Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age...

24
1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory Efferent System and Speech Intelligibility in Noise SungHee Kim a,c , Robert D. Frisinaª ,b.c and D. Robert Frisina c,a a Otolaryngology Division, b Departments of Surgery, Neurobiology &Anatomy and Biomedical Engineering, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, and c International Center for Hearing and Speech Research, National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester NY, USA Key words: Aging, Presbycusis, Olivocochlear bundle, Medial Efferent System, Hearing-in-noise, Speech perception emails: [email protected], [email protected] , [email protected] Corresponding Author: Robert D. Frisina, PhD Otolaryngology Assoc. Chair University of Rochester School of Medicine 601 Elmwood Avenue Rochester, NY 14642-8629, USA Phone: 585-275-8130 FAX: 585-271-8552 e-Mail: [email protected]

Transcript of Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age...

Page 1: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

1

Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners:

Relationships Between the Auditory Efferent System

and Speech Intelligibility in Noise

SungHee Kima,c , Robert D. Frisinaª,b.c and D. Robert Frisinac,a

aOtolaryngology Division, bDepartments of Surgery, Neurobiology &Anatomy and Biomedical Engineering, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, and cInternational Center for Hearing and Speech Research, National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester NY, USA Key words: Aging, Presbycusis, Olivocochlear bundle, Medial Efferent System,

Hearing-in-noise, Speech perception

emails: [email protected], [email protected], [email protected]

Corresponding Author: Robert D. Frisina, PhD Otolaryngology Assoc. Chair University of Rochester School of Medicine 601 Elmwood Avenue Rochester, NY 14642-8629, USA Phone: 585-275-8130 FAX: 585-271-8552 e-Mail: [email protected]

Page 2: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

2

ABSTRACT Human listeners are able to concentrate on listening to one voice amidst other

conversations and background noise, but not all of the neural mechanisms for this process

are understood. There is growing evidence in normal-hearing subjects that the medial

olivocochlear (MOC) auditory efferent system is involved in the detection of signals in

noise, such as speech sounds, by modulation of cochlear active physiological

mechanisms. The present investigation aimed to evaluate the MOC efferent involvement

in speech intelligibility in noise and spatial release from masking (RFM) in normal-

hearing adults of different ages. Contralateral suppression (CS) of distortion product

otoacoustic emission was used to measure MOC efferent system function. Using HINT

(Hearing in the Noise Test), we measured speech intelligibility in noise at 0 degree

azimuth (HINT N0) and the improvement of speech intelligibility in noise, i.e. release

from masking (RFM), when speech and noise were spatially separated. Correlation

analysis was applied to reveal relations between the MOC efferent system, speech

intelligibility in noise and spatial RFM. The findings suggest: (1) age-related difficulty

understanding speech in background noise is related to an age-related functional decline

of the MOC efferent system, (2) the higher frequency (4-6 kHz) range of the MOC

efferent function is correlated with speech processing in background noise, and (3) the 1-

2 kHz frequency range of the MOC efferent system is correlated with a spatial RFM, i.e.,

“cocktail party” processing capability based on binaural hearing. In conclusion, the

MOC efferent system can be characterized as a nonlinear adaptive filter activated during

speech processing in background noise and also as a cocktail party processor.

Page 3: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

3

I. INTRODUCTION

In realistic acoustical environments where various sounds reach our ears

simultaneously, we can listen adaptively to a particular sound in the mixture of sounds by

focusing our attention on it. This phenomenon is known as the “cocktail party” effect

(Cherry, 1953; Yost, 1997). Physiological correlates of this effect have not been

extensively studied yet. To date, no computer systems have had such an effective

adaptive sound selection mechanism, even though many signal processing studies have

been conducted on this topic (Giguere and Woodland, 1994; Cooke and Ellis, 2001;

Rouat and Pichevar, 2002).

