Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

download Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

of 22

Transcript of Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    1/22

    Modeling the Development Process ofDialogical Critical Thinking in PupilsAged 10 to 12 Years

    Marie-France Daniel, Louise Lafortune,Richard Pallascio, Laurance Splitter, Christina Slade, &Teresa de la Garza

    This research project investigated manifestations of critical thinking in pupils 10 to 12

    years of age during their group discussions held in the context of Philosophy for Children

    Adapted to Mathematics. The objective of the research project was to examine, through

    the pupils discussions, the development of dialogical critical thinking processes. The

    research was conducted during an entire school year. The research method was based on

    the Grounded Theory approach; the material used consisted of transcripts of verbal

    exchanges among the pupils (at the beginning, middle and end of the school year).Analysis of the transcripts revealed that: (1) critical thinking appears to the extent that a

    dia-logue is established among pupils; (2) on the cognitive level, dialogical critical

    thinking is comprised of four thinking modes: logical, creative, responsible and meta-

    cognitive; and (3) on the epistemological level, dialogical critical thinking is only

    manifested in a context where egocentricity of perspective and relativism of beliefs are

    transcended.

    Keywords: Critical Thinking; Philosophical Dialogue; Philosophy for Children;

    Discussion

    Marie-France Daniel (PhD, 1992, University of Quebec at Montreal) is Professor, Department of Kinesiology,

    University of Montreal; Louise Lafortune (PhD, 1992, University of Quebec at Montreal) is Professor,

    Department of Education, University of Quebec at Trois-Rivie res; Richard Pallascio (PhD, 1970, University of

    Montreal) is Professor, Department of Education, University of Quebec at Montreal; Laurance Splitter (PhD,

    1983, University of Oxford) is Professor, Department of Education, Hunter College; Christina Slade (PhD, 1982,

    Australian National University), is Professor, Department of Communication, Macquarie University; Teresa de la

    Garza (PhD, 1992, Universidad Iberoamericana) is Professor, Department of Philosophy, Universidad

    Iberoamericana. This research project was partially supported by a grant (410-98-1228) from the SocialSciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC). Marie-France Daniel can be contacted at

    [email protected]

    Communication Education

    Vol. 54, No. 4, October 2005, pp. 334/354

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    2/22

    Education is becoming increasingly complex world-wide, largely due to the

    expansion of knowledge combined with the accelerated development of technologies,

    which further complicate life in our society. In future, schools must provide younger

    generations with a form of education that allows them to successfully face these new

    challenges. In this regard, the development of critical thinking in pupils is necessaryto favour pupils true comprehension of events rather than developing and

    maintaining a simplifying vision of the information related to these events (Delors,

    1996, p. 47).

    In order to develop this true comprehension of events among children, and to

    stimulate their critical judgment, several specialists in the field of education are

    turning to socioconstructivist teaching approaches, in particular discussion among

    peers. Discussion, as a way of teaching, was valued by Socrates to elicit or give birth

    to young peoples knowledge (Plato, Theetete, 150b/151c). It was then taken up

    again by John Dewey (1929/1960), with a view to democratization of the classroom.These days, many researchers affirm the positive impact of class discussions, either by

    placing emphasis on the discussion techniques themselves (e.g., Brookfield & Preskill,

    1999) or by emphasizing the interrelations between social interaction and language

    abilities (e.g., Cazden, 1988; Garnier, Bednavz, & Ulanovskaya, 1991; Wells, 1999).

    Philosophy for Children (P4C) is among these approaches that cultivate learning

    through discussion. Numerous researchers have shown that this approach constitutes

    a significant tool for development of logical reasoning among young people

    (e.g., Lane & Lane, 1986; Camhy & Iberer, 1988; Gazzard, 1988; Lago-Bernstein,

    1990). The philosopher Matthew Lipman and his colleagues at Montclair State

    University (New Jersey) devised P4C in the 1970s (Lipman, Sharp, & Oscanyan, 1980).The approach is now used in nearly 50 countries, and the accompanying material

    (philosophical novels for the pupils and pedagogical guides for the teacher) has been

    translated into 20 languages. The essence of this approach is found in what Lipman

    calls the community of philosophical inquiry, which has its roots in the Deweyan

    concept of community (see Daniel, 1997). P4C is a so-called Socratic approach, since

    it aims to stimulate the thinking of young people by means of philosophical questions.

    The P4C teaching method is composed of three steps: (1) reading, (2) formulating

    questions, and (3) philosophical dialogue within a community of inquiry. The first

    step involves reading by the pupils of a chapter from a philosophical novel. Thereading is done aloud, with every pupil taking a turn. These two aspects are

    important to ensure peer co-operation. The second step implies that the pupils

    invest themselves in understanding the meanings of the chapter they have read, and

    that they question the concepts or the situations described in the novel. Questioning

    is the core of critical reflection, in that it incites the pupils to enter into a research

    process (Dewey, 1929/1960). Moreover, this step assigns responsibility to the pupils,

    placing them at the forefront of their own education, since through their questions

    the pupils (and not the teacher) develop the agenda for the coming weeks (see

    Cazden, 1988). The objective of the third step is to lead the pupils to hold a dialogue,

    so that together, within a community of inquiry, they can find elements of answersrelevant to the questions formulated in the preceding step. Dialogue is therefore

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 335

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    3/22

    situated within a perspective of cooperation, as each individual intervention

    contributes to enriching the groups perspective.

    A true community of inquiry is manifested when dialogue among peers is

    characterized by pluralism, reciprocity and tolerance (Lipman et al., 1980). Within

    the community of inquiry, pupils develop higher-order thinking skills and social-moral attitudes (Daniel, Lafortune, Pallascio, & Schleifer, 2000).

    P4C adapted to Mathematics (P4CM) is a philosophical approach that parallels the

    Lipmanian model, but which concentrates essentially on philosophical/mathematical

    ideas and concepts. P4CM was proposed by Daniel, Lafortune, Pallascio, & Sykes,

    (1996a,b) in order to foster more significant learning of mathematics among pupils

    10 to 12 years of age, and to stimulate their dialogical critical thinking, notably by

    assisting them in taming, together with their peers, prejudices (e.g., Is there such a

    thing as a mathematics wizard?), concepts (e.g., the value of zero, infinite vs.

    indefinite, probability vs. chance) and notions (e.g., the four mathematicaloperations) inherent in mathematics. The philosophical/mathematical material

    stimulates the young people to ask questions like, Does a perfect cube exist on

    Earth? If we had the chance to count all the stars in the sky, would we be able to say

    that they were infinite or indefinite in number? Does truth exist? Were mathematics

    invented or discovered? P4CM is a complementary tool (one hour per week) to be

    used in conjunction with the regular mathematics teaching program.

    This paper presents the results of a research project analyzing processes by which

    critical thinking develops in pupils aged 10 to 12 years when they use the P4CM

    material. The objective is not to test Lipmans teaching model, but rather to describe

    the cognitive development in pupils when they are significantly stimulated in thisdirection. We started off with the postulate that P4CM could fulfill this role.

