Effect of Long-term Outdoor Air Pollution and Noise on Cognitive And

11
 Int ern ati ona l Jou rnal of Hygien e andEnvir onmental Health218 (2015) 1–11 Con tents lists available at ScienceDirect International Journalof Hygieneand Env iron menta l Health  j ourna l h ome p a g e: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijheh Review Effectof long-termoutdoorairpollutionandnoiseoncognitiveand psychologicalfunctionsinadults LilianTzivian a,,AngelaWinkler b , Marth aDlugaj b ,TamaraSchikowski a,d,e , MohammadVossoughi a ,KaterynaFuks a ,GudrunWeinmayr a ,BarbaraHoffmann a,c a IUF- Leibni z Resea rch Insti tute for Enviro nment al Medicin e, Düss eldorf, Germa ny b Depar tment of Neuro logy, Univer sity Hospi tal of Essen , Univer sity of Duisb urg-Es sen, Essen , Germany c Medica l Facul ty, Heinric h Heine Univer sity of Düsse ldorf, Düsse ldorf, Germa ny d Swiss Tropic al and Public Healt h Instit ute, Basel , Switz erland e Univer sity of Basel , Basel , Switz erland articleinfo  Article history: Rec eived 2 June 2014 Rec eived in rev ise d for m 20 Augu st 2014 Acc ept ed 26 August 2014 Keywords: Cogni tive funct ion Depression Dementia Air pollut ion Noise Part iculat e matte r abstract Ithasbeenhypothesizedthatairpollutionandambientnoisemightimpactneurocognitivefunction.Early studiesmostlyinvestigatedtheassociationsof airpollutionandambientnoiseexposurewithcognitive developmentinchildren.Morerecently,severalstudiesinvestigatingassociationswith neuroc ogniti ve function,mooddisorders,andneurodegenerativediseaseinadultpopulationswerepublished,yielding inconsistentresults.Thepurposeof thisreviewistosummarizethecurrentevidenceonairpollutionand noise effec tsonmentalhealthin adu lts. Weincludedstudiesinadultpopulations(18yearsold)pub- lishedinEnglishlanguageinpeer-reviewed journals.Fifteenarticlesrelatedtolong-termeffectsof air pollutionandeightarticlesonlong-termeffectsof ambientnoisewereextracted.Bothexposureswere separatelyshowntobeassociatedwithoneorseveralmeasuresof globalcognitivefunction,verbaland nonverballearningandmemory,activitiesof dailyliving,depressivesymptoms,elevatedanxiety,and nuisance.Nostudyconsideredbothexposuressimultaneouslyand fewstudiesinvestigatedprogression of neurocognitivedeclineorpsychologicalfactors.Theexistingevidencegenerallysupportsassociations of environmentalfactorswithmentalhealth,butdoesnotsufceforanoverallconclusionaboutthe independenteffectof air pollut ionandnoise.Thereisa needforstudiesinvestigatingsimultaneouslyair pollutionandnoiseexposuresinassociationmentalhealth,forlongitudinalstudiestocorroboratend- ingsfromcross-sectional analyses,and for paralleltoxicologicalandepidemiologicalstudiestoelucidate mechanismsandpathwaysof action. ©2014ElsevierGmbH.Allrightsreserved. Introduction Whi le the ass oc iat ion s of air pol lut ion on car dio vas cul ar fun c- tion and dis ease ha ve be en s hown i n m any st u di es (Br ook et al., 201 0; R ück le r e t a l. , 201 1 ), compar at ivel y little is known about th e ef fect of long-term ai r poll ut ion on neurocogni ti ve func ti on, ev en t ho u gh s us t ai n ed n eu r oc og ni ti ve func ti on at ol d er ag e is a prominent el ement of heal thy ag ei ng an d wi ll be one of th e ma in ch al leng es in our ag ei ng societies. The rst link between ambient air pollu tionand neurotoxic ity was madeby Calderón-Garcidue˜ nas et al . (2 00 2) . Th is wo rk was ba sed on a hi st ol o gi c al c om pa ri so n of br ai n ti ssue from dogs , li vi ng in hi gh - and low- pollution ar eas, respec tiv ely. Dogs fro m hig hlypollu tedareas ha d more damages to Corre spon ding autho r. Tel.: +49 1522 9412016. E-mail addres s: [email protected] (L. Tzivian). theblood–br ainbarrier,degener atio n of cort icalneurons , apopt otic gli al white cells, non -neuritic pla que s and neu robri lla ry tan gle s compar ed to dog s fro m low-po lluted are as. Sin ce the n, sev era l epi - demiol ogi ca l and tox ico log ical stu die s have invest iga ted chronic eff ect s of air pol lut ion and ambien t noise on neu roc ogn iti ve fun c- tio n in chi ldr en and adu lts . Most of the epidemiologi cal st udies invest igated wh et her th e ef fect of ai r po ll ut ion was rela te d to th e deve lo pmen t of co gn i- ti on duri ng c hi ld ho od an d po ss i bl e c ha n ge s of it in ad ol e sc en ce (Edwards et al., 2010; Freire et al., 2010; Perera et al., 2008; Si ddique et al ., 2011 ). Invest igations on the ef fect of air poll ution on neurocognitive function in adults are still rare. In a recent review, Gux ens and Sun yer (20 12) summari zed studie s on neuro- psyc holo gica l effec ts of air poll ution, incl udin g 12 chil dren stud ies, and four studies of adult popula ti ons of wh ich one concentrated on sh or t- te rm air pol lut ion ef fe cts . In th emost cur r ent re vie w (Block et al., 2012), six st ud ies were related to ne ur oc og ni ti ve http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijheh.2014.08.002 1438-4 639/© 2014 Else vier GmbH. All right s rese rved.

description

Eng

Transcript of Effect of Long-term Outdoor Air Pollution and Noise on Cognitive And

  • International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    International Journal of Hygiene andEnvironmental Health

    jo u r n al homepage: www.elsev ier .com/ locate / i jheh

    Review

    Effect o oispsycho

    Lilian Tzi SchMohamm yra,a IUF-Leibniz Rb Department o yc Medical Faculd Swiss Tropicae University of

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:Received 2 June 2014Received in revised form 20 August 2014Accepted 26 August 2014

    Keywords:Cognitive funcDepressionDementiaAir pollutionNoiseParticulate ma

    a b s t r a c t

    It has been hypothesized that air pollution and ambient noise might impact neurocognitive function. Earlystudies mostly investigated the associations of air pollution and ambient noise exposure with cognitivedevelopment in children. More recently, several studies investigating associations with neurocognitive

    Introductio

    While thtion and di2010; Rckthe effect oeven thougprominent challenges air pollutionet al. (2002of brain tissrespectively

    CorresponE-mail add

    http://dx.doi.o1438-4639/ tion

    tter

    function, mood disorders, and neurodegenerative disease in adult populations were published, yieldinginconsistent results. The purpose of this review is to summarize the current evidence on air pollution andnoise effects on mental health in adults. We included studies in adult populations (18 years old) pub-lished in English language in peer-reviewed journals. Fifteen articles related to long-term effects of airpollution and eight articles on long-term effects of ambient noise were extracted. Both exposures wereseparately shown to be associated with one or several measures of global cognitive function, verbal andnonverbal learning and memory, activities of daily living, depressive symptoms, elevated anxiety, andnuisance. No study considered both exposures simultaneously and few studies investigated progressionof neurocognitive decline or psychological factors. The existing evidence generally supports associationsof environmental factors with mental health, but does not sufce for an overall conclusion about theindependent effect of air pollution and noise. There is a need for studies investigating simultaneously airpollution and noise exposures in association mental health, for longitudinal studies to corroborate nd-ings from cross-sectional analyses, and for parallel toxicological and epidemiological studies to elucidatemechanisms and pathways of action.

    2014 Elsevier GmbH. All rights reserved.

    n

    e associations of air pollution on cardiovascular func-sease have been shown in many studies (Brook et al.,ler et al., 2011), comparatively little is known aboutf long-term air pollution on neurocognitive function,h sustained neurocognitive function at older age is aelement of healthy ageing and will be one of the mainin our ageing societies. The rst link between ambient

    and neurotoxicity was made by Caldern-Garciduenas). This work was based on a histological comparisonue from dogs, living in high- and low-pollution areas,. Dogs from highly polluted areas had more damages to

    ding author. Tel.: +49 1522 9412016.ress: [email protected] (L. Tzivian).

    the bloodbrain barrier, degeneration of cortical neurons, apoptoticglial white cells, non-neuritic plaques and neurobrillary tanglescompared to dogs from low-polluted areas. Since then, several epi-demiological and toxicological studies have investigated chroniceffects of air pollution and ambient noise on neurocognitive func-tion in children and adults.

