Cognitive Biases 4

95
Cognitive Biases 4 Fallacies Involving Probability

description

Cognitive Biases 4. Fallacies Involving Probability. Homework 2. Assignment. W atch episode 1 of season 1 of “Ancient Aliens.” . Assignment. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of Cognitive Biases 4

Page 1: Cognitive Biases 4

Cognitive Biases 4

Fallacies Involving Probability

Page 2: Cognitive Biases 4

HOMEWORK 2

Page 3: Cognitive Biases 4

Assignment

Watch episode 1 of season 1 of “Ancient Aliens.”

Page 4: Cognitive Biases 4

Assignment

Find one thing that is said, shown, or presented in the episode that is misleading. I want you to describe it to me, then to explain why it is misleading.

Page 5: Cognitive Biases 4

Assignment

What you describe to me can be misleading for any reason, not just a reason we’ve talked about in class. Just describe it, and tell me the reason why it is misleading.

Page 6: Cognitive Biases 4

How to Get More Marks

I will award 1 mark to students who give me an example of something misleading in the show that is original—meaning that other students did not give me the exact same example.

Page 7: Cognitive Biases 4

PROBABILITY BIASES

Page 8: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

Which of the following is most likely to happen? a. There will not be a final exam in this class.b. There will not be a final exam in this class, because the instructor has to leave the country.c. Lingnan University closes and there will not be a final exam in this class.

Page 9: Cognitive Biases 4

The Killer

Ah Jong is a martial arts expert. He’s a high ranking member of the Triads, and he’s killed hundreds of people. Friends describe him as “dangerous.”

Page 10: Cognitive Biases 4

The Killer

Page 11: Cognitive Biases 4

What Is Most Likely?

(a) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses.(b) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses so the police won’t think he’s a gangster(c) Ah Jong sews women’s dresses as part of his court-ordered rehabilitation.

Page 12: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-nuclear demonstrations.Which is more probable?a. Linda is a bank teller.b. Linda is a bank teller and is active in the feminist movement.

Page 13: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

The correct answer is (a):

a. There will not be a final exam.a. Ah Jong sews women’s dresses.a. Linda is a bank teller.

Page 14: Cognitive Biases 4
Page 15: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

Suppose: 20,000 people meet the description (“31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy…”).

How many of them are feminist bank tellers? Pick any number: 5,000.

Page 16: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

How many of them are bank tellers (feminist or not feminist)?

It has to be more than 5,000. That’s the number of feminist bank tellers. You have to add in the number of non-feminist bank tellers who meet the description. Let’s say that’s only 1 person.

Page 17: Cognitive Biases 4

Conjunction Fallacy

Then the probability that Linda is a bank teller AND is active in the feminist movement is 5,000 out of 20,000 or 25%.

And the probability that she is a bank teller [maybe a feminist, maybe not] is 5,001 out of 20,000 > 25%.

Page 18: Cognitive Biases 4

Mathematics

Always, the probability of two events happening (Linda being a bank teller AND Linda being a feminist) is less than the probability of just one of those events happening (for example, Linda being a bank teller).

The illusion that the opposite is true especially occurs in cases where one event explains the other.

Page 19: Cognitive Biases 4

George

For example, suppose I tell you that there is a man named “George.” George turned water into wine, healed the sick, brought a dead person back to life, and came back to life himself after he died.

What is the probability that George did all these things? How likely is it?

Page 20: Cognitive Biases 4

George

You can say whatever you like. 1%, 10%, 99%.

But suppose I add to the story. I say “George was the son of God. That’s why he had all these powers.”

Page 21: Cognitive Biases 4

George

Many people will say that it’s more likely that George was the son of God AND did all these things than it is that he did all these things. But that can’t be true.

“A & B” is always less (or equally) probable than A, or than B. For A & B to happen, A has to happen and also B has to happen.

Page 22: Cognitive Biases 4

Debiasing

We can avoid this bias if we ask the question differently:

There are 100 persons who fit the description above (that is, Linda’s). How many of them are:Bank tellers? ____ of 100Bank tellers and active in the feminist movement? ____ of 100

Page 23: Cognitive Biases 4

Frequencies

This shows that it’s good to translate percentages and probabilities into frequencies (number of X out of number of Y).

We are less susceptible to representativeness bias when things are phrased in this way.

Page 24: Cognitive Biases 4

REPRESENTATIVENESS

Page 25: Cognitive Biases 4

Representativeness

Our (false) judgment that Linda is more likely to be a feminist bank teller than to just be a bank teller is an example of how we judge the truth of claims based on how “representative” they are.