    This article first presents definitions of critical thinking and presents a typology of

    exchanges among pupils. It then focuses on presenting a model of the development of

    the critical thinking process in pupils aged 10 to 12 years. An illustration of this

    developmental process is presented using transcripts of verbal exchanges among a

    group of Quebec pupils. Finally, a model of the process of learning to think critically

    is proposed, a model that brings together the types of exchanges among the pupils

    with the elements that constitute critical thinking.

    Critical Thinking

    Currently, critical thinking remains much discussed but ill-defined (see Kennedy,

    Fisher, & Ennis, 1990), with various conceptions coexisting. Psychology and

    philosophy have both contributed in their own way to our comprehension of critical

    thinking. Psychologists are mostly concerned with the thinking process and how this

    process can help people bring meaning to their experience. Psychologists emphasize

    problem solving (Catrambone & Holyoak, 1989; Needham & Begg, 1991).

    Philosophys contribution finds its roots in the writings of Socrates, Plato and

    Aristotle. Ever since these beginnings, philosophers have been interested in usinglogical reasoning as a moral force to promote good.

    336 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    4/22

    Critical Thinking as Concept

    A priori, we share Lipmans (1988) conception of critical thinking. From the

    perspective of pragmatist philosophy (Daniel, 1997), it clearly surpasses abstract

    logical analysis. Its starting point is situated in the interactions among peers, and itsintention is to improve individual and social experience. In other words, we share

    the Lipmanian definition of critical thinking because it is in keeping with the

    socioconstructivist triangle to which we subscribe: social interactions*/global

    development of the pupil*/social development (Daniel, 2005).

    Lipman (1988) regarded critical thinking as a useful tool for countering uncritical

    thinking and thoughtless action. Individuals need critical thinking to help them

    distinguish the most relevant information they receive (in relation to the objectives

    they pursue). Lipmans (1988, 1991, 1995) definition of critical thinking includes four

    criteria: (1) using particular criteria, (2) forming judgments, (3) self-correcting, and

    (4) sensitivity to context. The Lipmanian definition of critical thinking is well

    respected in North America (Johnson, 1992). Yet it should be noted that it is not the

    sole authority on the subject. Ennis (1993), Paul (1992), and Siegel (1988) have all

    promulgated alternatives to Lipmans approach to critical thinking, each of which

    places a greater emphasis on rationality.

    Teaching/Learning Critical Thinking

    The teaching of critical thinking can be conducted using three types of objectives

    that are not mutually exclusive, but are rather positioned on a continuum:(1) information-acquisition objectives; (2) self-development; and (3) personal and

    social development and higher-order thinking objectives. The information-acquisi-

    tion objective focuses on the transmission of knowledge and the evaluation of

    knowledge comprehension. This objective would be the equivalent of what Maier

    (1933) called reproductive thinking or learned behaviour, in contrast to

    reasoning or productive behaviour. Because of the sort of thinking skills required

    during repetition or repetitive exercises associated with information-acquisition goals

    (Newman, 1990), pedagogical activities oriented toward this goal primarily develop

    lower-order thinking skills.

    Teaching objectives centered on self-development highlight values such as respect

    and personal experience, as well as reflection concerning self-development according

    to these values. The development of critical thinking in this context occurs through

    comprehension of the environment, and lies within the scope of an intra-subjective

    perspective where each justification, each meaning, each interpretation, is accepted

    without question.

    Teaching objectives aimed at personal and social development and higher-order

    thinking consider the individual as part of society, and assume that the education of

    the former must serve to improve the latter. This strategy of development is heuristic

    in that it presupposes that the ultimate goal of teaching is to stimulate young peopleto question. Within this perspective, the development of critical thinking takes place

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 337

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    5/22

    via a praxis and lies within the scope of inter-subjectivity. Social interactions, and

    particularly discussions among peers, are fundamental here. Indeed, discussion

    among peers contributes to showing pupils their own limits and the importance of

    cooperation. Furthermore, discussion among peers stimulates higher-order thinking

    skills (Cazden, 1988).The preponderance of research projects dealing with learning critical thinking takes

    for granted that such a capacity cannot be developed in young subjects, particularly

    those of elementary school age. Literature reviews (e.g., Jones & Idol, 1990) do bring

    to light the development of logical reasoning, of creativity, and of meta-cognition in

    young adolescents, however. Although each of these thinking modes is a component

    of critical thought, when considered separately, they do not constitute critical

    thinking. Critical thinking is more complex, because it encompasses at least two

    cognitive modes (logical and creative) if not three (some add meta-cognitive or

    caring-thinking). The first objective of our project is thus to study whether youngpeople attending elementary school can demonstrate critical thought when

    adequately stimulated in this direction through a discussion-based curriculum.

    Our second objective is to discover the components of critical thinking as it is

    manifested in pupils aged 10 to 12 years. Our third objective is to introduce a model

    of the development process of critical thinking in these pupils.

    A Typology of Exchanges Among Pupils

    In order to fulfill these research objectives, we had to build our study on a concreteand observable basis that is, on the discourses of pupils (Van der Zee & Nikanne,

    2000). Another question was thus addressed: Are young people attending elementary

    school capable of holding a dialogical and critical exchange with their peers? Our first

    task thus consisted of studying the nature of the exchanges among pupils. This task

    was accomplished in a precursor study to the current one (Daniel et al., 2002). Five

    types of exchanges emerged from that discourse analysis: anecdotal, monological,

    non-critical dialogical, semi-critical dialogical and critical dialogical.

    Anecdotal Exchanges

    An anecdotal exchange refers to an exchange in which pupils are unconcerned by peer

    points of view. Also, the pupils do not justify their viewpoints, which they typically

    address only to the teacher. Another characteristic is that the anecdotal exchange

    takes place using the word I, the pupils having difficulty in turning their focus from

    their own experiences (Mexico, Group 1, first transcript):

    Facilitator: In the story, why didnt Ramon like mathematics exams?

    P1: I get nervous during exams.

    P2: Because sometimes I, because I worry.

    P3: Because I get nervous.

    338 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    6/22

    Monological Exchanges

    In a monological exchange, the discussion is focused on the question selected by the

    group. Furthermore, pupils interventions are brief; they resemble simple answers

    addressed to the teacher much more than well-thought-out statements aimed atgroup improvement. Also, in a monological exchange, pupils have difficulty in

    justifying their viewpoints (Quebec, Group 1, first transcript):

    P1 (showing the facilitator the cube he has just drawn): My cube is perfect.

    Facilitator: Tell us why its a perfect cube.

    P1: Im not sure.

    Facilitator: Its certainly a cube isnt it?

    P1: It looks like one.

    Facilitator: Is it a perfect cube?

    P1: Yes.

