Development and Preliminary Standardization of the Extended Complex Figure

download Development and Preliminary Standardization of the Extended Complex Figure

of 15

description

Development and Preliminary Standardization of the Extended Complex Figure

Transcript of Development and Preliminary Standardization of the Extended Complex Figure

  • This article was downloaded by: [Moskow State Univ Bibliote]On: 29 January 2014, At: 19:31Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

    Journal of Clinical and ExperimentalNeuropsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncen20

    Development and PreliminaryStandardization of the ExtendedComplex Figure Test (ECFT)Philip S. Fastenau aa Michigan State UniversityPublished online: 04 Jan 2008.

    To cite this article: Philip S. Fastenau (1996) Development and Preliminary Standardization of theExtended Complex Figure Test (ECFT), Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18:1,63-76, DOI: 10.1080/01688639608408263

    To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639608408263

    PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

    Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (theContent) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

    This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

  • Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1996, Vol. 18, NO. 1, pp. 63-76

    1380-3395/96/1801-63$12.00 0 Swets & Zeitlinger

    Development and Preliminary Standardization of the Extended Complex Figure Test (ECFT)*

    Philip S. Fastenau Michigan State University

    ABSTRACT

    Recognition and matching trials were designed for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT). Following pilot testing and expert review, they were standardized using 90 community-dwelling adults (58% female, ages 30 to 88). Recognition has 30 multiple-choice items for different figural elements; scores distributed normally with strong item-total correlations and with normally distributed item difficul- ties. Cronbach alphas were .84, .61, and .81 for the Total, Global, and Detail Scales. Recognition correlated .81 with ROCFT recall and .65 with Visual Reproductions. Matching has 10 multiple-choice items; scores were negatively skewed with a substantial ceiling effect. Alpha for Matching was .58, limited in part by few items. Matching correlated .h8 with Judgment of Line Orientation and .74 to .90 with copy trials. Both Recognition and Matching discriminated 34 patients with intractable epilepsy from 34 matched controls. Overall, Recognition appeared to be reliable and showed evidence of validity. By comparison, Matching reliability and validity were less impressive and warrant further examination.

    In addition to the psychometric standards that apply to all psychological and educational tests (American Psychological Association, 1985; Anastasi, 1982), a visual-spatial memory test should contain several standard features. First, comparison between immediate and delayed trials provides an index of consolidation (reten- tion or rate of forgetting). Second, comparison between free recall and recognition perfor- mances clarifies the relative contributions of encoding and retrieval processes to total memo- ry performance. Diagnostically, inclusion of both recall and recognition measures increases the sensitivity of the memory test:

    Failure to recognize words [or figures] as having been previously presented is a more

    reliable sign of memory disorder than failure to recall, and recognition failure denotes a more severe disorder as well. The use of re- call and recognition tests together makes it possible to detect subtle, early signs o f im- pairment (Squire, 1986, p. 280). Third, constructional and perceptual deficits

    must be ruled out as confounds in memory per- formance. Visual-spatial memory tests often require patients to draw figures f rom memory. Low memory scores may be indicative of motor or praxis limitations only, thereby masking in- tact visual memorization skills. Thus, a copy trial is essential to examine constructional abil- ity for the memory stimuli. In addition, a match- ing trial using those same stimuli is desirable to

    * This study was partially funded by the APA Science Directorate. The author acknowledges A1 Manning and Broughton Hospital, Morganton, 3, for supporting the pilot; Jane Holmes Bernstein for her expert input; Norm Abeles, Lauren Harris, Neal Schmitt, and Bert Karon for suggestions on the standardization study; John Fisk, Jeanne Bennett, and Henry Ford Hospital for supporting the clinical validation study; Natalie Denburg, Linda Sloan, Eric Fertuck, Jennifer Winer, Sandy Scott, Katy Parcells, Lidia Domitrovic, and Mike Finton for assisting with data collection and processing; Roger Halley for mobilizing material resources; and Dana Atkinson Fastenau for her loving support. Address correspondence to the author at University of Michigan Medical Center, Neuro- psychology Program, 480 Med Inn Building, Box 0840, 1500 East Medical Center Drive, Ann Arbor, Michigan,

    Accepted for publication: June 15, 1995. 48 109-0840, USA.

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 64 PHILIP S. FASTENAU

    verify accurate perception, which is integral to performance on all of the other trials (copy and recall).

    Therefore, any measure of visual-spatial me- mory should contain immediate and delayed tri- als, recall and recognition trials, and matching and copy trials. Of the many tests of visual-spa- tial memory that exist in our field, few even ap- proximate these standards. For example, the Re- curring Figures Test (Kimura, 1963), the Recog- nition Memory Test (Warrington, 1984), and the Delayed Recognition Span Test (DRST; Moss, Albert, Butters, & Payne, 1986) measure only recognition, without recall, and none of these includes a matching trial.

    The Benton Visual Retention Test (BVRT; Benton, 1974) measures only free recall, which depends on graphomotor output. It contains nei- ther a recognition trial nor a matching trial. Fur- thermore, the BVRT has been criticized for the simplicity of its stimuli (Hemsley, 1974; Zu- brick & Smith, 1978, as reported in Lezak,

    The Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS- R; Wechsler, 1987) contains two subtests that measure visual-spatial memory. Visual Repro- ductions (VR) and Figural Memory (FM) require recall and recognition, respectively, but use separate stimuli, thereby precluding direct comparisons between unfacilitated and facili- tated retrieval for the same figures. Several in- vestigators have expanded VR into a more com- prehensive memory tool. Kaplan (1988) describ- ed adding recognition items, and data have been reported for a recognition trial (Domitrovic, Denburg, & Fastenau, 1995; Hanger, Montague, & Smith, 1991) and for a matching trial (Domitrovic et al., 1995). Fastenau and Sloan (1993) added a copy trial.

