by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych)...

215
Benzodiazepine-related aggression: Consideration of intrapersonal factors by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Psychology (Forensic) Deakin University September, 2015

Transcript of by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych)...

Page 1: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Benzodiazepine-related aggression: Consideration of intrapersonal factors

by

Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons

Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of

Psychology (Forensic)

Deakin University

September, 2015

Page 2: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 3: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 4: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

4

Acknowledgements

Firstly, I would like to offer my sincerest gratitude to my supervisor, Prof. Petra

Staiger, for her unwavering support and guidance throughout my thesis journey. Her

belief in my ability, continued mentoring throughout the various challenges that

arose, and her genuine interest in seeing me improve as a researcher and a person

were experiences which I value highly and am extremely grateful for.

Second, I would like to thank those who took me under their proverbial wings and

supported me professionally and academically during the completion of my thesis,

particularly Dr Kate Hall, Dr Nicolas Kambouropoulos, Prof. David Best, and Prof.

Dan Lubman. Their varied areas of expertise greatly enhanced my awareness of the

implications of my research, encouraging me that my research is far more than just

completion of a thesis.

I thank my fellow doctorate-ees, for whom without their humour, friendship and

shared experience of thesis-related challenges, I would have struggled to complete

the degree in such a positive state of mind. The DPsych experience introduced me to

amazing and inspiring individuals, and I look forward to moving forward in our

careers, and personal lives, together.

Finally, I thank my beautiful family and partner who listened to my endless stressing,

understood the time commitment I had made, ensured that I took time out for myself,

and supported me physically and mentally to come out on the other side.

Page 5: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

5

Contents

Abstract 11

Chapter 1: Overview of Benzodiazepine Misuse 13

1.1. Introduction 13

1.1.1. Thesis overview 14

1.2. Medical Indications for Benzodiazepine Prescriptions 16

1.3. Prevalence of Non-medical Use of Benzodiazepines 19

1.3.1. Defining non-medical use 19

1.3.2. Australian data 19

1.3.2.1. General community 19

1.3.2.2. Clinical samples 20

1.3.2.3. Forensic samples 21

1.3.3. International data 22

1.4. Motivations to Misuse Benzodiazepines 23

1.4.1. Benzodiazepine sources 24

1.5. Benzodiazepine-related Harms 25

1.5.1. Medical effects of benzodiazepine use 27

1.5.2. Paradoxical behaviour 28

1.6. Summary 30

Chapter 2: Aggression, Intrapersonal factors and Rationale for Thesis 32

2.1. Understanding Aggression 32

2.1.1. A note on definitions 33

2.1.2. The importance of intrapersonal factors 34

2.1.2.1. Gray’s (revised) Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory 37

2.2. Summary and Thesis Rationale 42

2.2.1. Thesis aims, hypotheses, and chapters 44

Page 6: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

6

Chapter 3: Benzodiazepine Use and Aggressive Behaviour: A Systematic

Review 47

3.1. Rationale 47

3.2. Methods 49

3.2.1. Eligibility criteria 50

3.2.2. Information sources 50

3.2.3. Study selection 51

3.2.4. Data extraction 51

3.3. Results 51

3.3.1. Study selection 51

3.3.2. Study characteristics 52

3.3.3. Risk of bias within studies 54

3.4. Synthesis of Results 54

3.4.1. Experimental studies 54

3.4.2. Clinical studies 76

3.4.3. Prospective studies 78

3.4.4. Cross-sectional studies 80

3.5. Discussion 82

3.5.1. Sample 83

3.5.2. Benzodiazepine type 84

3.5.3. Benzodiazepine dose 84

3.5.4. Personality 84

3.5.5. Limitations 85

3.6. Conclusion 86

3.7. Chapter Summary 86

Chapter 4: Motivational drive and alprazolam use: A recipe for aggression? 88

4.1. Introduction 88

4.2. The Current Study 92

4.3. Method 93

4.3.1. Design and procedure 93

4.3.2. Materials 94

4.3.3. Statistical analyses 97

Page 7: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

7

4.3.3.1. Model specification and invariance testing 98

4.4. Results 99

4.4.1. Participant characteristics 99

4.4.2. Main analyses 102

4.4.2.1. Predicting benzodiazepine-related aggression 103

4.5. Discussion 113

4.5.1. Role of BAS-Dr in benzodiazepine-related aggression 113

4.5.2. Role of benzodiazepine type in benzodiazepine-related

aggression 115

4.5.3. Limitations and strengths 117

4.6. Implications and Conclusions 118

4.7. Chapter Summary 119

Chapter 5: Violent crime: A complex interplay of benzodiazepine use,

psychological distress, and problematic impulsive behaviours 121

5.1. Introduction 121

5.1.1. Benzodiazepines and violent crime 122

5.1.2. The current study 124

5.2. Method 125

5.2.1. Participants 125

5.2.2. Design and procedure 125

5.2.3. Measures 126

5.2.4. Statistical analyses 128

5.3. Results 129

5.3.1. Participant characteristics 129

5.3.2. Crime profile 129

5.3.3. Benzodiazepine use profile 132

5.3.4. Substance use profile 132

5.3.5. Mental health profile 135

5.3.6. Group differences 135

5.4. Discussion 139

5.4.1. Limitations and strengths 144

5.4.2. Implications and conclusions 146

5.5. Chapter Summary 147

Page 8: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

8

Chapter 6: Outcomes, Implications, and Future Directions 148

6.1. Clinical Implications 149

6.1.1. Role of alprazolam 149

6.1.2. Prescribing policy 150

6.1.3. Poly-substance use 152

6.1.4. Chicken-egg intervention 152

6.2. Theoretical Implications 154

6.3. Forensic Implications 157

6.4. Future Research 158

6.5. Conclusion 160

References 162

Appendices 200

Appendix A: Link to: Albrecht et al (2014) Benzodiazepine use and aggressive

behaviour: A systematic review.

Appendix B: Confirmatory Factor Analysis of BIS/BAS scales

Appendix C: Abstract of: Albrecht et al (under review) Motivational drive and

alprazolam misuse: A recipe for aggression?

Appendix D: Abstract of: Albrecht et al (under review) Violent crime: Could

benzodiazepine use be playing a role?

Page 9: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

9

List of Tables

Chapter 3

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies assessing non-specific benzodiazepine-

related aggressive behaviour, ordered by year of publication 55

Table 2. Characteristics of included studies assessing benzodiazepine-related

aggressive behaviour, ordered by benzodiazepine type and year of publication 63

Chapter 4

Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics 100

Table 2. Standardised questionnaire score ranges, means and standard

deviations 103

Table 3. Lifetime NMP use per benzodiazepine, and frequency of alprazolam and

diazepam use in past year 104

Table 4. Typical and maximum daily alprazolam and diazepam doses, with

approximate diazepam equivalent doses (DZM) 105

Table 5. Bivariate correlations between factors of interest 106

Table 6. Hierarchical multiple regression predicting benzodiazepine-related general

aggression 110

Table 7. Hierarchical multiple regression predicting benzodiazepine-related physical

aggression 112

Chapter 5

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample population 130

Page 10: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

10

Table 2. Participant substance use and mental health histories, and recent distress

ratings. 131

Table 3. Means (M), and standard deviations (SD) and bivariate correlations

between factors of interest 133

Table 4. Benzodiazepine use the month and day prior to the index offence 136

Table 5. Independent t-tests pertaining to substance use and commission of a violent

index offence 137

Table 6. Independent t-tests pertaining to impulsivity and commission of a violent

index offence 139

List of Figures

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram depicting the flow of information through the

stages of the systematic review. 53

Figure 2. Proposed relationships between psychological distress, impulsive coping,

benzodiazepine dependence and violence 141

Page 11: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

11

Abstract

Background: Benzodiazepines are commonly prescribed for the reduction or

management of anxiety and agitation. However, it is becoming increasingly apparent

that for some individuals, benzodiazepine use is associated with increased agitation,

and at times, aggressive or violent behaviour. The nature of aggressive behaviour,

and the potential medico-legal outcomes associated with such behaviour, means that

understanding of this response is warranted. Despite a number of studies being

conducted on the association between benzodiazepine use and aggression, the

response remains poorly understood. This thesis aims to enhance our understanding

of this response, specifically by critically reviewing the currently available literature

base and by conducting two original cross-sectional studies.

Methods: The literature exploring benzodiazepine-related aggression was explored

through a systematic review which was conducted in line with PRISMA guidelines.

A cross-sectional study of 204 community benzodiazepine users, aged 18-51, applied

the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory to test the predictive nature of motivational

tendencies in understanding aggression. Multiple hierarchical regression analyses

were conducted on this data. A second, smaller cross-sectional study of 82

community-based violent and non-violent offenders, aged 21-56, examined group

differences in relation to benzodiazepine use patterns and intrapersonal

characteristics. This data were analysed using independent samples t-tests and non-

parametric tests (i.e., when assumptions were violated).

Results: The systematic review identified that although a number of studies have

been conducted in this area, varying methodologies and the over-arching low quality

of studies which have been conducted prompts more questions than clear

conclusions. The research does however suggest that benzodiazepine-related

Page 12: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

12

aggressive responses may be linked most closely with alprazolam and diazepam use.

The community-based cross-sectional study suggested that benzodiazepine-related

aggression was more likely to be experienced by those who used alprazolam and

exhibited persistent approach tendencies, or motivational drive. The offender cross-

sectional study demonstrated that violent offenders were significantly more likely to

present with benzodiazepine dependence, alprazolam use at higher doses, depression

and personality diagnoses, sensation seeking, and a history of violent behaviour than

non-violent offenders.

Conclusions: Taken together, the systematic review and two studies showed

alprazolam use to pose a greater risk of subsequent aggression than diazepam use,

and demonstrated that intrapersonal factors can further our understanding of

benzodiazepine-related aggression. Specifically, the two empirical studies

highlighted the role of impulse control and maladaptive coping strategies to manage

negative affect in this response. Key implications include recommended changes to

benzodiazepine prescribing practices and policy, especially in forensic contexts; the

impact of such research on future medico-legal decision making; how these

outcomes can inform selection of appropriate treatment targets within addiction and

forensic mental health sectors; and future development of the literature base.

Page 13: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

13

Chapter One: Overview of Benzodiazepine Misuse

1.1 Introduction

Recent estimates suggest that between 1-20% of benzodiazepine users

experience some form of anger or aggressive response following consumption

(Lader, 2011), rather than the expected sedating and calming effects. Concern about

this response not only arises because it is paradoxical to the desired indications of

benzodiazepines, but because it has the potential for significant health, personal and

legal costs. Unfortunately, however, minimal research has explored the underlying

mechanisms for why this response occurs. Instead, the majority of related literature

merely assess whether or not an association exists between benzodiazepine use and

aggressive behaviour. It is argued throughout this thesis that without an

understanding of how benzodiazepine-related aggression occurs, or the factors

associated with this response, little change can be affected to reduce the likelihood of

related harms, or to improve the safety of benzodiazepine consumption.

Benzodiazepines are frequently being noted by users as influencing their

criminal and/or violent behaviour. For example, more than a quarter of Australian

benzodiazepine-using police detainees (27.1%) attribute their current offence to

benzodiazepine use, most commonly due to the psychopharmacological effect (74%;

Payne & Gaffney, 2012); benzodiazepine use has been frequently blamed for

unspecified crime (14%), unprovoked aggressive behaviour (20%) and fights (13%)

by injecting drug users (Smith, Miller, O’Keefe, & Fry, 2007); and incarcerated

young male violent offenders most frequently blame diazepam (in combination with

alcohol) as a facilitator of violent crime (Forsyth, Khan & McKinlay, 2011).

