start working

    on

    the

    test.

    I have

    to

    go

    to

    the

    principal's

    office

    to

    talk to

    someone.

    Please

    remember

    to keep

    your

    eyes

    on

    your

    own

    test.

    The

    teacher

    leaves

    the

    room

    and

    the class

    begins

    working

    on

    the test.

    Pat

    wants to

    do

    very

    good

    on

    the

    test,

    but

    the test starts

    getting

    really

    hard.

    Pat

    looks

    over

    and

    sees Chris

    opening

    a

    book

    under

    the

    desk

    and

    cheating

    and hears

    Chris

    say,

    Wow,

    this

    test

    is

    really easy

    now.

    Positive

    Reinforremen:

    Let's

    pretend

    that

    one

    day

    a

    class

    was

    about

    to

    take

    a

    very

    important

    test.

    Chris

    and Pat

    sit

    by each

    other

    in class.

    The

    teacher passes

    out the

    test

    to

    everyone

    and says

    Go ahead

    and

    start

    working

    on the test.

    I have

    to

    go

    to

    the principal's

    office

    to talk

    to

    someone.

    Please

    remember

    to keep

    your

    eyes

    on your

    own

    test.

    The

    teacher leaves

    the room

    and the

    class

    begins

    working

    on

    the test.

    Pat

    wants to

    do

    very

    good,

    but

    the test

    starts

    getting

    really

    hard.

    Pat

    looks

    over

    and

    sees Chris

    opening

    a

    book

    under

    the

    desk and

    cheating.

    Chris

    leans

    over

    and says

    Hey

    Pat

    this

    is

    a

    really

    hard test.

    Here,

    use

    my

    answers

    and

    you will do

    really

    good.

    Negative

    Reinforcement:

    Let's

    pretend

    that

    one day a

    class was

    about

    to

    take

    a

    very

    important

    test.

    Chris

    and

    Pat

    sit

    by each

    other

    in class. The

    teacher passes

    out the

    test

    to

    everyone

    and

    says

    Go

    ahead

    and

    start

    working

    on

    the test.

    I

    have

    to

    go

    to

    the principal's

    office

    to talk

    to

    someone.

    Please

    remember

    to keep

    your

    eyes

    on your

    own test.

    The

    teacher leaves

    the

    room

    and

    the class

    begins

    working

    on

    the

    test.

    Pat wants

    to do

    very good,

    but

    the

    test

    starts

    getting

    really

    hard.

    Pat

    looks

    over

    and

    sees

    Chris

    opening

    a book

    under

    the

    desk

    and

    cheating.

    Chris

    leans

    over

    and says

    Hey

    Pat

    this

    is a really

    hard test.

    If

    you do

    not

    cheat,

    you

    are

    going to

    fail.

    Referent:

    Let's

    pretend that

    one day

    a

    class was

    about

    to

    take

    a

    very

    important

    test. Chris

    and

    Pat sit

    by

    each

    other

    in

    class.

    The

    teacher

    passes

    out

    the

    test

    to

    everyone

    and

    says

    Go

    ahead

    and

    start working

    on

    the

    test.

    I

    have

    to go

    to the

    principal's office

    to talk

    to someone.

    Please

    remember

    to keep

    your

    eyes

    on

    your

    own

    test.

    The

    teacher leaves

    the

    room

    and

    the

    class begins

    working

    on

    the

    test.

    Pat wants

    to do

    very

    good

    on

    the

    test, but

    the

    test

    starts

    getting

    really

    hard.

    Pat

    looks

    over

    and

    sees

    Chris

    opening

    a

    book

    under

    the

    desk

    and

    cheating. Chris

    has

    lots

    of

    friends

    and

    everybody

    likes Chris.

    Pat wants

    to be just

    like

    Chris.

    Epert

    Let's

    pretend

    that

    one

    day

    a class

    was

    about

    to take

    a very

    important

    test.

    Chris

    and

    Pat sit by

    each

    other

    in class.

    The

    teacher

    passes

    out

    the test to

    everyone

    and

    says Go

    ahead

    and start

    working

    on

    the test.

    I

    have

    to

    go to

    the

    eyes

    on

    your

    Page

    251

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    18/22

    Burton Ray,

    Mehta

    APPENDIX

    C

    Peer

    Evaluation

    Questionnaires

    1.

    How

    much do

    Pat and

    Chris

    like each

    other?

    ..............

    3.

    4 2 5 : 6

    Not

    at all

    Very

    much

    2.

    Is

    Pat

    going

    to

    cheat

    like Chris?