There is growing evidence in normal hearing young adult subjects that the

auditory medial olivocochlear (MOC) efferent system is involved in the detection of

signals in noise (Micheyl et al., 1995; Micheyl and Collet, 1996), including signals such

as speech sounds (Giraud et al., 1997; Zeng et al., 2000), by modulation of the cochlear

active mechanisms. However, the full extent of the MOC system’s role in hearing is still

not well understood. Several hypotheses have been proposed for an efferent involvement

in anti-masking (e.g., Winslow et al., 1987; Kawase et al., 1993a,b; Micheyl et al., 1995;

Giraud et al., 1997; Heinz et al., 1998; Liberman 1988; Liberman and Guinan, 1998),

protection from damage due to loud noise (Cody and Johnstone, 1982; Handrock and

Zeiberg, 1982; Rajan 1990; Liberman and Gao, 1995), auditory and visual attention

(Igarashi et al., 1974; Oatman, 1976; Scharf et al., 1994, 1997), and auditory

development (Walsh et al., 1998) and degeneration with age (Kim et al., 2002). Among

these hypotheses, the anti-masking effect has received the most extensive investigation,

Page 4: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

4

and probably has the strongest empirical support. This effect is most likely mediated

through MOC innervations to the outer hair cells (OHCs).

Otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) are thought to be by-products of cochlear active

mechanisms, i.e., the motility of OHCs (Kemp, 1978; Brownell et al., 1985). Since Buño

(1978) and Murata’s work (1980) showing that acoustical stimulation of one cochlea can

modify the firing of afferent fibers in the contralateral cochlea, experiments have

indicated the feasibility of studying the MOC’s activity non-invasively by presenting

contralateral stimulation during OAE recordings (Littman et al., 1992; Williams et al.,

1994; Maison et al., 1997; Micheyl and Collet, 1996).

Recently, Kim et al. (2002) demonstrated that the function of the MOC efferent

system declines with age in human listeners with normal audiometric thresholds. MOC

efferent strength was measured by contralateral suppression (CS) of distortion product

otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) with a wideband noise. They found that the CS declines

at an earlier age than the age-dependent decrease in DPOAE amplitudes.

It has long been known that the elderly with and without hearing loss have more

difficulty in understanding speech than young listeners, especially in background noise

(CHABA, 1988). However, the exact relationship between speech recognition

performance and chronological age has not yet been determined, due to the combined

effects of peripheral hearing loss and age-related changes in the brain. Recently Kim et

al. (2003) studied the effect of age on binaural speech intelligibility in noise in normal

hearing subjects, using HINT (Hearing in Noise Test). Their findings suggested that age

degrades speech intelligibility in both quiet and noise. In addition, benefit from spatial

separation of speech and noise, i.e., spatial release from masking (RFM), declined with

Page 5: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

5

age. This RFM is probably one brainstem auditory system mechanism that contributes to

the “cocktail party effect” as one aspect of sound source determination.

The purpose of the present investigation was to evaluate the MOC auditory

efferent involvement in speech intelligibility in noise and spatial RFM, in normal hearing

human listeners of different ages.

II. METHODS

Subjects

This study was performed with 25 subjects, 18 to 75 years old. Table 1

summarizes the ages of the subjects who were classified as young (16 to 30 years old),

middle aged (38 to 52 years old) and old (greater than 60 years old). Their otological

histories indicated that they were clear of factors such as drug ototoxicity, long-term

noise exposure, or ear infections. A battery of hearing tests was completed in order to

establish integrity of their auditory systems. Pure tone audiometry was performed in a

sound-proof room, using a Grason-Stadler GSI 61 clinical audiometer, at frequencies

between 0.25 kHz and 8 kHz (0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 6, and 8 kHz). All subjects had pure tone

thresholds of 20 dB HL or better for standard audiometric frequencies up to 4 kHz (Fig.

1). They showed symmetric hearing within 10 dB. The target ear for assessing MOC

function was the better ear as determined by pure tone audiometry.

Page 6: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

6

Assessment of the MOC system function

The strength of the MOC auditory efferent system was evaluated by CS,

according to the method utilized by Kim et al. (2002). CS was calculated by subtracting

the DPOAE amplitude without noise from those with contralateral wide band noise

(WBN). The reduction in DPOAE amplitude due to the presence of contralateral noise

(CS) was presented as negative value.

All DPOAEs were recorded using an ILO 92 Otodynamics Ltd. Analyzer.