    Dialogical Exchanges

    A dialogical exchange, which presupposes listening skills, is one in which the

    participants listen to divergent perspectives and integrate them in order to enrich

    their viewpoints. Dialogical exchange is characterized by interdependence of view-

    points and respect for diversity. At this point, analysis of the transcripts showed that

    all dialogical exchanges are not necessarily critical, and can in fact be subdivided into

    non-critical, semi-critical, and critical. A non-critical dialogical exchange is a

    dialogue, but it remains simple, without evaluation of viewpoints (Mexico, Group2, second exchange):

    Facilitator: Why do you say that geometry is interesting?

    P1: Because its part of our everyday life.

    P2: Thats true because in school for example were now learning figures

    and when were older and want to buy some land we can figure out

    how much land area we own.

    P4: I agree with P2. And also because with geometry for example

    architects can build schools, buildings and everything, stores and

    everything we need in everyday life as P1 said.

    Semi-Critical Dialogues

    Other transcripts showed that the dialogue is sometimes verges on the critical, but

    not yet fully critical. In this type of exchange, the pupils dialogue and formulate

    criticisms in the form of oppositions, doubts, etc. Furthermore, these criticisms are,

    on one hand, more or less well-justified and, one the other hand, they do not succeed

    in influencing the pupils concerned. Consequently, the initial perspective remains

    unchanged at the end of the exchange we named this semi-critical dialogue (Mexico,

    Group 2, third transcript):

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 339

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    7/22

    P1: First you have to learn because if you dont learn how can you understand?P2: But I dont agree with P1 when he says first you have to learn. . .First you

    have to understand and figure out what youre going to do then you learn itso you can see if its right or not.

    P1: How can we understand mathematics if we dont learn it?P4: I think you have to learn things properly to be able to understand. Because

    first you learn the numbers and then you can understand how to use themhow to apply them.

    P5: I think that right now in the sixth grade what were doing is understanding.There are things that we have already learned. . .but maybe we understoodmore or less and maybe we have to learn them all over again to understandthem more clearly.

    P1: I think first we learn because how could I understand numbers if no-one evertaught them to me? To understand a formula like the base times the heightfirst you have to have learned it.

    Critical Dialogical Exchanges

    A critical dialogical exchange often takes on the appearance of a negotiation of

    viewpoints, a transaction among pupils, an open process in which the conclusions,

    when they are spoken, are open and temporary, serving as a hypothesis for future

    reflection. In this type of exchange, the initial idea that prevails at the beginning of a

    discussion is modified or nuanced at the end (Australia, Group 1, third transcript):

    Facilitator: Last week, we worked on the notion of order; the order of numbers

    and digits, and the hierarchy between humans and animals. Wouldsomeone like to summarize or pursue last weeks discussion?P1: It depends on the context. It depends if were talking about humans

    from the point of view of their inventiveness or of their instinct. And Ithink that humans are more intelligent than other animals in theirinventiveness. But then again, it might not be true. In other animalseyes, we may not be more intelligent, because other animals actaccording to their needs, not their desires, like us.

    P2: I think humans are the only ones that can do mathematics. Humansinvented English and mathematics. Math is like another language weinvented. We use it to understand things, to do the things we have todo well, to understand the reasons behind things. Like why the sky isblue and why cant we float or fly. So we invented mathematics toexplain these things. But animals, they just think sky and they dontreally think, they dont really think about the sky. Because they have, iffor us eating and mating are an instinct, for them, its their principalinstinct. . .If its about intelligence, I think humans are at the top ofthe list.

    Facilitator: Why? On what criteria do you base yourself?P2: On how complex they are. And also on the fact that we have other

    kinds of intelligence, like we said last week, empathy, sympathy andthings like that.

    P1: Well, for me, my theory is that we were a couple of different speciesplaced on Earth as a test, to see if we could evolve.

    Facilitator: Who placed them on Earth?

    340 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    8/22

    P1: The universe. The universe is like. . .were a cell inside billions of cells. Itslike a gigantic cell that will always keep on multiplying. And were just anordinary cell. Like a cell inside our body that is made of cells that are made ofcells. And it has nothing to do with intelligence. It has to do with whether wewill evolve or not.

    P3: Then there would be like two different paradigms.P4: Yes, theres the intelligence to think about how to make things and theres the

    intelligence about how youre going to use those things. Were both the moststupid and the most intelligent.

    (For the full text of this last extract, see Daniel et al., 2002.)

    The preceding typology of student exchanges, developed in earlier data analyses

    (Daniel et al., 2002), provided a comprehensive portrait of possible types of pupils

    exchanges in discussion. The typology permitted us to proceed to examine the central

    questions that concerned us: Can young people attending elementary school achieve

    critical thinking when adequately stimulated in this direction? If so, how does criticalthinking manifest itself and develop in these young people?

    Methods

    In accordance with the Grounded Theory approach (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), which

    inspires our process of analysis, we did not intend to deductively verify existing

    theoretical foundations regarding critical thinking. Neither did we aim to measure the

    impact of P4CM on the development of critical thinking (we did not use control

    groups). Instead, from the transcripts of exchanges among pupils, we intended to

    describe the manifestations of critical thinking in pupils*/using P4CM as apedagogical support*/in order to extract the elements likely to lead to a model of

    the process of development in critical thinking. Indeed, the purpose of the Grounded

    Theory is a construction of theories empirically based on social phenomena, about

    which few analyses have been articulated (Laperriere, 1997, p. 310). And to theorize,

    is to extract the meaning of an event, it is to link various elements of a situation in

    an explanatory schema, it is to renew the comprehension of a phenomenon by

    shedding a different light upon it (Paille, 1994, p. 149).

    Since diverse sources and data are crucial for qualitative research aimed at theory

    development (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), the research project was conducted in threegeographical contexts: Australia, Mexico and Canada (Quebec). These countries

    offered diversity in culture (see Gumperz & Hernandez-Chavez, 1972; Tsuneyoshi,

    2004) and in language (English, Spanish and French). In addition, the research sites

    offered diversity in curricula regarding teaching mathematics: Australias public

    school system has for a number of years placed particular emphasis on the

    development of logical reasoning in the learning of mathematics (Australian

    Educational Council, 1994). Mexicos public school curriculum has placed an

    emphasis on democratization and technological development (Ramirez, 1997).

    Quebec has oriented its mathematics programs toward the development of judgment

    and problem solving (Ministere de lEducation du Quebec, 2001). Diversity withregard to socio-economic status was likewise present. Four milieus were privileged,

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 341

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    9/22

    three were underprivileged, and one was poor (see Barraza & Walford, 2002; Chance,

    1986; Kennedy et al., 1990). Finally, diversity also extended to the childrens

    experience with the P4C approach. One group was composed of pupils, some of

    whom had practiced the P4C philosophical approach since preschool; another group

    was in its second year of practicing P4C; while the other six groups were novices tothe approach.