    Most of the epidemiological studies investigated whether theeffect of air pollution was related to the development of cogni-tion during childhood and possible changes of it in adolescence(Edwards et al., 2010; Freire et al., 2010; Perera et al., 2008;Siddique et al., 2011). Investigations on the effect of air pollutionon neurocognitive function in adults are still rare. In a recentreview, Guxens and Sunyer (2012) summarized studies on neuro-psychological effects of air pollution, including 12 children studies,and four studies of adult populations of which one concentratedon short-term air pollution effects. In the most current review(Block et al., 2012), six studies were related to neurocognitive

    rg/10.1016/j.ijheh.2014.08.0022014 Elsevier GmbH. All rights reserved.f long-term outdoor air pollution and nlogical functions in adults

    viana,, Angela Winklerb, Martha Dlugajb, Tamaraad Vossoughia, Kateryna Fuksa, Gudrun Weinma

    esearch Institute for Environmental Medicine, Dsseldorf, Germanyf Neurology, University Hospital of Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germanty, Heinrich Heine University of Dsseldorf, Dsseldorf, Germanyl and Public Health Institute, Basel, SwitzerlandBasel, Basel, Switzerlande on cognitive and

    ikowskia,d,e, Barbara Hoffmanna,c

  • 2 L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    and psychological functions of adults. These reviews hypothesizeda potential role of air pollution on white matter lesions andcerebrovascular pathology in adults that can lead to cognitivechanges in adults. A role of PM in cognitive decits and behaviorimpairmen2012; Guxe

    The assoless investinoise and n(Elmenhorseffect on irSalame andof the studibe related tand lead toVermeer an

    Knowledneurocognihow particuhypothesizevates pro-incausing an iInammatolation and Hirano et aties in the a

    The secovous systemPathologicastages of Al(UFP) wererons after e2008). Whiexposure adescribing paffecting ne

    Recentlyrocognitiveand Calderoparticipantsations withnoise levelmay have cgistic effectdate whicheffects on nreview the and ambienneurocognidisease in a

    Methods of

    We perfambient airnitive functadults usinScholar. Losure duringdays or hoFor this seadisease, anfunction, coimpairmendisease, pe

    noise exposure, particulate matter, particul*, ultrane particles,ne PM, trafc, alone and in combination. The exact searchhistory is available on request. An additional search was per-formed based on the references of the retrieved articles meeting

    lusio relares s (lish

    this .

    s

    liters werf air pient

    and Persthe 2Franius ere w

    andeffect noi

    esign

    e stues, oes inontroius epeat

    studerforere estudyrform. In tcted n (Ra

    on r et aeighon nrformcon

    2). ctiveda an

    asth

    re as

    g-terting f theckerre, tlongtternt in children and elderly was also proposed (Block et al.,ns and Sunyer, 2012).ciation of noise with neurocognitive function is evengated. Most studies on association between ambienteurocognitive functions measured short-term effectst et al., 2010; Schapkin et al., 2006) or investigated therelevant speech in adults (Clark and Srqvist, 2012;

    Baddeley, 1982; Smith and Broadbent, 1992). In mostes, occupational and environmental noise was found too annoyance that can disturb normal human activities

    somatic and psychosomatic health effects (Passchier-d Passchier, 2000).ge about biological pathways linking air pollution totive impairment is still scarce. Two possible pathways oflate matter PM can affect neurocognitive function wered (Genc et al., 2012). Respiratory intake of PM acti-ammatory cytokines in human macrophages, therebynammatory response and subsequent oxidative stress.ry compounds can spill over into the systemic circu-penetrate the bloodbrain barrier (Genc et al., 2012;l., 2003). This leads to different histological abnormali-nimal brain (Nemmar et al., 2001).nd pathway is related to direct entry of PM into the ner-

    through the olfactory bulb following nasal inhalation.l changes in the olfactory bulb were observed in earlyzheimers disease (AD) (Doty, 2008). Ultrane particles

    observed in human olfactory bulb periglomerular neu-xposure to air pollution (Caldern-Garciduenas et al.,le these pathways pertaining to long-term air pollutionre a major research subject, there is a lack of studiesossible pathways of long-term ambient noise exposureurocognitive functions., further epidemiological studies on long-term neu-

    effects of air pollution and noise were published (Blockn-Garciduenas, 2009). These studies investigated adult, used multiple cognitive tests and explored associ-

    various air pollutants and a wide range of ambients. As air pollution and ambient noise in many casesommon sources, such as trafc, they may have syner-s on neurocognitive function. There are no reviews to

    integrate the studies on both air pollution and noiseeurocognitive function. The purpose of this article is toup-to-date articles investigating effects of air pollutiont noise on different aspects of mental health, namelytive function, mood disorders and neurodegenerativedult populations.

    the search

    ormed a literature search of articles relating long-term pollution and ambient noise with mental health (cog-ions, neurocognitive diseases and mood disorders) ing the two major search engines PubMed and Googleng-term exposure was dened as the average expo-

    at least one year. Short-term exposures during theurs before outcome assessment were not included.rch, the following keywords were used: Alzheimersxiety, central nervous system, cognition, cognitivegnitive decline, depression, dementia, mild cognitive

    t, mood disorders, neurocognitive effect, Parkinsonsripheral nervous system, vertigo, air pollution, noise,

    the incstudiesexposuulationin Engfrom effects

    Result

    Thearticletions oof ambordersstudy (Six of 2013; Wellentherefo(2012)nitive aircraf

    Study d

    NinanalysanalyscasecWellenwith reIn thewere p187 wIn the was pevalue)conduwomeformed(Powe

    Of noise ies pea case(Tableprospe(Yoshi2007),gated.

    Exposu

    Lonestimatime oup (Dotherefoin the use pan criteria. Inclusion criteria for the articles were: (1)ted to the effect of air pollution and ambient noiseon mental health; (2) studies related to adult pop-18 years old); (3) epidemiologic studies publishedlanguage in peer-reviewed journals. We excludedreview ecologic studies and studies on short-term

    ature search was nished in November 2013. In total, 22e retrieved, of which 14 investigated long-term associa-ollution, and seven investigated long-term associationsnoise exposure on neurocognitive functions, mood dis-neurodegenerative disease in adults. Additionally, oneson et al., 2007) considered both, air pollution and noise.2 reviewed studies (Bocquier et al., 2013; Gatto et al.,ssen et al., 2013; Loop et al., 2013; Power et al., 2013;t al., 2012) were published within the last 2 years andere not included in the reviews of Guxens and Sunyer

    Block et al. (2012); four of them analyzed neurocog-ts of air pollution, and twothe effects of trafc andse.

    and exposures******

    dies related to air pollution conducted cross-sectionalf those six studies were carried out as cross-sectional

    ongoing cohort studies; two studies conductedl analysis (Table 1). In three studies (Lim et al., 2012;t al., 2012; Weuve et al., 2012), follow-up examinationsed assessments of cognitive function were performed.y of Lim et al. (2012), three follow-up examinationsmed, each 3 years apart, but from 560 participants onlyxamined all the three times during the study period.

    by Wellenius et al. (2012), the follow-up examinationed approximately 16.8 months after baseline (median

    he study by Weuve et al. (2012), a reexamination wasafter 4.3 years. Two studies were performed only innft et al., 2009; Weuve et al., 2012) and two were per-the same study population which included only menl., 2011, 2013).t studies investigating the association of long-termeurocognitive function or mood disorders, four stud-ed cross-sectional analyses, two studies conductedtrol analysis; and two studies were cohort studiesOne of them (Bocquier et al., 2013) had a retro-

    design. One study was performed only in womend Osada, 1997). In one casecontrol study (Persson et al.,matic versus non-asthmatic participants were investi-

    sessment

    m air pollution exposure is mostly approximated byaverage exposure at the address of residence at the

    outcome assessment or during the years of follow-y et al., 1993). The exposure contrasts investigated are,he spatial patterns of air pollution concentration which

    run tend to be relatively stable due to similar land-s and long-term meteorology (Cesaroni et al., 2012;

  • L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111 3

    Table 1Main characteristics of epidemiological studies exploring the association of long-term air pollution on cognitive and psychological functions in adults, by alphabetical orderof authors.

    Authors, year ofpublication, country

    Type of analysis Study population N(age)

    Exposure Principal outcomes/tests Investigated cognitive andpsychological functions

    Caldern-Garciduenaset al., 2004, Mexico

    Cross-sectional N = 19 (51.2 4.9) PM, O3 Alzheimer like pathology; COX2and mRNA levels

    Histological examinations

    Caldern-Garciduenaset al., 2009, Mexico

    Cross-sectional N = 87 (21.0 2.6) PM2.510, PM2.5 University of Pennsylvania smellidentication test (UPSIT)

    Olfactory function

    Chen and Schwartz,2009, USA

    Cross-sectional N = 1764(37.4 10.9)

    PM10, O3 Simple reaction time test (SRTT),Symbol-digit substitution test(SDST), Serial-digit learning test(SDLT)

    Attention and informationprocessing speed, verbalmemory

    Finkelstein and Jerrett,2007, Canada

    Casecontrol N = 52,986 (notavailable)

    NO2, Ambient Mn l-DOPA prescription; physiciansdiagnosis of Parkinson disease (PD)

    Gatto et al., 2013, USA Cross-sectional N = 1496(60.5 8.1)

    O3, NO2, PM2.5 Boston Naming Test, Symbol digitmodalities test, Trail Making Test(TMT) A/B, Letter-numbersequencing, Animal naming,California verbal learning test,Paragraph recall, Faces, Blockdesign, Judgment of lineorientation

    Attention, informationprocessing speed,confrontation naming,executive functions, verbalmemory, nonverbalmemory, visuoconstruction

    Lim et al., 2012,Republic of Korea

    Longitudinalfollow-up 3 years

    N = 357 (71.0 5.0) PM10, CO, SO2, NO2,O3

    Korean version of the Geriatricdepression scale-short form(SGDK-K)

    Depressive symptoms

    Loop et al., 2013, USA Cross-sectional N = 20,150(64.0 9.2)

    PM2.5 Incidence of cognitivedeclineSix-Item Screener

    Persson et al., 2007,Sweden

    Casecontrol N = 22,693(43.0 13.0)

    NOx, Woodburning; industrialsmells

    Psychic trait anxiety score Anxiety

    Power et al., 2011,USA

    Cross-sectional N = 680 (71.0 7.0) BC Mini-Mental State Examination(MMSE), Pattern comparison task,Digit span backward test,Long-term verbal immediate recall(IR) and delayed recall (DR),Constructional praxis

    Attention, workingmemory, long-term verbalmemory(immediate/delayed recall,consolidation andrecognition),visuoconstruction, globalcognition

    Power et al., 2013, USA Cross-sectional N = 629 (70.0 7.1) BC MMSE, Pattern comparison task,Digit span backward test,Long-term verbal IR and DR,Constructional praxis

    Attention, workingmemory, long-term verbalmemory,visuoconstruction, globalcognition

    Ranft et al., 2009,Germany

    Cross-sectional N = 399 (74.1 2.6) PM10 Neuropsychological CERAD-Plusbattery: MMSE, TMT, Stroopcolor-word test, long-term verbalIR and DR, Constructional praxis,DR of gures

    Attention, informationprocessing speed,confrontation naming,executive functions, verbalmemory, nonverbalmemory,visuoconstruction, globalcognition, depression,olfactory function

    Sun and Gu, 2008,China

    Cross-sectional N = 7358(83.6 11.4)

    API** MMSE; Activity of daily living(ADL); Self-reported health

    Global cognition,satisfaction with health,ability to perform everydayactivities

    Wellenius et al., 2012,USA

    Cohort follow-up(median), 16.8years

    N = 765 (78.1 5.4) Proximity tonearest road, BC

    MMSE; Hopkins verbal learningtest-revised (HVLT-R); TMT;Clock-in-the-box test (CIB);Category and letter uency tests

    Global cognition,long-term verbal memory,attention and informationprocessing speed,executive functions,cognitive exibility,divided attention

    Weuve et al., 2012, USA Cohort follow-up4.3 years

    N = 10,409(74.0 2.2)

    PM10; PM2.5,Coarse PM

    Telephone interview for cognitivestatus (TICS); 10-Words DR; EastBoston memory test (EBMT);Category uency; Digit spanbackward test, modied MMSE

    Global cognition, long termverbal memory, executivefunctions

    Zeng et al., 2010, China Cohort follow-up 7years

    N = 15,873 (86.3) API** ADL; MMSE; cumulative decitsindex (DI); telephone interview forcognitive status, Self-reportedhealth

    Global cognition, ability toperform everydayactivities, satisfaction withhealth

    *Additionally other environmental factors were measured.**Air pollution index that includes SO2, NO2, PM10, CO, O3.