Throughout the measurements, the ratio of f2/f1 was fixed at 1.22. The stimulus levels

were held constant, at L1=75 dB SPL and L2=65 dB SPL. The 2f1-f2 DPOAE amplitude

as a function of frequency was recorded at four points per octave to obtain a wideband

response in the 1 to 6-kHz range. DPOAE amplitude was measured for each frequency.

Contralateral acoustic stimulation was a 30 dB SL wideband noise, which was generated

by a Grason-Stadler GSI 61 clinical audiometer and applied via a 3A insert ear phone.

All measurements were done in an IAC sound-proof room with the subject seated

in an armchair comfortably and relaxed throughout the test session, which lasted for

approximately 30 min. Each test session consisted of three initial DPOAE measurements

without noise followed by three measurements with contralateral WBN exposures at 30

dB SL. WBN was presented 15 seconds prior to the beginning of the DPOAE stimulus

and continued until the DPOAE measurement was completed.

Assessment of the speech intelligibility

Page 7: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

7

To assess speech intelligibility, we performed the hearing in noise test (HINT).

HINT (Nilsson et al., 1994) was developed to provide a reliable and efficient measure of

speech reception thresholds for sentences (sSRT). Also, the free field and background

noise conditions of HINT provide an opportunity to get closer to real-life listening

situations.

Speech materials (sentences) were always presented at 0° azimuth. Sentence lists

were presented in the following conditions according to HINT instructions: (1) speech in

65 dB(A) noise at 0° azimuth (HINT N0), (2) speech in 65 dB(A) noise at 90° azimuth

(HINT N90), (3) speech in 65 dB(A) noise at 270° azimuth (HINT N270). The subject

was seated approximately 1 meter equidistant from three loudspeakers in a double-walled

sound booth.

An adaptive procedure (Levitt, 1971) without feedback was used to determine the

50% point on the psychometric function required for speech recognition thresholds. The

beginning intensity level of speech was 61 dB(A) and the noise channel was turned on

and remained at 65 dB(A). Noise onset preceded each sentence by 1 second and was

turned off 1 second after each sentence was completed. The first sentence in each list of

sentences was repeated at increased levels until identified correctly. The intensity level of

speech then was decreased by 4 dB and the second sentence presented. Stimulus level

was raised (incorrect response) or lowered (correct response) by 4 dB after subject’s

responses to the second and third sentences. The step size was reduced to 2 dB after three

sentences, and a simple up-down stepping rule was continued for the remaining 17

sentences. The calculation of the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) for 50 percent sentence

Page 8: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

8

recognition was based on averaging the presentation levels of sentences 4 through 20 for

the test list.

In this study, we evaluated speech intelligibility in noise using SNR of HINT N0

and RFM using an average SNR improvement of the HINT N90 and HINT N270

conditions relative to the HINT N0 condition.

All methods have been approved by our human subjects institutional review

boards and have been conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of

Helsinki.

Data Analyses

From the DP-gram, CS was obtained for the 1-6 kHz frequency range for f2. To

evaluate the effect of different frequency ranges, we arbitrarily divided the full frequency

range into 1-2 kHz and 4-6 kHz frequency ranges. To reveal the MOC auditory efferent

functional involvement in the cocktail party effect, different frequency ranges of CSs

were compared with HINT N0 and RFM using correlation analysis. To evaluate the age

effect on speech understanding in noise, age was also made comparisons between the

HINT N0 and RFM.

III. RESULTS

Table 2 showed average CS in the different frequency ranges for each age group.

And Table 3 showed the results of speech intelligibility of HINT N0 and speech

intelligibility gain when speech and noise were spatially separated (RFM).

Page 9: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

9

The average CS in the higher frequency (4-6 kHz) range is correlated with HINT

N0 (Pearson r=0.4295, p<0.05), as shown in Fig. 2. Note that the greater the strength of

the MOC efferent system, the more negative the value of the CS. A zero or positive CS

value means that there was no change or there was an enhancement of the DPOAE

amplitude, respectively, when the contralateral noise was applied. The relative amplitude

of the CS in the 4-6 kHz ranged from –1.97 to 1.62 dB. The SNR of HINT N0 ranged

from –0.8 to –4.4 dB. Every subject within normal hearing level performed 50 % correct

speech intelligibility at less than noise level, i.e., negative SNR. According to HINT

procedural conventions, the wideband noise was fixed at 65 dB(A). The speech reception

thresholds for sentences in the background noise at 0 degree azimuth ranged from 61.6 to

64.2 dB(A).

