DataDrivenLanguageLearning:** LearnerProficiency ... · DataDrivenLanguageLearning:**...

52
DataDriven Language Learning: Learner Proficiency, Performance, and Percep9on Nina Vyatkina University of Kansas Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin December 3, 2014

Transcript of DataDrivenLanguageLearning:** LearnerProficiency ... · DataDrivenLanguageLearning:**...

Data-­‐Driven  Language  Learning:    Learner  Proficiency,  Performance,  and  

Percep9on  

Nina  Vyatkina  University  of  Kansas    

Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin December 3, 2014

Overview  

•  Results  of  two  empirical  studies  that  explored  the  effects  of  Data-­‐Driven  Learning  (DDL)  of  German  lexico-­‐gramma?cal  construc?ons  (verb-­‐preposi?on  colloca?ons)  by  North  American  university  students    

2

Corpus  research  and    L2  teaching  and  learning  

3

applied  corpus  research  

direct  applica?ons:  DDL  

computer-­‐based  (hands-­‐on)  

paper-­‐based  (hands-­‐off)  

indirect  applica?ons:  grammars  and  textbooks  

Theore?cal  background:    Usage-­‐Based  Grammar  (UBG)  

•  language  is  learned  induc?vely  through  repeated  exposure  to  and  prac?ce  with  specific  language  models    

•  Langacker  1987;  Robinson  &  Ellis  2008;  Bybee  2008  •  inseparability  of  grammar  and  the  lexicon  

•  Conrad,  2000;  Chambers,  2005;  Flowerdew,  2011  

•  primacy  of  target  language  input  •   Krashen  1982;  Doughty  &  Long  2003;  Schmidt  1990;  Sharwood  Smith  1993  

4

DDL  pedagogical  applica?ons  •  Benefits:    

–  work  with  aZested  usage  examples  (Jones,  1986;  1991)  –  induc?ve  (discovery)  learning  (Bernardini,  2000;  2002;  Ellis,  2002;  

Richards  et  al.,  1992;  Thornbury,  2006)    –  developing  analy?cal  and  problem-­‐solving  skills  (Johns,  1991,  1997;  

Qiao  &  Sussex,  1996;  Kennedy  &  Miceli,  2002)  

•  Challenges:  –  Language  learners  are  not  expert  corpus  users  

•  “most  widely  accessible  corpora  were  created  as  tools  for  linguis?c  research  and  not  with  pedagogical  goals  in  mind”  (Brown,  2007)  

–  Corpus  examples  are  ‘genuine’  but  not  ‘authen?c’  for  language  learners  (Widdowson,  2000;  2003)  

5

DDL  research  •  DDL  effec?ve  for  L2  learning  (vocabulary,  colloca?ons,  

improving  student  wri?ng)  –  e.g.  Horst,  Cobb  &  Nicolae  2005;  Lin  2008;  Sun  &  Wang  2003;  Yoon  2008  

•  Hands-­‐on  DDL  superior  or  equivalent  to  tradi?onal  instruc?on  –  e.g.  Cobb  1997,  1999;  Daskalowska  2013;  Garner  2013;  Kaur  &  Hegelheimer  2005;  Liou  et  al.  2012  

•  Hands-­‐off  DDL  superior  or  equivalent  to  tradi?onal  instruc?on  –  Allan  2006;  Boulton  2008,  2009,  2010;  Koosha  &  Jafarpour  2006,  Tian  2005  

6

Does  DDL  work?  

•  Cobb  &  Boulton  (in  press):  meta-­‐analysis  – up  to  2012,  further  analysis  currently  ongoing  

•  Is  DDL  effec?ve?  – Within  groups  effect  size:  1.68  SD  units  

•  SD  d  =  .84,    95%  CI  =  [1.36,  2.00]  

•  Is  DDL  efficient?  – Within  groups  effect  size:  1.04  SD  units  

•  SD  d  =  .73,    95%  CI  =  [.83,  1.25]  

•  =>  high  ES  (Oswald  &  Plonsky  2010)  7

Research  gaps  •  morpho-­‐syntac?c  features  •  non-­‐English  •  correla?ons  with  proficiency  and  percep?ons  •  hands-­‐on  vs.  hands-­‐off  •  delayed  effects  •  low  L2  proficiency  levels  •  applica?ons  with  ‘real’  teachers  •  new  vs.  previously  learned  language  features  