    Even with the recognition, matching, and copy trials, VR may be an inadequate measure of visual-spatial memory in many cases. First, the figures are relatively simple, and most are symmetrically organized. Consequently, they are more likely to be encoded verbally (see Reed, 1974) and they may not sufficiently tax the upper range of visual-spatial memory capac- ities (see Palmer, 1977). Second, although the four or five items in Kaplans and Hangers rec-

    1983, pp. 450-451).

    ognition trials may suffice as screening instru- ments, they may be too few for reliable and sen- sitive diagnostics.

    The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941; Rey & Osterrieth, 1993), like VR, measures free recall only. As an advantage over VR, the ROCFT uses an intricate stimulus that is asymmetrical in its design. The complexity of this stimulus seems to better tax the upper range of visual- spatial processing as compared to the VR stim- uli. Furthermore, it appears to be more resistant to verbal mediation (Casey, Winner, Hurwitz, & DaSilva, 1991). As a product of this complexity, patterns of fragmentation, neglect, rotation, and distortion on the ROCFT correspond to some degree with the location and type of neurologi- cal insult (e.g., Binder, 1982; Brouwers, Cox, Martin, Chase, & Fedio, 1984; Kaplan, 1988; Lezak, 1983; Milberg, Hebben, & Kaplan, 1986).

    The ROCFT administration that is most popu- lar (Knight, Kaplan, & Ireland, 1994; Lezak, 1983) includes a copy and immediate recall trial, followed 20 to 60 min later by a delayed recall trial. However, there is neither recognition nor matching. In the three studies presented here, recognition and matching trials were developed to supplement the ROCFT. These trials were designed to follow the copy, immediate free re- call, and delayed free recall trials. This elabo- rated administration will be called the Ex- tended Complex Figure Test (ECFT). This ar- ticle describes the design of the ECFT, pilot re- sults and expert review, preliminary standard- ization with a relatively healthy population, and preliminary validation with a clinical sample.

    PILOT STUDY

    DESIGN OF THE NEW MEASURES

    Initially, 20 recognition items were designed for the ROCFT using theory, findings in the litera- ture, and patient records. Figure l exemplifies the format. Each item consisted of a vertical ar- ray of five choices: one element from the origi- nal ROCFT stimulus and four distractors, or

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE ECFT 65

    U

    Fig. 1. Sample recognition item featuring an Outer Configurational element in a left-specific design (Left Detail subscale item).

    foils, that contained common and/or clinically significant errors.

    Because of its complexity, the Rey figure can be processed in a variety of ways. However, analysis of common errors (e.g., Binder, 1982; Kaplan, 1988; Lezak, 1983) and analysis of pat- terns by which people organize the stimulus fea- tures while drawing them (Waber & Holmes, 1985, 1986) support the inference that people tend to perceive and encode the Rey figure ac- cording to the 18 units identified by Osterrieth (Osterrieth, 1944; Rey & Osterrieth, 1993).

    The classification of the constructional ele- ments by Waber and Holmes (1985, 1986) fur- ther guided the development of the instrument. Base rectangle (BR) and main substructure (MS) elements comprised the Global Scale; these in- cluded the large rectangle, the diagonal cross, and the horizontal and vertical midlines. Outer configuration (OC; e.g., cross at far left, dia- mond at far right) and internal detail (ID; e.g., circle with three dots, five horizontals in upper left quadrant) elements comprise the Detail Scale. The items sampled fairly representatively from the different scorable constructional ele- ments of the complete stimulus figure (Oster- rieth, 1944; Rey & Osterrieth, 1993).

    Within the Detail Scale, some items were de- signed to be left-specific for the assessment of left-side neglect (Figure 1). For these items, the discriminating features were concentrated on the left side of the drawing for all of the choices so that the patient would have to attend to that side of the page and that side of each figure to dis- criminate between the choices.

    For subjects 13 years of age and older, BR and MS units are typically drawn first, followed by the more detailed elements (Milberg et al., 1986; Waber & Holmes, 1985, 1986). Conse- quently, for the recognition task, Global ele- ments were presented before Detail elements. In addition, different constructional elements were alternated (e.g., BR, MS, BR, MS; OC, ID, OC, ID) to prevent comparisons across consecutive items and to limit the extent to which previous choices could provide cues in successive re- sponses.

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 66 PHILIP S. FASTENAU

    METHOD

    Subjects Using the 20 items described above, Fastenau and Manning (1992) conducted a pilot study to examine the psychometric properties of the new recognition items. The sample consisted of 42 nonimpaired volun- teers. Twenty-nine subjects (69%) were hospital em- ployees; 13 volunteers were solicited from an intro- ductory psychology class at a nearby university. Many of the participants were White (93%) and female (76%). Age ranged from 18 to 55 years with a mean of 32.5 (SD = 12.2); education varied from 12 to 18 years with a mean of 13.6 (SD = 1.6). The group had mean peer-equivalent scaled scores of 10 (SD = 2.2, range 6-15) for the Picture Completion subtest and 10.5 (SD = 2.2, range 6-15) for the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Re- vised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981). A demographic- based formula (Wilson, Rosenbaum, & Brown, 1979) yielded a mean estimated IQ of 104.5 (SD = 5.0, range 91-1 16).

    Procedure Subjects were tested individually. The ROCFT copy trial was immediately followed by a free recall trial. During a 20-min delay, subjects completed Picture Completion and Block Design from the WAIS-R. A delayed free recall trial for the ROCFT was then ad- ministered, followed by the recognition task.