  • 4 L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    Table 2Main characteristics of epidemiological studies exploring the association of ambient noise on cognitive and psychological functions in adults.

    Authors, year of publication,country

    Type of analysis Study population N Exposures Principal outcomes/tests Investigated functions

    Bocquier et 6h#

    Brand et al., oise

    Brink, 2011, fc;; aircr

    Chiovenda e vel fo

    Franssen et LAEQ, 2, 22-23

    Hardoy et al noise

    Persson et a soundrs; traentila

    Yoshida and

    *Restricted to # LAEQequiva## LDENday, e

    Wang et al.tory of parinto accounstudies (Caviduals fromin low-pollZeng et al.,low, mediuair pollutioDatabase.

    Eleven sdifferent tecmeasured cand Schwar2009), threitoring statPersson et use regressof residentiet al., 2012;studies useroad or prosure to trafWellenius e

    Particulalate matter most frequethem was pet al., 2007from industthe studiespollution indioxide (SOand O3 was

    biens (Past cof th

    (Fraoise. al., 2013, France Retro-prospectivelongitudinal follow-up1 year

    N = 190,617(41.5 12.5)

    LAEQ, 22-

    2009, Switzerland Qualitative-quantitativecasecontrol

    N = 63 (56.4 16.0) Trafc n

    Switzerland Longitudinal follow-up5 years

    N > 8000 (45.2) Road trarailwaynoise

    t al., 2007, Italy Cross-sectional N = 81 (42.2 7.7) Noise le8 h/day

    al., 2013, Netherlands Cross-sectional N = 11,812 (>18.0) LDEN##;h#; LAEQ

    ., 2005, Italy Cross-sectional N = 355 (1875) Aircraft

    l., 2007, Sweden Casecontrol N = 22,693(43.0 13.0)

    LDEN##, neighbonoise; v

    Osada, 1997, Japan Cross-sectional N = 366 (20.0-60.0) LDEN##

    psychological outcomes.lent sounds levels calculated over the corresponding time period.vening, night equivalent sound level over 24 h.

    , 2013). In most studies, the individual residential his-ticipants before the outcome assessment is not takent. Four studies exploited contrasts between cities: twoldern-Garciduenas et al., 2004, 2009) compared indi-

    highly polluted cities with those from communitiesution areas. The two other studies (Sun and Gu, 2008;

    Amsourcethe moSeven studiescraft n 2010) compared participants living in the cities withm and high gross domestic products, using citywiden indices obtained from the Chinese Natural Resources

    tudies exploited within-city exposure contrasts usinghniques, alone or in combination. Four studies assignedoncentrations from the nearest monitoring site (Chentz, 2009; Lim et al., 2012; Loop et al., 2013; Ranft et al.,e studies interpolated residential exposure from mon-ions using GIS-based techniques (Gatto et al., 2013;al., 2007; Weuve et al., 2012), ve studies used land-ion models to estimate small-scale spatial contrastsal exposure (Finkelstein and Jerrett, 2007; Wellenius

    Power et al., 2011, 2013; Ranft et al., 2009) and threed trafc indicators such as proximity to the nearestximity to high trafc as markers of long-term expo-c-related air pollution (Finkelstein and Jerrett, 2007;t al., 2012, Ranft et al., 2009).te matter less than 2.5 m in diameter (PM2.5), particu-less than 10 m in diameter (PM10) and ozone (O3) werently investigated in the reviewed studieseach one ofresented in four different studies. One study (Persson) investigated exhaust fumes, wood burning and smellrial production without specifying the PM fractions. In

    of Sun and Gu (2008) and Zeng et al. (2010), an airdex (API) assessing combined concentrations of sulfur2), nitrogen dioxide (NO2), PM10, carbon monoxide (CO)

    used.

    from neighTrafc noismental noiet al., 2007,et al., 2009databases (surrogate mexposure mequivalent during the ies, noise l(Bocquier estudy (Chioofcers wercraft noise e2013; Hardaddressed tegories of 5mathemati

    Outcome as

    In mostogical testsdomains, sufunctions, wterm noisemostly useMean number ofanxiolytic-hypnotic purchases

    Migraine; headache; depressivedisorders; anxiety disorders*

    Emotional status

    aftSelf-reported health* Satisfaction with

    health

    r Digit span forward WAIS-R;attentive matrices; Digit spanbackward; arithmetic reasoning;Raven PM38; Stroop color-wordtest; State-trait anxietyinventory; prole of moodstates*

    Attention andinformationprocessing, executivefunctions, workingmemory, inhibition,verbal memory,emotional status

    3-7

    h#

    Self-reported healthquestionnaire (VOEG)*

    Satisfaction withhealth

    Generalized anxiety disorder(GAD); anxiety disorder nototherwise specied (NOS); majordepressive disorder; depressivedisorder NOS; eating disorder

    Emotional status

    fromfction

    Psychic Trait Anxiety score * Emotional status

    General nuisance, interferencewith daily activities,psychological symptoms*

    Satisfaction withhealth

    t chronic noise is the sound generated by environmentalsschier-Vermeer and Passchier, 2000). Road trafc wasommonly investigated environmental source of noise.e eight studies investigated road trafc noise and twonssen et al., 2013; Hardoy et al., 2005) investigated air-In one study (Franssen et al., 2013), the additional noise

    borhood and from ventilation systems was assessed.e assessment was performed using dedicated environ-se prediction models (Bocquier et al., 2013; Persson

    Yoshida and Osada, 1997), direct measurements (Brand), mandatory noise mapping available through ofcialBrink, 2011; Chiovenda et al., 2007), or by the use ofarkers such as occupation. Most commonly, the noiseetrics dayeveningnight noise level, dened as the

    sound level over 24 hLDEN (Leq(24 h)), or the noise levelnight period (226 h)LNIGHT were used. In three stud-evels were stratied for analysis to groups of 5 dB(A)t al., 2013; Brink, 2011; Yoshida and Osada, 1997). In onevenda et al., 2007), high road trafc noise exposed policee compared to low noise exposed ofce employees. Air-xposure for two cross-sectional studies (Franssen et al.,oy et al., 2005) was assessed by proximity of residentialo the nearest airport, and additionally stratied into cat-

    dB(A), derived from the National Aerospace Laboratorycal noise model (Franssen et al., 2013).

    sessment

    of the studies related to air pollution, neuropsychol- investigating the impairment of different cognitivech as working memory, verbal memory or executiveere used. Studies investigating the association of long-

    exposure with the central nervous system functionsd different assessment methods than studies on air

  • L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111 5

    pollution. Only two cognitive tests (Digit span test and Stroopcolor-word test) were used both in air pollution and noise stud-ies, making direct comparisons between air pollution and noiseeffects difcult. Few studies investigated mood disorders, clinicaldiagnosis o

    Most stusensitivity tMini-Menta2011, 20132012; Weu30-item selassessed wItem ScreenIn this test,orientation

    Among tion and infrequently. 2013; Ranftof targets hcorrectly peof attentionpattern comstitution tetest (Chen athe symbolattention anused to cheeffects is thtest assesse

    Anotherbal memorywords) thatpant (GattoWellenius etrial, the nuof verbal mlist, the parwords (delarecognitionshown to thously learnefor verbal iand needs tusing a storined using study of Cheassessed wiof eight num

    Some stufor nonverb2009) and frecall test ccommon rof the facessented, andrecognized ticipants.

    The exaoutcome innitive exibTrail Makinet al., 2012)in order witnumber of exibility a

    assessed using a semantic category test in which a high numberof items generated for the same category (for instance, animals)indicated a 2012; Weu

    phoe nure ofd w

    et aand tcase,rgansesset, thelor nk insred. ithm

    cognral st2013

    or eral ss nsis optionn-Gmatimonino anal sfactocatio

    ility -item

    cogew snal ance

    press009

    logicic Deinclutivity

    usinvssony-relontrxiet

    Hard Mea2013State

    ivity heck010, or o

    at letrast, addplaied.