8

Target  structure:    verb-­‐preposi?on  colloca?ons  

•  some  verbs  subcategorize  preposi?onal  arguments    •  form-­‐meaning  mismatches  lead  to  L2  errors    

–  to  wait  for  =  warten  auf  (not  für)    •  e.g.  Nesselhauf  2003  

•  German  pronominal  adverbs  extremely  frequent    –  davon,  dabei  -­‐  thereof,  thereby…  

•  either  the  verb  or  the  preposi?on  also  assigns  gramma?cal  case  =>  a  lexico-­‐gramma9cal  construc9on  

•  warten  +  auf  +  accusa?ve  NP  •  Difficult  construc?ons  for  L2  German  learners  

•  e.g.  Baten  2011,  Diehl  et  al.  2000,  Vinagre  and  Muñoz  2011  

9

Instruc?onal  sewng  

•  large  public  US  university  •  intact  classes  of  German  as  a  Foreign  Language    •  age:  21  mean  (range  18-­‐35)  •  L1:  American  English  •  gender:  approx.  equal  #  female  and  male    •  no  familiarity  with  corpora  

10

Study  1  

•  Short-­‐term  and  delayed  effects  of  two  DDL  methods  for  teaching  L2  colloca?ons:  –   computer-­‐based  (hands-­‐on)  – paper-­‐based  (hands-­‐off)  

•  Correla?on  of  learners’  performance  with  their  L2  proficiency  and  DDL  recep?vity  (percep?ons)    

11

Par?al  replica?on  of    Boulton  (2012)  

•  DDL  of  English  verb  structures  by  high-­‐intermediate  college-­‐level  ESL  students  (L1  French)  

• hands-­‐on  and  hands-­‐off  DDL  equally  effec?ve  • only  hands-­‐off  results  correlated  with  proficiency  

•  correla?on  with  recep?vity  posi?ve  but  not  significant  

12

Par?cipants  and    instruc?onal  sewng  

•  9  German  majors  and  minors,  1  High  School  student  •  Enrolled  in  16-­‐week-­‐long  ‘Advanced  German’  course  •  L2  proficiency:  B1  (CEFR)  average  •  Course  goals:  

–  development  of  L2  proficiency  AND  corpus  literacy:  the  ability  to  use  corpora  for  consulta?on  and  language  analysis  (Mukherjee,  2002)    

–  regular  hands-­‐on  and  hands-­‐off  DDL  ac?vi?es  in  addi?on  to  tradi?onal  extensive  reading,  discussion,  and  gramma?cal  analysis  

13

DDL  instruc?on  

•  DWDS  core  corpus  of  the  20th  cent.  German  (www.dwds.de);  100  M  words  

•  Biweekly  computer  lab  mee?ngs,  in-­‐class  and  homework  corpus-­‐based  assignments  

•  Teaching  method:  –  DDL  “TRIPLE  I”  principle:  Illustra?on-­‐Interac?on-­‐Induc?on  (McCarthy  

and  Carter  1995)    –  Focus  on  forms  encountered  by  learners  during  reading  the  course  

novel:  top-­‐down,  text-­‐to-­‐corpus  approach    (Charles  2007;  Frankenberg-­‐Garcia  2012;  Tyne  2012)    

–  The  en?re  text  of  the  novel  is  archived  in  DWDS  

•  Study:  20  verb-­‐preposi?on  colloca?ons  from  the  novel  14

The  course  novel  

15

Example  worksheet  

16

Research  ques?ons  1.  Did  learner  wriZen  performance  on  the  gap-­‐filling  task  and  the  

sentence-­‐wri?ng  task  improve  following  DDL  instruc?on  and  were  the  gains  retained  a  month  later?  

2.  Which  DDL  method  was  more  effec?ve:  hands-­‐on  or  hands-­‐off?  3.  Did  performance  outcomes  correlate  with  learner  proficiency?  4.  Did  learners  prefer  hands-­‐on  or  hands-­‐off  DDL,  did  their  

preferences  change  during  the  course,  and  did  their  preferences  correspond  with  their  performance  and  proficiency?  