Although these data rest on self-reported (and potentially biased) attributions, they

provide an insight into the potentially dangerous effects that benzodiazepines may

Page 14: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

14

have in influencing aggressive behaviour, despite prior findings that benzodiazepines

are mostly associated with acquisitive or property crime (e.g., Bradford & Payne,

2012; Darke & Ross, 1994; Darke et al., 2010; Horyniak, Reddel, Quinn, & Dietze,

2012; Payne & Gaffney, 2012). Of note, crimes against the person are the second

most commonly reported criminal behaviour in Australia, behind public order

offences (Australian Bureau of Statistics [ABS], 2013), and assault offences alone

are estimated to cost the nation $3.03b annually (Smith, Jorna, Sweeney, & Fuller,

2014). The potential legal and financial ramifications of this response (in addition to

the medical and social costs) highlight the need to further understand

benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Further knowledge and understanding about benzodiazepine-related

aggression is important in a number of ways. First, as a problematic substance-

related outcome, there will be implications for substance use treatment and relapse

prevention strategies. Second, risks of violent behaviour could be identified and

become violent offender rehabilitation treatment targets. Third, medical prescription

and regulation of benzodiazepines in the greater community, addiction medicine, and

in justice health (where a higher rate of violent tendencies may naturally be

observed) are likely to be impacted. Finally, it is likely that such knowledge will

inform future medico-legal decisions regarding an offender’s culpability, and their

punishment and rehabilitation needs.

1.1.1 Thesis overview

The current thesis aims to enhance understanding of the relationship between

benzodiazepine use and subsequent interpersonal aggression and violence. As will be

argued in the following chapters, this seemingly paradoxical response is of particular

concern given the frequency of benzodiazepine prescription coupled with the

Page 15: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

15

increasing misuse of benzodiazepines across healthy, clinical, and forensic

populations. Within this overarching aim, the following research has three specific

directives:

1. To systematically review the benzodiazepine-aggression literature base to

specifically identify benzodiazepine type and dose, and individual

characteristics associated with benzodiazepine-related aggression.

2. To enhance the theoretical rigour of benzodiazepine-aggression literature.

3. To identify characteristics associated with benzodiazepine-related violence in

a benzodiazepine-using, community-based criminal justice sample.

In order to achieve these aims, the thesis is structured as follows. Chapter 1

provides an introduction to benzodiazepine use and misuse, and argues the

importance of research exploring problematic, or contra-indicative, outcomes

associated with benzodiazepine use. Chapter 2 follows with an introduction to

interpersonal aggression, and its links with benzodiazepine use, and argues that

intrapersonal factors are an important consideration when investigating aggressive

behaviour. This chapter also introduces a well-validated theory of motivational

tendencies, the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory, which is used to enhance the

theoretical rigour of the literature base (i.e., Aim 2). An argument is presented

regarding how this theory specifically adds to our understanding of benzodiazepine-

related aggression. Chapter 3 presents a systematic review of the currently available

literature exploring the association between benzodiazepine use and subsequent

aggression or violence, providing a critical overview of what is currently understood

about this response, and identifying key gaps in the literature base, some of which

are then targeted by two original studies (i.e., Aim 1). Chapter 4 presents an original,

cross-sectional study of community members which assesses the relative importance

Page 16: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

16

of motivational tendencies (as defined by the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory),

compared to benzodiazepine-related factors (i.e., type, dose), in predicting

aggression. Chapter 5 presents a second, smaller cross-sectional study which

examines the differences between violent and non-violent offenders’ benzodiazepine

use patterns and intrapersonal characteristics (i.e., Aim 3). Chapter 6 combines the

findings of Chapters 3-5 with a discussion of how the thesis furthers our

understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression. Implications relating to

benzodiazepine prescribing practices and policy, especially in forensic contexts,

medico-legal decision making, and selection of appropriate treatment targets within

addiction and forensic mental health sectors are discussed. Opportunities for further

developing the literature base regarding benzodiazepine-related aggression are also

considered.

In order to understand the nature and depth of these implications, however, it

is important to first understand the nature of benzodiazepine (mis)use. The following

sections provide an overview of benzodiazepine use and misuse, including a

discussion of why benzodiazepines are so frequently misused, common side effects,

and the occurrence of behavioural disinhibition following benzodiazepines, which

includes aggressive behaviour. It is then argued that although informative and

necessary, neurological understandings of benzodiazepine-related aggression are

currently insufficient and impractical platforms on which to base intervention, policy

or management strategies, warranting closer investigation of other contributory and

explanatory factors in this response.

1.2 Medical Indications for Benzodiazepine Prescriptions

Benzodiazepines are commonly prescribed to treat symptoms of stress,

anxiety, and sleep disorders (Bisaga, 2008), as well as to assist in the management of

Page 17: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

17

symptoms associated with alcohol (Ashton, 2002) and heroin dependence (Fry,

Smith, Bruno, O’Keefe, & Miller, 2007). Benzodiazepines are also used for

anaesthesia, epilepsy (Drugs and Crime Prevention Committee [DCPC], 2007), and

acute psychosis with hyperexcitability and aggressiveness (Ashton, 2002). Despite

their wide range of indications, which informs the doses at which they are prescribed,

benzodiazepines are generally used for their anxiolytic and sedative properties.

Benzodiazepines can be short- (e.g., oxazepam, temazepam, alprazolam) or long-

acting (e.g., diazepam), and bind primarily to the GABAA receptor (Paton, 2002),

potentiating the inhibitory action of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA; Lader,

2011). This suppresses the central nervous system (CNS), slowing down the

messages received and sent from the brain (DCPC, 2007).

Benzodiazepines are widely prescribed. For example, during 2005,

benzodiazepines were among the most commonly prescribed pharmaceuticals in

Victoria (DCPC, 2007) and across Australia (Nicholas, 2010). Between 2002 and

2007 there was a national increase (from 23.76 to 24.11 defined daily dose/1000

population/day) in the prescription of anxiolytics, sedatives, and hypnotics

(Hollingworth & Siskind, 2010). Although it appears that the total amount of

benzodiazepines dispensed has since decreased, the quantity of benzodiazepines

prescribed per script has increased (Islam, Conigrave, Day, Nguyen, & Haber, 2014).

Notably, rates of diazepam prescriptions have remained stable, and alprazolam

prescriptions have increased eight-fold between 1992 and 2011 (Islam et al., 2014).

A separate examination of prescription data indicated that in 2011, diazepam was the

most commonly dispensed anxiolytic, and temazepam was the most commonly

dispensed sedative (Stephenson, Karanges, & McGregor, 2013). Although less

readily available, international data also indicate a high rate of benzodiazepine

Page 18: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

18

prescription and dispensing. For example, Norwegian rates of benzodiazepine

dispensing are increasing (Bjørner, Tvete, Aursnes, & Skomedal, 2013),

benzodiazepine prescriptions are written at a rate of 37.6 per 100 persons across

America (Paulozzi, Mack, & Hockenberry, 2014), and dispensing of diazepam and

lorazepam significantly increased in England between 2011 and 2012 (The Health

and Social Care Information Centre [HSCIC], 2013).

Poor adherence to prescribing guidelines and protocols partially explains the

high rate of benzodiazepine prescriptions. For example, an examination of

Tasmanian nursing home residents’ medication (n = 2345) identified that

benzodiazepines were among the most frequently inappropriately prescribed

medications (Stafford, Alswayan, & Tenni, 2011). Prescribing guidelines suggest

that benzodiazepines should only be used for short-term treatment, and that careful

dose titration is necessary when coming off benzodiazepines, due to their

dependence potential (e.g., Jones, Nielsen, Bruno, Frei, & Lubman, 2011; Nicholas,

Lee, & Roche, 2011). Alprazolam is widely considered to have the most abuse and

harm potential of the benzodiazepines, due to its short-acting effects, and has

therefore been recently up-scheduled in Australia to a controlled substance (Schedule

8).The only other benzodiazepine to fall under Schedule 8 restrictions is

flunitrazepam. Australia’s rescheduling of alprazolam is despite unchanged

classification of alprazolam as a drug of moderate risk of harm or dependence (i.e.,

Class C) in other countries (United Kingdom: Misuse of Drugs Act 1971 UK; New

Zealand: Misuse of Drugs Act 1975 NZ). Such rescheduling attempts to reduce the

accessibility of alprazolam, and therein reduce the associated harmful sequelae.

However, as discussed by Islam and colleagues (2014), such rescheduling often leads

to compensatory increased (mis)use of a substitute benzodiazepine, and a broader

Page 19: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

19

policy regarding the prescribing and regulation of benzodiazepines as a psychotropic

drug class is required.

1.3 Prevalence of Non-Medical Use of Benzodiazepines

The following sections outline the widespread misuse of benzodiazepines in

Australia and internationally. Importantly, the Australian rates below were recorded

prior to the recent rescheduling of alprazolam, and therefore it is as yet unclear how

benzodiazepine use patterns have been impacted.

1.3.1 Defining non-medical use.

The terms ‘misuse’ and ‘non-medically prescribed use’ are used

interchangeably within this thesis to refer to benzodiazepine use which is not within

the explicit boundaries of a prescription from an appropriately qualified prescriber

(except where otherwise stated). This may include using a higher dose than detailed

on the prescription, extending use past the period (or the reason) for which

benzodiazepines were indicated, or using non-prescribed (i.e., black market)

benzodiazepines.

1.3.2 Australian data.

1.3.2.1 General community. The Australian Institute of Health and Welfare’s

(AIHW) National Drug Strategy Household Survey (NDSHS) is conducted on a

regular basis to explore the nature of substance use by Australian individuals aged 14

years and over. They define non-medical use of a substance as use to enhance or

induce a drug experience, enhance performance, or for cosmetic reasons (AIHW,

2011). Unfortunately, this survey does not explicitly report on benzodiazepine use,

but instead describes pharmaceutical medication misuse, which includes pain killers,

tranquillisers, steroids, methadone and buprenorphine, and other medical opiates.

Page 20: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

20

Therefore, both the specificity and the scope of the findings is greatly reduced, and

the following rates are commented on with caution.

The rate of pharmaceutical misuse in the general community has been

steadily and significantly increasing over the past decade. In 2010, 7.4% of the

general Australian population reported ever having misused pharmaceutical

medications, with 4.2% reporting such use in the 12 months prior to the survey

(AIHW, 2011). By 2013, 4.7% reported misusing pharmaceuticals in the past 12

months, representing a 1% increase since 2007 (AIHW, 2014). Use of tranquilisers

specifically displayed a non-significant increasing trend, though they were the

second most frequently misused pharmaceutical drug following pain-

killers/analgesics, with 1.6% of people aged 14 years or older reporting their misuse

(AIHW, 2014). Inspection of age and gender indicates that tranquilisers were

predominantly misused by females aged 20-29 years, and males aged 30-39 years

(AIHW, 2014). Compared to the 2010 data (AIHW, 2011), a new cohort of older

males appear to be reporting tranquiliser misuse, potentially warranting targeted

attention in community prevention strategies. Recent estimates however, indicate that

benzodiazepine misuse occurs at a considerably greater rate in both clinical and

forensic populations.