    (Brn onf disease, medication intake or pathological ndings.dies used comprehensive, but unspecic tests with lowo assess global cognitive function. In seven studies, thel State Examination (MMSE) was used (Power et al.,; Ranft et al., 2009; Sun and Gu, 2008; Wellenius et al.,ve et al., 2012; Zeng et al., 2010), which consists of af-administered questionnaire, and which can also beith a telephone interview. One study applied the Six-er to assess global cognitive function (Loop et al., 2013).

    three items have to be recalled, and additionally the to year, month and day of the week was checked.more specic test of neurocognitive domains, atten-formation processing speed was investigated mostSeveral studies used the Trail Making Test A (Gatto et al.,

    et al., 2009; Wellenius et al., 2012), in which a numberave to be connected in the correct order. The number ofrformed connections during certain time was a marker

    and psychomotor speed. Other studies applied a test onparison (Power et al., 2011, 2013), a symbol-digit sub-

    st (Chen and Schwartz, 2009), a simple reaction timend Schwartz, 2009) and a complex attention test like

    digit modalities test (Gatto et al., 2013) to check thed memory of participants. An additional test that wasck participants attentive capacity in studies of noisee attentive matrices test (Chiovenda et al., 2007); thisd information processing as well.

    frequently tested domain of cognitive function is ver-. It was in most cases assessed using word lists (1016

    were presented once or several times to the partici- et al., 2013; Power et al., 2011, 2013; Ranft et al., 2009;t al., 2012; Weuve et al., 2012). After every learningmber of immediately recalled words serves as measureemory. After a delay and/or an additional interferenceticipants were asked to recall the previously learnedyed recall). Furthermore, most researchers included a

    trial at the end of the test in which a list of words wase participants, and asked which ones were on the previ-d list. One study (Gatto et al., 2013) examined memory

    nformation that exceeds the immediate memory spanhe contribution of meaning to retention and recall byy recall test. In this study comprehension was exam-recall questions and multiple-choice questions. In then and Schwartz (2009), short-term verbal memory wasth a serial-digit learning test by reproducing a sequencebers.dies additionally examined the memory performanceal material like previously drafted gures (Ranft et al.,aces (Gatto et al., 2013). Faces immediate and delayedonsisted of 16 colored pictures of faces together withst and family name were presented. Later recognition

    was checked using 12 faces from 16 that were pre- additional 12 distracted faces. The amount of correctlyfaces was a marker of visual episodic memory of par-

    mination of executive function was another main many studies, assessed with various specic tests. Cog-ility and divided attention were measured using theg Test B (Gatto et al., 2013; Ranft et al., 2009; Wellenius. In this test, a number of targets need to be connectedh numerous letter targets on alternating sequence. Thecorrectly performed connections measured cognitivend divided attention of participants. Verbal uency was

    using atest, thmeasuassesseWeuve2013) latter ning, owas asthis tesin a coblue inmeasuwas ar

    Thein seve2011, design

    Sevto assediagnoprescriCalderinamples. A42-amadditiothe olIdentipossibized 40in mild

    A fadditiodisturbsis. Deet al., 2demioGeriatrwhich and ac(2007)(Gustaanxiet

    In cined an2007; 1997).et al., Mood only.

    Actwere cet al., 2factorshavingin constudiescal comexaminstudiesliving ibetter performance (Gatto et al., 2013; Wellenius et al.,ve et al., 2012). Similarly, verbal uency was checkednematic category test (Wellenius et al., 2012). In thismber of words beginning with a given letter served as

    phonematic uency. Moreover, working memory wasith the digit span backwards (Power et al., 2011, 2013;l., 2012), a letternumber sequencing task (Gatto et al.,he Clock-in-the-Box test (Wellenius et al., 2012). In the

    not only examining working memory, but also plan-ization and visuoconstruction were tested. Inhibitiond with the Stroop color-word test (Ranft et al., 2009). In

    name of a color (e.g., blue, green or red) is printedot denoted by the name (e.g., the word red printed intead of red ink), and a reaction time to name a color isAn additional outcome used in studies of noise effectsetic reasoning (Chiovenda et al., 2007).itive domain of visual construction was also assessedudies (Gatto et al., 2013; Ranft et al., 2009; Power et al.,) using the Judgment of Line Orientation task, blockgure drawing (constructional praxis).studies used clinical diagnoses or pathological ndingservous system effects of air pollution. A physiciansf Parkinsons disease (PD) and related medication

    was investigated in Finkelstein and Jerrett (2007).arciduenas et al. (2004) investigated postmortem brainon and Alzheimer like pathology in autopsy brain sam-g others, cyclooxygenase-2 (COX2) expression and thecid form of -amyloid (A42) were examined. In antudy performed by Caldern-Garciduenas et al. (2009),ry functions using University of Pennsylvania Smelln Test (UPSIT) test were investigated as a marker of

    of AD. This test includes self-administrated standard-s, of which 10 items strongly predict conversion to AD

    nitive impairment (MCI) patients.tudies also investigated mood disorders, either asoutcomes or to statistically control for psychologicals in the air pollution-neurocognitive function analy-ion was assessed in two studies (Lim et al., 2012; Ranft). It was identied by application of the Center of Epi-al Studies Depression Scale (Ranft et al., 2009) or of thepression Scale-Short Form (SGDS-K) (Lim et al., 2012),des questions about general life attitude, satisfaction. Anxiety was investigated in the study of Persson et al.g the Swedish Universities Scales of Personality score

    et al., 2000) that summarizes positive answers to sevenated statements.ast to air pollution studies, most noise studies exam-y and depression (Bocquier et al., 2013; Chiovenda et al.,oy et al., 2005; Persson et al., 2007; Yoshida and Osada,n number of anxiolytic-hypnotic purchases (Bocquier), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Prole of

    (Chiovenda et al., 2007) were assessed in noise studies

    of daily living and instrumental activities of daily livinged in two air pollution studies (Sun and Gu, 2008; Zeng). These self-reported activities included personal-careutdoor possibilities. For these instruments, participantsast one activity limitation were dened as disabled,

    to persons without any disabilities. In these Chineseitionally self-reported health, as a number of physi-nts (e.g. back pain) and symptoms (e.g. tiredness), wasSelf-reported health was also investigated in two noiseink, 2011; Franssen et al., 2013), and activity of dailye study (Yoshida and Osada, 1997).

  • 6 L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    Reported associations

    The reviewed studies reported their results in a very heteroge-neous way (Table 3). Even if the same instruments were used, theresults wercomparisonanalysis of

    Association In 10 stu

    formance wperformancperformancLongitudinaa positive cindex (API)supported bpopulation (Zeng et al.,outcome asassociated 18 determin

    Similar rmatter withGeographic20,150 aduof PM2.5 onand mixed (were suppoStudy Cogntion of highdecline wa4.3 years. Ghighest ver

    Inconsiseffect of pawas associaanalysis of tversus LateUS Americalevels weretion processand socio-eNational HeUSA residen

    Associatshowed incof Gatto et cognitive fucic cognitiincreasing utive functiassociated wwas associaanother croterm exposattention aeven after health covation assessmsuch incons

    Associatnitive functto the nearestudies shoPower et a

    (NAS) cohort participants, an adverse association between BC con-centration and global cognitive function assessed by MMSE testwas found. This adverse effect of BC on cognitive function wasfound predominantly in overweight and obese individuals (p of

    tioner, n, sm

    n the012)

    scortests, butts wTrafnitiverforuen

    (SALIen oius e

    assosACaldmerslutiond aan astratitionsociatsion

    studring t assncenSun a

    in c) ands). Thted uppo

    API ity o010)s of Aith

    with in tea (Lunddditsociaerlipiound

    (CV

    tion y onenctior (Chfor aroop c

    ociattudie013;e generally not presented in a way that allows direct between studies. This prohibits a systematic meta-results.

    with air pollutiondies, the effect of ambient air pollution on cognitive per-as investigated. In the earliest studies, global cognitivee tests were used. In the rst study on global cognitivee conducted in 7358 Chinese adults from the Chinesel Health Longevity Survey (CLHLS) (Sun and Gu, 2008),ross-sectional association of the citywide air pollution

    with reduced MMSE was found. These results werey a consecutive longitudinal analysis in a larger sub-

    of 15,873 participants from the same CLHLS population 2010), using similar methods both for exposure and forsessment. A one unit increase in API in this study waswith a 0.9% increase in odds for MMSE score less thaned as cognitive impairment.esults were obtained for the association of particulate

    cognitive functions in the cross-sectional Reasons for and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) study oflts in USA (Loop et al., 2013), where a signicant effect

    incidence of cognitive decline was observed for urbanurbanrural) areas, but not for rural areas. These resultsrted by the longitudinal analysis of the Nurses Healthitive Cohort (Weuve et al., 2012), in which an associa-er levels of PM2.5-10 and of PM2.5, with global cognitives found in 10,409 women over a follow-up period oflobal cognitive decline was signicantly bigger in thesus lowest quintile of exposure.tent results were found in studies investigating therticulate matter on specic cognitive domains. PM2.5ted with lower verbal learning in the cross-sectionalhe Women Isoavone Soy Health Trial (WISH) and Early

    Intervention trial With Estradiol (ELITE) study of 1499n adults (Gatto et al., 2013). In contrast, increased PM10

    not associated with reduced attention and informa-ing speed and verbal memory after adjustment for raceconomic status (SES) in the cross-sectional study of thealth and Nutrition Examination Survey study of 1764ts (Chen and Schwartz, 2009).ions of gaseous pollutants with cognitive function alsoonsistent results. Although in the cross-sectional studyal. (2013), O3 and NO2 were not associated with globalnction, they were associated with the functions of spe-ve domains. In this study at high O3 levels (49 ppb),ozone concentration was associated with lower exec-on, while at the intermediate level (3449 ppb) it wasith higher logical memory. Exposure to ambient NO2

    ted with lower logical memory (Gatto et al., 2013). Inss-sectional study (Chen and Schwartz, 2009), long-ure to high levels of O3 was associated with a lowernd information processing speed and verbal memory,adjustment for age, sex, gender, SES, life-style andriates. Differences in tests applied for cognitive func-ent in these two studies can be a possible reason for

    istent results.ions of other trafc-related exposures with global cog-ion were investigated in four studies, using proximityst road and concentration of BC as exposures. All thesewed similar results. In two cross-sectional studies ofl. (2011, 2013) performed on Normative Aging Study

    interacHowevtensionseen. Iet al., 2MMSEnitive MMSEticipanyears. in cogwas pethe InAging in womWellen

    Thediseaseies of Alzheiair polsion afound concencentra

    Assdepresin fourcompanicanNOx costudy, cultieseatingclotheassociawere sing thein activet al., 2ciationcities wthose tigatedof Korwere foPM10 aThe asof hypwere fdisease

    AssociaOnl

    tive fuworkefound the Strlevel.