    .................

    2..

    3.

    4.

    5..

    6

    Not

    likely

    Highly

    likely

    3.

    How

    upset

    does

    Pat

    feel

    1 2.

    3.

    4.

    5..

    6

    Not

    Upset

    Very

    Upset

    4.

    How

    often

    does

    this

    sort

    of

    thing

    really

    happen?

    .

    .............. 

    2..

    3.

    4.

    5..

    6

    Not ever

    All

    the time

    5. How

    upset

    would

    you

    be watching

    this

    happen?

    .

    ..............

    .

    2.

    3.

    4.

    5.G

    6

    Not Upset

    Very

    Upset

    6. If

    you

    were

    Pat,

    wouldyou

    cheat? ...........

    2......2.

    3.

    4.

    5..

    6

    Highly

    unlikely

    Highly

    likely

    P.age

    252

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    19/22

    CHILDSTUDYJOURNAL/

    Volume 33/No.

    4/2003

    References

    Akers,

    R.

    L. (1977).

    Deviant

    behavior

    A social

    earningpersecdive.

    Belmont,

    CA:

    Wadsworth.

    Bandura, A.

    (1977). Social

    earning

    theoy.

    Englewood

    Cliffs,

    NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Barrett,

    M.

    E., Simpson,

    D.

    D.,

    Lehman, W.

    E.

    (1988).

    Behavioral

    changes

    of

    adoles-

    cents

    in drug

    abuse intervention

    programs.

    Jouirnal

    of

    Youth andAdolescence,

    15,

    345-353.

    Berndt,

    T.

    (1979).

    Developmental

    changes

    in

    conformity

    to peers

    and

    parents. Developmen-

    tal

    Psybhology,

    15,

    608-616.

    Berndt,

    T.

    (1989,

    Mv arch).

    Effects

    offiiendshbp

    on achievement

    motivation

    and classroom

    behavior.

    Paper

    presented

    at the

    annual

    meeting

    of

    the American

    Educational

    Research

    Association,

    San

    Francisco.

    Berndt,

    T.

    J., Savin-Williams,

    R

    C. (1993).

    Peer

    relations

    and

    friendships.

    In

    P.

    H. To-

    Ian

    B.

    J.

    Cohler

    (Eds.),

    Handbook

    of

    clinical

    research

    andpractie

    with

    adolescents.

    (pp.

    203-219).

    New

    York:

    Wiley

    Sons.

    Bemd;

    T.j.

    Keefe,

    K

    (1994).

    Friend s

    influence

    on

    adolescents

    adjustment

    to

    school.

    ChildDevelopment,

    66, 1312-1329.

    Bemdt,

    T. J.

    (1996).

    Transition

    in

    friendship

    and

    friends

    influence.

    In

    J.

    A.

    Graber,

    .

    Brooks-Gunn,

    A.

    C.Peterson

    (Eds.),

    Transitions

    hbi gh

    adolescence:

    Interpersonal

    Domains

    and

    Context

    (pp. 57-84).

    Mahwah, New

    Jersey:

    Lawrence

    Erlbaum

    As-

    sociates,

    Inc.,

    Publishers.

    Billy,

    J. G.,

    Udry,

    J.

    R

    (1985).

    Pattems

    of

    adolescent

    friendship

    and effects

    on

    sexual

    behavior.

    Social

    Psycholo gy.Qarterly,

    48,27-41.

    Bronfrenbrenner,

    U.

    (1970).

    Tov

    worlds

    of

    hildren.

    New

    York:

    Sage.

    Brown, B.

    B.,

    Clasen,

    D.

    R,

    Eicher,

    S.

    A.

    (1986).

    Perceptions

    of

    peer

    pressure,

    peer

    conformity

    dispositions,

    and

    self-reported

    behavior

    among

    adolescents.

    Develop

    mental

    Prycbolog,,

    22, 521-530.

    Cohen,

    J.

    (1983).

    Commentary:

    The

    relationship

    between

    friendship

    selection

    and

    peer

    influence.

    In J. L

    Epstein

    N.

    Karweit

    (Eds.),

    Friendsin

    school

    (pp.1

    6

    3

    -

    174

    ).

    New

    York.

    Academic

    Press.

    Crick,

    N.

    R, Dodge,

    K

    A. (1994).

    A review

    and

    reformulation

    of

    social

    information-

    processing

    mechanisms

    in children s

    social

    adjustment.

    PsychologicalBulletin,

    115,

    74-101.

    Davies,

    M., Kandel,

    D.

    B.

    (1981).