Page 26: Cognitive Biases 4

Representativeness

Consider again our case of coin flips that seem non-random, due to clustering.

Since coins land 50% heads and 50% tails, “XO” and “OX” are representative of this even split, whereas “XX” and “OO” don’t represent it. So sequences with clustering seem non-random, even if they are (random).

Page 27: Cognitive Biases 4

Representativeness

Representativeness influences our other judgments as well.

It’s hard to accept that two very tall parents tend to, on average, have less tall children (as regression to the mean requires). Children who are as tall as their parents are more representative of their parents’ heights.

Page 28: Cognitive Biases 4

Heuristics and Biases

Representativeness is often a good heuristic.

A heuristic is a strategy that is easy to use in problem solving but doesn’t always work when applied.

There is often no good reason to distinguish between heuristics and biases.

Page 29: Cognitive Biases 4

Representativeness is a good heuristic (sometimes) because (sometimes) things are representative.

Page 30: Cognitive Biases 4

• Sometimes small effects have small causes. Burnt toast can be caused by leaving bread in the toaster for too long.

• Sometimes complex effects have complex causes. World War I (a complex effect) was caused by a very complex set of factors, only one of which was the assassination of Archduke Ferdinand.

Page 31: Cognitive Biases 4

However:• Sometimes large effects have small causes. An

outbreak of a disease may be caused by a tiny virus or bacterium.

• Sometimes complex effects have simple causes. For instance, introducing a foreign species into a new land may cause radical changes in the ecosystem.

Page 32: Cognitive Biases 4

THE BASE RATE NEGLECT FALLACY

Page 33: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Fallacy

• There are ½ million people in Russia are affected by HIV/ AIDS.

• There are 150 million people in Russia.

Page 34: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Fallacy

Imagine that the government decides this is bad and that they should test everyone for HIV/ AIDS.

Page 35: Cognitive Biases 4

They develop a test with the following features:• If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then 95% of the time

the test will be positive (correct), and only 5% of the time will it be negative (incorrect).

• If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then 95% of the time the test will be negative (correct), and only 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect).

Page 36: Cognitive Biases 4

The Test

If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then :• 95% of the time the test

will be positive (correct)• 5% of the time will it be

negative (incorrect)

Page 37: Cognitive Biases 4

The Test

If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then:• 95% of the time the test

will be negative (correct)

• 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect)

Page 38: Cognitive Biases 4

You Get Tested

Suppose you are a Russian who gets tested for HIV/ AIDS under the government program.

The test comes out positive. How likely are you to have HIV/ AIDS?

Page 39: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

True Positives

False Negatives

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

True Negatives

Page 40: Cognitive Biases 4

True Positives

We know that there are 500,000 people in Russia who have HIV/AIDS. How many will get a positive test result?

Page 41: Cognitive Biases 4

The Test

If someone has HIV/ AIDS, then :• 95% of the time the test

will be positive (correct)• 5% of the time will it be

negative (incorrect)

Page 42: Cognitive Biases 4

True Positives

500,000 x 95% = 475,000

Page 43: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

True Positives

False Negatives

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

True Negatives

Page 44: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 False Negatives

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

True Negatives

Page 45: Cognitive Biases 4

False Negatives?

How many people who have HIV/AIDS will test negative?

500,000 – 475,000 = 25,000500,000 x 5% = 25,000

Page 46: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 False Negatives

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

True Negatives

Page 47: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

True Negatives

Page 48: Cognitive Biases 4

True Negatives

We also know that there are 150 million – 500,000 people in Russia who do not have HIV/AIDS.

How many of them will correctly test negative?

Page 49: Cognitive Biases 4

The Test

If someone does not have HIV/ AIDS, then:• 95% of the time the test

will be negative (correct)

• 5% of the time will it be positive (incorrect)

Page 50: Cognitive Biases 4

True Negatives

(150,000,000 – 500,000) x 95% = ?

Page 51: Cognitive Biases 4

True Negatives

149,500,000 x 95% = ?

Page 52: Cognitive Biases 4

True Negatives

149,500,000 x 95% = 142,025,000

Page 53: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Negatives

True Negatives

Page 54: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Negatives

142,025,000

Page 55: Cognitive Biases 4

False Positives

How many people who do not have HIV/AIDS will falsely test positive for the disease?