    Assthree set al., 2 = 0.1) and in ever smokers (p for interaction = 0.07).o evidence for effect modication by diabetes, hyper-oking or body-mass index (BMI) on the MMSE score was

    MOBILIZE Boston Study of 765 participants (Wellenius, an increase in BC level was also associated with loweres and worse performance of additional specic cog-. Proximity to major road was associated with a lower

    affected only a part of the other cognitive tests in par-ith at least college education and those younger than 77c exposure was associated with reduced performance

    tests as well in the study of Ranft et al. (2009) thatmed on 399 Germany women, a part of the Study ofce of air pollution on Lung function, Inammation andA) cohort. In this study, the association was not presentlder than 74 years, which corresponds to the study oft al. (2012).ciation of air pollution with two neurodegenerativeD and PDwas investigated in three studies. Two stud-ern-Garciduenas et al. (2004, 2009) indicated more-related changes in individuals with higher exposure ton (reduced olfactory functions, increased COX2 expres-ccumulation of A42). Finkelstein and Jerrett (2007)sociation with prevalence of PD only for an increasedon of ambient manganese (Mn), but not for NO2 con-.ions of air pollution with mood disordersanxiety and, and with activities of daily livingwere investigatedies. In the casecontrol study of Persson et al. (2007)22,693 asthmatic and non-asthmatic participants, a sig-ociation of exhaust fumes from trafc approximated bytrations with anxiety was found. In a cross-sectionalnd Gu (2008) found that API was associated with dif-

    oping with personal care activities (bathing, dressing, instrumental activities (cooking, shopping, washingis study also showed poor self-reported health to bewith an increase in air pollution index. These resultsrted by the longitudinal study of Zeng et al. (2010), link-with poor self-reported health and increased difcultiesf daily living at 4.3 years over a follow-up period (Zeng. However, in the study of Sun and Gu (2008), the asso-PI with activity of daily living were found only for the

    high gross domestic product (GDP), and not found for low GDP. An association with depression was inves-he longitudinal study in 357 inhabitants of Republicim et al., 2012). Increased levels of PM10, O3 and NO2

    to increase the emotional symptoms of depression, andionally increased the somatic and affective symptoms.tion with O3 was stronger in participants with a historydemia than among those without, and little differences

    between participants with and without cardiovascularD) or myocardial infarction (Lim et al., 2012).

    with noise study investigated the association of noise with cogni-ns in 81 police ofcers from high trafc areas and ofceiovenda et al., 2007). A signicant effect of noise wasithmetic reasoning, difculties in logical reasoning andolor-word test adjusting for gender, age and education

    ions of noise with health status were investigated ins, reporting inconsistent results (Brink, 2011; Franssen

    Yoshida and Osada, 1997). In the earliest cross-sectional

  • L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111 7

    Table 3Results of studies on long-term air pollution and noise effect on cognitive and psychological functions in adults, by outcome.

    Authors, year of publication Covariates Obtained results*

    Effect of air pollutionNeurocognitive assessmentCognitive functionsSun and Gu, 2008 P, L, E 1 unit air pollution index (API): For high gross domestic product (GDP): MMSE score = 2.67,

    p < 0.001. For medium GDP: MMSE score = 1.84, p < 0.001Chen and Schwartz, 2009 P, L, H Increased PM10: SDLT =0.48, 95% CI 0.270.68; SDST = 0.10, 95% CI 0.050.15. After adjustment

    for race and SESnon-signicant effect. Increased O3: SDST = 0.11, 95% CI 0.010.22; SDLT = 0.52, 95% CI 0.031.01

    Ranft et al., 2009 P, L, H, E Trafc exposure: CERAD-plus battery b = 3.8, p < 0.1; Stroop test b = 5.1, p < 0.01; Snifng testb = 1.3, p < 0.05 (age 74). No independent effect of PM10

    Zeng et al., 2010 P, L 1 unit API: cognitive impairment OR = 1.009, p < 0.05Power et al., 2011 P, L, H BC (doubling concentration, g/m3): MMSE OR = 1.3, 95% CI 1.11.6Wellenius et al., 2012 100 m from major road: MMSE < 26 for at least college education OR = 1.54, 95% CI 1.102.17; for

    77 years OR = 1.34, 95% CI 1.011.76. Not associated with HVLT-R recognition, TMT Part A ad CIB.Interquartile increase in BC (0.11 g/m3): MMSE < 26, OR = 1.15, p = 0.06; worse performance ofHVLT = R immediate recall, p = 0.046

    Weuve et al., 2012 P, L PM2.510: worse global cognitive score (p for trend 0.01); worse for highest vs. lowest level(p = 0.003). Highest vs. lowest quintile of PM2.5: changes in global cognitive score for women(p = 0.03). Global cognitive score (SD/2 years) per 10 g/m3 increment: PM2.510 0.020 (95% CI0.32 to 0.008); PM2.5: 0.018 (95% CI 0.035 to 0.002)

    Gatto et al., 2013 P, L None association with global cognition. Per 10 g/m3 PM2.5: lower verbal learning ( = 0.32,p = 0.05). NO2 > 20 ppb: lower logical memory ( = 0.62, p = 0.095). O3 > 49 ppb: lower executivefunction ( = 0.66, p = 0.059). O3 range 3449 ppb: higher logical memorywomen ( = 0.46, 95%CI 0.090.83), adults 60 y. o. ( = 0.51, 95% CI 0.110.91)

    Loop et al., 2013 P, L, H, E Per 10 g/m3 PM2.5: for urban areaincident cognitive impairment OR = 1.40 (95% CI 1.061.85);for mixed areasincident cognitive impairment (OR = 0.32, 95% CI 0.110.98). No associations forrural area and total population

    Power et al., 2013 P, L, H BC (doubling concentration, g/m3): for lacked an HFE C282Y low MMSE (OR = 1.37, 95% CI1.081.73); for at least one HFE H63D variant (OR = 1.74, 95% CI 1.06, 2.87). HFE C282 modies theassociation between BC and global cognitive function

    Neurodegenerative diseasesAlzheimers diseaseCaldern-Garciduenas et al., 2004 Frontal cortex tissue: Elevation of COX2 mRNA in high-exposure group (p = 0.009); elevation of

    COX2 immunoreactivity (p = 0.01). Hippocampus tissue: Elevation in COX2 mRNA in high-exposuregroup (p = 0.045); no differences in COX2 immunoreactivity between high and low-exposuregroups (p = 0.37)

    Caldern-Garciduenas et al., 2009 Mean UPSIT scores lower for high-exposure group (p = 0.03). No differences in UPSIT scores indifferent APOE statuses (p = 0.52)

    Parkinson disease (PD)Finkelstein and Jerrett, 2007 P 10 g/m3 increases in Mn: PD or Dopa prescription for men OR = 1.041, 95% CI 0.9971.09; for

    female: OR = 1.035, 95% CI 0.971.10. With type of clinic as confounder OR = 1.044 (95% CI1.001.09)

    Activity of daily living and mood disordersAnxietyPersson et al., 2007 P, H Exhaust form trafc: anxiety OR = 1.66, p = 0.001Activity of daily livingSun and Gu, 2008 P, K, E 1 unit API: For high GDP: difculties in ADL ( = 1.41, p < 0.01); instrumental ADL ( = 0.98,

    p < 0.001), self-related health (OR = 2.20, p < 0.001). For medium GDP instrumental ADL = 0.6,p < 0.001, self-related health, (OR = 1.87, p < 0.001), no associations with ADL

    Zeng et al., 2010 P, L 1 unit API: increased ADL disability (25%, p < 0.001); increased health decits (8%, p < 0.05)DepressionLim et al., 2012 P, L, H Interquartile increase of PM10: Increase in composite score of emotional symptoms: 38.2%, p < 0.01,

    NO2 118.2%, p < 0.05. Increase in somatic symptoms score 38.9%, p < 0.05. Increase in affectivesymptoms score 11.5%, p < 0.01. O3: Increase in composite score of emotional symptoms 132.5%,p < 0.05

    Effect of noiseNeurocognitive assessmentCognitive functionsChiovenda et al., 2007 P Differences between police ofcers from high trafc areas and ofce workers: Raven PM38

    (t = 3.24, p = 0.002); arithmetic reasoning (t = 2.30, p = 0.024), Stroop color-word test (t = 2.02,p = 0.047)

    Activity of daily living and mood disordersActivity of daily livingYoshida and Osada, 1997 P Noise 70 dB(A): appetite loss (OR = 5.2***)AnxietyHardoy et al., 2005 General anxiety disorder OR = 2.0 (95% CI 1.04.2); Anxiety disorder NOS OR = 2.9 (95% CI 1.04.1)Persson et al., 2007 P Associated with anxiety: trafc noise 55 dB(A), OR = 1.32, p = 0.009; sound from neighbors

    OR = 1.47, p = 0.001; trafc vibration OR = 1.37, p = 0.042; sound (other) OR = 1.72, p = 0.003. Soundfrom ventilation not associated with anxiety

    General nuisanceYoshida and Osada, 1997 P Noise 70 dB(A): OR = 5.2***Brand et al., 2009 Difference between groups with different noxious environment (X2 = 34.0, p < 0.001)**DepressionYoshida and Osada, 1997 P Noise 70 dB(A): OR = 2.9***

    Hardoy et al., 2005 P Major depressive disorder and depressive disorder NOSnot related to aircraft noise

  • 8 L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    Table 3 (Continued)

    Authors, year of publication Covariates Obtained results*

    Bocquier et al., 2013 P, H Censors blockneighborhood environment: low deprivation, intermediate deprivation, highdeprivation. Regardless deprivation: RR = 1.33 (95% CI 1.021.24) for LAEQ, 22-6 h# 55 dB(A). For

    ow deate anations

    Health statuYoshida and e OR =Brink, 2011 d modFranssen et ted to

    61, (95I 1.26% CI 1

    Ppersonal fa hnicitchildren, coun ng, exBMI. Hhealth , diabehypertension, olluti*Results relate**Results are r***Condence# LAEQequiva## LDENday, e

    study perfowomen, an status, adjuhouse was only effect inhibiting adinal studyyears of follels was fouroad noise Lstatus and sFrassen et astatus and within a radwas observand LDEN health. Highand tirednedegree of uOsada (199