    Parental

    and

    peer

    influences

    on

    adolescents

    educa-

    tional plans:

    Some further

    evidence.

    American

    Journal

    of

    Sociology,

    87,

    363-387.

    Duck,

    V. (1996).

    A

    lIfetime

    of

    relationships.

    New

    York:

    Brooks/Cole

    Publishing

    Company.

    Ellio;

    D. S.,

    Huizinga,

    D.,

    Ageton,

    S. S.

    (1985).

    Explaining

    delinquengy

    and drog

    nse.

    Bev-

    erly Hills,

    CA: Sage.

    Epstein,

    J.

    L

    (1983).

    The

    influence

    of friends

    on

    achievement

    and affective

    outcomes.

    In

    J. L

    Epstein

    N.

    Karweit

    (Eds.),

    Friends

    in

    school:

    Patterns

    of selection

    and

    nfluence

    in

    secondary

    schools (pp.177-

    20 0

    ).

    New York:

    Academic

    Press.

    Fisher,

    L

    A.,

    Bauman,

    K.

    E.

    (1988).

    Influence

    and

    selection

    in

    the

    friend-adolescent

    relationship:

    Findings

    from

    studies

    of

    adolescent

    smoking

    and

    drinking.

    Journal

    ofAppliedSodialPsychologV, 18,

    289-314.

    French, J.

    R

    P.,

    Raven,

    B. (1959).

    The

    bases

    of social

    power.

    In

    D.C.

    Cartwright

    (Ed.),

    Page253

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    20/22

    Burton,

    Ray,

    Mehia

    Hartup,

    W.

    W.

    (1983).

    Sodalization,

    personality,

    and

    social

    development

    In

    E.

    M.

    Hetherington

    (Ed.),

    Handbook

    of

    childpsychology:

    Vol.

    4.

    New York.

    Wiley.

    Hartup, W.

    W. (1992).

    Conflict

    and

    friendship

    relations.

    In

    C. U.

    Shantz

    W.

    W. Hartup

    (Eds.),

    Conflict

    in

    child

    and

    adolescentdevelopment

    (pp.

    186-215).

    Cambridge, UK

    Cambridge

    University

    Press.

    Huston,

    A.

    C. (1985).

    The

    development

    of

    sex-typing:

    Themes

    from recent

    research.

    Develop,mental

    Recien,

    5,

    1-17.

    Kandel,

    D.

    B.

    (1978).

    Homophile,

    selection

    and

    socialization

    in adolescent

    friendships.

    AmericanJournalof

    ociology,

    84,

    427-436.

    Kopfstein,

    D. (1972).

    Effects

    of

    accelerating

    and

    decelerating

    consequences

    on the

    sodal

    behavior

    of

    trainable

    retarded

    children.

    ChildDevelopment,

    43,

    800-809.

    Maltz, D.

    N.,

    Borker,

    R A.

    (1983).

    A cultural

    approach

    to

    male-female

    miscommunica-

    ion.

    In

    J. A.

    Gumperz

    (Ed.), Language

    and social

    identiy

    (pp.

    195-216).

    New

    York: Cambridge University Press.

    Maccoby,

    E.

    E.

    (1988).

    Gender

    as

    a

    social

    category.

    DevelopmentalPycholog,

    24,

    755-765.

    Maccoby,

    E.

    E.

    (1990).

    Gender

    and

    relationships:

    A

    developmental

    account

    American

    Pychbologist,

    45,

    513-520

    Matthys,

    W.,

    Cohen-Kettenis,

    P.,

    Berkhout,

    J.

    (1993).

    Boys'

    and

    girls'

    perceptions

    of

    peers

    in middle

    childhood:

    Differences

    and

    similarities.

    TheJournal

    of GeneticPry-

    chology,

    155,

    15-24.

    McDavid,

    J.

    W. (1959).

    Personality

    and

    situaional

    determinants

    of

    conformity.

    Jouirnal

    of

    AbnonmalrPychology,

    67, 286-289.

    Miller,

    W.

    B. (1958).

    Lower

    class

    culture

    as

    generating

    milieu

    of gang

    delinquency.

    Journa/

    of

    SocialIssues,

    14,

    5-19.

    Millstein,

    S. G.,

    Petersen,

    A. C.,

    Nightingale,

    E. 0.

    (Eds.).

    (1993).

    Promoting he

    health

    of

    adolescents:

    New

    directionsfor

    he

    tventyfirst

    centu_y.

    New

    York.

    Oxford

    University

    Press.

    Morgan,

    M.,

    Grube,J.