149,500,000 – 142,025,000 = 7,475,000149,500,000 x 5% = 7,475,000

Page 56: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

False Positives

142,025,000

Page 57: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

7,475,000 142,025,000

Page 58: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

7,475,000 142,025,000

Page 59: Cognitive Biases 4

You tested positive. What percentage of people who tested positive truly had HIV/AIDS?

Page 60: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

7,475,000 142,025,000

Totals

Page 61: Cognitive Biases 4

Test = Yes Test = No

HIV/AIDS = Yes

475,000 25,000

HIV/ AIDS = No

7,475,000 142,025,000

Totals 7,950,000

Page 62: Cognitive Biases 4

Your Chances

True Positives ÷ All Positives =475,000 ÷ 7,950,000 = 6%

Page 63: Cognitive Biases 4

Whether a test is good or worth doing depends not only on how accurate it is (95% true positive, 95% true negative), but also on how prevalent the condition being tested for is.

Very rare conditions require very sensitive tests, whereas very prevalent conditions only need minorly accurate tests.

Page 64: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rates

The “base rate” is the percentage of people in the population who have a certain property.

The base rate of HIV/AIDS cases is the percentage of people who have HIV/AIDS in the population. The base rate of bearded people is the percentage of people who have beards in the population, etc.

Page 65: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Neglect

The “base rate neglect fallacy” is the fallacy of ignoring the base rate when making a judgment.

For example, if I assumed you were a terrorist, because you tested positive, I would be committing the base rate neglect fallacy. I should assume you’re still probably not a terrorist.

Page 66: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rates

As we have seen, base rates matter. If the base rate of a condition is very low (small percentage of AIDS cases), then even very accurate tests (95% true positive, 95% true negative) can be useless.

In our example only 6% of people who tested positive for AIDS had the disease!

Page 67: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Neglect

The base rate neglect fallacy has nothing at all to do with the number of people there are in the world. Nothing.

It has to do with the probability of a variable taking on a certain value, for instance, the probability that someone’s height = 1.5m, the probability that someone has AIDS, etc.

Page 68: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rates

This is the “base rate” of people who are 1.5m tall, and the “base rate” of people with AIDS.

If 1 in 100 people are terrorists, then the rate of terrorists is 1 in 100 and the probability that a randomly selected person is a terrorist is 1 in 100.

Page 69: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rates

We call this the base rate, because it is the probability that someone is a terrorist when we don’t know anything else about them.

It might be that the base rate of terrorists is 1 in 100, but the rate of terrorists among people who are holding rocket launchers is 1 in 2, and the rate of terrorists among retirees is 1 in 500.

Page 70: Cognitive Biases 4

Tests

The base rate neglect fallacy happens when we have a test that is meant to detect the value of a variable.

For example we might have a test that tells us whether someone has AIDS or not, or whether someone is driving over the speed limit, or whether they are drunk.

Page 71: Cognitive Biases 4

Reliability of Tests

Here is the important, and crucial fact. Please learn this:

As the base rate of X = x decreases, the # of false positives on tests for X = x increases.

Tests are less reliable when the condition we are testing for becomes rare (low base rate).

Page 72: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Neglect Fallacy

The base rate neglect fallacy happens when:

1. There is a low base rate of some condition.2. We have a test for that condition.3. Someone tests positive.4. We assume that means they have the

condition, ignoring the unreliability of tests for conditions with low base rates.

Page 73: Cognitive Biases 4

Base Rate Neglect

Humans have a tendency to ignore base rates.

For example, Kahneman and Tversky (1973) conducted a study in which participants were supposed to estimate the GPAs of certain (fictional) students.

Page 74: Cognitive Biases 4

Kahneman & Tverskey 1973

Some of the participants were given good evidence that students had high (or low) GPAs. In particular, they were given the students’ percentiles (95th percentile, for example).

Other participants were given only very weak evidence: the scores that the students got on a test of humor.

Page 75: Cognitive Biases 4

All the participants were given the base rate of students with various GPAs. For example, 20% A, 40% B, 30% C, 10% D.

But all of the participants ignored the base rate.

Page 76: Cognitive Biases 4

A good test for a prevalent condition (like number of people with A’s, not like being a terrorist) gives you lots of information.

If someone is in the 99th percentile, for instance, you can be sure that they got an A. If they’re in the bottom quartile, you know that they did not get an A.

Page 77: Cognitive Biases 4

But scoring high on a test of humor is not a good indicator of your GPA. Maybe people with a good sense of humor are a little bit more likely to get better grades, but not much more likely.

Given only such information, your guess should be very close to the base rate (for example, it’s 40% likely the student has a B GPA).