    Three stsion (Yoshid2005). Yoshciated withbeing a criti190,617 Fredepressive of low sociointermediasignicant. disordersotherwise sin the crossthe effect ovicinity of Ecities or rur

    While Hsion, anxietnoise (Hardand no accinhibits a cbetween aigated in a cno signica

    er rod soted ted

    tionsdies on n. Onlf boiffertion

    noistion

    ssion

    his rend snt asoods th, andunctre oisenLAEQ, 22-6 h# 55 dB(A) in l70 dB(A), with the noise level of 70 dB(A)cal threshold. Bocquier et al. (2013) found in a cohort ofnch that the effect of trafc noise on purchase of anti-

    howevtion anassociaassocia

    AssociaStu

    noise scarceation owith dassocialution,interac

    Dis cu

    In t2013 adifferetion, mexpand(2012)nitive fexposufar to ddrugs was only positive in participants living in areaseconomic deprivation, while for participants living in

    te and high deprivation areas such an effect was non-In contrast to these studies, two forms of depressivemajor depressive disorder and depressive disorder notpeciedwere not found to be related to aircraft noise-sectional study of Hardoy et al. (2005). In this study,f aircraft noise was investigated in people living in thelmas airport in contrast to a sample of people living inal or mining villages of Italy.ardoy did not nd associations of noise with depres-y disorders were found to be associated with aircraftoy et al., 2005). No multiple models were constructedurate noise levels were specied in this study, whichomparison with other mentioned studies. Associationrcraft noise and anxiety disorders were also investi-asecontrol study of Persson et al. (2007) that foundnt effect of aircraft noise on the trait anxiety (p = 0.101),

    and outcomexpand the

    All preseand noise wleast for onations diffein the cognet al., 2013)in air pollutcomes wasassociationdepressionair pollutiolittle, mostexposures s

    Differenmental heaTill now, tprivation level RR = 1.16 (95% CI 1.011.32) (reference LAEQ, 22-6 h#

    d high deprivation and for other noise levels (4550 andnon-signicant effect

    3.0***. No associations with listlessnessel

    noise exposure during the night. Noise 60 dB(A): poor% CI 1.012.56). LDEN##: headache (OR = 1.16, 95% CI 1.011.34),1.70), listlessness (OR = 1.17, 95% CI 1.011.36), not fully rested in.031.41). Remaining seven complainsnon-signicant

    y, childhood SES, education level, marital status, number of survivingercise, leisure activities, dark sh consumption, computer experience,tes, incident stroke, presence of depressive symptoms, dyslipidemia,on, temperature, season.

    ad trafc noise, sounds from neighbors, trafc vibra-und from other sources were shown to be signicantlywith anxiety. Sound from ventilation systems was notwith anxiety.

    with air pollution and noiseinvestigating the concurrent effects of air pollution andeurocognitive outcomes in the same population arey one of the reviewed studies investigated the associ-th air pollution and trafc noise (Persson et al., 2007)ent aspects of mental health in the same study. Thes were calculated for each one of the exposures (air pol-e) separately. However, possible synergistic relations ors between these two exposures were not investigated.

    view, we analyzed 22 studies published until Novemberummarized the effect of environmental exposures onpects of mental health, namely neurocognitive func-

    disorders and neurodegenerative disease. This reviewe works of Guxens and Sunyer (2012) and Block et al.

    supports a possible role of air pollutants on neurocog-ion in the adult population and a role of long-term noisen mood disorders. However, no evidence is available sotangle the effects of these two closely related exposures

    es. In this review, we add newly published studies and

    investigated outcomes.nted studies, assessing the association of air pollutionith neurocognitive functions, found an association at

    e of the investigated pollutants. However, these associ-red in effect size (Gatto et al., 2013; Loop et al., 2013) anditive domain that showed positive associations (Gatto

    and are therefore not easily comparable. Additionally,ion studies, an association with AD- and PD-related out-

    observed for highly exposed participants. A positive of air pollution and noise was also found for anxiety,

    and impaired activities of daily living. Associations ofn and noise with mental health outcomes overlappedly due to a lack of common outcomes to both types oftudied.t associations of air pollution and noise with aspects oflth can also be a result of different biological pathways.he biological pathways of an effect of long-term air

  • L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111 9

    pollution or noise exposure on the brain are not completelyclaried. Two different biological pathways were proposed forlong-term effects of air pollution. First, the direct respiratory intakeof air pollutants can provoke a local inammatory effect that leadsto productithe systemand damagshown in tolevels of dieexpression brain micromay affect Ranft et al.that were increase in the hippoca

    Possibletion have nHowever, etion and nopollution dewell as noistion memoris known asing and mem2005). Thuswhich showneurotransmndings bylarly, associfunctions wmale mice eto sub-chrolike symptothe biologicical functioshown thatin human (the release thalamus ththat is respo

    Some coand they hi

    Concurrent

    Only onand noise ainformationpollution anhigh correlaent noise arin time andnoise, and able. It is theexposure in

    Diversity of

    Studies rlution use dthe effect oflution used studies, invtive functiodifferent te

    necessary to conduct studies on air pollution and noise using thesame clinical and subclinical outcomes to be able to adequatelycompare and tease apart the effect of either exposure.

    ual s

    en innoiseimpopairm009;lutiobgroutivelyge mg therly p

    dina

    y fewciat

    th of lutiofunct

    al ceuv

    cogf Chwas alent tf Wes pe

    to tpart

    assoitive

    nal c

    st stuersondern wortates .ilarlyt bodnd S2009eridenuat

    theynicalise c

    gend

    of tn. Sexeen s-Levnmen

    pot2013on of pro-inammatory cytokines which can pass intoic circulation and can disrupt the bloodbrain barriere brain cells (Genc et al., 2012). This pathway wasxicological studies. For instance, rats exposed to highsel exhaust showed elevated levels of interleukin IL-1and elevated levels of tumor-necrosis factor TNF inglia (Levesque et al., 2011). Secondly, air pollution

    olfactory functions (Caldern-Garciduenas et al., 2004;, 2009), similar to that found in animal models. Dogshighly exposed to air pollution showed a signicantDNA damage in olfactory bulbs, the frontal cortex andmpus (Caldern-Garciduenas et al., 2002).

    associations between noise and neurocognitive func-ot been investigated in epidemiological studies so far.xperimental studies showed similar effects of air pollu-ise on animals. For example, 10 months exposure to aircreased spatial learning in mice (Fonken et al., 2011), ase exposure for 15 days produces working and recogni-y decits in rats (Haider et al., 2012). The hippocampus

    the key region critically involved in both, spatial learn-ory formation (Broadbent et al., 2004; Kahlmann et al.,

    , the toxicological ndings from experiments in rodents, that noise exposure causes changes in hippocampalitter signaling in rats (Cui et al., 2012), support the

    Fonken et al. (2011) and Haider et al. (2012). Simi-ations of both air pollution and noise with psychologicalere also demonstrated in animal studies. For example,xposed to long-term PM2.5 exposure and rats exposednic noise exposure were found to display depressive-ms (Fonken et al., 2011; Naqvi et al., 2012). However,al pathways of possible effects of noise on psycholog-n are not well established yet. Briey, animal studies

    noise provokes mild acute stress similar to that occurArnsten and Goldman-Rakic, 1998). This stress evokesof high levels of noradrenaline and dopamine in hypo-at in turn impairs the regulation of prefrontal cortexnsible for cognitive abilities (Arnsten, 2009).

    mmon problems were identied in the reviewed studiesnder a full comparison of results of these studies.

    environmental exposures

    e of the reviewed studies analyzed both air pollutionssociation (Persson et al., 2007). However, even though

    on both exposures was available, no analysis with aird noise simultaneously was conducted, possibly due totion of these two exposures. Air pollution and ambi-e both related to trafc and often occur simultaneously

    space. The effect of air pollution may be modied by synergism between the two exposures seems possi-refore necessary to include both air pollution and noiseto account when investigating the effect of either.

    outcomes

    elated to cognitive and psychological effect of air pol-ifferent panels of tests than studies, which investigated

    noise. Most of the studies exploring the effect of air pol-a large battery of objective cognitive tests, while in noiseestigators mostly used self-reported outcomes. Cogni-n was less investigated in noise studies, and completelysts were performed for its assessment. It is therefore

    Individ

    Whtrafc is also ing imet al., 2air polthe surespecolder adilutinof elde

    Longitu

    Onlits assostrengair polnitive to norm2009; Wrelatedstudy oozone equivastudy ochangesimilaryears aing theof cogn

    Additio

    Motook pconfouisfactioan impcovariastudies

    SimcurrenChen aet al., triglycan atteyses asone cliand no

    Role of

    Fivewomehave bMeyerenvirotigatedet al. (usceptibility

    vestigating the effect of trafc proximity (indicator for as well as for trafc-related air pollution) or noise, itrtant to take into account a rising prevalence of hear-ent with age. In two out of 22 reviewed studies (Ranft

    Wellenius et al., 2012), an association of trafc-relatedn with decreased cognitive function was found only inp of younger participants (74 years and 77 years,), but not in the older group. A decline in hearing inay lead to a lower sensitivity to trafc noise, thereby

    effect of noise in study populations with a large fractionarticipants.

    l data on cognitive decline

    studies on progression of cognitive impairment andion with air pollution and ambient noise also limit thethe existing evidence. In adults, long-term exposure ton and ambient noise may have a negative effect on cog-ion that will result in cognitive impairment additionalognitive aging. Only two studies (Chen and Schwartz,e et al., 2012) performed a comparison of air pollution-

    nitive decline with normal aging-related decline. In theen and Schwartz (2009), an increase in 10 ppb of annualssociated with the decline in cognitive functions that iso 3.5 till 5.3 years normal-aging decline. Similarly, in theuve et al. (2012), differences in rates of global cognitiver 10 g/m3 of PM2.510 and PM2.5 exposures were foundhe difference in rates between women who were 12

    in age. There is an urgent need for studies investigat-ciation of environmental exposures with progression

    decline in older age.