1.3.2.2 Clinical samples. National figures demonstrate high use of

benzodiazepines in illicit drug-using populations, as 83% of Australian people who

inject drugs (PWID) have used benzodiazepines (Stafford & Burns, 2012). Within

Victoria alone, the majority of PWID interviewed between 2008 and 2010 for the

Illicit Drug Reporting System (IDRS) reported recent benzodiazepine use (74%),

with 6% injecting benzodiazepines (Horyniak et al., 2012). Moreover, interviews

with drug treatment clients have demonstrated that only 14% are using

Page 21: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

21

benzodiazepines as prescribed, and that whilst only 7% present solely for

benzodiazepine-related treatment, an additional 37% report benzodiazepine use to

also cause concern (Nielsen et al., 2008). More recent national figures suggest that

benzodiazepines are commonly used in the context of other substance use, as 85% of

those who seek treatment primarily for benzodiazepine use report additional drugs of

concern (mostly alcohol and cannabis; AIHW, 2013a). It is noted, however, that the

number of people presenting for treatment for benzodiazepine misuse has reduced,

with only 2% of national drug and alcohol treatment episodes targeting

benzodiazepines as the principal drug of concern, and a further 7% of episodes

targeting benzodiazepines as an additional drug of concern (AIHW, 2013a).

Historical data suggests that females were more likely to report difficulties with

benzodiazepines (DCPC, 2007), however recent figures are suggesting males are

now reporting slightly higher rates of benzodiazepine-related problems than females

(AIHW, 2013a). Alprazolam and diazepam appear to be the favoured

benzodiazepines in clinical drug-using populations (Nielsen et al., 2008; Stafford &

Burns, 2012).

1.3.2.3 Forensic samples. The Drug Use Monitoring in Australia (DUMA)

program, an initiative of the Australian Institute of Criminology, reviews the

substance use and crime patterns of police detainees on a quarterly basis. Compared

to 2007, when 15% of police detainees reported having used illegal (i.e., non-

prescribed) benzodiazepines in the previous 12 months (Loxley, 2007), recent figures

indicate that 25% of adult police detainees now report non-medical benzodiazepine

use (Ng & Macgregor, 2012). This rate is also higher than that pertaining to

tranquiliser or sleeping pills misuse reported by new prison entrants (16%; AIHW,

2013b). Importantly, benzodiazepines are the most commonly used type of

Page 22: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

22

pharmaceutical among police detainees, often used in combination with other drugs

(McGregor, Gately & Fleming, 2011; Ng & Macgregor, 2012). Specifically,

alprazolam and diazepam are preferred (McGregor et al., 2011; Sweeney & Payne,

2012).

The recent inclusion of urinalysis into the DUMA program provides

corroboration of self-reported substance use. Consistent with self-reported rates,

nearly one in five (23%) detainees tested positive for benzodiazepines between 2009

and 2010 (Sweeney & Payne, 2012). During this period, benzodiazepines were the

second most commonly detected drug (following cannabis), and were most

commonly detected among females (36%), detainees aged 31-35 years old (32%),

and property offenders (31%; Sweeney & Payne, 2012). Of note, 20% of violent

detainees tested positive for benzodiazepines (Sweeney & Payne, 2012), highlighting

the importance of elucidating the link between benzodiazepine use and violent

behaviour.

1.3.3 International data.

Similar to Australia, benzodiazepines are among the most commonly misused

pharmaceuticals in New Zealand (Sheridan, Jones, & Aspden, 2012), England

(Home Office Statistics, 2012; HSCIC, 2011), America (Substance Abuse and

Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA], 2011), and Thailand (Kerr et al.,

2010; Puangkot, Laohasiriwong, Saenqsuwan, & Chiawiriyabunya, 2011).

Furthermore, illicit benzodiazepines are being seized at increasing rates in European

countries, and benzodiazepine trafficking and abuse across the Middle East is also

rising (International Narcotics Control Board, 2014). Given such international use of

benzodiazepines, enhanced understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression is

likely to have wide-reaching implications.

Page 23: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

23

1.4 Motivations to Misuse Benzodiazepines

Despite their widespread misuse, few studies have assessed why people use

benzodiazepines for non-medical reasons. There is some indication that

benzodiazepines are used as relief from negative emotions and life experiences,

predominantly by individuals with low levels of novelty seeking (Adams et al.,

2003). However, studies of clinical and forensic populations suggest that

benzodiazepine use may be more complex, as findings indicate that benzodiazepines

are used for multiple reasons. Non-medical use of benzodiazepines has been

primarily attributed to the reduction of anxiety and stress by American street-based

illicit drug users, methadone maintenance patients, and residential drug treatment

clients (Rigg & Ibañez, 2010), and Australian adult police detainees (n = 986;

McGregor et al., 2011) and injecting drug users (n = 102; Best, Wilson, Reed, &

Harney, 2012). Subsidiary reasons for non-medical use of benzodiazepines include

pain relief, drug substitution, to get high (Rigg & Ibañez, 2010; Nielsen et al., 2013),

and managing alcohol and drug withdrawal (Best et al., 2012; McGregor et al., 2011;

Nielsen et al., 2008; Nielsen et al., 2013). At other times, benzodiazepines are used

merely due to availability and curiosity (McGregor et al., 2011), and specifically to

improve the effects of heroin (Best et al., 2012). As such, opioid users may be more

likely to use benzodiazepines for recreational reasons (i.e., enhance their opioid

intoxication; Jones, Mogali, & Comer, 2012), although one French study of opiate-

dependent individuals (n = 92) suggested that benzodiazepines were most commonly

used for a combination of self-therapeutic and hedonistic motivations over time

(Fatséas, Lavie, Denis, & Auriacombe, 2009). This deviation may reflect cultural

differences, or be an effect of self-report data where recall bias may have impacted

the various outcomes. The literature, however, does paint a concerning picture of

Page 24: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

24

benzodiazepines which are inappropriately prescribed and then used to excess for

both self-medication and to enhance other substance use. As discussed by Bennett,

Holloway, Brookman, Parry, and Gorden (2014), users may apply ‘techniques of

neutralization’ to justify their misuse of prescription medication. Introduced as a

method to explain away delinquent criminal behaviour (Sykes & Matza, 1957),

techniques of neutralization enable the individual to dismiss any social or legal

constraints on their behaviour (Bennett et al., 2014). Initially, these included denial

of responsibility, denial of injury, denial of victim, condemnation of condemners,

and appeals to higher loyalties (Sykes & Matza, 1957). Application of the concept to

cannabis use has resulted in substance use specific ‘risk denial techniques’ (e.g.,

scapegoating, comparing risk, emphasising personal control; Peretti-Watel, 2003;

Sandberg, 2012). Indeed, to justify prescription medication misuse, university

students most commonly referenced biological need (i.e., desperate for medication),

legitimacy (i.e., person had superior knowledge or experience of the drug), or denial

of choice (i.e., GP unavailability, cost of prescription; Bennett et al., 2014).

Unfortunately, however, although sedatives and tranquilisers were misused by this

sample, the results are not medication type specific. Regarding benzodiazepines

specifically, some Australian drug users perceive them to be an “entitlement” as they

are legally available (Nielsen et al., 2008). This sentiment is likely to impact the

effectiveness of public health promotion and awareness strategies regarding

benzodiazepine-related aggression.

1.4.1 Benzodiazepine sources.

Not only are benzodiazepines inappropriately prescribed and misused, but so

too are they acquired from a number of sources. Consequently, there is concern about

a large hidden population of pharmaceutical misusers who may be unable to be

Page 25: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

25

accessed through regular health care streams. Analysis of drug trends in Melbourne,

Hobart, and Darwin demonstrate that benzodiazepines are often diverted through the

black market, commonly through forged prescriptions or doctor shopping (Fry et al.,

2007). Indeed, although reflecting a reduction since the 2011-2012 period,

benzodiazepines were the most commonly detected pharmaceuticals on the

Australian border during 2012-2013 (Australian Crime Commission [ACC], 2014). It

appears, however, that substance users are using a combination of legitimate

prescriptions and illegitimate methods (i.e., street dealers, theft, from friends, as a

gift) to source benzodiazepines (Best et al., 2013; Havens, Walker, & Leukefeld,

2010; Nielsen et al., 2013). Poor adherence to prescribing protocols appears to be

inadvertently supporting benzodiazepine diversion onto the black market. For

example, Ibañez, Levi-Minzi, Rigg, and Mooss (2013) recently identified that

diversion of benzodiazepines from healthcare providers is resulting in the provision

of more benzodiazepine pills (on average) than street dealers. Their survey of various

drug users (n = 1207) indicated that although patients were accessing healthcare

providers at a lesser frequency than non-healthcare sources, they were receiving

greater quantities of benzodiazepines per visit. In addition, healthcare providers were

more likely to be accessed by higher income participants (Ibañez et al., 2013), again

highlighting the likelihood of a hidden population of benzodiazepine users who are

unlikely to be accessed through health care streams. Combined, the findings

demonstrated that benzodiazepines are easily accessible, and such ease appears to be

resulting in increasing incidence of benzodiazepine-related harms.

1.5 Benzodiazepine-related Harms

Benzodiazepines are being increasingly linked with medical emergencies and

harm. Worryingly, between 2000 and 2009, benzodiazepine-related cases made up

Page 26: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

26

the highest proportion of pharmaceutical drug-related ambulance attendances in

Melbourne, and there was a significant increase in the proportion of benzodiazepine-

related cases requiring transport to hospital (Lloyd & McElwee, 2011). These rates

continue to rise across Victoria, with benzodiazepines becoming the second most

common drug category overall (after alcohol) involved in attendances (Lloyd,

Matthews, & Gao, 2014). Worse, coronial data from 2011 indicates that half (50.3%)

of Victorian drug-related deaths involve benzodiazepines; a figure second only to

opioid analgesics (51.4%; Coroners Court of Victoria, 2013). Benzodiazepines are

similarly reported at high, and increasing, rates in hospitalisation and/or mortality

data in America (SAMHSA, 2012), England and Wales (Office for National

Statistics, 2014), and Scotland (Zador et al., 2007). International findings

demonstrate benzodiazepine use is common in non-fatally injured emergency room

patients (e.g., Rockett, Putnam, Jia, & Smith, 2006), especially those with violent

injuries (e.g., Kurzthaler et al., 2005) and overdose (Hamad, Al-Ghadban, Carvounis,

Soliman, & Coritsidis, 2000). Injection of benzodiazepines can also lead to a number

of intravenous-related health complications (Breen, Degenhardt, Roxburgh, Bruno,

& Jenkinson, 2004). In addition, benzodiazepines increase the risk of being involved

in a traffic accident by 60-80%, with a 40% increase in responsibility (Dassanayake,

Michie, Carter, & Jones, 2011). Indeed, benzodiazepines are the second most

prevalent form of drug found in the blood of Victorian drivers injured in motor

vehicle collisions, following cannabis (Ch’ng et al., 2007). Of note, benzodiazepine-

related harm data appears to suggest that females may be more likely than males to

require ambulance or medical attention following benzodiazepine use (Longo,

Hunter, Lokan, White, & White, 2000; Lloyd & McElwee, 2011; Lloyd et al., 2014).

This gender inequality reflects a population experiencing drug-related harm which is

Page 27: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

27

not reflected in the illicit drug research. Consideration of specific benzodiazepines

has suggested that alprazolam may be the most toxic benzodiazepine (Isbister,

O’Regan, Sibbritt, & Whyte, 2004). Indeed, since 2005, and the rise of alprazolam

prescribing, alprazolam has been increasingly associated with heroin-related deaths

in Victoria (Rintoul, Dobbin, Nielsen, Degenhardt, & Drummer, 2010). However,

recent coronial data from Australia and England indicate diazepam to be the most

commonly identified benzodiazepine in drug-related deaths (Coroners Court of

Victoria, 2013; Office for National Statistics, 2014). Through reviewing such

national and international data on benzodiazepine-related harms, a number of

improvements to benzodiazepine prescribing protocols have been suggested.