    Eight groups of pupils were studied, two in Australia, three in Mexico and three in

    Quebec. Each group was composed of an average of 30 pupils aged 10 to 12 years, for

    a total of 240 pupils. The classes were mixed, with approximately 50% girls and 50%

    boys. The analysis did not focus on specific individuals, but attempted to bring to

    light manifestations of critical thinking in the groups of pupils as a whole.

    The experimental protocol consisted of conducting philosophical discussions

    among peers as recommended by the P4CM approach (Daniel et al., 1996b). The

    program took place during one full school year, at a frequency of one hour per week.For each of the groups, three video recordings were prepared of the philosophical

    exchanges among the pupils. Each recording was 60 minutes, the length of the pupils

    weekly philosophical exchanges. The first recording was conducted at the beginning

    of October in Quebec and Mexico, and at the beginning of February in Australia. The

    second recording was completed at the beginning of February in Quebec and Mexico,

    and at the end of May in Australia. The third and final recording was conducted at

    the end of May in Quebec and Mexico, and at the end of September in Australia. In

    total, we obtained 24 video recordings, each representing 30 to 32 pages of transcript

    (approximately 8,500 words). A professional transcribed the video recordings in full.

    The transcripts were then returned to the teachers for verification of discussioncontent. Although the head of research, responsible for transcript analysis, spoke all

    three languages, to ensure greater rigor in the analysis, the 24 transcripts were

    translated into her mother tongue.

    To establish data reliability within a qualitative framework, the 24 transcripts of

    exchanges among pupils were first blind coded by the principal researcher, that is, all

    data that could allow the pupils or the groups or the time period to be identified were

    removed. Then, six weeks later, the same person again subjected the transcripts to a

    second blind coding. Finally, excerpts from each transcript were submitted to the co-

    researchers in their original languages for analysis and subsequent coordination.Where a divergence arose, the researchers exchanged views until a consensus was

    reached. Below are the coding stages we followed:

    (1) We coded each discussion to determine the inherent cognitive skills displayed

    (statement, justification, example, etc.). Subsequently, we grouped these emergent

    codes (and skills) into categories. Four temporary categories related to thinking

    modes (logical, creative, responsible, and meta-cognitive) emerged. We were familiar

    with certain traditional definitions associated with each thinking mode (e.g., Jones &

    Idol, 1990). However, in accordance with the Grounded Theory approach, we

    attempted to de-emphasize these, and to focus as much as possible on the pupils

    contextualized manifestations of critical thought, our objective being to analyze dataarising from the elementary school pupils group discussions.

    342 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    10/22

    At the conclusion of this stage of coding, a general definition was attributed to each

    of these thinking modes. Thus, logical thinking referred to informal logic that

    presupposes convergence and coherence in language. Creative thinking referred to a

    quest for meaning. It was manifest in the production of items or relations that were

    more or less original or divergent, and likely to orient the groups perspectivedifferently. Thinking was defined as responsible to the extent that pupils invested

    themselves in reflection with regard to human behavior and to moral rules. Meta-

    cognitive thinkingreferred to a capacity to evaluate thinking and viewpoints in order

    to improve them (for more details of this coding see Daniel et al., 2004).

    (2) We analyzed the transcripts of the exchanges once more, this time paying

    particular attention to the manner in which these four modes of thinking increased in

    complexity as the exchanges progressed, moving from anecdotal or monological

    toward critical dialogical. This analysis led us to identify three epistemological

    perspectives, which we named egocentricity, relativism and inter-subjectivity. Withregard to egocentricity, within this perspective the pupils viewpoints were mere

    statements grounded in their own experience; they were not justified. In relativism,

    tolerance and open-mindedness toward plurality characterized the exchanges, but

    reflection did not include doubt nor evaluation of the points of view. As for inter-

    subjectivity, this perspective was only manifested at the end of the school year, and

    not in all the groups of pupils. The inter-subjectivity perspective presupposed that

    pupils were motivated to construct meanings with the help of their peers; their

    privileged instrument was critical evaluation; their statements were justified and

    regularly characterized by uncertainty (for more details see Daniel et al., 2004).

    (3) We cross-tabulated the cognitive modes (logical, creative, responsible, and

    metacognitive) and the epistemological perspectives (egocentricity, relativism, and

    inter-subjectivity) inherent in the development of critical thinking processes. In this

    process it proved helpful to characterize a distinction between content and form. The

    resulting matrix appears in Table 1.

    (4) In the final phase of the analysis, reported in the present paper, we applied

    the constituent elements of the dialogical critical thinking process (Table 1) to

    the transcripts of pupils exchanges. That is, we used the 4 (cognitive mode)/3

    (epistemological perspective) matrix devised in Phase 3 as an instrument for

    analyzing each groups transcripts. The objective was to better comprehend and tomodel the development of critical thinking processes among groups of pupils. The

    following section of the paper is based on this fourth phase of the analysis.

    Results

    Illustration of the Development of the Dialogical Critical Thinking Process

    To present an illustration of the childrens development of the dialogical critical

    thinking process as shown in Table 1, we used excerpts from the exchanges of a group

    of Quebec pupils. In the first transcript (October), the pupils discussed the followingquestion:Does a perfect cube exist?In the second transcript (February), they turned

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 343

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    11/22

    Table 1 Development of the Dialogical Critical Thinking Process */C

    Types of thought

    Epistemologicalperspectives Logical Creative Responsib

    1. EgocentricityContent Statement based on sensory

    observation of a personalor of a particular fact

    Statement that givesmeaning to the personalpoint of view

    Response related own behavior

    Form No justification Statement of units Simple response

    2. RelativismContent Statement based on

    generalizations stemming fromthe senses and reason

    Statement that givesmeaning to anothersviewpoint

    Response relatedpeers particular b

    Form Incomplete or concretejustification

    Contextual relations Attempt to undepeers responses

    3. Inter-subjectivityContent Statement based on

    simple reasoning(conceptualization)

    Statement that conveys adivergent meaning(transformation)

    Response relatedrules (categorizat

    Form Spontaneous and elaboratedjustification

    Evaluation of meanings Manifestations of

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    12/22

    their attention to the question:When you draw shapes, are you doing geometry? And

    in the third transcript (May), they discussed the game of chess and strategies for

    success. The sequence showed the increasing complexity of thought on the

    epistemological level as the group became anchored in philosophical praxis.

    Epistemological Complexity of Four Thinking Modes

    Logical thinking.At the level of content, logical thinking tends toward conceptualiza-

    tion, and at the level of form, toward the ability to justify ones viewpoints. In the

    transcripts for the entire school year from one of the groups of Quebec pupils, logical

    thinking developed in the following manner.

    At the time of the first recorded small group interaction in October, the group of

    pupils exchanged using mostly the logical thinking mode, neglecting other thinking

    modes. At the level of content, the pupils contributions were based either on a

    concrete referent or on a generalization that combined observation by the senses withreasoning, whereas with regard to form, the majority of pupils did not justify their

    statements:

    Facilitator: Does a perfect cube exist?P1: A die could be a perfect cube.Facilitator: P2, does a perfect cube exist?P2: I dont want to say there is no such thing, but I havent really seen

    one.