Fig. 3 shows that the average CS in the 1-2 kHz frequency range was correlated

with the RFM (Pearson r=-0.4455, p<0.05). The relative amplitude of CS in the1-2 kHz

bandwidth ranged from –1.99 to 0.94 dB. The RFM is the improvement of speech

intelligibility in noise resulting from the spatial separation of the noise speaker from the

speech speaker at N0. The range of RFM was found to be from 1.80 to 7.55 dB. The

larger the RFM, the greater the benefit in speech understanding in background noise

resulting from moving the noise source away from the speech source.

Age was found to be correlated with the HINT N0 thresholds and the RFM

measurements (Pearson r=0.5470, p<0.01, and Pearson r=0.4059, p<0.05, respectively).

Page 10: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

10

IV. DISCUSSION

Using the HINT test, speech intelligibility in noise was measured using an

adaptive procedure to determine the SNR for 50% intelligibility. The SNRs typically

obtained are in the range of –2 to -5 dB (Glasberg and Moore, 1989; Plomp, 1994) when

the background sound is a steady noise with the same long-term average spectrum of

speech (called speech-shaped noise). Even though SNR differences are not large in terms

of dB, a 1dB improvement of SNR improves speech intelligibility by 11% to 19 %

(Plomp and Mimpen, 1979; Laurence et al., 1983; Moore et al., 1992; Nilsson et al.,

1994).

The HINT N0 condition is one in which speech and noise are presented binaurally

with no interaural differences. In the present study it was found that the CS in the 4-6

kHz frequency range is statistically correlated with SNR for HINT N0. This finding

suggests that the higher frequency range of the MOC efferent system function is related

with speech processing when the background noise and speech come from similar

locations. In a real listening environment, though, it is hard to have identical sound

sources (distance and azimuth of the signal and noise).

Cherry (1953) coined the term “cocktail party effect” to describe the auditory

system’s ability to determine the sources of sounds in a multi-source acoustical

environment. In psychophysical terms, signal detection and speech perception improve

when the target and competing noise are spatially separated, thereby demonstrating the

advantage provided by binaural hearing. However not all the neural mechanisms for this

process are completely understood.

Page 11: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

11

Spatial RFM is the measured improvement of speech intelligibility when spatially

separating a signal from the background noise. The CS in the 1-2 kHz frequency range

was statistically correlated with this spatial RFM in the present investigation. This

finding suggests the importance of the lower frequency range (1-2 kHz) of the MOC

efferent system’s involvement in the cocktail party neural processors as they pertain to

binaural hearing.

The first clue regarding the possible role of centrifugal pathways in

discriminations of signals in noise came from the work of Dewson in 1968. He trained

monkeys to pick out vowel sounds such as ‘I’ and ‘U’ presented to the animal in the

presence of white noise. He then measured the threshold of the discrimination between

these sounds in noise. Following surgical section of the crossed olivo-cochlear bundle at

the midline on the floor of the fourth ventricle, Dewson found a rise in masked threshold

of up to 15 dB. There is a possiblility therefore that other centrifugal pathways may also

be involved in ‘signal in noise’ discriminations.

Nieder and Nieder (1970) were likely the first to coin the term ‘anti-masking’

when they observed that efferent stimulation significantly increased neural response to

loud click in noise. Comis (1973) studied influence of atropine effect on the cochlear

nucleus on detection of signals in noisy background. Micheyl and Collet (1996) found a

possible relationship between detection of tones in noise and the strength of efferent

activation, as measured by contralateral suppression of otoacoustic emissions (Littman et

al., 1992; Williams et al., 1994; Maison et al., 1997).

Similar to Dewson’s (1968) finding, Heinz et al., (1998) showed that vowel

formant discrimination in cats was adversely affected by bilateral efferent section only in

Page 12: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

12

high-level noise background but not at low noise levels. Giraud et al. (1997) were the

first clinical investigators to demonstrate efferent influences on human speech perception.