Where benzodiazepines are prescribed, Dobbin (2014) suggests that

benzodiazepine-related harms can be reduced via thorough assessments of patient

needs and drug risk, as well as a comprehensive management plan involving

pharmaceutical medication not subject to abuse and non-medicinal approaches.

Lader (2014) further argues that a combined pharmaceutical and psychological

strategy is most appropriate, and that benzodiazepine prescription should only follow

the exhaustion of alternative options. Such recommendations are made not only in

response to the concerning harms identified above, but also the more common side

effects associated with benzodiazepine use.

1.5.1 Medical effects of benzodiazepine use.

Benzodiazepines can produce multiple short- and long-term side effects.

Other than the intended anxiolytic and sedative effects, short term effects can include

drowsiness, vertigo, motor incoordination, mild impairments in memory and

concentration, and emotional depression (Longo & Johnson, 2000). Aggravation or

production of depressive symptoms may be due to serotonin and norepinephrine

Page 28: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

28

inhibition, whilst blunted pleasure and pain may be due to the inhibition of the

emotional centers in the brain (Ashton, 2002). At very high doses the depressing

effects of benzodiazepines can induce unconsciousness, coma, and even death when

used in combination with other drugs (DCPC, 2007). Furthermore, benzodiazepines

can induce neurological changes leading to tolerance, withdrawal, and dependence

(DCPC, 2007). Withdrawal symptoms can include anxiety, insomnia, autonomic

hyperactivity, and seizures (Longo & Johnson, 2000).

Continued use of benzodiazepines can result in effects such as avolition,

memory loss, personality change, anxiety, irritability, sleep problems, somatic

problems, and increased aggressiveness (DCPC, 2007). Moreover, benzodiazepine-

induced impaired concentration and attention can negatively affect memory,

especially episodic memory, which may influence uncharacteristic behaviours

(Ashton, 2002). As such, benzodiazepine use has been frequently associated with

increased paradoxical behaviour.

1.5.2 Paradoxical effects of benzodiazepines.

Benzodiazepines have been associated with paradoxical effects, such as

excitement, irritability, aggression, hostility, and impulsivity (Longo & Johnson,

2000). Such disinhibition, or the loss of restraint over behaviour, is often socially

inappropriate, unpredictable, and uncharacteristic of the individual (Bond, 1998).

Whilst only in a minority of cases (Paton, 2002), disinhibitory reactions (e.g.,

aggression, hyperactivity, inappropriate sexual behaviour) have been observed

during benzodiazepine use and withdrawal (Bond, 1998), and may lead to

involvement with the criminal justice system (Redman, 1994). This so called ‘Rambo

effect’ can occur at both therapeutic and higher doses (DCPC, 2007), and has been

suggested to be more common following intravenous administration (Ashton, 2002).

Page 29: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

29

Two main physiological mechanisms have been hypothesised to explain such

disinhibition. In some cases, benzodiazepines can stimulate the CNS, leading to

increased talkativeness, mania, anxiety, restlessness, sleep disturbances, acute rage,

and extreme aggression (DCPC, 2007). This can also include nightmares,

hallucinations, exacerbated seizures, and hyperactive behaviour (Ashton, 2002).

Individuals report feeling invisible and invincible, and are sometimes unaware of

committing a crime (Fry et al., 2007). Conversely, the Rambo effect may result from

CNS depression which reduces inhibitions, leading to impaired judgement (DCPC,

2007) and the suppression of external social cues which would normally guide

behaviour (Longo & Johnson, 2000). By decreasing the restraining influence of the

cortex, such depression can provoke increased excitement, psychosis, anxiety,

hostility, rage, and alcohol use (Paton, 2002). It is unclear what influences these two

seemingly opposite effects.

Additional neurological theories specifically attempting to account for

aggressive behaviour post-benzodiazepine consumption include discussion of genetic

factors, GABA-related inhibition of neurotransmission, and disruption of the

endogenous anxiety/threat-detection system (for reviews, see Essman, 1978; Hoaken

& Stewart, 2003; Paton, 2002; van der Bijl & Roelofse, 1991). The majority of

related research has been conducted on animals, and therefore has limited

generalizability to humans. These studies have noted, however, that certain subunits

on the GABAA receptors (γ, α) may be implicated in aggression mediation (e.g., Lee

& Gammie, 2010; Miczek, Fish, & DeBold, 2003), with one study suggesting low

doses of the receptor agonists (i.e., midazolam, triazolam) can significantly increase

aggression duration and frequency (i.e., biting; Gourley, DeBold, Yin, Cook, &

Mizek, 2005). Combining benzodiazepines with alcohol has also been demonstrated

Page 30: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

30

to enhance aggressive responding in rodents (de Almeida, Saft, Rosa, & Miczek,

2010; Miczek, Weerts, & DeBold, 1993), and some evidence suggests a history of

(Ferrari, Parmigiani, Rodgers, & Palanza, 1997), or predisposition towards (Weerts,

Miller, Hood, & Miczek, 2010), aggressive behaviour enhances benzodiazepine-

related aggression. As informative as such findings are, however, the majority of

such research provide little options in the way of realistic screening or intervention

targets for benzodiazepine-related aggression in humans, or have yet to be replicated

in human research (i.e., role of pre-existing aggressive tendencies). Dose seems

equally unable to assist our understanding, as both high (Paton, 2002) and low doses

(Hoaken & Stewart, 2003) of benzodiazepines have been implicated in subsequent

aggressive responding. It is therefore important to extend beyond neurological

reasoning to explore more easily measurable, and potentially changeable, factors,

such as situational and intrapersonal elements which are associated with this

response, and base such research on examination of human participants. It is

precisely these factors that have been argued to be more important in understanding

the benzodiazepine-aggression relationship (Hoaken & Stewart, 2003; Lion,

Azcarate, & Koepke, 1975), but which have failed to attract much investigation.

1.6 Summary

Benzodiazepines are frequently prescribed and misused, often in an attempt

to secure a physiological high and/or reduce negative affective experiences.

However, benzodiazepines have increasingly become associated with medical and

psychosocial harm on a global level, with ambulance and morbidity data reflecting a

rise in benzodiazepine use in their patients. Of note, approximately one fifth of users

experience increased aggressive behaviour following benzodiazepine use, and such

incidents are as yet poorly understood. Animal data suggests that those who

Page 31: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

31

experience an aggressive response may be physiologically vulnerable; however the

applicability of such findings to human screening and prescribing policies is limited.

This thesis aims to improve our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression

by exploring the role of contributory factors (such as personality or intrapersonal

characteristics), in an effort to provide risk indicators more amenable to rapid

screening (for prescribers) and intervention. The following chapter begins by

defining the concept of aggression in general in an attempt to provide a broader

context, before presenting an argument that intrapersonal factors are a highly

relevant and important concept to explore in order to more fully understand

benzodiazepine-related aggression. A well-validated theory of personality and

motivational tendencies which will be used to explore the role of such factors in

benzodiazepine-related aggression is then introduced. The chapter concludes with a

more in-depth discussion of the aims of the thesis and how three unique studies (a

systematic review and two cross-sectional studies) were developed to attend to these

aims.

Page 32: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

32

Chapter Two: Aggression, Intrapersonal factors and Rationale for Thesis

2.1 Understanding Aggression

Like all behaviour, aggression cannot be explained through recourse to one

explanatory factor. Instead, it is multiply-determined, influenced by a complex

interplay of internal (i.e., beliefs, neurobiology, personality, intoxication) and

external (i.e., environment, opportunity, frustration) factors. The general aggression

model (GAM; Anderson & Bushman, 2002), for example, proposes that situational

(e.g., aggression cues, drugs, frustration, incentives, pain) and person inputs (e.g.,

traits, beliefs, values, attitudes, goals) inform behavioural outcomes (i.e., an

aggressive episode) through the internal states that they create. Such internal states

may include cognitions such as hostile thoughts or aggressive scripts, feelings such

as anger or hostility, and heightened physiological arousal (Anderson & Bushman,

2002). Factors which have been empirically linked with aggressive behaviour include

certain neurotransmitters and brain structures (i.e., serotonin, GABA, frontal lobe,

limbic system), mental health diagnoses and distress, sociological factors (i.e., peer

pressure, social information processing), personality predispositions (i.e.,

impulsivity, hostility, antisociality, anger), and criminal history (for reviews, see

Anderson & Bokor, 2012; Chereji, Pintea, & David, 2012; Schenk & Fremouw,

2012). Notably, associations between illicit drugs and violent crime have been well

established in the literature (Friedman, 1998), with particular reference to alcohol

(e.g., Lennings, Copeland, & Howard, 2003), methamphetamines (e.g., McKetin et

al., 2014), and anabolic steroids (e.g., Klötz, Petersson, Isaacson, & Thiblin, 2007;

Lundolm, Kall, Wallin, & Thiblin, 2010). Surprisingly, however, examinations of

benzodiazepine-related aggression or violence have rarely included exploration of

potential contributory factors such as those listed above (i.e., mental health,

Page 33: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

33

sociological factors, intrapersonal differences). By improving our understanding of

benzodiazepine-related aggression, we can hope to affect change in reducing the

frequency of this response, reducing health care costs (financial and personal), and

reducing forensic spending for judiciary, incarceration, and rehabilitation phases.

2.1.1 A note on definitions.

There is contention within the literature regarding the definitions of

‘aggression’ and ‘violence’. This includes various definitions of aggression as

involving anger, verbal, psychological, indirect (or social), and physical aspects (e.g.,

Björkqvist, 1994; Buss & Perry, 1992); instrumental or reactive motives (Buss,

1961); and highly specific types of violence (i.e., intimate partner violence; e.g.,

Straus & Mickey, 2012; Grych & Swan, 2012). Violence has been described as a

more severe type of aggression (DeWall, Anderson & Bushman, 2011), and is

therefore more frequently used in forensic contexts than research arenas (which

favour the term ‘aggression’), although the two terms have and can be used

interchangeably (e.g., Anderson & Bokor, 2012).

The current thesis adopts the definition of ‘aggression’ proposed by Baron

and Richardson (1994), which is commonly referenced in aggression and violence

literature (e.g., Anderson & Bokor, 2012; Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). However, as

physical aggression is the topic of interest, some modifications to the definition have

been made, in order to exclude instances and discussion of verbal or psychological

aggression, as it is considered that they reflect very different types of aggression, and

likely include different psychological barriers and different consequences. For the

purposes of this thesis, aggression is therefore defined as physical force directed

towards a person motivated to avoid such force (i.e., psychological or verbal

aggression/violence was excluded). The current thesis also uses ‘violence’ to refer to

Page 34: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

34

more severe or officially documented acts of aggression (i.e., resulting in

conviction). To not include ‘violence’ within our definition would exclude the

forensic studies which explored ‘violent behaviour’ or ‘violent crime’, and to

exclude studies exploring ‘aggression’ would exclude a wealth of data from clinical

and healthy populations (including the experimental data), involving a somewhat

lower severity of violent behaviour.

2.1.2 Importance of intrapersonal factors.

Personality traits and motivational tendencies provide indicators of a person’s

characteristic manner of interacting with the world. They are pervasive and endure

across time and situations, and can inform the likelihood of engagement in certain

behaviours. Research in the addiction field has demonstrated the applicability of an

individual’s personality traits to problematic substance use, related problems, and

treatment (e.g., Staiger Kambouropoulos, & Dawe, 2007). Similar findings have also

been demonstrated in the aggression and violence literature (e.g., Hosie, Gilbert,

Simpson, & Daffern, 2014; Jones, Miller, & Lynam, 2011; Miller, Lynam, &

Leukefeld, 2003). Not surprisingly, such intrinsic person characteristics have been

argued to be highly influential in the benzodiazepine-violence relationship (Hoaken

& Stewart, 2002; Lion et al., 1975). Interestingly, however, little research has been

conducted into these potential explanatory factors.