•  Modifica?on  of  Boulton  2012  

17

Data  collec?on    •  All  students:  hands-­‐on  and  hands-­‐off  DDL  condi?on  •  Language  background  ques?onnaire  •  Performance:  pretest  –  post-­‐test  -­‐  delayed  post-­‐test  

–  10  colloca?ons  per  condi?on  –  gap-­‐filling  and  sentence-­‐wri?ng  test  tasks  –  10  possible  pts  per  test  /  condi?on  /  task  

•  Proficiency:  pre-­‐course  –  post-­‐course    –  standardized  online  diagnos?c  test  (www.ondaf.de)  

•  Recep?vity:  pre-­‐course  –  post-­‐course    –  wriZen  ques?onnaires  

 18

Aug. 28 (SW1)   Nov. 6 (SW11)   Nov. 11 (SW12)   Dec. 4 (SW15)   Dec. 9 (SW16)  

Proficiency and receptivity pretest; language background questionnaire  

Performance hands-on: pretest, treatment, and posttest  

Performance hands-off: pretest, treatment, and posttest  

Proficiency and receptivity posttest  

Performance: delayed posttest  

Gap-­‐filling  test  task  

19

Sentence-­‐wri?ng  test  task  

20

Performance  outcomes:  raw  scores  

21

0  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

7  

8  

9  

10  

pretest   posZest   delayed  

On-­‐gap  

Off-­‐gap  

On-­‐sentence  

Off-­‐sentence  

Methods  

•  Raw  test  scores  –  abnormally  distributed  ⇒ mul?level  Poisson  regression  (z-­‐tests)  

•  Gain  scores  –  normally  distributed  ⇒ mul?level  linear  regression  (t-­‐tests)  

22

Performance  outcomes  by  task  •  Pretest-­‐PosZest:  

– gap:      z  =  4.94*,  p  <  .0001  (+1.97)  – sentence:  z  =  3.52*,  p  =  .0004  (+1.71)  

•  Pretest-­‐Delayed  PosZest:  – gap:      z  =  2.05*,  p  =  .04  (+1.35)      – sentence:    z  =  0.91,  p  =  .36  (+1.16  )    

•  PosZest-­‐Delayed  PosZest:  – gap:      z  =  −3.01*,  p  =  .004  – sentence:  z  =  −2.65*,  p  =  .01  

  23

Performance  outcomes:    gains  by  condi?on  

24

0  

0.5  

1  

1.5  

2  

2.5  

3  

3.5  

4  

4.5  

Hands-­‐off   Hands-­‐on  

immediate  gap  

immediate  sentence  

delayed  gap  

delayed  sentence  

Interaction between conditions not significant: Immediate gains: t(34) = −0.80, p = .43 Delayed gains: t(34) = 0.47, p = .64

Proficiency  outcomes  

onDaF Test

N Mean Min Max SD N  in  CEFR  bands    

A2 B1 B2

Pre-course

9 73.78 (B1.1)

36 115 26.88 3 4 2

Post-course

10 80.70 (B1.2)

48 118 22.94 1 6 3

25

Correla?ons  (r)  between  DDL  performance  and  proficiency    

measure N method pretest immediate posttest

delayed posttest

total

Gains 9 hands-on N/A 0.30 -0.16 N/A

hands-off N/A 0.70a 0.58 N/A

26

a: two-tailed t-test: p=.046 (t=2.37; df=8)

Recep?vity  outcomes  

Recep9vity  to  corpus  use     Pre-course   Post-course  

Mean (1-5)  

SD   Mean (1-5)  

SD  

1   The German corpus (DWDS) will be / has been easy to use for language learning purposes.

3.7   0.82   3.7   0.82  2   The German corpus (DWDS) will be / has been useful for

learning German. 3.6   0.96   3.9   1.10  

3   Corpus work will be / has been interesting.   3.4   1.08   4.1   1.20  4   I liked doing corpus activities on computer. N/A   N/A   3.5   1.43  5   I liked doing activities with corpus concordances on paper. N/A   N/A   3.8   1.14  

Overall mean: 3.57   0.13   3.8   0.22  

27

Individual  recep?vity  

28

0  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

S1   S2   S3   S4   S5   S6   S7   S8   S9   S10  

Precourse  

Postcourse  

Recep?vity:  correla?ons  •  Strong,  significant:  