    RESULTS

    Cronbachs alpha for the recognition task was 0.68 (p < .001). Three items detracted from the overall reliability; deletion of those three items would raise alpha slightly (0.70). Recognition total scores (number correct) correlated moder- ately with raw scores on Picture Completion ( r = .62, p < .001) and Block Design ( r = .60, p < .001). For Recognition, most corrected item-to- tal correlations ranged from .26 to .60 (p < .05); four were not significant ( r < .200, p > .05).

    Recognition scores distributed fairly normally between 12 and 20 with the exception of two outliers in the lower tail (scores of 5 and 8) and a slight ceiling effect. The mean was 15.7 (SD = 3.0) with the outliers and 16.2 (SD = 2.2) with- out the outliers.

    DISCUSSION

    As evidence of reliability, an index of internal consistency for the new instrument indicated fairly homogeneous content. With regard to con- struct validity, modest but significant relation- ships between the two WAIS-R visuospatial subtests and the recognition task were indicative of convergence in a common domain (visuo- spatial functions) yet without complete redun- dancy. In addition to reliability and validity, this pilot study showed that most items effectively discriminated between good and poor perform- ers on the task. Therefore, gross psychometric indices with a small sample of nonimpaired adults justified further development of the rec- ognition task.

    PRELIMINARY STANDARDIZATION

    FINAL REVISIONS IN ITEM DESIGN

    Recognition Some revisions were made based on the pilot results and based on other research. One item was dropped because the item-total correlation was negative, indicating that those subjects with good overall performances tended to fail that item. Right-specific items were added based on the results obtained by Ogden (1987). She found that, when speech impairments could be con- trolled in studies of neglect, neglect in the right visual field was as frequent as neglect in the left visual field, although right neglect tended to be less severe and less enduring. Because the rec- ognition trial can circumvent language deficits, it seemed especially important to add right-spe- cific items to the Detail Scale. These revisions expanded the set to 27 items.

    Expert appraisal was solicited for initial eval- uation of the instruments content validity and for suggestions to improve on its design. Hol- mes-Bernstein has examined the design qualities of the ROCFT and has used it extensively with children (see Waber, Bernstein, & Merola, 1989; Waber & Holmes, 1985, 1986). She reviewed the recognition items and suggested additional modifications, including the addition of some

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE ECFT 67

    distractors that would reflect idiosyncrasies in normal and neurological children (J. H. Bernstein, personal communication, September 12, 1990). In addition to these revisions, some original items were modified to better reflect errors made by the normal pilot group. This was expected to eliminate the ceiling effect observed in the pilot study.

    The final revised set contained 30 recognition items, which formed the Total Scale. As further evidence of content validity, the proportions of BR items (.lo), MS items (.13), OC items (.40), and ID items (.37) were shown to approximate the proportions in Osterrieths criteria used for scoring copy and recall constructions (.06, .17, .44, and .33, respectively; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey & Osterrieth, 1993). Seven items comprised the Global Scale; 23 items comprised the Detail Scale. Nine of the 23 detail items were left-spe- cific (Left-Detail Subscale); 1 1 were right-spe- cific (Right-Detail Subscale).

    Matching Matching items were created from I0 of the 30 recognition items by placing each vertical array next to a reproduction of the standard (Figure 2 ) . The matching set included one base rectangle, one main substructure, four left-detail, and four right-detail items, All 10 items constituted the Total Scale; the left-detail items and the right- detail items comprised the Left- and Right-De- tail Subscales, respectively. The total adminis- tration of the ROCFT and the two supplemen- tary trials will hereafter be referred to as the Extended Complex Figure Test (ECFT; Fastenau & Denburg, 1994).

    HYPOTHESES

    Recognition ECFT Recognition Total scores were expected to distribute normally in this normal sample, with no ceiling or floor effects. Every item was expected to correlate positively with the Total score, indicating that each item discriminates between good and poor performance overall. In addition, there was an attempt to achieve a wide range of item difficulties (easy items to foster a

    m m m Fig. 2. Sample matching item featuring a Main Sub-

    structure element.

    sense of confidence in less-able learners and difficult items to challenge the most skillful lea- rners) with a mean and mode approaching .50 (50% of the subjects answering an item incor- rectly), the point of maximum discrimination. Cronbachs alpha reliability coefficient, an in- dex of internal consistency, were expected to be very good for the Total Scale and moderate for the other scales and subscales.

    As evidence of construct validity, ECFT Rec- ognition was expected to correlate positively with other measures of visual-spatial memory (convergent validity). Discriminant validity would be demonstrated by much smaller corre- lations with non-memory visual-spatial mea- sures.

    Matching ECFT Matching was designed to be a much ea- sier task. Total scores were expected to skew negatively with a prominent ceiling effect. It was predicted that difficulty indices would be very low and that item-total correlations would be limited by a restriction of range on the Total

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 68 PHILIP S. FASTENAU

    scores. Cronbachs alpha was expected to b e good for the Total Scale and modest for the subscales, limited substantially by the few num- ber of items. ECFT Matching was expected to correlate highly with another measure of visual- spatial perception, as evidence of convergent validity. For discriminant validity, lower corre- lations were expected with visual-spatial mem- ory measures.

    METHOD

    Subjects The normative sample was comprised of 90 healthy community-dwelling adults who reported no recent or active central nervous system conditions and who lived independently in the community. Volunteers with uncorrected visual or hearing impairment or with impaired use of the preferred hand were excluded. Subjects were solicited from four religious organiza- tions in a midwestern city of 150,000 residents. The organizations received a monetary contribution for each of the participants from their group, together with a bonus for recruiting equal numbers of men and women from each of ten 5-year age bands (30-34,35- 39, .._ 70-74, 75-and-over). This incentive created an age- and sex-stratified sample; furthermore, because each organization was equally represented among men and women and across age groups, potential socioeco- nomic differences between organizations were un- likely to confound age and sex analyses.