    In this excerpt, the pupils needed to base their comments on concrete observation.

    They drew little upon abstraction, stemming from reasoning alone. Also, the pupils

    simply stated their viewpoints, they did not justify them using reasons or criteria.With reference to Table 1, and regarding the logical thinking mode, the group was

    therefore situated within perspective 1, egocentricity, for both content and form.

    By February the pupils thinking had become more complex, and generally

    displayed more than one cognitive mode in its expression. Regarding logical thinking,

    which still dominated the discourse, the transcript was characterized by thinking that

    was slightly more abstract than in the first transcript (see Klausmeier, 1990). Pupils

    did question one another without the facilitators intervention, and reasoning was

    more explicit in their answers, although pupil interventions were still based on a

    system of reference rooted in observation:P1: When you draw, you make shapes, but then are you doing geometry at the

    same time?P2: Before grade 6, before we started talking about this [P4CM], I drew like this.

    But ever since we started talking about it, when I draw, I always questionmyself.

    P3: Before, I drew like this and I didnt think it was geometry, but now since westarted talking about it, I tell myself it always has to be well done.

    The answers were no longer short sentences comprised of simple words, but rather

    consisted of one or two complete sentences with logical relations of time, of cause to

    effect, etc. At the level of form, in February the pupils generally supported theirstatements with justifications (. . .because . . .), but only to the extent that an adult

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 345

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    13/22

    prompted them to do so. With reference to Table 1, we estimated that the group was

    situated within perspective 2, relativism, for both content and form.

    At the time of the final recorded interaction in May, the pupils had progressed in

    the logical mode; their reasoning was more elaborate in the sense that the basis of

    their argumentation rested on concepts. For example, while the pupils discussed thequalities necessary for success in chess, P1 mentioned self-confidence, and others

    added, in a critical manner:

    P2: [Among] the qualities to play chess, there is self-confidence, yes, but you also

    have to pay attention. If you dont pay attention, if you listen to someone

    talking and you dont pay attention to his move, you wont know whats

    happening and then. . .

    P3: You also need logic. Lets say you place your queen on a diagonal to a bishop,

    it isnt logical because the bishop is going to take your queen. And this is one

    of the main rules. Which means you have to have a lot of logic.

    P4: You have to know how to think, its good for logic. Also to be able to keep

    quiet during the game, to concentrate. If just as you are ready, you have to

    speak with someone who tells you Youre no good, youre no good you

    wont see whats going on in the game. This is why keeping quiet during a

    game of chess is good.

    In this excerpt, we noted that pupil contributions were based on abstract concepts

    (self-esteem, paying attention, being logical, knowing how to think), which led pupils

    to specify, to infer, and to make significant judgments. At the level of form, we also

    noted that pupils spontaneously supported their points of view by justifications.

    According to Table 1, the group was situated within perspective 3 regarding contentand form, which we refer to as inter-subjectivity.

    Creative thinking. At the level of content, creative thinking strives toward the

    transformation of ideas and meanings, and at the level of form, toward their

    evaluation. In the transcripts from the same group of Quebec pupils, creative

    thinking developed during the year in the following manner.

    In the October transcripts we very rarely noted manifestations of creative thinking.

    The pupils discussions remained relatively convergent. That is, they reproduced

    rather than transformed meanings.By February, expressions of creative thinking were more frequent. Such thinking

    was mostly centered on comprehension (as opposed to active transformation) of

    meanings, and was often manifested as a question:

    P1: Before, you said that when you were drawing and that you thought about it,

    you were doing geometry, but when you didnt think about it, you were doing

    some. I dont understand what youre saying. . .When youre drawing, even if

    you arent thinking about it, you still think its geometry?

    Here, P1 was not trying to evaluate his peers point of view. Rather, he wanted to

    understand the meaning of his peers intervention, and to do so, he contextualized hiscomments. Indeed, the theme discussed created a problem in P1s mind, and he

    346 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    14/22

    searched for meaningful relations. With reference to Table 1, we assigned the group to

    perspective 2, relativism, at the content and form levels.

    At the final recorded discussion (May), creative thinking appeared in a great many

    conversational turns. Creation of meaning through the use of an example to illustrate

    a personal point of view (egocentrism) or a peer point of view (relativism) was veryevident. At the level of content, we also observed several contributions that we

    considered to be more complex, because of the divergence of perspective they

    included (inter-subjectivity). By virtue of this divergence, the pupil remarks enriched

    the exchanges. On the other hand, the pupils contributions did not succeed in

    modifying the exchange because, at the level of form, the divergent viewpoints were

    merely juxtaposed (relativism), and were not used as criteria in order to evaluate, to

    place in a hierarchy, to prioritize, or to choose the most relevant definition (as would

    be required to demonstrate inter-subjectivity).

    P1: The strategy is the plan to reach a solution.P2: Yes, but sometimes the strategy can be the solution.P3: Yes, but you have to create a strategy to find the solution you want.

    This group was at least occasionally characterized by creative thinking, which

    establishes critical relations between the various meanings given, and which

    contributes to modifying either the context or the initial idea. The following

    example illustrates high-level creative thinking.

    P1: Its about the reasons why your strategy might not work. Sometimes, itswhen your strategy isnt strong enough for your opponent, or maybe its that

    hes better than you are, or that he moved a game piece that, according toyou*/in your mind, he wasnt supposed to move for your strategy to work.So thats one thing that can make your strategy fail. Then theres alsoconcentration that can keep your strategy from working. You tell yourselfIm not sure this is going to work. Those that get here do this, but Im notsure. Youre not giving yourself any self-confidence.

    In the preceding excerpt, the pupil not only presented points of view that were

    different from those previously stated in the community of inquiry, but she also

    presented them as hypotheses (or. . .or. . .or. . .) that she related to a perspective of

    critical evaluation of peer points of view. Here, complexity of creative thinking was

    therefore manifested in the transition from the search for a convergent meaning to

    the divergent evaluation of meanings. This situated the discourse within perspective

    3, inter-subjectivity.

    Responsible thinking.At the level of content, responsible thinking appears when pupils

    invest themselves in reflection*/first, with regard to human behavior, and then to

    moral rules (categorization of specific actions). At the level of form, responsible

    thinking appears when pupils question and evaluate behaviors, rules, or principles.

    Below is the progression in which responsible thinking was manifested in the group.

    In October and February, responsible thinking did not constitute a cognitivemodality that was characteristic of the group.

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 347

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    15/22

    In May, however, responsible thinking was manifested when some pupils reflected

    upon the causes of defeat in the game of chess.

    Facilitator: In this instance, what would be the causes of defeat?P1: It depends on what the other one did.

    P2: Maybe its because. . .

    Lets say that you were looking at what theother one just did and you told yourself: on my next turn I willcertainly play this move, to be sure to get it. And the opponentdoesnt play it (as expected). Then its your fault too, you know. Youtold yourself he could do that but he didnt.