Investigation of the benzodiazepine-aggression relationship has tended to

focus on establishing and replicating associative findings. As will be discussed in

greater depth in Chapter 3, cross-sectional and laboratory studies have found positive

findings between certain benzodiazepines, such as diazepam and alprazolam, and

self-reported aggression or behavioural proxies of aggressive behaviour. However,

only cursory investigation of contributory or explanatory factors has occurred.

Page 35: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

35

Notably, only a handful of cross-sectional and experimental studies have assessed the

potential role of intrapersonal characteristics, including psychiatric vulnerability or

personality traits, when examining benzodiazepine-related aggression. For example,

Dåderman and colleagues explored the case histories of Swedish male forensic

populations (via file review and semi-structured interviews) to identify whether there

was a link between flunitrazepam use, personality and violent offending. Although in

one study they found no personality differences between flunitrazepam users and

non-users (Dåderman & Edman, 2001), they suggested that certain personality traits

(i.e., boredom susceptibility, verbal aggression, Dåderman & Lidberg, 1999; anxiety,

low self-esteem, Dåderman, Fredriksson, Kristiansson, Nilsson, & Lidberg, 2002;

psychopathy characteristics, Dåderman, Edman, Meurling, Levander, &

Kristiansson, 2012) may influence an aggressive response to flunitrazepam.

However, their conclusions relied on targeted examination of flunitrazepam-using

violent offenders (Dåderman & Lidberg, 1999; Dåderman et al., 2002) or violent

offenders without non-violent control groups (Dåderman & Edman, 2001; Dåderman

et al., 2012), failed to statistically account for poly-substance use, have limited

generality, and cannot suggest temporal causality between flunitrazepam use and

violent offending. However, the use of more stringent, controlled designs has

provided little further explanation of the role of intrapersonal factors in this response.

To date, only two experimental studies have explicitly examined the role of

personality in benzodiazepine-related aggression (Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002;

Wilkinson, 1985), though an additional early study did identify interesting

personality-related results post-hoc (Cherek, Steinberg, Kelly, Robinson, & Spiga,

1990). Wilkinson (1985), using double-blind and placebo-controlled methods, and a

competitive reaction time task, identified that diazepam-related aggression may be

Page 36: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

36

mediated by trait anxiety. In her study, undergraduates who were less cautious of

their environment (i.e., low trait-anxious) displayed the greatest enhancement in

responding indicative of aggression following diazepam consumption, compared to

high trait-anxious undergraduates. In a similarly designed study, Ben-Porath and

Taylor (2002) suggested that diazepam-related aggression may be related to hostility,

as undergraduates who exhibited higher scores on a standardised hostility measure

displayed greater increases in aggression following diazepam use than those with

lower hostility scores. This group difference, however, failed to reach significance.

Cherek and colleagues (1990) also highlighted the role of hostility, as increased

aggressive responding following diazepam use was only observed in participants

with high hostility scores. However, the sample on which this conclusion is based

was very small (n = 9), making overall conclusions regarding the role of hostility in

this response tentative at best. Unfortunately, additional laboratory studies which

reported to measure personality characteristics failed to analyse or discuss whether

individual differences affected aggressive responding (Bond, Curran, Bruce,

O’Sullivan, & Shine, 1995; Bond & Lader, 1988; Bond & Silveira, 1993; Pietras et

al., 2005).

Research into benzodiazepine-related aggression has rarely explored the role

of contributory factors, such as intrapersonal characteristics. As noted above, the

handful of studies which have included such analysis examined various

characteristics, often with minimal (if any) theoretical reasoning as to the relevance

or importance of the characteristic(s) selected. The current thesis aims to build on

this research, by taking a theory-driven approach to understanding the personality

and motivational factors associated with benzodiazepine-related aggression. Such an

approach can provide treatment and risk management outcomes which are based

Page 37: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

37

upon a strong theoretical basis, are testable, and can inform the development of

further research. As will be demonstrated in Chapter 4, a large community-based

study will use a well-validated theory of approach and avoidance motivation, the

Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST), to measure the role of motivational

tendencies in benzodiazepine-related aggression. The main assumptions of this

theory are presented below, with commentary on how this theory may be valuable in

furthering our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression.

2.1.2.1 Gray’s (revised) Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. The RST is a

neuropsychological theory of emotion, motivation, learning processes, and

personality (Corr, 2009). The theory has undergone substantial revisions since its

original conceptualisation (Gray, 1982), and the current thesis applies the Gray and

McNaughton (2003) revision (rRST). The theory postulates that individual

sensitivities to punishment and reward lead to motivations to engage in approach or

avoidance behaviour. An individual’s tendency to engage in approach or avoidance

behaviour provides an indication of their underlying personality (i.e., trait

impulsivity or anxiety, respectively; Corr, 2009). This theory was purposefully

selected due to its strong validation across research of problematic substance use and

related outcomes (e.g., Booth & Hasking, 2009; Dissabandara, Loxton, Dias,

Daglish, & Stadlin, 2012; Loxton et al., 2008), its biological underpinnings which

specifically account for the impact of benzodiazepines on the functioning of certain

motivational tendencies (see discussion below; Gray & McNaughton, 2003), and its

conceptually sound nature. In addition, the current application of the rRST not only

enhances our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression and theoretical

rigour of related research, but it also expandes the rRST literature to include

benzodiazepine use, providing precedence for future comparative studies. The theory

Page 38: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

38

proposes three separate but interacting motivational systems which make up

personality (Corr, 2004), and influence approach, avoidant, and cautious behaviour

(Corr, 2008).

Approach tendencies. The behavioural approach system (BAS) mediates

reactions to appetitive stimuli (Corr, 2004), and is thought to be primarily modulated

by dopamine (Pickering & Corr, 2008). Approach motivation promotes anticipatory

pleasure, and is associated with the personality traits of optimism, hope, reward

orientation, and impulsivity (Corr & Perkins, 2006). However, it has been suggested

that impulsivity does not explain the full range of processes involved in this system,

such as restraint and planning (Corr, 2009), and that extraversion (Pickering & Corr,

2008) or reward learning (Berkman, Lieberman, & Gable, 2009) may mediate

activation of the system instead. This contention is reflected in the two main

measures of the BAS, where Torrubia, Ávila, Moltó, and Caseras (2001)

conceptualise the system as primarily involving sensitivity to reward cues in their

Sensitivity to Punishment Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ), whilst

Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/BAS scales include three components which seem to

involve the processes mentioned above – drive (i.e., goal pursuit, functional

impulsivity), fun seeking (i.e., dysfunctional impulsivity, minimal thought to

consequences), and reward responsiveness (i.e., positive energy and affect in

response to reward cues; Tull, Gratz, Latzman, Kimbre, & Lejuez, 2010). Clinically,

approach motivation has been associated with addictive behaviours, high-risk

impulsive behaviours, and some aspects of mania (Corr & Perkins, 2006). It has been

suggested that impulsivity, more than reward responsiveness, underlies the approach

motivation and substance use relationship (Corr, 2008). Of note, and as will be

discussed in Chapter 4, approach motivations have been consistently associated with

Page 39: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

39

aggressive behaviour, most notably the BAS characteristic Drive. Reflecting a

tendency to be persistent in the pursuit of desired goals, Drive has been positively

associated with anger (Smits & Kuppens, 2005), the tendency to not suppress anger

or its expression (Cooper, Gomez, & Buck, 2008), and aggressive behaviour

(Harmon-Jones, 2003; Seibert, Miller, Pryor, Reidy, & Zeichner, 2010), and appears

to operate along similar neural structures and pathways as does aggressive behaviour

(Beaver, Lawrence, Passamonti, & Calder, 2008). Due to its strong association with

aggressive tendencies, particular attention is paid to whether Drive can contribute to

our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Aversive (inhibitory) tendencies. The fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS)

mediates reactions to both conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli, and

produces escape or avoidance behaviours (Corr, 2004). This system is associated

with the emotion of fear (Corr, 2004), and fearful and avoidant personality traits

(Corr & Perkins, 2006). When threat is distal, this system produces flight or freezing,

whilst in the face of proximal danger, it produces a fight reaction (Jackson, 2009).

Empirical research has demonstrated that individuals highly sensitive to fear are

more likely to engage in threat magnification, perceiving threats as especially close

and intense (Perkins, Cooper, Abdelall, Smilie, & Corr, 2010). Clinically, a strong

fear system is associated with phobia and panic (Corr & Perkins, 2006).

The behavioural inhibition system (BIS) aims to resolve goal conflict which

generates anxiety (Corr, 2004). Thought to be distributed over a number of neural

structures (Pickering & Corr, 2008), this system assesses risk and works to increase

risk aversion (Corr, 2008). Conflicts are usually approach-avoidance, but can also be

approach-approach, or avoidance-avoidance (Corr & Perkins, 2006). Simultaneous

and similar activation of the above two systems stimulates this conflict resolution

Page 40: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

40

system (Pickering & Corr, 2008). The system increases arousal (Tull et al., 2010),

initiates risk assessment, scans memory (Corr, 2004), and increases the negative

valence of stimuli in order to resolve the goal conflict (Corr & Perkins, 2006).

Subjectively, the process presents as rumination and worry, with a sense of possible

danger or loss (Corr, 2004), and is predominantly associated with trait anxiety (Corr,

2002). Clinically, inadequate goal resolution presents as generalised anxiety and

obsessive-compulsive disorder (Corr & Perkins, 2006). An important distinction

between the BIS and FFFS is that in situations of threat which do not need to be

faced, fear mediates avoidant behaviour (Perkins, Kemp, & Corr, 2007), yet when

threatening stimuli must be faced, the BIS is activated, producing anxiety-mediated

risk assessment, cautious approach behaviour, or withheld entrance (Perkins et al.,

2007). Anxiolytics (i.e., benzodiazepines) have been demonstrated to selectively

impact the BIS, and reduce the salience of threats of punishment, failure, or the

uncertainties of novel situations, reducing inhibited behaviour and promoting a more

care-free attitude (Gray & McNaughton, 2003).

Collectively, the approach and avoidance motivations driven mediated by the

BIS and FFFS in response to aversive stimuli are referred to as punishment

sensitivity. Individuals who display strong punishment sensitivity only are argued to

be least likely to engage in aggressive behaviour following benzodiazepine

consumption. Although benzodiazepines may induce a change from threat-reactive

behaviour to neutral, pre-threat behaviour, it is suggested that in most individuals this

does not extend to violent behaviour. These individuals are not sensitive to reward

cues or impulsive by nature, and therefore would be less likely to act with minimal

thought to consequences, such as responding aggressively to slights or frustration. It

is acknowledged that some trait-fearful individuals may engage in aggressive

Page 41: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

41

behaviour when they perceive a threat to be especially close or intense (Jackson,

2009), though this behaviour presents only in a minority of cases. However, as

outlined below, if a punishment sensitive individual also exhibits strong approach

motivation, benzodiazepine-related aggression is argued to be increasingly likely.

System interactions and frustrative non-reward. Initially, the RST conceived

reward and punishment sensitivity as separate motivational systems

(Kambouropoulos & Staiger, 2004). However, inconsistent findings in the literature

regarding individual responses to reward and punishment prompted a different

conceptualisation (Corr, 2004). The Joint Systems Hypothesis (JSH) proposes that

under certain conditions, reward and punishment can exert interdependent effects, as

the related sensitivities are both facilitative and antagonistic (Corr, 2004). This

hypothesis describes the ability of aversive stimuli to facilitate fear and anxiety

whilst antagonising approach motivation, and of appetitive stimuli to facilitate

approach behaviour whilst inhibiting avoidance (Corr, 2004). Specifically,

individuals with high levels of impulsivity (or approach motivation) and low levels

of trait anxiety will demonstrate the strongest reactions to appetitive stimuli, and vice

versa for aversive stimuli (Kambouropoulos & Staiger, 2004).