Of particular significance is the link they established between cochlear de-efferentation in

vestibular nerve neurectomized (VNT) patients and a loss of the ability to recognize

monosyllabic words in background noise. They showed that speech-in-noise

intelligibility is positively correlated with the strength of OAE suppression in normal

hearing humans. As a more direct test of the importance of efferent feedback in speech

processing, these investigators also found that unilateral VNT patients scored better in

speech-in-noise recognition tasks when stimuli were presented to the ear with intact

olivocochlear projections. Zeng et al., (2000) conducted behavioral studies in VNT

patients to evaluate anti-masking function proposed for the efferents. They found poorer

speech in noise recognition was observed in the surgery ear than the non-surgery ear, but

that finding was confounded by hearing loss. The present study using normal hearing

humans of different age shows that age-related difficulty understanding speech in

background noise is related to an age-related functional decline of the MOC efferent

system

Older adults are known to have significantly greater difficulty in recognizing

speech in the presence of background noise than young adults have (CHABA, 1988).

Although, in the main, sensorineural hearing loss contributes to the problems, absolute

pure tone thresholds are poor predictors for speech intelligibility in noise (Beattie et al.,

1997; Studebaker et al., 1997; Halling &Humes, 2000; Noorddhoek et al., 2001; Pittman

& Wiley, 2001). There are probably a number of age-related changes in both peripheral

Page 13: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

13

and central auditory processing contribute to this difficulty (Gordon-Salant S, 1987;

Humes LE, 1996; Frisina DR and Frisina RD, 1997).

The present study shows that age itself is one of the important factors to affect

speech understanding in noise, consistent with the previous study (Kim et al., 2003).

Subjects in the old group in this study were selected on the basis of their relatively good

hearing. This was done to rule out any significant peripheral hearing loss. For example,

less than 10 % of subjects who are over the age of 60 years have less than 25 dB HL from

0.25 to 8 kHz. Therefore, the poor performance of speech understanding in noise by the

elderly generally, must consider the possible effect of central presbycusis.

V. SUMMARY and CONCLUSIONS

It is well known that older individuals have more difficulty in understanding

speech in background noise than young adults. Interestingly, a recent study shows that

the function of the MOC efferent system declines with age (Kim et al., 2002). The

present study investigated the relationship between the MOC efferent system and speech

understanding in noise in normal hearing humans of different ages. Our findings suggest

that (1) age-related difficulties in understanding speech in background noise are related to

the age-related functional decline of the MOC efferent system, (2) the higher frequency

(4-6 kHz) range of the MOC efferent function is correlated with speech processing when

the background noise and speech come from similar spatial locations (HINT N0), and (3)

the 1-2 kHz frequency range of the MOC efferent system is correlated with spatial RFM

based on binaural processing, i.e., cocktail party processor neural mechanisms. In

conclusion, our findings suggest that the auditory MOC efferent system might function as

Page 14: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

14

a nonlinear adaptive filter during speech processing in background noise and also as a

cocktail party processor.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Prof. Jean Rouat, Prof. Jacqueline Walker, and Dr. Jiefu

Zheng for fruitful discussions and Dr. Arthur S. Hengerer for great support for study. The

work was supported by NIH Grant P01 AG09524 from the National Institute on Aging,

and the International Center for Hearing and Speech Research, Rochester NY, USA

VI. REFERENCES

Beattie, R.C. 1989. Word recognition functions for the CID W-22 test in multitalker

noise for normally hearing and hearing-impaired subjects. Journal of Speech & Hearing

Disorders. 54(1):20-32.

Brownell, W.E., Bader, C.R., Bertrand, D., de Ribeaupierre, Y. 1985. Evoked mechanical

responses of isolated cochlear hair cells. Science 227, 194-196.

Buño, W. 1978. Auditory nerve fiber activity influenced by contralateral ear sounc

stimulation. Experimental Neurology 59, 62-74.

Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics and Biomechanics (CHABA) 1988. Speech

understanding and aging. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 83, 859-820.

Cherry, C. 1953. Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two

ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25, 975-979.

Page 15: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

15

Cody, A.R., Johnstone, B.M. 1982. Temporary threshold shift modified by binaural

acoustic stimulation. Hearing Research 6, 199-206.