    The total normative sample consisted of 38 men and 52 women. Age ranged 30 to 80 years with one 88-year-old ( M = 55.9, Mn = 54.5, Sf) = 14.1). The younger group (ages 30 to 54, n = 47) was 55% fe- male, and the older group (ages 55 and beyond, n = 43) was 61 % female. These sex ratios closely approxi- mate the 1990 U.S. census (51% and 57%, respecti- vely; U. S. Department of Commerce, 1990). Educa- tion ranged 8 to 25 years (M and M n = 15.2, SD = 3.0). In this sample, 97% had at least a high school education, which is higher than the 75% observed na- tionally among people over age 25 (U. S. Department of Commerce, 1990). Among the older adults, 88% had 12 or more years of schooling, compared to 56% nationwide (U. S. Department of Commerce, 1990). Therefore, this sample was more educated than the average U.S. citizen. Age-corrected WAIS-R Vocabu- lary scale scores ranged 5 to 19 ( M and Mn = 12.5, SD = 2.3).

    A structured interview assessed the past history of potentially confounding health conditions. It address- ed the following conditions (percent of the sample with a positive history): closed-head injury with loss of consciousness (16%). unexplained loss of con-

    sciousness @%), cerebrovascular disease (4%), hydro- cephalus (O%), seizures (1 %), intracranial surgery ( 1 %), hypertension (3 I%, all well-controlled), coro- nary artery disease (18%), diabetes (lo%, all well- controlled), pulmonary disease (lo%), renal disease (16%), and hepatic disease (2%).

    Levels of depression were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, 1978); the mean and median scores were well within normal limits (5.3 and 4, respectively). Eight percent of the sample scored in the mildly depressed range, and 2% scored in the moderate to severe range; these percentages correspond with national incidence rates, indicative that the sample is representative on this dimension. Levels of reactive and chronic anxiety were assessed using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 39833; summary indices for State scores ( M = 30.8, Mn = 29, SD = 8.8) and for Trait scores ( M = 32.8, Mn = 31.5, SD = 8.9) were virtually identical to those for the standardiza- tion sample.

    Instruments Table 1 provides the descriptive data for all of the measures analyzed in this study. The battery included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Vo- cabulary subtest (Wechsler, 198 1) and Judgment of Line Orientation Test (JOLO; Benton, Hamsher, Var- ney, & Spreen, 1983). Wechsler Memory Scale-Re- vised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) Visual Reproduc- tions (VR) Immediate and Delayed Recall were also included, followed by a copy trial for those same stim- uli (Fastenau & Sloan, 1993).

    The ECET trials were administered in the follow- ing order: Copy, Immediate Recall (no latency after the copy trial), Delayed Recall (20-min latency), Rec- ognition, and Matching. Scoring criteria for ECFT drawings were modeled after the WMS-R VR scoring (Wechsler, 1987). Interrater reliability (two raters) on 23 sets of drawings that spanned a wide range of abil- ity was good for the copy drawings (Pearson product moment r = .90) and very good for immediate and delayed recall drawings (Y = .97 for both).

    Procedure All subjects were tested individually. Most subjects completed the exam in one session; several older sub- jects required two sessions for optimal testing. The testing for all subjects consisted of two segments of cognitive testing (each lasting 50 to 75 min), sepa- rated by a break during which they completed the emotional inventories. The ECFT was administered in one segment, and the WMS-R was administered in the other segment. The order of the segments (ECFT first or WMS-R first) was counterbalanced; subjects were assigned to the two segment conditions blindly, strati- fied within each age-sex cell.

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE ECFT 69

    Table 1. Demographic and Test Data, by Group, and T Tests Comparing Patients to Controls. ~ ~~

    Standardization Epilepsy Matched Sample Patients Controls (n = 90) (n = 34) ( n = 34)

    Measure M SD M SD M SD 1 P ~ ~ ~~ ~~ ~ ~

    Demographics

    Age (Years) 55.9 14.1 40.4 9.9 43.9 9.3 -1.49 .I4 Education (Years) 15.2 3.0 13.9 2.3 14.8 2.1 -1.61 . 1 1 % Female 57.8 70.6 64.7 .51 .61 % Left-Handed 7.8 8.9 15.6 -0.84 .40

    Test Scores

    ECFT Copy 47.4 6.1 44.7 8.9 48.0 5.4 -1.83 .04 ECFT Delayed Recall 26.9 9.7 23.3 12.3 28.1 10.3 -1.77 .04 ECFT Recognition 16.0 5.7 15.0 5.8 17.4 5.7 -1.75 .04 ECFT Matching 9.4 1 .0 9.0 1.3 9.5 0.9 -1.62 .05 VR Copy 38.0 3.5 VR Delayed 27.5 9.0 JOLO 25.5 3.9

    Note. T tests were two-tailed for demographics and one-tailed for test scores. ECFT = Extended Complex Figure Test; VR = WMS-R Visual Reproductions; JOLO =Judgment of Line Orientation.

    RESULTS

    The analyses in this study were conducted using SPSS (SPSS, Inc., 1990). Because of the relatively large number of significance tests performed on this data set, precautions were taken to control for alpha inflation. Hypotheses were clearly articu- lated at the outset of the study. Also, a conserva- tive alpha was adopted: Results significant at .05 were regarded as trends; results with p < .01 only were considered to be reliable.

    ECFT Recognition Total scores distributed normally. Descriptives are presented in Table 2; where age or sex ef- fects neared significance (p < .05), the results were stratified accordingly.