    In this example P2, unlike P1, tried to understand the causes of this defeat by

    examining both parties at issue. To do so, at the level of content, what she attempted

    to explain was related to the behavior of a peer. Furthermore, she was concerned with

    making P1 understand her point of view. The intervention was thus in all respects

    situated within relativism.

    Meta-cognitive thinking. At the level of content, meta-cognitive thinking signifies

    thinking about thinking (ones own and that of peers) and exercising a certain control

    over it instead of simply being pulled along by it. At the level of form, meta-cognitive

    thinking presupposes evaluation and correction of ones thinking. Below is the manner

    in which meta-cognitive thinking developed in the group as the months passed.

    In October, meta-cognitive thinking was rarely observed. In February, however, at

    the level of content meta-cognitive thinking was manifested when pupils agreed or

    disagreed with the points of view stated by their peers (relativism). Participants

    typically began their comments by explicitly repeating a preceding remark on which

    they based their comments, and sometimes by naming the pupil who voiced it and

    describing the point of view at issue (Before, you said that . . . or, I dont agree

    with her because. . .). At the level of form, the pupils began their intervention by

    reformulating, as exactly as possible, the comments of their peers (relativism). In

    other words, they were situated more within description than within evaluation. Thus

    in February, the pupils discourse was not random; on the contrary, they controlled it

    quite well. With reference to Table 1, we considered meta-cognitive thinking to be

    expressed primarily within perspective 2 of relativism.

    In the final set of transcripts recorded in May, since the exchange was of adialogical nature, that is to say, each successive student remark rested on the

    preceding one, we could say that meta-cognitive thinking was an integral part of the

    pupils discourse. At the level of content, meta-cognitive thinking was manifested

    when making references to strategies or mental processes, and to their construction.

    P1: Its about the reasons why your strategy might not work. Sometimes, its whenyour strategy isnt strong enough for your opponent, or that he moved agame piece that, according to you*/in your mind, he wasnt supposed tomove for your strategy to work. So thats one thing that can make yourstrategy fail. Then theres also concentration that can keep your strategy fromworking. You tell yourself Im not sure this is going to work. Youre notgiving yourself any self-confidence.

    348 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    16/22

    In this instance, at the level of form, it was not only the statement of mental processes

    that was at stake, but also the evaluation of their relation. A high degree of conceptual

    complexity was evident. The group was therefore ascribed to perspective 3, inter-

    subjectivity, for both content and form.

    In this group of Quebec pupils, the preceding series of discussion extracts revealsthe increasing complexity of dialogical critical thinking during their philosophical

    praxis. On one hand, thinking modes were increasingly varied; at the beginning of the

    school year, pupil interventions mostly reflected one mode of thinking (logical), while

    at the end of the year, all four modes of thinking were displayed. On the other hand,

    the groups epistemological perspective increased in complexity between the

    beginning and the end of the year, shifting from egocentricity to relativism and

    even, in some cases, to inter-subjectivity.

    The preceding section, because it was meant to illustrate the development of the

    dialogical critical thinking process, was based on transcripts of the exchanges within asingle group of pupils. In the following section, we introduce elements of a full model

    of dialogical critical thinking. To do so, all 24 of the transcripts of exchanges between

    pupils in the eight groups were considered.

    Modeling of Dialogical Critical Thinking

    In response to our objective of modeling the learning process of critical thinking in

    pupils, analysis of the transcripts revealed two fundamental criteria: multi-modality

    of thought and epistemological complexity.

    Multi-modality. Unlike the works of authors such as Lipman (1988, 1991, 1995),

    Ennis (1993), Paul (1992), and Siegel (1988), in our study, multi-modality refers to

    four thinking modes (logical, creative, responsible, and meta-cognitive). Analyses of

    the transcripts of exchanges, as illustrated above, revealed the prevalence of multi-

    modal thinking. For example, logical thinking was present in all of the exchanges,

    whether of the anecdotal, the monological or the dialogical type of exchange. On the

    other hand, we observed meta-cognitive thinking only when the exchanges between

    pupils were of the dialogical type. This is because dialogue is a discursive co-

    construction that presupposes not only listening, but also recognizing the soundnessof others points of view and perspectives (Buchler, 1978).

    And we observed that the dialogical type of exchange became critical when the

    young people, aware of the distinctions between various verbalized perspectives,

    adjusted or modified their viewpoints accordingly (meta-cognitive thinking). These

    two elements (taking into account the comments of others and correcting) are

    inherent in meta-cognitive thinking as it emerged from our analyses. This finding

    brings us to posit that, without skills related to meta-cognitive thinking in the

    discourse, there cannot be a critical dialogical type of exchange. However, the

    converse also applies. That is, along with Roubtsov (1991), Lipman et al. (1980),

    Cazden (1988), Wells (1999) and other socio-constructivists who follow Vygotsky, weconsider that communication among peers is what ensures complexity of reflection.

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 349

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    17/22

    Furthermore, we observed in the student discussions that creative thinking was

    instrumental in transforming the level of exchange and in enabling a transition from

    monological to dialogical exchange. Conversely, the more the dialogue reflected the

    criteria for critical status, the more creative thinking became manifest in the pupils

    discourse. The explanation for this finding is that skills related to creative thinking arepresent when providing divergent meanings, unexpected points of view, and critical

    evaluations of meanings. Critical evaluation often produces cognitive conflict in

    peers, which in turn triggers a more elaborate process of reflection, that is, creative

    thinking.

    Finally, we observed among the pupils who had experience with P4C that in their

    most critical exchanges, responsible thinking was present (e.g., the extract presented

    in the section Critical Dialogical Exchange). We explain this presence by the fact that

    truly dialogical critical thinking does not aim for personal victory over others points

    of view, but rather improvement of the groups, or of societys perspective (see Paul,1992; Daniel et al., 2000). In other words, development of the responsible mode

    among the pupils tempers the exchanges and orients the discussion toward a vision of

    mutual understanding and of active participation in the construction of the moral

    rules inherent in every group and in every society.

    Epistemological complexity.Along with multi-modality, the discussion data revealed

    another concomitant of the development of critical thinking: epistemological

    complexity. For dialogical critical thinking to develop, the simple presence of the

    four thinking modes in the pupils discourses may be necessary, but it is not

    sufficient. In our transcripts, dialogical critical thinking was observable only when wedetected also the capabilities associated with inter-subjectivity, that is, conceptualiza-

    tion, transformation, categorization and correction. It is within inter-subjectivity

    that statements are argued and justified, relations are seen as divergent and meanings

    are evaluated, judgments are inclusive and social-moral rules are questioned, and

    perspectives are evaluated and corrected. From the perspective of dialogical critical

    thinking, multi-modality and epistemological complexity are two inseparable

    indicia.