However, the systems do not always interact in such an antagonistic fashion.

Whilst it has been suggested that a personality characterised by both high trait

anxiety and high impulsivity is unlikely (Pickering & Corr, 2008), individuals with

such a trait pattern have been found to demonstrate the greatest level of behavioural

inhibition (Kambouropoulos & Staiger, 2004), neuroticism (Perkins et al., 2007),

and quick response times during goal pursuit (Berkman et al., 2009). Moreover, such

an interaction between appetitive and aversive motivations is evident in frustrative

non-reward (FN), experienced when the expected reward is higher than the actual

Page 42: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

42

reward (Corr, 2002). FN is a signal of non-reward (i.e., punishment) which

implicates aversive motivation, and also reflects a sensitivity to reward cues and

expectancies, implicating appetitive motivation; an individual with high appetitive

motivation (i.e., BAS) would be the first to detect non-reward and experience FN

(Corr, 2002). This suggests that FN may be caused by reward sensitivity and

mediated by punishment sensitivity, and therefore levels of FN should be greatest in

individuals with both high approach and avoidance motivation (Corr, 2002). It is

these individuals who we would expect to be most likely to experience aggressive

responses in the face of frustration or goal blocking.

Such interactive effects have yet to be explored in relation to substance-

related aggression, much less benzodiazepine-related aggression. The complexity of

the antagonistic and facilitative interactions between human motivational systems

demonstrates why it is essential to more deeply explore the intrapersonal

characteristics of those who experience benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour,

in order to provide more educated treatment and pharmaceutical management

options. The original study presented in Chapter 4 was specifically designed in order

to elucidate such deeper understanding of this response.

2.2 Summary and Thesis Rationale

A number of serious medical and forensic outcomes are associated with

benzodiazepine use. Most notably, benzodiazepine-related disinhibition is a cause for

concern, due to its association with motor vehicle accidents, risk taking, aggressive

incidents, and subsequent memory deficits, which can have implications in both

medical and forensic arenas. In particular, benzodiazepine-related aggression is

poorly understood, despite the high rate of benzodiazepine misuse in criminal justice

samples (including violent offenders), in countries where violent crime remains

Page 43: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

43

among the most commonly recorded types of crime. The potential legal, medical and

social ramifications of violent behaviour, and the increasing diversion of

benzodiazepines onto the black market and subsequent increases in misuse, indicates

a need to further understand the relationship between benzodiazepine use and

violence. The complexity likely involved in this response has impacted the literature

base, as there are a number of explanatory gaps and flaws in what we understand

about benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour. One core gap in the literature is

the identification of clear, consistent risk indicators for this response.

It has been suggested that a drug can alter the occurrence of aggressive

behaviour in an individual, depending on the individual’s underlying personality

(Paton, 2002). This premise is supported by laboratory findings demonstrating

individuals with certain underlying characteristics (i.e., hostility, low trait anxiety) to

be more likely to experience significant increases in aggressive behaviour following

benzodiazepine consumption (e.g., Cherek et al., 1990; Wilkinson, 1985). However,

despite such early theorizing (Lion et al., 1975), surprisingly little empirical literature

has been conducted into contributory factors such as personality or motivational

states. Without clear understanding of such contributory mechanisms, efforts cannot

be made to curtail benzodiazepine-related violence and its harmful medical and legal

sequelae. Furthermore, prescribing policy cannot be informed about potential risks

and contraindications for the prescription of benzodiazepines. This thesis will

therefore investigate the premise that intrapersonal factors (i.e., personality,

motivational factors, mental health) provide strong indicators of whether aggressive

behaviour is experienced post benzodiazepine consumption. Examining such

individual differences provides clear, easily identifiable risk factors to be considered

when prescribing benzodiazepines.

Page 44: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

44

2.2.1 Thesis aims, hypotheses and chapters.

The overall aim of the following research is to further understand the

relationship between benzodiazepine use and subsequent interpersonal aggression

and violence. This seemingly paradoxical response is of concern given the reasons

that benzodiazepines are generally prescribed, the context in which they are

prescribed, and the increasing misuse of benzodiazepines in healthy, clinical, and

forensic populations. Within this overarching aim, the following research has three

specific directives:

1. To systematically review the benzodiazepine-aggression literature base to

specifically identify benzodiazepine type and dose, and individual

characteristics associated with benzodiazepine-related aggression.

2. To enhance the theoretical rigour of benzodiazepine-aggression literature.

3. To identify characteristics associated with benzodiazepine-related violence in

a benzodiazepine-using, community-based criminal justice sample.

Attending to Aim 1, Chapter 3 presents a systematic review of the currently

available literature exploring benzodiazepine-related aggression. Particular attention

is paid to the role of different benzodiazepine types and dose, as well as participant

characteristics, in understanding this response. As will be discussed, the current

literature base, whilst informative, is flawed and often inconsistent, and highlights

the need for further systematic research of the various types of benzodiazepines and

greater statistical and measurement control.

Attending to Aim 2, Chapter 4 presents an original, cross-sectional study of

community members which tests a well-validated theory of motivational tendencies

against benzodiazepine-related aggression, and assesses the relative importance of

such tendencies, compared to benzodiazepine-related factors (i.e., type, dose), in

Page 45: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

45

predicting general aggression (i.e., including anger, verbal aggression) and physical

aggression specifically. Participants are community members over the age of 18

years, who report having used benzodiazepines at some point during the past year.

Through the use of hierarchical multiple regression, the study tests the hypotheses

that:

1. Motivational factors will significantly predict engagement in benzodiazepine-

related aggressive behaviour over and above benzodiazepine type or use;

2. BAS-Drive will significantly predict general aggression and physical

aggression; and

3. BAS-Drive will moderate the relationship between BIS and aggressive

outcomes. Specifically, it is predicted that the relationship between BIS and

aggressive behaviour will be stronger for individuals with high levels of

BAS-Drive.

Attending to the final aim, Chapter 5 presents a second, smaller cross-

sectional study which examines the differences between violent and non-violent

offenders’ benzodiazepine use patterns and intrapersonal characteristics (i.e., mental

health, personality). Participants are community-based offenders who committed a

crime in the six months prior to data collection, and who report using

benzodiazepines on a regular (at least monthly) basis. The study predicts that:

1. Individuals engaged in violent crime will report greater benzodiazepine use,

and higher doses, than non-violent offenders; and

2. Violent offenders will display a more complex psychiatric and social history

than non-violent offenders.

Page 46: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

46

The findings of Chapters 3-5 will be combined in Chapter 6 in a discussion of

how the thesis furthers our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Based on the combined research, the chapter will discuss the implications for

benzodiazepine prescribing practices and policy, provide commentary on the recent

rescheduling of alprazolam to a controlled substance (Schedule 8), discuss access to

benzodiazepines in forensic contexts, and indicate treatment targets both within the

addiction and forensic mental health sectors. It is hoped that this research will also

inform legal decision making about the potential culpability of an offender, and the

use of (in)appropriate legal defences.

Page 47: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

47

Chapter Three: Benzodiazepine Use and Aggressive Behaviour: A Systematic

Review

Please note, this chapter presents an expanded version of a systematic review

published in the Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry (2014), due to

copyright reasons. A link to the original article has been appended for your

consideration (Appendix A).

3.1 Rationale

Benzodiazepines are one of the most commonly prescribed pharmaceuticals

in developed countries (AIHW, 2011; Home Office Statistics, 2012; HSCIC, 2011;

Stephenson, Karanges, & McGregor, 2013; SAMHSA, 2011). They are commonly

prescribed for the management of anxiety, sleep disorders, agitation and alcohol

withdrawal (Ashton, 2002). Although the most common effects of benzodiazepines

include sedation and reduced anxiety, there have been reports of some users

experiencing behavioural disinhibition following consumption, which includes

aggressive behaviour (Bond, 1998; Fry, Smith, Bruno, O’Keefe, & Miller, 2007;

Paton, 2002). In fact, it is estimated that anywhere between 1-20% of benzodiazepine

users experience some form of increased anger or express aggression (Lader, 2011).

Furthermore, a number of clinical case studies report that individuals experience

aggressive responding following administration of a range of benzodiazepines

including diazepam (Gardos, 1980; Lion, Azcarate, & Koepke, 1975; Zisook and

DeVaul, 1977), clorazepate (Karch, 1979), alprazolam (Rosenbaum, Woods, Groves,

& Klerman, 1984), clonazepam (Binder, 1987; Kalachnik, Hanzel, Sevenich, &

Harder, 2003), and flunitrazepam (Dåderman, Stridlund, Wiklund, Fredriksen, &

Lidberg, 2003). Such incidents occurred at both high and low doses (approximate

diazepam equivalent doses (DZM) ranging between 5-240mg), and by individuals

Page 48: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

48

with (Kalachnik et al., 2003; Lion et al., 1975) and without (Karch, 1979) histories of

aggressive behaviour. Despite this accumulating clinical evidence, there has been no

systematic review of the existing literature to inform policy or practice.

A complicating factor is the variable definitions of aggression used within the

literature. For the purposes of this report, aggressive behaviour is defined as physical

behaviour directed toward another person with the goal of harming or injuring that

person, who is motivated to avoid such behaviour (Baron & Richardson, 1994). The

term ‘violence’ is often used interchangeably with ‘aggression’, although commonly

reflects more serious harm (Anderson & Bushman, 2002) and forensic contexts

(Anderson & Bokor, 2012). Our deliberate and sole focus on physical aggression

directed toward another person is due to the potential severe medical and legal

consequences of such responses (i.e., hospitalisation and mortality; legal

responsibility, sentencing, and rehabilitation considerations).

Of note, this issue has significant implications for judicial decision-making

pertaining to violent offending behaviour. For example, a study of 102 injecting drug

users (with a range of criminal histories) reported that benzodiazepine use was

associated with unprovoked aggressive behaviour (20%), fights (13%) and crime

(14%; Smith, Miller, O’Keefe, & Fry, 2007). Furthermore, self-reported substance

use in adult police detainees (n = 1884) indicated that 27% of users attributed their

current offence to benzodiazepine use (Payne & Gaffney, 2012). Although limited

by the use of retrospective self-attributions of behaviour and the lack of control for

concurrent substance use, these findings do highlight that disentangling these issues

via a systematic and critical analysis of the literature is timely.

In terms of how benzodiazepines increase aggressive responding, Lion and

colleagues (1975) suggest an interaction between drug use, personality, and

Page 49: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

49

environmental factors, with more recent research highlighting that personality may

be as, or more, important than pharmacological factors (Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002;

Hoaken & Stewart, 2003; Paton, 2002; Wilkinson, 1985). The latter premise is

highlighted by research implicating both high (Paton, 2002) and low doses (Hoaken

& Stewart, 2003) of benzodiazepines in subsequent aggressive responding. The

circumstances under which aggressive behaviour is likely to result following

benzodiazepine consumption are poorly understood and the literature lacks a

systematic review of the empirical research investigating the relationship between

benzodiazepine consumption and subsequent aggressive or violent behaviour. This

relationship needs to be understood when considering prescribing protocols, and the

potential clinical and legal implications of aggressive responses. This chapter

therefore reports on a systematic review addressing the question: Does

benzodiazepine consumption increase aggressive behaviour in adult humans?