    Point-biserial correlations between each item and the Total score were corrected by partialling out the variance in the Total score that was due to the item itself. All coefficients were positive: 26 were significant at p < .01 (rs = .24 - .60); two approached significance ( r = .22 and .18; p < .05); and two others were not statistically sig-

    nificant ( r = .12 and .09; p > .05). Item-diffi- culty indices (percent of the sample that respon- ded incorrectly) ranged from 3% to 85% and distributed roughly normally. The mean item difficulty for the 30 items was 46.5%.

    Correlations among scales and subscales are presented in Table 3. The Global Scale and De- tail Scale correlated moderately but not per- fectly (.75, corrected for unreliability). The Left- and Right-Detail Subscales correlated per- fectly with one another (.99, corrected). Cron- bachs alpha reliabilities are presented on the diagonal. Within the scales and subscales, no item detracted from any of the reliabilities ex- cept for one item on the Left Detail Subscale, and that alpha was reduced by less than .02. Odd-even and split-half reliability coefficients were similar to alpha (.81 and .78, respectively) after the Spearman-Brown correction for length- related attenuation.

    ECFT Recognition scores were correlated with other measures of visual memory and with measures of visual perception (Table 4). After correcting for attenuation due to imperfect reli-

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 70 PHILIP S. FASTENAU

    Table 2. ECFT Recognition Raw Scores. ~

    Group M SD

    Total Scalea

    Total Younger

    Men Women

    Men Women

    Older

    16.0

    20.0 16.1

    14.4 13.7

    5.7

    4.5 5.7

    5 .o 5.6

    Detail Scalea

    Total 11.3 4.6 Younger

    Men 14.9 3.5 Women 11.6 4.4

    Men 9.6 3.6 Women 9.0 4.5

    Older

    Right Detail Subscale'

    Total Younger Older

    6.1 7.0 5.2

    2.4 2.3 2.3

    ~

    Left Detail Subscalea

    Total 3.6 2.0 Younger

    Men 5.1 1.7 Women 3.7 1.8

    Men 3.2 1.6 Women 2.4 1.8

    Older

    Global Scaleb

    Total 4.7 1.8

    Note. Sample sizes were: Total, 88; Younger, 47; Older, 41; Men, 37; Women, 5 1 ; Younger Men, 21 ; Younger Women, 26; Older Men, 16; Older Women, 25. a Age and sex effects (p < .05).

    No age, sex effects (p > .05). Age effect only (p < .05).

    ability, ECFT Recognition Total scores corre- lated positively and highly with ECFT Delayed Recall and with VR Delayed Recall (.81 and .65, respectively). Correlations with immediate trials of ECFT and VR were virtually redundant of those with delayed trials and, therefore, were not

    tabulated. ECFT Recognition correlated with ECFT Matching, ECFT Copy, and VR Copy (.26 - .46) more weakly than it correlated with mem- ory measures ( t > 2 . 3 7 , ~ '. .01, one-tailed), as de- terminedusing the t test for dependent correlations (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). However, the correlation

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE E C R 71

    Table 3. Correlations Among ECFT Scales and Subscales by Trial.

    Recognition Trial

    GLO DET L-DET R-DET TOT

    Global (GLO) .61 .75 .65 .77 1.02 Detail (DET) .53 .81 1.24 1.24 1.18 Left Detail (L-DET) .39 .86 .59 .99 1.15 Right Detail (R-DET) .49 .9 1 .62 .66 1.18 Total (TOT) .73 .97 .8 1 .88 .84

    Matching Trial

    L-DET R-DET ~~

    TOT

    Left Detail (L-DET) .40

    Total (TOT) .82 Right Detail (R-DET) .55

    3.07 .08 .92

    .99 4.27 .58

    Note. Diagonal values (italicized) are Cronbachs alpha reliabilities. Values above the diagonals are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the scales; values below the diagonal are not corrected. All correlations are significant (p < ,0005, one-tailed). GLO = Global Scale, DET = Detail Scale, L-DET = Left-Detail Subscale, R- DET = Right-Detail Subscale, TOT = Total Scale.

    of ECFT Recognition with JOLO was not signifi- cantly weaker than its correlation with VR De- layed Recall ( t = 1.238, p > .05, one-tailed).

    ECFT Matching Total scores were negatively skewed. They cor- related negatively with age ( r = --.25, p = .Ol) , but not with sex or the interaction term 0, > .05). Scores for the total sample ranged from 6 to 10 ( M = 9.4, Mn = 10.0, SD = 1.0). Scores for the younger group ranged from 7 to 1 0 ( M = 9.6, M n = 10.0, SD = 0.8), whereas scores for the older group ranged from 6 to 10 ( M = 9.3, Mn = 10.0, SD = 1.2).

    Corrected item-total correlations were posi- tive and significant, ranging from .24 (p < .01) to .41 (p < .001). Three items were answered correctly by everyone in the sample, resulting in zero variance. Difficulty indices were very low (ranging from 0% to 18%).

    Cronbachs alphas are presented in Table 3. The Total Scale had a modest inter-item reliabil- ity ( . 58 ) , suppressed in part by three items with zero variance. None of the other items detracted from the Total Scale alpha coefficient. Subscale reliabilities were limited by too few items and

    by items with zero variance. Correlations among scales and subscales can

    be found in Table 3. Correlations between the Total Scale and the other scales are very large, of course, because each subscale is nested within the Total Scale. The correlation (correc- ted for attenuation) between the two unnested subscales, Left Detail and Right Detail, exceeds 1 .OO because of their unreliability.

    The ECFT Matching Total score correlated highly with JOLO (.68) and with perception-in- tensive copy trials (.74 - .90). With one excep- tion, all of ECFT Matching correlations with memory measures (.26 - .64) showed at least a trend toward being significantly smaller than all of its correlations with perception measures (VR Copy vs. VR Delayed, t > 1.66, p \ .05, one- tailed; other comparisons, t > 2.37, p .05, one-tailed).