Page 78: Cognitive Biases 4

But, as I said, participants ignored the base rate. They guessed that people who did very well on the humor test had high GPAs, and people who did poorly on the test had low GPAs.

Page 79: Cognitive Biases 4

Prosecutor’s Fallacy

The base rate neglect fallacy is often called the prosecutor’s fallacy, as I shall explain.

Page 80: Cognitive Biases 4

Murder!

Let’s suppose that there has been a murder.

There is almost no evidence to go on except that the police find one hair at the crime scene.

Page 81: Cognitive Biases 4

You are the Suspect

If someone is the killer, there is a 100% chance that their DNA will match the hair’s DNA.

The police have a database that contains the DNA of everyone in Hong Kong.

They run the DNA in the hair through their database and discover that you are a match!

Page 82: Cognitive Biases 4

Comprehension Question

If you have been following along you should be able to answer this question:

What is the probability that you are the murderer, given that you are a DNA match for the hair?

Page 83: Cognitive Biases 4

Answer

If you said 100%, then you have just committed the base rate neglect fallacy.

The correct answer is “Much lower, because the base rate of people who committed this murder out of the Hong Kong population as a whole is 1 in 7 million.”

Page 84: Cognitive Biases 4

Perfect Conditions for Fallacy

Here’s what we have:

1. A low base rate (only 1 person who committed this murder in the world).

2. A test for whether someone is the murderer.3. You, who’ve tested positive on this test.4. And the police who think you did it!

Page 85: Cognitive Biases 4

Let’s Look at the Numbers

We know that if you are the murderer, then there is a 100% chance of a DNA match.

But what is the false positive rate? How likely is a randomly selected person will match the DNA?

Page 86: Cognitive Biases 4

False Results

Here’s a quote from “False result fear over DNA tests,” Nick Paton Walsh, The Guardian:

“Researchers had asked the labs to match a series of DNA samples. They knew which ones were from the same person, but found that in over 1 per cent of cases the labs falsely matched samples, or failed to notice a match.”

Page 87: Cognitive Biases 4

Let’s assume that half of the cases where “labs falsely matched samples, or failed to notice a match.” were cases where they falsely matched samples.

So the probability of a false positive is ½ x 1% = 0.5%, or 5 in 1,000.

Page 88: Cognitive Biases 4

Since there are 7 million people in Hong Kong, we expect about 0.5% x 7 million = 35,000 of them to match the hair’s DNA.

Actually, it’s 35,000 + 1, because the true killer is a match, and not by accident.

Page 89: Cognitive Biases 4

So we expect that there are 35,001 DNA matches in all of Hong Kong.

And only one of them is the murderer. So what is the probability that you are the murderer?

1 in 35,001. That’s way less than 100%.

Page 90: Cognitive Biases 4

Important Things to Remember

There are three important things to remember:

1. If the test is more accurate (fewer false positives), then it’s more reliable

2. If the base rate is higher, the test is more reliable.

3. If the police have other reasons to suspect you, the test is more reliable.

Page 91: Cognitive Biases 4

1. If the test is more reliable…

Theoretically, DNA tests only return a false positive about 1 in 3 billion times.

In that case, we’d expect only .002 false positives in all of Hong Kong.

So your chances of being guilty would be 1 in 1.002, or 99.8%. Still, that’s lower than 100%.

Page 92: Cognitive Biases 4

2. If there base rate is higher…

Maybe the person who died was stabbed 5,000 times, once each by 5,000 different people. So there are 5,000 murderers.

Then with the previous false positive number at 35,000, you have a 5,000 in 40,000 chance of being one of the killers, or 12.5%.

Page 93: Cognitive Biases 4

3. If the police have some other reason to suspect you…

To figure out your chances of being guilty, we looked at the probability that a randomly selected person from HK would be a DNA match. We were assuming you were randomly selected.

But what if you weren’t randomly selected? What if the police tested you because you had a reason to kill the victim?

Page 94: Cognitive Biases 4

Reason to Suspect You

Then we would have to look at not the probability that a randomly selected person would match, but the probability that a person who had reason to kill the victim would match.

Suppose there are 5 people who had reasons to kill the victim, and the killer is one of them.

Page 95: Cognitive Biases 4

Much Higher Chance

Then your chances are:Let K = you’re the killer and M = you’re a matchP(K/ M) = [P(K) x P(M/ K)] ÷ P(M)= [(1/5) x 100%] ÷ P(M)= 0.2 ÷ [(1 + 0.025) ÷ 5]= 97.6%