3.2 Methods

A systematic review was designed and reported according to the Preferred

Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement

(Liberati et al., 2009; Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altmann, & The PRISMA Group,

2009). The Maryland Scale of Scientific Rigor (Sherman et al., 1998) was applied to

assess each study’s internal validity. According to this scale, studies are ranked from

1 (weakest) to 5 (strongest), with the main considerations pertaining to control of

extraneous variables, minimisation of error variance, and sufficient power for

meaningful statistical differences. Randomised clinical trials (RCT) and well

controlled experimental studies provide the strongest level of scientific rigor

(Sherman et al., 1998). Although experimental studies can inform the impact of acute

doses, they provide limited information on chronic use, and the findings are based on

Page 50: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

50

highly contrived circumstances. Therefore, less rigorous designs (i.e., cross-

sectional, prospective) were also included in this review in order to enhance the

ecological and clinical validity of the findings. However, at the lowest level of

evidence (Merlin, Weston, & Tooher, 2009), case studies were excluded from

consideration due to their poor generalizability (Evans, 2003).

3.2.1 Eligibility criteria.

Studies investigating the relationship between benzodiazepines and

aggression in adult human populations were located. Inclusion criteria included

English language and peer-reviewed journal articles. Studies examining self-

reported, observed, and behavioural analogues of other-directed aggression, defined

as physical behaviour directed towards another person with the goal of harming or

injuring that person who is motivated to avoid such behaviour, were included.

Exclusion criteria included animal studies, unrelated neurobiological studies, case

studies, and clinical trials which did not explicitly measure aggression. Articles were

excluded if they only investigated hostility, anger, self-directed aggression (e.g., self-

harming behaviours), the intent to act aggressively, or verbal or psychological

aggression. Articles were also excluded if they investigated non-benzodiazepine

sedatives, hypnotics, or anxiolytics. Synthesis articles (i.e., reviews, meta-analyses)

were excluded following examination of their reference lists for original articles

meeting the above criteria.

3.2.2 Information sources.

The search was applied to Medline Complete, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO,

Academic Search Complete, and the Psychology and Behavioural Sciences

Collection electronic databases. Search terms used were: benzo*, BZD, sedative,

hypnotic, anxiolytic, anti-anxiety medication, anti-anxiety drug, tranquilisers,

Page 51: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

51

tranquilizers, diazepam, lorazepam, temazepam, alprazolam, flunitrazepam,

oxazepam, clonazepam, nitrazepam, aggress*, and violen*. The limiter ‘English

language’ was used for PsycINFO, Academic Search Complete, and Medline

Complete. The limiter ‘peer reviewed’ or ‘scholarly (peer reviewed) journals’ was

used for all databases except Medline as this was not available. The limiter of

‘human’ or ‘human population’ was used for PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, and

Medline. The final search was run on 7 December, 2012.

3.2.3 Study selection.

Two authors (BA and PS) independently screened the titles and abstracts of

all publications obtained by the search strategy (2492), and assessed the full text of

selected articles (64) for inclusion. In questionable cases, the authors discussed the

inclusion and exclusion requirements and came to a consensus.

3.2.4 Data extraction.

Information was extracted from each study on: (1) participant characteristics

(including gender, age, clinical diagnosis, behavioural history if reported); (2) study

design and method (including benzodiazepine type, dose, frequency; versus placebo;

corroboration of self-reported information; single- or double-blinded techniques);

and (3) type of outcome measure (including behavioural rating scale, behavioural

analogues, self-report).

3.3 Results

3.3.1 Study selection.

The initial database search revealed a total of 2492 articles. Of these, 2428

studies were discarded as duplicates or because they did not meet the criteria (see

Fig. 1). The full text of the remaining 64 citations was examined in more detail. An

additional 15 articles were identified by examining the reference lists of these

Page 52: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

52

citations and through previous literature searches. Thirty articles did not meet the

inclusion criteria as described, including 19 non-systematic reviews, and a review

investigating the behavioural side effects of benzodiazepines in individuals with an

intellectual disability (Kalachnik, Hanzel, Sevenich, & Harder, 2002). Upon detailed

study, two studies were excluded (Cherek, Steinberg, & Kelly, 1987; Cowdry &

Gardner, 1988) because they reported the same data as a further two (Cherek,

Steinberg, Kelly, Robinson, & Spiga, 1990; Gardner & Cowdry, 1985, respectively);

the latter two contained more methodological and statistical detail. One experimental

study (Brown, 1978) was excluded as it failed to report methodological or statistical

outcome detail. Hence, 46 studies met the inclusion criteria.

3.3.2 Study characteristics.

The 46 studies varied considerably in terms of study design, type of samples,

the range of benzodiazepines examined and doses considered. Due to the

heterogeneity of study design and benzodiazepine type and dose, it was not possible

to conduct a meta-analysis on the reviewed data. Design: The association between

benzodiazepine consumption and subsequent aggressive behaviour had been

explicitly investigated in two prospective studies, 25 cross-sectional studies, six

clinical studies, and 13 experimental studies. Sample: The studies investigated

clinical (n = 15), forensic (n = 12), and healthy community (n = 19) samples. Drug

Type: Benzodiazepines investigated across the studies included diazepam,

alprazolam, flunitrazepam, triazolam, temazepam, clonazepam, oxazepam,

lorazepam, and clorazepate. Diazepam and alprazolam received the most attention (9

and 6 studies, respectively), and 43.5% (n = 20) studies explored non-specific

benzodiazepine use. Dose: The included studies investigated both therapeutic and

higher doses; approximate DZM equivalent doses for the clinical and experimental

Page 53: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

53

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram depicting the flow of information through the

stages of the systematic review.

Records identified through database searches

(n = 2492)

Scre

enin

g In

clud

ed

Elig

ibili

ty

Iden

tific

atio

n Additional records identified through other sources

(previous searches n = 6; reference list examination n =

9)

Records after duplicates removed (n = 2087)

Records screened (n = 2087)

Records excluded (biological or

animal n = 583; not relevant n = 1193; victim use n = 15;

treatment of aggression n = 84;

using benzodiazepines as treatment n = 85; alcohol-related

aggression n = 6; editorial/letter to

editor n = 23; suicide/self-harm n = 11, case reports n

= 8)

Full-text articles assessed for eligibility (n = 79)

Full-text articles excluded, with

reasons (letter to editor n = 2; not relevant n = 6; children n = 2;

duplicate n = 2; lack of detail n = 1; reviews n = 20)

Studies included in qualitative synthesis

(n = 46)

Page 54: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

54

studies ranged between 3.3-100mg and 2-20mg, respectively. The experimental

studies examined acute benzodiazepine administration, whilst the clinical studies

explored chronic administration ranging from four days to three months. Cross-

sectional and prospective studies did not specify dose. Table 1 and 2 detail the

characteristics of the included studies.

3.3.3 Risk of bias within studies.

There were multiple risks of bias in each article reviewed, ranging from

limited generalizability to potential confounding by drug interactions, as detailed in

the tables. Application of the Maryland Scale of Scientific Rigor (Sherman et al.,

1998) demonstrated that overall, 56.5% (n = 26) of the included studies are of a

Level 1 standard, and only 6.5% (n = 3) are of a Level 5 standard, indicating that the

quality of the evidence base surrounding the benzodiazepine-aggression relationship

is poor.

3.4 Synthesis of Results

Does benzodiazepine consumption increase aggressive behaviour in adult

humans?

The reviewed literature demonstrated a moderate relationship (with some

inconsistency) between benzodiazepine consumption and subsequent aggression.

Diazepam demonstrated the greatest association with increased aggression (n = 6

studies), and has been examined in more high level (Levels 4 and 5; n = 3) studies

than any other benzodiazepine.

3.4.1 Experimental studies.

The 13 experimental studies utilised either the Taylor Aggression Paradigm

(TAP; n = 10) or the point subtraction aggression paradigm (PSAP; n = 3), with the

majority utilising placebo-controlled (n = 11) and double-blind (n = 9) methods. The

Page 55: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

55

Table 1

Characteristics of included studies assessing non-specific benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour, ordered by year of publication.

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

Haller &

Deluty,

1990

Cross-

sectional

100

cases

(80

clients;

49%

F)

- Severely

assaultive

psychiatric

patients

File

examination

Specificity –

“anxiolytics”

No temporal causality

Anxiolytic prescription

positively related to assault

severity 1

Dawson,

1997

Cross-

sectional

18352 18 or older Current drinkers Self-report

questionnair

e

Specificity – “sedatives”,

“tranquilisers”

Reliance on self-report

Exclusion of licit use of

substances

0.4% used sed/tranq only

Sed/tranq use not predictive of

past-year alcohol or drug-

related fighting

1

Ryden et

al., 1999

Cross-

sectional

116

(73%F

)

85.4 (7.9) Cognitively

impaired,

consistently

File

examination

Sampling bias

No control group (i.e.,

non-agg sample)

41.4% received anxiolytics

(most common L & A)

Receiving anxiolytics more

1

Page 56: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

56

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

aggressive,

nursing home

residents

Behavioural

observation

No temporal causality agg & more likely to be

restrained but n.s.

Anxiolytic &/or psychotropic

sig predicted physical agg

Shah et

al., 2000

Prospective

(6 months)

412

(64%

F)

82.3 (9.5) Melbourne

nursing home

residents

SOAS

RAGE

Measurement variability

(sensitivity)

Lack of temporal

causality

SOAS: Agg patients prescribed

greater no. of BZD

RAGE: most likely to be agg

not prescribed BZD

3

Friedman

et al.,

2003

Cross-

sectional

612

(50%F

)

26.23

(1.52)

African-

American, low

socio-economic

status, young

adults

Database

analysis

Specificity –

“seds/tranqs”

Cultural and SES

generality/confounds

Degree of sed/tranq use

predicted number of assault &

weapon possessions offences,

& total violence score for high

delinquent sub-group

No association for low-

delinquent group

1

Voyer et Cross- 2633 65 or older Patients of long- Structured Limited generality BZD not associated with 1

Page 57: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

57

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

al., 2005 sectional (75.4%

F)

term care

facilities

(Quebec)

interview

Systematic

medical file

review

Contextual factors not

assessed/controlled

Questionable data quality

– busy, time-limited staff

physical aggression (only

verbal)

Giancola

& Parrott,

2005

Experiment

(PC)

Mixed

design

330

(50%F

)

23.04

(2.85)

Healthy social

drinkers

TAP

Self-report

questionnair

e

Specificity – “sedatives”

Reliance on self-report

Not double-blind

Sedative use not related to

agg/extreme agg

Alc did not influence relation

between sedative use & agg

3

Haggård-

Grann et

al., 2006

Cross-

sectional

133 M 35.3 (12.0) Convicted

violent offenders

& offenders

undergoing

forensic

psychiatric

evaluation

Structured

interviews

Case-

crossover

design

Recall bias

No corroboration via

blood/urine tests

BZD alone at regular doses =

lower violence risk

BZD & alcohol = increased

risk but not greater than

alcohol alone

1

Whitty & Cross- 295 - Incident reports Retrospectiv Limited generality Of clients involved in physical 1

Page 58: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

58

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

O’Connor

, 2006

sectional cases

(157

clients)

for out-patient

methadone

stabilisation &

detox program

e file

examination

Small sample - only 20

clients provided urine

sample 24hrs prior to

incident

No control group

agg, 80% of those tested were

positive for BZD

Feingold

et al.,

2008

Cross-

sectional

150 M 28

(0.5)

At least 1 long-

term relationship

(1 year)

Database

analysis

Limited generality

Low statistical power

Specificity – “sedatives”