Comis, S.D. 1973. Detection of signals in noisy backgrounds: a role for centrifugal fibres.

J Laryngol Otol. 87(6):529-534

Cooke, M., Ellis, D. 2001. The auditory organization of speech and othersources in

listeners and computational models. Speech Communication 34, 267-285.

Dewson, J.H. 1968. Efferent olivocochlear bundle: some relationships to stimulus

discrimination in noise. J. Neurophysiol 31:122-130

Frisina, D.R., Frisina, R.D. 1997. Speech recognition in noise and presbycusis: relations

to possible neural mechanisms. Hearing Research 106, 95-104.

Giguere, G., Woodland. P.C. 1994. A computational model of the auditory periphery and

speech and hearing research. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 95, 331-349.

Giraud, A.L., Garnier, S., Micheyl, C., Lina, G., Chays, A., Chery-Croze, S. 1997.

Auditory efferents involved in speech-in-noise intelligibility. Neuroreport 8, 1779-1783.

Glasberg, B.R., Moore, B.C. 1989. Psychoacoustic abilities of subjects with unilateral

and bilateral cochlear hearing impairments and their relationship to the ability to

understand speech. Scand Audiology Supplement 32, 1-25.

Gordon-Salant S. 1987. Age-related differences in speech recognition performance as a

function of test format and paradigm. Ear and Hearing 8, 270-276.

Halling, D.C., Humes, L.E. 2000. Factors affecting the recognition of reverberant speech

by elderly listeners. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research 43, 414-431

Page 16: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

16

Handrock, M. Zeiberg, J. 1982. The influence of the efferent system on adaptation,

temporary and permanent threshold shift. Archives of Otorhinolaryngology 234, 191-195.

Heinz, R.D., Stiles, P., May, B.J. 1998. Effects of bilateral olivocohlear lesions on vowl

formant discrimination in cats. Hearing Research 116, 10-20.

Humes, L.E. 1996. Sppech understanding in the elderly. Journal of the American

Academy of Audiology 7, 161-167.

Igarashi, M., Alford, B.R., Gordon, W.P., Nakai, T. 1974. Behavioral auditory function

after transection of crossed olivocochlear bundle in the cat. Acta Otolaryngolica 73, 311-

317.

Kawase, T., Delgutte, B., Liberman, M.C. 1993a. Antimasking effects of the

olivocochlear reflex. II. Enhancement of auditory-nerve response to masked tones.

Journal of Neurophysiology 70(6), 2533-2549.

Kawase, T. Liberman, M.C. 1993b. Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex. I.

Enhancement of compound action potentials to masked tones. Journal of

Neurophysiology 70(6), 2519-2532.

Kemp, D.T. 1978. Stimulated acoustic emissions from within the human auditory system.

Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 64, 1386-1391.

Kim, S., Frisina, D.R., Frisina R.D. 2002. Effects of Age on Contralateral Suppression of

Distortion-Product Otoacoustic Emissions in Human Listeners with Normal Hearing.

Audiology and Neuro-otology 7, 348-357.

Page 17: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

17

Kim, S., Frisina, R.D., Mapes, F., Hickman, E., Frisina, D.R. 2003. Effect of age on

binaural sentence intelligibility in noise in normal hearing listners. ARO midwinter

meeting #727.

Laurence, R.F., Moore, B.C.J., & Glasberg, B.R. 1983. A comparison of behind-the-ear

high-fidelity linear aids and two-channel compression hearing aids in the laboratory and

everyday life. British Journal of Audiology 17, 31-48.

Levitt, H. 1971. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. Journal of the

Acoustical Society of America, 16, 331-353

Liberman, M.C. 1988. Response properties of cochlear efferent neurons: monaural vs.

binaural stimulation and the effects of noise. Journal of Neurophysiology 60, 1779-1798.

Liberman, M.C. Gao, W.Y. 1995. Chronic cochlear de-efferentation and susceptibility to

permanent acoustic injury. Hearing Research 90, 158-168.

Liberman, M.C. Guinan, J.J. 1998. Feedback control of the auditory periphery:

antimasking effects of middle ear muscles vs. olivocohelar efferents. Journal of

Communication Disorders 6, 471-482.