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 72 PHILIP S. FASTENAU ~

    Table 4. Correlations Among ECFT Trials and Related Measures.

    Memory Perception/Construction

    Memory 1 . ECFT Recognition 2. ECFT Delayed 3. VR Delayed Recall Perception/Construction 4. ECFT Matching 5. JOLO 6. ECFT Copy 7. VR Copy

    1

    .84

    .7 1

    .57

    .18* .46 .38 .39

    2 3 4 5 6 7

    .81 .65 .26** .56 .44 .46 .91 .67 .40 .64 .62 .55 .6 1 .92 .64 .58 .54 .66

    29*** .47 .58 .68 .90 .74 .55 .50 .46 .80 .71 .60 .56 .49 .65 .60 .89 .73 .48 .58 .52 .49 .63 .84

    Note. Diagonal values (italicized) are Cronbachs alpha reliabilities. Values above the diagonals are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measures; values below the diagonal are not corrected. ECFT = Extended Complex Figure Test; VR = WMS-R Visual Reproductions; JOLO = Judgment of Line Orientation. * p < .OS ** p < .01 *** p < ,005 All other correlations, p < .0005, one-tailed.

    DISCUSSION

    ECFT Recognition Predictions regarding the distribution of Total scores and regarding item characteristics were completely supported. Total scores distributed normally; they correlated significantly with age, so descriptives were tabulated for younger and older subjects. Corrected item-total correlations showed that all but two items effectively dis- criminated between higher and lower perfor- mance on the Total score. Item-difficulty indices reflected that the test samples a broad range of ability levels so that there were items within vir- tually everyones capability and items to chal- lenge people with even very good memory abili- ties. Yet, the set of items converges on an item difficulty of 50%, where discrimination is maxi- mized.

    Reliability hypotheses were also well sup- ported. The ECFT Recognition Total Scale had very homogeneous content. Subscale alphas ranged from moderate to high, in rough relation to the number of items on each scale. Each of the items contributed substantially to the integ- rity of its host scale. Correlations among scales and subscales supported the uniqueness of the Global and Detail Scales, but the Left Detail and Right Detail Subscales did not measure any unique variance in this healthy sample. These

    findings were expected because Global-Detail distinctions have been observed elsewhere with nonneurological (albeit much younger) samples (Waber et al., 1989; Waber & Holmes, 1985, 1986). By contrast, left and right hemi-inatten- tion syndromes are rarely observed in healthy persons.

    As predicted, preliminary evidence of validity was obtained. As an index of convergent valid- ity, ECFT Recognition correlated highly with ECFT free recall and moderately with VR free recall. Discriminant validity can be inferred from weaker relationships between Recognition and non-memory trials of the ECFT and VR, although the correlation with JOLO was stron- ger than expected.

    ECFT Matching As predicted, Total scores were negatively skewed. They declined with age, so descriptives were provided for younger and older subjects. Although the relationship with age was statisti- cally significant, the actual raw score diffe- rences were minimal (approximately one-quarter point). The Total sample descriptives should serve the clinician well for preliminary norms.

    Predictions regarding item characteristics were supported. Seven coefficients were posi- tive and moderate, indicating that the items ef- fectively discriminated between higher and

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE ECFT 73

    lower performance on the task as a whole. Three item-total correlation coefficients could not be computed due to zero variance for those items. Item-difficulty indices showed that, unlike ECFT Recognition, ECFT Matching was very easy for most healthy adults and did not discrim- inate among these individuals very well. Data comparing clinical samples with healthy sam- ples and comparing patients with different types or degrees of impairment will be necessary to demonstrate the utility of this measure for dis- criminating perceptual performance in lower ranges of ability.

    The Total Scale had modest internal consis- tency, suppressed in part by three items with zero variance; all of the remaining items con- tributed to the integrity of the scale. The Left- and Right-Detail Subscales were not expected to be reliable due to the very few number of items on each subscale and due to the infrequency of hemispace inattention among healthy adults. This trial will need to be administered to a clini- cal sample, where more variability can be ex- pected, in order to examine the reliabilities of the scale and subscales more fully.

    As expected, convergent and discriminant validity were successfully demonstrated. There was convergence between Matching and an es- tablished measure of visual-spatial perception and with perception-intensive construction mea- sures. As evidence of discriminant validity, cor- relations with memory measures were consider- ably lower, with the exception of a relatively strong relationship with VR Delayed.

    CLINICAL VALIDATION STUDY

    With regard to criterion-related validity, concur- rent validation is considered the most appropri- ate evidence for diagnostic tests (American Psy- chological Association, 1985; Anastasi, 1982). Consequently, the ECFT was administered to a group of patients with intractable epilepsy. In- tractable epilepsy of various etiologies frequent- ly interferes with memory efficiency. Therefore, it was predicted that a group of matched controls would exceed patients on ECFT Recognition. Although epilepsy has not been shown to pro-

    duce discrete or profound deficits in perceptual abilities, it was expected that ECFT Matching would nonetheless grossly differentiate neuro- logical patients from nonneurological patients.

    METHOD

    Subjects The clinical sample was comprised of 34 patients who were undergoing evaluation for surgical treatment of intractable epilepsy. Data on the localization of the foci were not available for this study. A subset of the previously described standardization sample was matched to the patient sample on age, education, sex, and handedness. The descriptives and t-test values (two-tailed) are presented in Table 1.

    Procedure The patients were administered the ECFT as part of a more comprehensive clinical test battery. Most were tested presurgically; for 6 patients, however, only postsurgical data were available. The testing of matched controls is described in the standardization study above.