    W.

    (1991).

    Closeness

    and

    peer

    group

    influence.

    BritishJournalof

    SocialPsychology,

    30,

    159-169.

    Mounts,

    N. S.,

    Steinberg,

    L.

    (1995).

    An

    ecological

    analysis

    of peer

    influence

    on

    adoles-

    cent

    grade

    point

    average

    and

    drug

    use.

    DevelopmentalPRychology,

    31,

    915-922.

    Newcomb,

    A. F.,

    Bukowski,

    W.

    M.,

    Hartup,

    W. W.

    (1996).

    The

    coVnpany

    they

    kee:

    Friendshb

    in

    childhood

    and

    adolescence.

    New York:

    Cambridge

    University

    Press.

    Newcomb,A.

    F,

    Bagwell,

    C.

    L.

    (1995).

    Children's friendship

    relaions: A

    Meta-Analyic

    Review.

    P.yhbologicalBulletin,

    117,

    306-347.

    O Connor,

    R.

    (1969).

    Modification

    of

    social

    withdrawal

    through

    symbolic

    modeling.

    Jour-

    nal

    ofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,

    2

    15-22.

    Parker,J.

    G.,

    Asher,

    S.

    R

    (1987).

    Peer

    relationships

    and

    later

    personal

    adjustment

    Are

    low-accepted

    children

    at

    risk?

    PsybhologicalBulletin,

    102,

    357-389.

    Ray,

    G.

    E., Graham,

    J. A.,

    Cohen,

    R (2001).

    The

    importance

    of

    relationship

    informa-

    tion

    for

    children's

    evaluations

    of

    peers

    and

    social

    situations.

    In F.

    Columbus

    (Vol.

    Ed.),

    Advances

    inpsychology

    researrh:

    Vol.

    5.

    (pp.

    209-230).

    Huntington,

    NY:

    Nova

    Sdence Publishers.

    Ray,

    G.

    E.,

    Cohen,

    R

    (1992,

    April).

    Children s

    e4pectations

    and

    awareness

    of

    theirfriendship

    networks.

    Paper

    presented

    at the

    biennial

    meeing

    of the

    Conference

    on Human

    Page

    254

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    21/22

    CHILD STUDYJOURNAL/IVol

    me

    33/No. 4/2003

    Savin-Williams,

    R. C., Berndt,

    T.J. (1990).

    Friendships

    and peer relations

    during adoles-

    cence.

    In

    S.S. Feldman G.

    Elliot (Eds.),

    At

    the

    threshold:

    The developing adolescent

    (pp.

    277-307). Cambridge,

    MA: Harvard

    University

    Press.

    Sherif,

    M.,

    Sherif,

    C.

    (1964).

    Reference

    groups:

    Exploration

    into

    confonrni y

    and

    deviance

    of

    adoles-

    cents.

    NY: Harper

    Row.

    Steinberg,

    L.

    (1986). Latchkey

    children and

    susceptibility

    to peer pressure:

    An ecological

    analysis. DevelopmentalRPychology,

    22,

    433-439.

    Steinberg,

    L.,

    Silverberg,

    S.

    B. (1986). The

    vicissitudes of

    autonomy

    in early adolescence.

    Child

    Development,

    57 841-851.

    Sullivan,

    H. S.

    (1953). The interpersonal

    heory ofpychiat?y.

    New York: Norton.

    Sutherland, B.,

    Cressey,

    D. (1970) Criminology. New

    York Lippincott.

    Youniss,

    J.

    (1980).

    Parentsandpeers in

    soial

    development. A

    Sullivan-Piagetperspecive.

    Chicago:

    University

    of Chicago

    Press.

    Footnotes

    We gratefully acknowledge

    the assistance and

    support of

    the Principal,

    and

    the

    teachers of Dannelly Elementary

    School,

    Montgomery,

    Alabama.

    Correspondence concerning

    this article

    should be addressed

    to:

    Glen

    E. Ray

    Page255

  • 8/18/2019 Enaluation of Peer Influence

    22/22

    COPYRIGHT INFORMATION

    TITLE: Children’s Evaluations of Peer Influence: The Role of 

    Relationship Type and Social Situation

    SOURCE: Child Study J 33 no4 2003

    WN: 0300403908003

    The magazine publisher is the copyright holder of this article and it

    is reproduced with permission. Further reproduction of this article in

    violation of the copyright is prohibited. To contact the publisher:

    http://www.socialwork.buffalo.edu

    Copyright 1982-2004 The H.W. Wilson Company. All rights reserved.