    The sequence of learning dialogical critical thinking. In all eight groups participating in

    the experiment, the development of critical thinking followed the same sequenceregardless of culture or language. The only difference noted concerned groups

    which had previous experience with P4C, as these began with a head start and

    finished one step ahead. Following is a description of the general sequence we

    observed.

    At the beginning of the school year, the pupils communication demonstrated two

    types of exchange: anecdotal (rarely) and monological (mostly). Analysis of the

    transcripts that reflect these types of exchanges indicated that the pupils discourses

    reflected a preponderance of concrete logical thinking.

    At mid-year, the pupils exchanges were dialogical. However, the dialogue was non-

    critical. Analysis of the transcripts that reflected this type of exchange indicated thatthe pupils discourses reflected the use of three thinking modes: semi-abstract logical

    350 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    18/22

    thinking, contextualized creative thinking, and meta-cognitive thinking. Overall, the

    non-critical dialogical type of exchange reflected a relativism of perspective in which

    the pupils enjoyed accumulating as many points of view as possible.

    At the end of the school year, the transcripts revealed exchanges that were largely of the

    semi-critical dialogical type. Discourse analysis reflected all four thinking modes: logicalthinking, metacognitive thinking, creative thinking, and responsible thinking. In this

    phase, the groups wavered between relativism and inter-subjectivity, between unreflective

    acceptance of peers points of view and conscious evaluation of those points of view.

    Those groups with prior experience in P4C ended the year more solidly grounded

    in critical dialogue and in inter-subjectivity.

    Conclusions

    The starting point for this study was exchanges among peers, since discourse is

    considered to be the concrete manifestation of thought. In contrast to numerous

    existing studies on social interactions, and specifically on classroom communication

    between peers, we emphasized the critical aspect of these exchanges. A first important

    contribution of this study is that it succeeded in inducing the emergence of a process

    of learning critical dialogue among young people aged 10 to 12 years when they

    employed a philosophical approach centered on mathematical concepts.

    Although assessing the impact of P4CM was not among our research objectives, it is

    nonetheless interesting to note the role of these classroom discussions in the pupils

    cognitive development. In this research project, the steps in the development process

    were achieved with the help of a philosophicalpraxis(rather than a rigid technique),which was regular (one hour per week) and continuous (extending over at least one

    school year). Moreover, it appealed to teachers Socratic abilities, which consist of

    stimulating thought in their pupils with questions such as: What is your justification

    when you say. . .? What criteria are you basing this on? Do you have a counter-

    example? What could you add to improve this point of view? Do you agree with

    what was just said? How would you organize the criteria that were just voiced? How

    has the groups perspective evolved between the beginning and the end of the

    exchange? It would be opportune to verify, in a future research project, the impact

    of other non-philosophical pedagogical approaches centered on class discussions on thedevelopment of young people critical thinking. A priori, two conditions seem

    necessary: (1) that these approaches lie within the scope ofpraxis, and (2) that their

    objectives encompass scaffolding young people in the development of multimodal and

    complex thought. This scaffolding is necessary, as these dimensions of thought not

    infrequently plunge early adolescent pupils into ambiguity and uncertainty, which can

    at times render the exchangeuncomfortable. Indeed, multimodal and complex thinking

    presuppose not only a cognitive investment, but also an affective and social investment.

    With Dewey (1929/1960), Lipman (1995), Wells (1999) and others, we consider that

    classroom discussions must be situated within the cooperative context of a community

    of inquiry in order to better stimulate pupils toward a common reflection that favorsaccess to inter-subjectivity.

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 351

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    19/22

    A second major contribution of our study resides in the modeling of the process of

    learning dialogical critical thinking among young adolescents 10 to 12 years of age. In

    most studies, critical thinking is thought to emerge within the context of the cognitive

    development of young adults attending college and university. In contrast, our study

    focused on preadolescents attending elementary school. The results showed that withdiscussion-based praxis, such pupils can develop higher-order thinking skills and

    attitudes related to critical. While a number of other studies have examined the

    development of various thinking modes (in particular logical reasoning, conceptualiza-

    tion, meta-cognition, creativity and problem solving) in young people, these thinking

    modes have been generally examined separately (e.g., Jones & Idol, 1990). Our model is

    more global, in that it integrates four thinking modes and shows them operating

    concurrently. In addition, the model explicated here has the merit of combining these

    four modes with a second theoretical axis related to epistemology, in order to better

    capture the complexity of the development of critical thinking. Following is ourdefinition of dialogical critical thinking that summarizes the constituent elements:

    Dialogical critical thinking is a process of (e)valuating the object of thought, incooperation with peers, in an attempt to eliminate irrelevant criteria in order tocontribute to improvement of experience. Dialogical critical thinking is a process thatensues from a common quest, which manifests itself in cognitive skills and attitudesrelated to conceptualization, transformation, categorization and correction. It there-fore requires the contribution of four cognitive modalities, namely logical, creative,responsible and meta-cognitive thinking, which are conjoined with a complexepistemological perspective, inter-subjectivity oriented toward meaning. As a result, anew understanding of the object of thought is generated, and a modification of the

    initial idea occurs.

    Learning to dialogue in a critical manner and learning to reflect critically is a complex

    task for young people, and takes time. Nevertheless, the ensuing learning acquired

    (that is, learning to manage diversity of opinions for collective enrichment;

    understanding that uncertainty and ambiguity are a necessary transition toward

    attaining a significant solution; perceiving the evaluation of acquired knowledge as a

    fundamental activity; grasping that criticism is a privileged instrument in the

    evolution of viewpoints and perspectives, etc.) is in keeping with a form of education

    that will allow young people to successfully face the challenges that are inherent in the

    expansion of knowledge and in the increasing complexity of life in society.

    References

    Australian Education Council. (1994). Mathematics: A curriculum profile for Australian schools.

    Carlton: Curriculum Corporation.

    Barraza, L., & Walford, R. (2002). Environmental education: A comparison between English and

    Mexican school children. Environmental Education Research, 8, 171/186.

    Brookfield, S., & Preskill, S. (1999). Discussion as a way of teaching. Tools and techniques for

    democratic classrooms. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

    Buchler, J. (1978). What is a discussion? Thinking, 1 (1), 49/54.

    Camhy, D., & Iberer, G. (1988). Philosophy for children: A research project for further mental and

    personality development of primary and secondary school pupils. Thinking, 7(4), 18/26.

    352 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    20/22

    Catrambone, R., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving

    transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, & Cognition,15, 1147/1156.

    Cazden, C. (1988). Classroom discourse. The language of teaching and learning. Portsmouth, NH:

    Heinemann.

    Chance, P. (1986). Introduction: The thinking movement. In P. Chance & R. S. Brandt (Eds.),

    Thinking in the classroom: A survey of programs(pp. 1/9). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Daniel, M.-F. (1997). La philosophie et les enfants [Philosophy and children]. Montreal: Logiques.

    Daniel, M.-F. (2005). Pour lapprentissage dune pensee critique au primaire[For the apprenticeship

    of critical thinking in elementary school]. Quebec City: Presses de lUniversitedu Quebec.