    ovariates

    dies investigating air pollution or noise associationsnal characteristics and life-style factors as potentials into account. However, depressive symptoms, sat-ith health and activities of daily living might playnt role in cognitive performance. The effect of theseis poorly investigated both in air pollution and noise

    , the role of comorbidities is poorly investigated in they of evidence. Five of reviewed studies (Brink, 2011;

    chwartz, 2009; Loop et al., 2013; Lim et al., 2012; Ranft) included factors related to CVD (BMI, hypertension,

    level) as intermediates in sensitivity analyses, showingion of effect in some studies. These are important anal-

    suggest that general or cardiovascular health may be pathway through which a possible effect of air pollutionan be mediated.

    er in studies of neurocognitive functions

    he 22 reviewed studies were performed only in men or differences in different domains of cognitive function

    described in several studies (Darley and Smith, 1995;y, 1989; Torres et al., 2006). However, few studies oftal exposures and cognitive function specically inves-

    ential effect modication by sex. For example, Gatto) showed that logical memory was more affected by

  • 10 L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111

    long-term air pollution exposure in women than in men. In mousemodels, sex differences in behavioral patterns and neuron differ-entiation due to exposure to nano-sized particulate matter werefound (Davis et al., 2013). The role of sex in modifying effect esti-mates is largbiological mand stratic

    A lack of inf

    Althougair pollutioaddress is nyears of livfor participthe whole pestimates fo

    Conclusion

    Investigtal health rexposures rshown to benitive funcin several sof reviewedambient airtive functiolong-term eies investigin associatiroborate ntoxicologicaand pathwa

    Acknowled

    Lilian Tzdoctoral feJerusalem, IFoundation

    References

    Arnsten, A.F.T.structure a

    Arnsten, A.F.Tcognitive fArch. Gen.

    Block, M.L., Caammatio

    Block, M.L., ED.A., CostH.A., KaufNadadur, Wright, Rhttp://dx.d

    Bocquier, A., CGaudart, Jrisk factorJ. Public H

    Brand, S., HelSchwarzenwith envirdiagnostic

    Brink, M., 201tionship wSwitzerlan

    Broadbent, N.Jand the hi

    Brook, R.D., Rajagopalan, S., Pope III, C.A., Brook, J.R., Bhatnagar, A., Diez-Roux,A.V., Holguin, F., Hong, Y., Leupker, R.V., Mittleman, M.A., Peters, A., Siscov-ick, D., Smith, S.C., Whitsel, L., Kaufman, J.D., et al., 2010. Particulate matterair pollution and cardiovascular disease. An update to the scientic state-ment from the American heart association. Circulation, DOI: 10.1161/CIR.

    3e31-Garc

    aya, Newca

    -Garcez, R.s, M., mmatolluti-Garcerritt., Gon. Lon

    ltered particildren-Garc

    zalez-ks, Don, R.,

    Dotypatho://dx.d., Schitive da, P.apozze-expssmen, Srqvbehav

    u, Me desphory

    .K., S: an eertisin.A., BBerhaatal el diffe://dx.di, G., stiere

    modrt st://dx.d, D.W.morta., 200le? An, S.C.,, E., Le polyin a p133rst, Easnere follo

    Arch. Oein, M

    and mo Can

    L.K., Xollutiocam987., E.A.Me arouicatio., Ramandezn wit232.M., Mack

    in m://dx.d

    Zadeohe nerely unclear but may provide some insights into possibleechanisms. Investigations of mixed gender populationsation of results by sex are therefore necessary.

    ormation on the length of residential exposure

    h the reviewed studies assessed long-term exposure ton and noise, length of residence at the same residentialot mentioned in most of these studies. Information oning at the same address, and stratication of analysisants that did or did not change the living address duringeriod of exposure can help to provide the more accurater the relevant exposureoutcome associations.

    s

    ations on the association of air pollution and noise men-eceived increased attention during recent years. Botheviewed in this article (air pollution and noise) were

    associated with one or several indicators of neurocog-tion, mood disorders and neurodegenerative diseasetudies. Despite some limitations and inconsistencies

    studies, they generally support the hypothesis that pollution and noise are associated with neurocogni-ns, mood disorders and neurodegenerative disease inxposed persons. Nevertheless, there is a need for stud-ating simultaneously air pollution and noise exposureson with mental health, for longitudinal studies to cor-dings from cross-sectional analyses, and for parallell and epidemiological studies to elucidate mechanismsys of action.

    gements

    ivian gratefully acknowledges the support by a post-llowship from the Environmental and Health Fund,srael. This work was supported by the German Research

    (DFG; HO 3314/2-1).

    , 2009 June. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortexnd function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (6), 410422.

    ., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1998. Noise stress impairs prefrontal corticalunction in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism.

    Psychiatry 55, 362369.lderon-Garciduenas, L., 2009. Air pollution: mechanisms of neuroin-n and CNS disease. Trends Neurosci. 32 (9), 506516.lder, A., Auten, R.L., Bilbo, S.D., Chen, H., Chen, J.-C., Cory-Slechta,a, D., Diaz-Sanchez, D., Dorman, D.C., Gold, D., Gray, K., Jeng,man, J.D., Kleinman, M.T., Kirshner, A., Lawler, C., Miller, D.S.,S., Ritz, B., Semmens, E.O., Tonelli, L.H., Veronesi, B., Wright, R.O.,., 2012. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop,oi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2012.08.014.ortaredona, S., Boutin, C., David, A., Bigot, A., Sciortino, V., Nauleau, S.,., Giorgi, R., Verger, P., 2013. Is exposure to night-time trafc noise a

    for purchase of anxiolytichypnotic medication? A cohort study. Eur.ealth, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckt117.ler, P., Bicher, A.J., Braun-Fahrleander, C., Huss, A., Niederer, M.,bach, S., Waeber, R., Wegmann, L., Kuechenhoff, J., 2009. Patientsonment-related disorders: comprehensive results of interdisciplinarys. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 212, 157171.1. Parameters of well-being and subjective health and their rela-ith residential trafc noise exposurea representative evaluation ind. Environ. Int. 37, 723733.., Squire, L.R., Clark, R.E., 2004. Spatial memory, recognition memory,ppocampus. PNAS 101 (40), 1451514520.

    0b01Caldern

    OsnA., R373.

    CaldernChavForeinaair p

    CaldernB., HD.M2008an anein ch

    CaldernGonBrooJardR.R.,and http

    Chen, J.Scogn

    ChiovenG., Cnoisasse

    Clark, C.and

    Cui, B., Wnitivpho

    Darley, WgiesAdv

    Davis, DJ.C., Prenronahttp

    CesaronForasioncohohttp

    Dockeryand

    Doty, R.Lviab

    EdwardsFlakbornage 1326

    ElmenhoM., Bin thInt.

    Finkelsteasein tw

    Fonken,Air phipp973

    Franssennoismed

    Freire, CFernlutio64, 2

    Gatto, NJ.C., tionhttp

    Genc, S.,on t81dbece1.iduenas, L., Azzarelli, B., Acuna, H., Garcia, R., gambling, T.M.,., Monroy, S., Tizapanzi, M.D.R., Carson, J.L., Villarreal-Calderon,stle, B., 2002. Air pollution and brain damage. Toxicol. Pathol. 30,

    iduenas, L., Reed, W., Maronpot, R.R., Henriques-Roldan, C., Delgado-, Caldern-Garciduenas, A., Dragustinovis, I., Francco-Lira, M., Aragon-Solt, A.C., Altenburg, M., Torres-Jardon, R., Swenberg, J.A., 2004. Brainion and Alzheimers-like pathology in individuals exposed to severeon. Toxicol. Pathol. 32, 650.iduenas, L., Solt, A.C., Henrquez-Roldn, C., Torres-Jardn, R., Nuse,, L., Villarreal-Caldern, R., Osnaya, N., Stone, I., Garca, R., Brooks,zlez-Maciel, A., Reynoso-Robles, R., Delgado-Chvez, R., Reed, W.,g-term air pollution exposure is associated with neuroinammation,

    innate immune response, disruption of the blood-brain barrier, ultra-ulate deposition, and accumulation of amyloid -42 and -synuclein

    and young adults. Toxicol. Pathol. 36 (2), 289310.iduenas, L., Franco-Lira, M., Henrquez-Roldan, C., Osnaya, N.,Maciel, A., Reynoso-Robles, R., Villarreal-Caldern, R., Herritt, L.,., Keefe, S., Palacios-Moreno, J., Villarreal-Calderon, R., Torres-

    Medina-Cortina, H., Delgado-Chavez, R., Aiello-Mora, M., Maronpot,, R.L., 2009. Urban air pollution: Inuences on olfactory functionlogy in exposed children and young adults. Exp. Toxicol. Pathol.,oi.org/10.1016/j.etp.2009.02.117.wartz, J., 2009. Neurobehavioral effects of ambient air pollution onperformance in US adults. Neurotoxicology 30, 231239., Pasqualetti, P., Zappasodi, F., Ercolani, M., Milazzo, D., Tomei,ella, A., Tomei, F., Rossini, P.M., Tecchio, F., 2007. Environmental

    osed workers: event-related potentials neuropsychological and moodt. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 65, 228237.ist, P., 2012. A 3 year update on the inuence of noise on performanceior. Noise Health 14, 292296.., She, X., Liu, H., 2012. Impulse noise exposure in rats causes cog-cits and changes in hippocampal neurotransmitter signaling and taulation. Brain Res. 1427, 3543.mith, R.E., 1995. Gender differences in information processing strate-mpirical test of the selectivity model in advertising response. J.g XXIV (1), 4156.ortolato, M., Godar, S.C., Sander, T.K., Iwata, N., Pakbin, P., Shih,ne, K., McConnell, R., Sioutas, C., Finch, C.E., Morgan, T.E., 2013.xposure to urban air nanoparticles in mice causes altered neu-rentiation and depression-like responses. PLoS One 8 (5), e64128,oi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064128.Porta, D., Badaloni, C., Stafoggia, M., Eeftens, M., Meliefste, K.,, F., 2012. Nitrogen dioxide levels estimated from land use regres-els several years apart and association with mortality in a largeudy. Environ. Health: A Global Access Science Source 11 (48),oi.org/10.1186/1476-069X-11-48., Pope III, C.A., Xu, X., et al., 1993. An association between air pollutionlity in six U.S. cities. N. Engl. J. Med. 329, 17531759.8. The olfactory vector hypothesis of neurodegenerative disease: is itn. Neurol. 63, 715.