Littman, T.A., Cullen, J.K., Bobbin, R.P. 1992. The effect of olivocochlear bundle

transection on tuning curves and acoustic distortion products. Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America 92:1945-1952.

Maison, S., Micheyl, C., Chays, A., Collet, L. 1997. Medial olivocochlear system

stabilizes active cochlear micromechanical properties in humans. Hearing Research 113,

89-98.

Micheyl, C., Collet, L. 1996. Involvement of the olivocochlear bundle in the detection of

tones in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 99, 1604-1610.

Page 18: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

18

Micheyl, C., Morlet, T., Giraud, A.L., Collet, L., Morgon, A. 1995. Contralateral

suppression of evoked otoacoustic emissions and detection of a multi-tone complex in

noise. Acta Otolaryngolica 14, 6992-7007.

Moore, B.C.J., Lynch, C., Stone, M.A. 1992. Effects of fitting parameters of a two-

channel compression system on the intelligibility of speech in quiet and in noise. British

Journal of Audioloty, 26, 369-379.

Murata, K., Tanahashi, T., Horikawa, J., Funai, H. 1980. Mechanical and neural

interactions between binaurally applied sounds at cat cochlear nerve fibers. Neuroscience

Letters 18, 289-294.

Nieder, P., Nieder, I. 1970. Antimasking effect of crossed olivocochlear bundle

stimulation with loud clicks in guineapig. Exp Neurol 28:179-188

Nilsson, M., Soli, S.D., Sullivan, J.A. 1994. Development of the hearing in noise test for

the measurement of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. Journal of the

Acoustical Society of America 95, 1085-1099.

Noordhoek, I.M., Houtgast, T., Festen, J.M. 2001. Relations between intelligibility of

narrow-band speech and auditory functions, both in the 1-kHz frequency region. Journal

of the Acoustical Society of America 109, 1197-1212.

Oatman, I.C. 1976. Effects of visual attention on the intensity of auditory evoked

potentials. Experimental Neurology 51, 41-53.

Pittman, A.L., Wiley, T.L. 2001. Recognition of speech produced in noise. J of speech

language and hearing Research 44, 487-96.

Page 19: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

19

Plomp, R. 1994. Comments on "Evaluating a speech-reception threshold model for

hearing-impaired listeners" [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 93, 2879-2885 (1993)] Journal of the

Acoustical Society of America. 96, 586-589

Plomp, R., Mimpen, A.M. 1979. Improving the reliability of testing the speech-reception

threshold for sentences. Audiology 18, 43-52.

Rajan, J. 1990. Functions of the efferent pathways to the mammalian cochlea.In: Rowe M

and Aitkin L. (Eds.), Information processing in Mammalian Auditory and Tactile

Systems. Alan R. Liss, New York, pp 81-96.

Rouat, J. Pichevar, R. 2002. Nonlinear speech processing techniques for source

segregation. In EUSIPCO2002.

Scharf, B., Magnan, J., Collet, L., Ulmer, E., Chays, A. 1994. On the role of the

olivocochlear bundle in hearing: a case study. Hearing Research 75, 11-26.

Scharf, B., Magnan, J., Chays, A. 1997. On the role of the olivocochlear bundle in

hearing: 16 case studies. Hearing Research 103, 101-122.

Studebaker, G.A., Sherbecoe, R.L., McDaniel, D.M., Gray, G.A. 1997. Age-related

changes in monosyllabic word recognition performance when audibility is held constant.

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 8, 150-162.

Walsh, E.J., McGee, J., McFadden, S.L., Liberman, M.C. 1998. Long-term effects of

sectioning th olivocochlear bundle in neonatal cats. Journal of Neuroscience 18, 3859-

3869.

Williams, E.A., Brookes, G.B., Prasher, D.K. 1994. Effects of olivocochlear bundle

section on otoacoustic emissions in humans: efferent effects in comparison with control

subjects. Acta Otolaryngolica 114, 121-129.

Page 20: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

20

Winslow, R.L., Sachs, M.B. 1987. Effect of electrical stimulation of the crossed

olivocochlear bundle on auditory nerve response to tone in noise. Journal of

Neurophysiology 57, 1002-1021.