    Daniel, M.-F., Lafortune, L., Pallascio, R., & Sykes, P. (1996a). Les aventures mathematiques de

    Mathilde et David [The mathematical adventures of Mathilde and David]. Quebec City: Le

    Loup de Gouttiere.

    Daniel, M.-F., Lafortune, L., Pallascio, R., & Sykes, P. (1996b). Philosopher sur les mathematiques et

    les sciences[Philosophize on mathematics and science], Accompanying Guide. Quebec City:

    Le Loup de Gouttiere.

    Daniel, M.-F., Lafortune, L., Pallascio, R., & Schleifer, M. (2000). Developmental dynamics of a

    community of philosophical inquiry in an elementary school mathematics classroom.

    Thinking, 15(1), 2/10.

    Daniel, M.-F., Splitter, L., Slade, C., Lafortune, L., Pallascio, R., & Mongeau, P. (2002). Are the

    philosophical exchanges of pupils aged 10 to 12 relativistic or intersubjective? Creative and

    Critical Thinking, 10(2), 1/19.

    Daniel, M.-F., Splitter, L., Slade, C., Lafortune, L., Pallascio, R., & Mongeau, P. (2004). Dialogical

    critical thinking: Elements of definitions emerging in the analysis of transcripts from pupils

    aged 10 to 12 years. Australian Journal of Education, 48(3), 295/313.

    Delors, J. (Ed.). (1996). lEducation, un tresor est cache dedans. Rapport a lUNESCO de la

    Commission Internationale sur lEducation pour le Vingt et Unieme Siecle[Education, in it a

    treasure is hidden. Report to UNESCO from the International Commission on Education for

    the 21st Century]. Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Dewey, J. (1960).The quest for certainty: A study of the relation between knowledge and action. New

    York: Capricorn Books. (Originally published 1929)

    Ennis, R. (1993). Critical thinking assessment. Theory into Practice, 32(3), 179/186.

    Garnier, C., Bednarz, N., & Ulanovskaya, I. (Eds). (1991). Apres Vygotski et Piaget. Perspectives

    sociale et constructiviste. Ecoles Russe et Occidentale[After Vygotsky and Piaget. Social and

    constructivist perspectives. Russian School and Occidental School]. Brussels: De Boeck

    University.

    Gazzard, A. (1988). Evidence of effectiveness of the Philosophy for Children program: Quantitative

    studies. Thinking, 7(4), 13/15.

    Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Strategies for qualitative

    research. Chicago: Aldine.

    Gumperz, J., & Hernandez-Chavez, E. (1972). Bilingualism, bidialectalism, and classroom

    interaction. In C. Cazden, V. John, & D. Hymes (Eds.), Functions of language in the

    classroom (pp. 84 /108). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Jones, B. F., & Idol, L. (Eds). (1990).Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction. Hillsdale, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Kennedy, M., Fisher, M., & Ennis, R. (1990). Critical thinking: Literature review and needed

    research. In B. F. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp.

    11/41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Klausmeier, H. J. (1990). Conceptualizing. In B. F. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and

    cognitive instruction (pp. 93/138). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Lago-Bernstein, J. C. (1990). The community of inquiry and the development of self-esteem.

    Thinking, 9(1), 12/17.

    Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years 353

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    21/22

    Lane, N. R., & Lane, S. A. (1986). Rationality, self-esteem and autonomy through collaborative

    enquiry. Oxford Review of Education, 12, 263/275.

    Laperriere, A. (1997). La theorisation ancree: Demarche analytique et comparaison avec dautres

    approches apparentees [Grounded theory: Analytical process and comparison with similar

    methods]. In J. Poupart, J.-P. Delauriers, L. H. Groulx, A. Laperriere, & A. Pires (Eds.), La

    recherche qualitative: Enjeux epistemologiques et methodologiques [Qualitative research:Epistemological and methodological stakes] (pp. 309/346). Boucherville: Gaetan Morin.

    Lipman, M. (1988). Critical thinking: What can it be? Educational Leadership, 46(1), 38/43.

    Lipman, M. (1991). Thinking in education. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Lipman, M. (1995). Good thinking. Inquiry. Critical Thinking Across Disciplines, 15, 37/41.

    Lipman, M., Sharp, A. M., & Oscanyan, F. (1980). Philosophy in the classroom . Philadelphia, PA:

    Temple University Press.

    Maier, N. (1933). An aspect of human reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 24, 144/155.

    Ministere de lEducation du Quebec. (2001). Programme de formation de lecole Quebecoise:

    education prescolaire, enseignement primaire[Program of education in Quebec: Kindergarten

    and elementary school]. Quebec City: Les publications du Quebec.

    Needham, D. R., & Begg, I. R. (1991). Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogicaltransfer. Memory & Cognition, 19, 543/557.

    Newman, F. M. (1990). Higher-order thinking in teaching social studies: A rationale for the

    assessment of classroom thoughtfulness. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 22, 41/46.

    Paille, P. (1994). Lanalyse par theorisation ancree [Analysis with grounded theory]. Cahiers de

    Recherche Sociologique[Journal of Research in Sociology], 23 , 147/181.

    Paul, R. (1992). Critical thinking: What, why and how. New Directions for Community College,77,

    3/25.

    Platon. (1950). Theetete. In Platon (Trans. Leon Robin), uvres Completes [Complete Works]

    (pp. 83/193). Paris: Gallimard.

    Ramirez, G. (1997). Leducation aux droits de lhomme et a la democratie: Defis de la societe

    mexicaine [Education to human rights and to democracy: Challenges of the Mexicansociety],Revue des Sciences de lEducation[Journal of Educational Sciences], XXIII(1), 113/

    123.

    Roubtsov, V. (1991). Lactivitedapprentissage et les problemes de formation de la pensee theorique

    des ecoliers [Learning activity and the problems related to the development of theoretical

    thinking in students]. In C. Garnier, N. Bednarz, & I. Ulanovskaya (Eds.), Apres Vygotski et

    Piaget. Perspectives sociale et constructiviste. Ecoles Russe et Occidentale [After Vygotsky

    and Piaget. Social and constructivist perspectives. Russian School and Occidental School]

    (pp. 151/162). Brussels: De Boeck University.

    Siegel, H. (1988). Educating reason: Rationality, critical thinking and education. New York:

    Routledge.

    Tsuneyoshi, R. (2004). The new foreigners and the social reconstruction of differences: Thecultural diversification of Japanese education. Comparative Education, 40, 55/81.

    Van der Zee, E., & Nikanne, U. (2000). Cognitive interfaces: Constraints on linking cognitive

    information. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry. Towards a sociocultural practice and theory of education.

    Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Received November 5, 2003

    Accepted February 18, 2005

    354 M. Daniel et al.

  • 8/11/2019 Dialogical Critical Thinking in Pupils Aged 10 to 12 Years

    22/22