–  hands-­‐on  –  hands-­‐off  recep?vity    p=.03  (r=0.68,  t(8)=3.0)    –  overall  recep?vity  –  hands-­‐on  recep?vity    p=.001  (r=0.87,  t(8)=5.8)  –  overall  recep?vity  –  hands-­‐off  recep?vity    p=.003  (r=0.83,  t(8)=4.27)    

•  Moderate  but  not  significant    –  pretest  –  posZest  recep?vity        (r=0.48,  t(8)=1.57)  –  recep?vity  –  proficiency        (r=0.46,  t(8)=1.46)    

•  Weak,  not  significant:  –  overall  recep?vity  –  performance,  both  posZests  –  hands-­‐on  recep?vity  –  performance,  both  posZests  –  hands-­‐off  recep?vity  –  performance,  immediate  posZest    

•  (moderate,  not  significant  for  the  hands-­‐off  condi?on  on  the  delayed  posZest)  

29

Study  1  results:  summary  •  Did  learner  wriZen  performance  on  the  gap-­‐filling  task  and  the  

sentence-­‐wri?ng  task  improve  following  DDL  instruc?on?    -­‐>  Yes  •  Were  the  gains  retained  a  month  later?  -­‐>  Yes  for  gap-­‐filling  results    •  Which  DDL  method  was  more  effec?ve:  hands-­‐on  or  hands-­‐off?  -­‐>  No  difference  •  Did  performance  gains  correlate  with  overall  proficiency  gains?  -­‐>  Yes  for  hands-­‐off,  no  for  hands-­‐on  •  Did  preferences  correspond  with  performance  and  proficiency?  -­‐>  No  

30

Study  2  

•  Comparison  of  short-­‐term  effects  of  a  paper-­‐based  DDL  method  and  a  non-­‐DDL  method  for  teaching  L2  colloca?ons  to  low-­‐proficiency  learners  –  lexical  vs.  syntac?c  gains  – new  vs.  previously  learned  colloca?ons  

31

Par?cipants  and    instruc?onal  sewng  

•  Intact  classes  of  intermediate-­‐low  (3rd  semester)  and  intermediate-­‐mid  (4th  semester)  German  

•  L2  proficiency:  <  or  =  A2  (CEFR)  •  Mul?-­‐sec?on  program,  uniform  syllabus  •  Students  enrolled  to  fulfill  a  language  requirement  

32

Design  •  4  groups:  LD  (n=15);  LT  (n=13);  MD  (n=13);  MT  (n=16)    

–  Instruc?onal  level  (“proficiency”):  L  (low)  and  M  (mid)  –  Treatment  condi?ons:  D  (DDL)  and  T  (textbook-­‐based)  

•  Instruc?on:    –  10  verb-­‐preposi?on  colloca?ons  from  regular  textbook  –  D:  III  (Illustra?on-­‐Interac?on-­‐Induc?on,  McCarthy  &  Carter  1995)    –  T:  PPP  (Presenta?on-­‐Prac?ce-­‐Produc?on,  DeKeyser  1998)  

•  Timeline:  –  day  1:  pretest  (10  min.),  language  background  ques?onnaire  –  day  2:  instruc?on  (40  min.),  posZest  (10  min.)  

•  Only  gap-­‐filling  produc?on  data:  –  10  possible  pts  for  accurate  lexical  items  (preposi?ons)  –  5  possible  pts  for  accurate  syntac?c  items  (ar?cle  inflec?on)  

 

33

DDL  worksheet  I  

34

DDL  worksheet  II  

35

Test  task  I  

36

Test  task  II  

37

Research  ques?ons  1.  Did  learner  wriZen  performance  on  the  gap-­‐filling  task  

improve  following  focused  instruc?on?  2.  Were  the  gains  higher  for  the  DDL  or  the  non-­‐DDL  group?  3.  Was  there  interac?on  among  the  factors  treatment,  course  

level,  and  linguis?c  items  (lexical  or  syntac?c)?  