Yost, W.A., 1997. The cocktail party problem: Forty years later, In: Gilkey, R., Anderson,

T.R. (Eds.), Binaural and Spatial Hearing in Real and Virtual Environments. Lawrence

Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 329-348.

Zeng, F-G., Martino, K.M., Linthicum, F.H., Soli, S.D. 2000. Auditory perception in

vestibular neurectomy subjects. Hearing Research 142, 102-112.

Page 21: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

21

Table 1. Age and sex distribution and thresholds of pure tone average (0.5, 1, and 2 kHz)

for three age groups

Group Age range N (female/male)

PTA average of both ears for HINT

PTA average of the target ear for CS (Right/Left)

Young 18-27 9 (5/4) 3.87 ± 2.37 SD dB HL 3.78 ± 1.76 SD dB HL (4/5)Middle-aged 41-49 6 (3/3) 5.97 ± 2.60 SD dB HL 5.83 ± 2.30 SD dB HL (4/2)Old 62-75 10 (5/5) 9.23 ± 4.02 SD dB HL 7.80 ± 3.76 SD dB HL (5/5)

Table 2. The results of contralateral suppression (CS) for three age groups (unit: dB).

Group CS in 1-6 kHz of f2 CS in 1-2 kHz of f2 CS in 4-6 kHz of f2 Young -0.82 ± 0.61 SD -1.01 ± 0.48 SD -0.54 ±0.89 SD Middle-aged -0.11 ± 0.13 SD * 0.08 ± 0.35 SD * -0.16 ± 0.59 SD Old 0.03 ± 0.52 SD ** -0.28 ±0.85 SD 0.40 ± 0.75 SD * One-way ANOVA F(2,22)= 7.51, p<0.01 F(2,22)=5.85, p<0.01 F(2,22)=3.63, p<0.05

Indicate, statistically significant effect:

* post-hoc t-test, p<0.05, re: young, ** post-hoc t-test, p<0.01, re: young.

Table 3. The results of speech intelligibility in HINT N0 and spatial RFM (unit: dB)

Group HINT N0 (SNR) RFM Young -2.67 ± 0.87 SD 5.14 ± 1.21 SD Middle-aged -2.78 ± 0.52 SD 4.23 ± 0.56 SD Old -1.76 ± 0.76 SD * 3.79 ± 1.57 SD One-way ANOVA F(2,22)=4.81, p<0.05 ns

Indicate, statistically significant effect:

* post-hoc t-test, p<0.05, re: young, ns; not significant

Page 22: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

22

Pure Tone Audiogram

Frequency (Hz)

.25 K .5 K 1 K 2 K 3 K 4 K 6 K 8 K

dB

HL

0

20

40

60

80

100

Young GroupMiddle Aged GroupOld Group

Fig. 1. Audiometric thresholds averaged for the right and left ears of the young adult

(black circles), middle-aged (grey circles) and old (open circles) groups. Error bars

indicate standard deviations. HL – dB of hearing level, K – kHz.

Page 23: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

23

CS of DPOAE at 4-6 kHz frequency range of F2

-3 -2 -1 0 1 2

HIN

T N

0 (d

B S

NR

)

-5

-4

-3

-2

-1

0

Young groupMiddle-aged group Old group

Fig. 2. Correlation between the speech intelligibility in noise at 0 degree azimuth, SNR of

HINT N0º, and CS of DPOAEs in the 4-6 kHz frequency range (f2). Pearson r=0.4295,

p<0.05. CS – contralateral suppression, DPOAE – distortion product otoacoustic

emission, SNR – signal-to-noise ratio.

Page 24: Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing … · 2004. 1. 26. · 1 Effects of Age on Speech Understanding in Normal Hearing Listeners: Relationships Between the Auditory

24

CS of DPOAE on 1-2 kHz frequency range of F2

-2.5 -2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5

RF

M (

dB

)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

Young groupMiddle-aged groupOld group

Fig. 3. Correlation between the spatial RFM and CS of DPOAE at 1-2 kHz frequency

range of f2. Pearson r=-0.4455, p<0.05. CS – contralateral suppression, DPOAE –

distortion product otoacoustic emission, RFM – release from masking.