    RESULTS

    The analyses in this study were conducted using SPSS 6.1 (SPSS, Inc.; 1994). Predictions were directional so one-tailed tests were used. Results at p < .05 were considered reliable.

    ECFT Recognition In this mixed sample, corrected item-total corre- lations were positive: 26 were significant at p < .05 (rs = .24 - .61); four were not statistically significant (i-s = .06 - .18; p > .05). Item-total correlations were similar when each group was analyzed separately. Item-difficulty indices (percent of the sample that responded in- correctly) ranged from 9% to 8 I % and distrib- uted roughly normally. These ranged 15% to 82% for patients and 3% to 82% for controls. The mean item difficulty for the 30 items was 46.0% for the mixed sample, proving slightly more difficult for patients (51.1% for patients, 41.9% forcontrols). Cronbachs alpha reliability was .83-.84 when the groups were analyzed sep- arately and .84 when they were analyzed together. None of the items detracted from alpha

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 74 PHILIP S . FASTENAU

    for either group. Comparisons of the distributions of the two

    groups showed considerable overlap in scores. Nonetheless, the patients scored slightly, and significantly, lower than did matched controls. The differences between the two groups is per- haps best reflected by the patients median and modal score of 14 in comparison to a median of 18 and mode of 2 1 for the matched controls. The other descriptives and t-test values (one-tailed) are presented in Table 1.

    ECFT Matching In the mixed sample, corrected item-total corre- lations were positive: Six were significant at p < .05 (TS = .25 - .49); two were not statistically significant ( T S = .04 - .13; p > .05); two had zero variance (answered correctly by all). Analyzed by subgroup, similar results were obtained from the patient group; for the controls, five items were statistically significant while the other five had zero variance. Item-difficulty indices ranged from 0% to 22%, and their distribution was neg- atively skewed. The mean item difficulty for the 10 items was 9.4% (9.9% for patients, 5.4% for controls). Cronbachs alpha reliability was .58 for the patient group and for the combined group; the control group yielded an alpha of .47, suppressed in part due to half of the items being answered correctly by all controls. None of the items detracted significantly from alpha for ei- ther group.

    Similar to the recognition trial, comparisons of the matching trial distributions of the two groups showed considerable overlap in scores. The patients scored significantly lower than matched controls, but the difference in scores is smaller than that which could be detected clini- cally (one-half point). The median and mode for both groups was 10, a perfect performance. The other descriptives and t-test values (one-tailed) are presented in Table 1.

    DISCUSSION

    For the patient sample, item characteristics and alpha reliability coefficients were virtually iden- tical to those obtained from the matched control

    group and the standardization sample. As one exception, item-difficulty indices were higher for patients. Comparisons between patients and matched controls showed both trials to be sensi- tive to the diffuse disruption of intractable epi- lepsy, although there was considerable overlap between patient and control performances, espe- cially for the matching task.

    GENERAL DISCUSSION

    In this project, recognition and matching trials were designed to supplement the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. Pilot test results were used to refine the original test items. Data from an age- and sex-stratified sample of relatively healthy community-dwelling adults were used to describe the psychometric properties of the in- strument and to generate preliminary norms, although it should be noted that this sample was better educated than the average U.S. citizen. For ECFT Recognition, reliability was observed in the form of internal consistency. Content va- lidity was supported by initial expert review and by comparisons to Osterrieth s scoring system. Construct validity was demonstrated by conver- gence with similar measures and by discrimina- tion from dissimilar measures. As preliminary evidence of concurrent validity, as a group, pa- tients scored lower than did their matched con- trols.

    Psychometric evidence on ECFT Matching was less impressive, perhaps due to the small number of items on the measure. The ceiling effect on the matching trial also limited the psy- chometric characteristics, especially with 30% of the items being answered correctly by every- one in the sample. The distribution of scores did not become any less skewed when patient data were analyzed, and reliabilities remained mod- est. Nonetheless, ECFT Matching did correlate highly with other perception tasks, while corre- lating lower with memory trials. Also, it did dis- criminate between patients and controls in the present study, even if the differences were sub- tle.

    Other forms o f reliability and validity were not demonstrated here. Test-retest reliability is

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • DEVELOPMENT OF THE ECFT 75

    usually an important index, showing the degree of temporal stability. For the ECFT, this may be inappropriate because of the incidental nature of the administration. That is, subjects are not told that they are to remember the figure until after the stimulus has been removed. Upon presenta- tion of the stimulus in the second testing, sub- jects will likely approach the lask differently and encode information intentionally during the copy trial. Another form of reliability, inter- scorer agreement, is less relevant to the new tri- als presented here because they use an objec- tively scored multiple-choice format.

    With regard to validity, formal testing of the construct validity of ECFT Recognition and Matching via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) would be especially valuable. CFA could be used to further verify that Recognition and Matching assess memory and perceptual acuity, respectively. In addition, with CFA one could test whether the ECFT is a measure of visual- spatial functioning, as distinct from verbal func- tioning. Finally, CFA could be applied to con- firm the a priori assignment of the recognition items onto their respective scales and subscales. The size of the present sample was insufficient for those analyses. In addition, more compelling evidence of concurrent validation is warranted. More elaborate clinical trials for ECFT Recogni- tion and ECFT Matching with different diagnos- tic groups are needed to examine diagnostic sen- sitivi ty.

    REFERENCES

    American Psychological Association. (1985). Stan- dards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: Author.

    Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological resting (5th ed.). New York: Macmillan.

    Beck, A. T. (1978). Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.

    Benton, A. L. (1974). Revised Visual Retention Test (4th ed.). New York: Psychological Corp.

    Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney, N. R., & Spreen, 0. (1983). Contributions to neuropsycho- logical assessment: A clinical manual. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Binder, L. M. (1982). Constructional strategies on Complex Figure drawings after unilateral brain

    damage. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4 ,

    Brouwers, P., Cox, C., Martin, A,, Chase, T., & Fedio, P. ( 1 984). Differential perceptual-spatial impair- ment in Huntingtons and Alzheimers dementias. Archives of Neurology, 41, 1073-6.

    Casey, M. B., Winner, E., Hurwitz, I., & DaSilva, D. (1991). Does processing style affect recall of the Rey-Osterrieth or Taylor Complex Figures? Jour- nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol- ogy, 13,600-606.

    Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple re- gression/ correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl- baum.

    Domitrovic, L. A,, Denburg, N. L., & Fastenau, P. S. ( 1 995, February). Recognition and matching trials for WMS-R Visual Reproductions: Psychometric properties. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Seattle, WA.

    Fastenau, P. S. , & Denburg, N. L. (1994, February). Reliability and validity of the Extended Complex Figure Test (ECFT). Paper presented at the meet- ing of the International Neuropsychological Soci- ety, Cincinnati, OH.

    Fastenau, P. S., & Manning, A. A. (1992, February). Development of a recognition task for the Complex Figure Test. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, San Diego, CA.

    Fastenau, P. S., & Sloan, L. E. ( 1 993, February). Copy trial for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Vi- sual Reproductions subtest: Normative study. Pa- per presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Galveston, TX.

    Hanger, P. A., Montague, J. R., & Smith, M. (1991, February). A recognition memory test for the Vi- sual Reproduction subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Paper presented at the annual meet- ing of the International Neuropsychological Soci- ety, San Antonio, Texas, USA.

    Hemsley, D. ( 1 974). Relationship between two tests of visual retention. Perceptual and Motor Skills,

    Kaplan, E. ( 1988). A process approach to neuropsy- chological assessment. In T. Boll & B. K. Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain func- tion: Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 125- 167). Washington, DC: American Psychologi- cal Association.

    Kimura, D. (1963). Right temporal-lobe damage. Ar- chives of Neurology, 8,264-27 I .

    Knight, J. A., Kaplan, E. F., & Ireland, L. D. (1994, February). Survey$ndings of Rey-Osterrieth Com- plex Figure use among the INS membership. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neu- ropsychological Society, Cincinnati, OH.

    Lezak, M. D. ( 1983). Neuropsychological assessment

    5 1-58.

    39, 1132-1 134.

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014

  • 76 PHILIP S. FASTENAU

    (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Milberg, W. P., Hebben, N., & Kaplan, E. (1986). The

    Boston Process Approach to neuropsychological assessment. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiat- ric disorders (pp. 65-86). New York: Oxford Uni- versity Press.

    Moss, M. B., Albert, M. S., Butters, N., & Payne, M. (1986). Differential patterns of memory loss among patients with Alzheimers disease, Hunting- tons disease, and alcoholic Korsakoffs syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 43, 239-246.

    Ogden, J. A. (1987). The neglected left hemis- phere and its contribution to visuospatial neglect. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp. 215-233). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test du copie dune figure complexe. Archives of Psychology (Chicago). 30,

    Palmer, S. E. (1977). Hierarchical structure in percep- tual representation. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 441 - 414.

    Reed, S. K. (1974). Structural descriptions and the limitations of visual images. Memory and Cogni- tion, 2(2), 329-336.

    Rey, A. (1941). Lexamen psychologique dans les cas dencephalopathie traumatique. Archives of Psy- chology (Chicago), 28,286-340.

    Rey, A., & Osterrieth, P. A. (1993). Translations of excerpts from Andre Reys Psychological exami- nation of traumatic encephalopathy and P. A. Osterrieths The complex figure copy test (J. Corwin & F. W. Bylsma, Trans.). Clinical Neuro- psychologist, 7,3-21. (Original works published in 1941 and 1944, respectively).

    Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    SPSS, Inc. (1990). Statistical Package for the Social Sciences: Release 4 . I (Mainframe version, IBM VM/CMS) [Computer program]. Chicago, IL: Au-

    206-356.

    thor. SPSS, Inc. (1994). SPSS 6.1 for Windows [Computer

    software]. Chicago, IL: Author. Squire, L. R. (1986). The neuropsychology of mem-

    ory dysfunction and its assessment. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assess- ment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 268-299). New York: Oxford University Press.

    U. S. Department of Commerce ( I 990). I990 census o f p o p u l a t i o n : S o c i a l a n d e c o n o m i c characteristics--United States. Washington, DC: Author.

    Waber, D. P., Bernstein, J. H., & Merola, J. (1989). Remembering the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: A dual-code, cognitive neuropsychological model. Developmental Neuropsychology, 5( I), 1-15.

    Waber, D. P., & Holmes, J. M. (1985). Assessing chil- drens copy productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experi- mental Neuropsychology, 7,264-280.

    Waber, D. P., & Holmes, J. M. ( I 986). Assessing chil- drens memory productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experi- mental Neuropsychology, 8, 563-580.

    Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition Memory Test manual. Windsor, Berkshire: NFER-NELSON Publishing Company.

    Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Wechsler, D. ( 1 987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora- tion.

    Wilson, R. S., Rosenbaum, G., & Brown, G. (1979). The problem of premorbid intelligence in neuro- psychological assessment. Journal of Clinical Neu- ropsychology, 1,49-53.

    Zubrick, S., & Smith, A (1978, February). Factors uffecting BVRT performance in adults with mute ,focal cerebral lesions. Paper presented at the meet- ing of the International Neuropsychological Soci- ety, Minneapolis, MN.

    Dow

    nloa

    ded

    by [M

    osko

    w St

    ate U

    niv B

    ibliot

    e] at

    19:31

    29 Ja

    nuary

    2014