38

Lexical  gains  

39

0  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

7  

pretest   posZest  

MT  

MD  

LT  

LD  

Syntac?c  gains  

40

0  

0.5  

1  

1.5  

2  

2.5  

3  

pretest   posZest  

MT  

MD  

LT  

LD  

Results  •  Overall  gains:  

–  lexical:      z  =  6.14*,  p  <  .0001  (by  1.94)  – syntac?c:  z  =  3.63*,  p  =  .0002  (by  1.87)  

•  DL  beZer  than  TL:  –  lexical:      t(104)  =  -­‐2.39*,  p  =  .02  (by  1.66)      – syntac?c:    no  difference  

•  DM  vs.  TM:  –  lexical:      no  difference  – syntac?c:    no  difference  

41

Study  2b:  DL  (n=16),  TL  (n=15)  Lexical  gains  

42

0  

0.5  

1  

1.5  

2  

2.5  

3  

pretest   posZest  

TL  

DL  

Study  2b:  DL  (n=16),  TL  (n=15)  Syntac?c  gains  

43

0  

0.2  

0.4  

0.6  

0.8  

1  

1.2  

1.4  

1.6  

1.8  

pretest   posZest  

TL  

DL  

Results  •  Pretest:  

–  lexical:      no  difference  between  groups  – syntac?c:    no  difference  between  groups  

•  Overall  gains:  –  lexical:      z  =  4.13*,  p  <  .0001  (by  3.16)      – syntac?c:    z  =  3.66*,  p  =  .0002  (by  4.63)  

•  DL    beZer  than  TL:  –  lexical:      t(42)  =  3.81*,  p  =  .0004  (by  1.73)      – syntac?c:    t(42)  =  2.58*,  p  =  .01  (by  1.17)  

44

Study  2  results:  summary  •  Did  learner  wriZen  performance  on  the  gap-­‐filling  task  

improve  following  focused  instruc?on?  -­‐>  Yes  •  Were  the  gains  higher  for  the  DDL  than  the  non-­‐DDL  group?

Was  there  interac?on  among  the  factors  treatment,  course  level,  and  linguis?c  items  (lexical  or  syntac?c)?  

-­‐>  DDL  was  more  effec?ve  than  non-­‐DDL  only  for  the  lower-­‐level  group  (for  learning  new  colloca?ons)  -­‐>  DDL  advantage  retained  on  a  delayed  posZest  

45

Scores  for  the  same  10  colloca?ons  across  3  course  levels  

46

0  

2  

4  

6  

8  

10  

12  

14  

Pretest   PosZest  

DH  

DM  

TM  

DL  

TL  

Study  1  revisited:    Learner  comments  

47

Study  1  revisited:    Learner  comments  (cont.)  

48

…a  coda  

•  all  five  students  who  con?nued  in  the  next  semester  advanced  German  course  (with  no  explicit  focus  on  corpora)  reported  having  used  it  as  a  reference  resource  independently  throughout  the  course.  

49

Conclusion  •  DDL  effec?ve  for  teaching  colloca?ons  and  well  received  by  learners  

=  Boulton  2012;  Cobb  1997,  1999;  Daskalowska  2013;  Liou  et  al.  2012  

•  can  be  applied:    –  to  languages  other  than  English  –  to  linguis?c  targets  beyond  vocabulary  (morpho-­‐syntax)  –  as  regular  course  component  and  one-­‐?me  interven?ons  –  with  regular  teachers  

•  lexical  gains  higher  than  morpho-­‐syntac?c  gains  •  both  hands-­‐on  and  hands-­‐off  DDL  is  effec?ve  and  can  be  alternated  to  

teach  structures  resistant  to  tradi?onal  teaching  methods  •  hands-­‐on  method  equally  beneficial  for  faster  and  slower  learners  •  more  efficient  than  non-­‐DDL  for  learning  new  structures  

–  vs.  Nesselhauf  2004  

•  benefits  of  guided  induc?on  and  consistent  prac?ce  -­‐>  learner  autonomy  

50

Future  research  •  More  replica?ons  •  Longitudinal  study  •  Singling  out  DDL  effects    

–  input  richness  and  enhancement  (Frankenberg-­‐Garcia  2014)  –  guided  and  unguided  induc?on  (Smart  2014)  –  learner  corpus  in  DDL  (Cotos  2014)  

•  Hands-­‐on  for  lower-­‐level  learners  •  More  aZen?on  to  morpho-­‐syntax  •  Transfer  to  wri?ng  and  other  free  produc?on  •  More  integra?on  into  regular  L2  syllabi  

51

Acknowledgements  

•  Terrence  D.  Jorgensen  Jr.  for  help  with  sta?s?cal  analysis  

•  The  department  of  Germanic  Languages  and  Literatures,  University  of  Kansas  

52