    Jedrychowski, W., Butscher, M., Camann, D., Kieltyka, A., Mroz, E.,i, Z., Wang, S., Rauh, V., Perera, F., 2010. Prenatal exposure to air-cyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and childrens intelligence at 5 years of

    rospective cohort study in Poland. Environ. Health Perspect. 118 (9),1..M., Elmenhorst, D., Wenzel, J., Quehl, J., Mueller, U., Maass, H., Vejvoda,, M., 2010. Effects of nocturnal aircraft noise on cognitive performancewing morning: doseresponse relationships in laboratory and eld.ccup. Environ. Health 83, 743751.

    .M., Jerrett, M., 2007. A study of relationships between Parkinsons dis-arkers of trafc-derived and environmental manganese air pollutionadian cities. Environ. Res. 104 (3), 420432.u, X., Weil, Z.M., Chen, G., Sun, Q., Rajagopalan, S., Nelson, R.J., 2011.on impairs cognition, provokes depressive-like behaviors and alterspal cytokine expression and morphology. Mol. Psychiatry 16 (10),

    ., van Wiechen, C.M.A.G., Nagelkerke, N.J.D., Lebret, E., 2013. Aircraftnd a large international airport and its impact on general health andn use. Occup. Environ. Med. 61, 405413.os, R., Puertas, R., Lopez-Espinosa, M., Julvez, J., Aguilera, I., Cruz, F.,, M.-F., Sunyer, J., Olea, N., 2010. Association of trafc-related air pol-h cognitive development in children. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health28.Henderson, V.W., Hodis, H.N., St John, J.A., Lurmann, F., Chen,, W.J., 2013. Components of air pollution and cognitive func-iddle-aged and older adults in Los Angeles. Neurotoxicology,oi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2013.09.004.glulari, Z., Fuss, S.H., Genc, K., 2012. The adverse effects of air pollutionvous system. J. Toxicol., http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/782462.

  • L. Tzivian et al. / International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 218 (2015) 111 11

    Gustavsson, J.P., Bergman, H., Edman, G., Ekselius, L., von Knorring, L., Linder, J.,2000. Swedish universities Scales of Personality (SSP): construction, internalconsistency and normative data. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 102, 217225.

    Guxens, M., Sunyer, J., 2012. A review of epidemiological studies on neuropsychol-ogical effects of air pollution. Swiss Med. Weekly 141, w13322.

    Haider, S., Naqvi, F., Batool, Z., Tabassum, S., Perveen, T., Saleem, S., Haleem, D.J., 2012.Decreased hippocampal 5-HT and DA levels following sub-chronic exposure tonoise stress: impairment in both spatial and recognition memory in male rats.Sci. Pharm. 80, 10011011.

    Hardoy, M.C., Carta, M.G., Marci, A.R., Carbone, F., Cadeddu, M., Kovess, V., DellOsso,L., Carpiniello, B., 2005. Exposure to aircraft noise and risk of psychiatric disor-ders: the Elmas survey. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 40, 2426.

    Hirano, S., Furuyama, A., Koike, E., Kobayashi, T., 2003. Oxidative-stress potency oforganic extracts of diesel exhaust and urban ne particles in rat heart microves-sel endothelial cells. Toxicology 187 (23), 161170.

    Kahlmann, S., Piel, M., Wolf, O.T., 2005. Impaired memory retrieval after psycholog-ical stress in healthy young men. J. Neurosci. 25, 29772982.

    Levesque, S., Taetzsch, T., Lull, M.E., Kodavanti, U., Stadler, K., Wagner, A., Johnson,J.A., Duke, L., Kodavanti, P., Surace, M.J., Block, M.L., 2011. Diesel exhaust acti-vates and primes microglia: air pollution, neuroinammation, and regulation ofdopaminergic neurotoxicity. Environ. Health Perspect. 119, 11491155.

    Lim, Y-H., Ho Kim, J.H., Bae, S., Park, H.Y., Hong, Y-C., 2012. Air pollution and symp-toms of depression in elderly adults. Environ. Health Perspect. 120, 10231028.

    Loop, M.S., Kent, S.T., Al-Hamdan, M.Z., Crosson, W.L., Estes, S.M., Estes, M.G., Quat-trochi, D.A., Hemmings, S.N., Wadley, V.G., McClure, L.A., 2013. Fine particulatematter and incident cognitive impairment in the REasons for geographic andracial differences in stroke (REGARDS) cohort. PLoS One 8 (9), e75001.

    Meyers-Levy, J., 1989. Gender differences in information processing: a selectiv-ity interpretation. In Cafferata, P., Tybout, A.M. (eds.), Cognitive and AffectiveResponses to Advertising. Lexington Books, Canada.

    Naqvi, F., Haider, S., Batool, Z., Perveen, T., Haleem, D.J., 2012. Sub-chronic exposureto noise affects locomotor activity and produces anxiogenic and depressive likebehavior in rats. Pharmacol. Rep. 64 (1), 6469.

    Nemmar, A., Vanbilloen, H., Hoylaerts, M.F., Hoet, P.H., Verbruggen, A., Nemery,B., 2001. Passage of intratracheally instilled ultrane particles from the lunginto the systemic circulation in hamster. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 164 (9),16651668.

    Passchier-Vermeer, W., Passchier, W.F., 2000. Noise exposure and public health.Environ. Health Perspect. 108 (1), 123131.

    Perera, F., Li, T., Zhou, Z., Yuan, T., Chen, Y., Qu, L., Rauh, V.A., Zhang, Y., Tang, D., 2008.Benets of reducing prenatal exposure to coal-burning pollutants to childrensneurodevelopment in China. Environ. Health Perspect. 116 (10), 13961400.

    Persson, R., Bjrk, J., Ard, J., Albin, M., Jakobsson, K., 2007. Trait anxiety and modeledexposure as determinants of self-reported annoyance to sound, air pollution andother environmental factors in the home. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 81(2), 1791

    Power, M.C., Weisskopf, M.G., Alexeeff, S.E., Coull, B.A., Spiro III, A., Schwartz, J.,2011. Trafc-related air pollution and cognitive function in a cohort of oldermen. Environ. Health Perspect. 119, 682687.

    Power, M.C., Weisskopf, M.G., Alexeeff, S.E., Wright, R.O., Coull, B.A., Spiro III., A.,Schwartz, J., 2013. Modication by hemochromatosis gene polymorphisms ofthe association between trafc-related air pollution and cognition in older men:a cohort study. Environ. Health 12, 16.

    Ranft, U., Schikowski, T., Sugiri, D., Krutmann, J., Kramer, U., 2009. Long-termexposure to trafc-related particulate matter impairs cognitive function in theelderly. Environ. Res. 109, 10041011.

    Rckler, R., Schneider, A., Breitner, S., Cyrys, J., Peters, A., 2011. Health effect of par-ticulate air pollution: a review of epidemiological evidence. Inhal. Toxicol. 23(10), 555592.

    Salame, P., Baddeley, A.D., 1982. Disruption of short-term memory by unattendedspeech: implications for the structure of working memory. J. Verb Learn. VerbBehav. 21, 150164.

    Schapkin, S.A., Falkenstein, M., Marks, A., Griefahn, B., 2006. Executive brain func-tions after exposure to nocturnal trafc noise: effects of task difculty and sleepquality. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 96, 693702.

    Smith, A.P., Broadbent, D.E., 1992. Non-auditory effects of noise at work: a reviewof the literature. HSE Contract Research Report No. 30. HMSO, London.

    Siddique, S., Banerjee, M., Ray, M.R., Lahiri, T., 2011. Attention-decit hyperactivitydisorder in children chronically exposed to high level of vehicular pollution. Eur.J. Pediatrics 170 (7), 923929.

    Sun, R., Gu, D., 2008. Air pollution economic development of communities, andhealth status among the elderly in urban China. Am. J. Epidemiol. 168,13111318.

    Torres, A., Gmez-Gil, E., Vidal, A., Puig, O., Boget, T., Salamero, M., 2006. Genderdifferences in cognitive functions and inuence of sex hormones. Actas Esp.Psiqiatr. 34 (6), 408415.

    Wang, R., Henderson, S.B., Sbihi, H., Allen, R.W., Brauer, M., 2013. Temporal stabilityof land use regression models for trafc-related air pollution. Atmos. Environ.64 (0), 312319.

    Weuve, J., Puett, R.C., Schwartz, J., Yanosky, J.D., Laden, F., Grodstein, F., 2012. Expo-sure to particulate air pollution and cognitive decline in older women. Arch.Intern. Med. 172 (3), 219227.

    Wellenius, G.A., Boyle, L.D., Coull, B.A., Milberg, W.P., Grypais, A., Schwartz, J., Mit-telman, M.A., Lipsitz, L.A., 2012. Residential proximity to nearest roadway andcognitive function in community-dwelling seniors: results from the MOBILIZEBoston Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60 (11), 20752080.

    Yoshida, T., Osada, Y., 1997. Effects of road trafc noise on inhabitants of Tokyo. J.Sound Vibr. 205, 517522.

    Zeng, Y., Gu, D., Purser, J., Hoenig, H., Christakis, N., 2010. Associations of environ-mental factors with elderly health and mortality in China. Am. J. Public Health100, 298305.91.

    Effect of long-term outdoor air pollution and noise on cognitive and psychological functions in adultsIntroductionMethods of the searchResultsStudy design and exposures******Exposure assessmentOutcome assessmentReported associationsAssociation with air pollutionAssociation with noiseAssociations with air pollution and noise

    DiscussionConcurrent environmental exposuresDiversity of outcomesIndividual susceptibilityLongitudinal data on cognitive declineAdditional covariatesRole of gender in studies of neurocognitive functionsA lack of information on the length of residential exposure

    ConclusionsAcknowledgementsReferences