Sedative problems not

predictive of IPV 1

Stalans &

Ritchie,

2008

Cross-

sectional

19338

cases

(54.7F

)

18-50 Living with

intimate partners

No treatment for

substance use in

past year

Database

analysis

Specificity –

“sedatives/pain relievers”

Reliance on self-report

Limited measure of IPV

perpetration (1 item)

Use of sedatives independently

and sig increased likelihood of

perpetrating IPV after

controlling demographic/social

factors

1

Moore et

al., 2010

Cross-

sectional

1937

cases

- Violence report Database

analysis

Reliance on adverse

incidence database –

confounding factors,

TZ, D, A, C disproportionately

associated with violence (4/31

drugs)

1

Page 59: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

59

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

unreported events

No temporal causality

Nabors,

2010

Cross-

sectional

1635

(61%F

)

19 Undergraduates Self-report

questionnair

e

Specificity –

“depressants”

Limited generality

No temporal causality

Use of depressants 57% more

likely to perpetrate IPV

Depressant use strongest

predictor of F IPV perpetration

1

Rouve et

al., 2011

Cross-

sectional

56

cases

(48M)

46 Physical

aggressiveness

against others

Database

analysis

Case/non-

case method

Reliance on adverse

incidence database –

confounding factors,

unreported events

BZD most frequent nervous

system drugs documented

Physical agg sig assoc with A

& bromazepam

1

Hakansso

n et al.,

2011

Cross-

sectional

5659 29.7-34.8 Swedish CJS

clients

Database

analysis

Specificity –

“tranquilizers”

Limited generality

No temporal causality

Lack of sample

26% reported tranq use

Tranqs one of the predictors of

difficulty controlling violent

behaviour

1

Page 60: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

60

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

homogeneity

Recall bias – interviews

between 6mth-2years

after intake into CJS

Shin et

al., 2012

Prospective

(5-12

month)

376

(49.5%

F)

41.15

(13.33)

Help-seeking US

veterans with

PTSD

Self-report

questionnair

e

Small subsample with

BZD prescription (23%)

No temporal causality

Reliance on self-report

BZD prescription did not

significantly explain variance

between baseline & follow-up

agg

BZD associated with increased

agg among those high in

baseline agg

2

Lundholm

et al.,

2012

Cross-

sectional

194

(22F)

F:

30.68

(9.64)

M: 30.51

(10.88)

Remand

prisoners

suspected of

violent crime

Structured

interviews

Case-

crossover

Recall bias

No corroboration with

blood/urine records

High BZD dose associated

with violence

Regular BZD dose associated

with reduced risk of violence

BZD expectations: feel better,

1

Page 61: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

61

Source Design

Participant Data Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes

Q.I

. No. Mean age

(SD) Details

design reduced anxiety

Afifi et

al., 2012

Cross-

sectional

25778

(54%F

)

20 and

above

12 month

romantic

relationship

Database

analysis

Low prevalence &

specificity (‘sedatives’,

‘tranquilisers’)

Underpowered model

No temporal causality

Covariates – only select

mental dx

Sed/tranq use increased odds

of IPV perpetration than non-

users – however n.s. in fully

adjusted model

F with sed/tranq use less likely

to perpetrate IPV than M

counterparts

1

Mattson

et al.,

2012

Cross-

sectional

181 F: 39.9

(9.3)

M: 42.7

(8.9)

Married/

cohabiting

heterosexual

couples

M: substance

abuse treatment

M: alcohol

dependence

Individual

interview;

self- and

other-

reported

aggression

(highest

value used)

Gender bias in model

Specificity of “sedatives”

No temporal causality

Use of highest agg value

– overestimate agg?

No direct association between

sedative use & physical agg

F: sed use indirectly increased

minor physical agg

F: sed use indirectly predicted

decreased severe physical agg

Effects of F sed use eliminated

when agg levels combined

1

Page 62: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

62

Notes. Q.I. = quality indicator; A = alprazolam; D = diazepam; C = clonazepam; TZ = triazolam; L = lorazepam; P = participants; DB = double-blind;

PC = placebo-controlled; SOAS = Staff Observation Aggression Scale; RAGE = Rating Scale for Aggressive Behavior in the Elderly; agg = aggression;

F = female, M = male; IPV = intimate partner violence; CJS = criminal justice system; n.s. = non-significant; B/subjects = between-subjects design;

W/subjects = within subjects design.

Page 63: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

63

Table 2

Characteristics of included studies assessing benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour, ordered by benzodiazepine type and year of publication.

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

Diazepam

Feldman,

1962

Clinical

B/subjects

87

(31F)

37(-) Patients

with

hypoactive

syndrome

Diazepam

– dose

unclear

Tx approx.

3 months

Evaluator

assessment

Potential drug interactions

Limited information re

control, dose, responses,

inter-rater reliability

No comparison group,

blinding methods

Progressive development

of “hatefulness” which

led to violence 1

Wilkinso

n, 1985

Experimen

t (DB, PC)

W/subjects

60 M 18-24 Undergrad

uates

Diazepam

– 10mg

TAP

Limited generality

Ecological validity

D increased agg over

placebo (p < .01)

D-related agg enhanced

in low-anxious Ps (p <

.05)

4

Gantner

& Taylor,

Experimen

t (PC,

36

(50%

19 or

over

Undergrad

uates

Diazepam

– 10mg

TAP

Not double-blind –

expectancy bias?

D increased agg

behaviour (p <.01) 3

Page 64: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

64

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

1988 random

allocation)

B/subjects

F) 60-min between D & task

Cherek et

al.,1990

Experimen

t (DB, PC)

W/subjects

9 M - Healthy

volunteers

Diazepam

– 2, 5,

10mg/

70kg

PSAP

Successive drug doses

separated by 96hrs

Small sample

Self-selection (response bias)

Ecological validity

D decreased agg (p

<.025)

2P increased agg

responding (5mg,

10mg/70kg)

- high hostility

3

Ben-

Porath &

Taylor,

2002

Experimen

t (DB, PC,

random

allocation)

B/subjects

60 M 18-24 Undergrad

uates

Diazepam

– 10mg

TAP

Limited generality

Reliance on self-reported

hostility

Ecological validity

D increased agg

compared to placebo (p =

.05)

- influence of hostility

(n.s)

5

Wallace

& Taylor,

Experimen

t (DB, PC)

30 M 18 or

over

Undergrad

uates

Diazepam

– 10mg

TAP

Limited generality

Self-selection (response bias)

D significantly increased

agg compared to placebo 3

Page 65: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

65

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

2009 W/subjects (p <.05)

Forsyth et

al., 2011

Cross-

sectional

202

M

16-20 Young

serious

offenders

Diazepam Qualitative

interviews

Self-report

questionnaire

Reliance on self-report

Limited generality

D drug most often

blamed for crime

D more likely to facilitate

violence when with

alcohol

1

Flunitrazepam

Dåderma

n &

Lidberg,

1999

Cross-

sectional

19 M 14-20

M =

17

(1.2)

Juvenile

offenders

(serious

offences),

Fl abusers

Fl Structured

interviews

File

examination

Questionable temporal

causality

Reliance on self-report

Confounding variables?

Limited generality

Targeted sample without

comparison group

All Fl-abusers committed

violent offences

High boredom

susceptibility, verbal agg

associated with high Fl

use

No sig diff re. type or no.

of crime for abusers/non-

abusers

1

28 M 14-20

M =

17

(1.3)

Juvenile

offenders,

non-Fl

abusers

Page 66: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

66

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

Dåderma

n &

Edman,

2001

Cross-

sectional

60 M 27

(5.7)

Non-

psychotic

forensic

psychiatric

patients

Fl Structured &

open-ended

interviews

File

examination

Limited generality

Reliance on self-report

Low statistical power

No personality difference

between Fl non/abusers

Fl abusers more likely to

commit robbery, weapon,

theft crimes

No sig difference re

violent crime

1

Dåderma

n et al.,

2002

Cross-

sectional

5 M 23-26 Swedish

offenders

who had

used Fl

Fl File

examination

Confounded – concurrent

drug use

No controls (i.e., non-anxious

Fl users)

Limited generality

Small, targeted sample

Violence promoted in

individuals with

psychiatric vulnerability

(i.e., anxiety) 1

Bramness

et al.,

2006

Cross-

sectional

415

cases

(16%

30

(9.3)

DUI under

Fl only

Fl Database

analysis

Social/environment/personal

factors not available

Reliance on adverse

Fl not significantly

different between violent

& non-violent cases

1

Page 67: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

67

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

F) incidence database –

confounding factors,

unreported events

7

cases

(M)

21.9

(2.6)

Violent

crime

where Fl

only drug

Dåderma

n et al.,

2012

Cross-

sectional

114

M

22.5

(6.6)

Non-

psychotic

offenders

& juvenile

delinquents

Fl Interviews

File

examination

Small sample pure Fl users –

limited power to assess

unique characteristics

No temporal causality

34% used Fl

Fl use associated with

Facet 4 psychopathy

(poor behavioural

control, early behavioural

problems, criminal

versatility)

1

Clonazepam

Karson et

al., 1982

Clinical

(DB, PC)

W/subjects

13

(3F)

21-45 Inpatients

-chronic

schizophre

C 1mg/o.d.

(gradually

increased,

Staff

behavioural

ratings

2wk stabilisation period prior

to C – sufficient?

Drug interactions?

4 patients had agg

behaviour during study

(only 1 other-agg during

2

Page 68: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

68

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

A-B-A nia tapered)

28 days tx

Limited generality C tx, at 4mg)

Rosenfel

d et al.,

1987

Cross-

sectional

38

(18F)

13-41 Intractable

seizure

disorder

Clonazepa

m

File

examination

Drug interaction?

Variations in testing – prior to

or during C tx

Limited generality

1P displayed “marked

agg”

Sig diff in VIQ-PIQ

discrepancy compared to

non-side effect patients

(not specific to

behavioural side effect)

No personality

differences

1

Alprazolam

Gardner

&

Cowdry,

1985

Clinical

(DB, PC)

W/subjects

16 F 24-42 Outpatients

Borderline

personality

disorder

Alprazolam

– FDS (1-

6mg; M =

4.7mg o.d.)

Self-report

Behavioural

observation

Histories of dyscontrol

1wk washout – carry over

effects?

Limited generality

A increased severe

dyscontrol over placebo

1P displayed other-

directed agg

2

Page 69: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

69

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

6 weeks tx

Noyes et

al., 1988

RCT

(DB, PC,

random

allocation)

B/subjects

525

(67%

F)

M:

36.8

(11.1)

F:37.1

(10.2)

Panic

disorder,

agoraphobi

a & panic

attacks

Alprazolam

– FDS

(1-10mg)

8 weeks tx

Behavioural

observation

Self-report

1wk medication washout –

sufficient?

Limited generality

Aggressive/violent

behaviour reported by 1P

(4mg o.d.)

Dose reduction – no

recurrence of behaviour

3

Bond &

Silveira,

1993

Experimen

t (DB, PC,

random

allocation)

B/subjects

48

(50%

F)

- Moderate

social

drinkers

Alprazolam

– 1mg

(with &

without

alcohol)

Modified

TAP

Ecological validity A did not increase agg

compared to placebo

Alcohol & A = increased

agg more than the sum of

the single effects (p <.01)

4

O’Sulliva

n et al.,

1994

RCT (DB,

PC,

random

allocation)

154

(81%

F)

35 Inpatients

Panic

disorder

with

Alprazolam

– FDS

(1-10mg)

8 wks tx (8

Structured

interview

Assessor &

Limited generality

A increased agg in 2Ps

No increase in hostility

3

Page 70: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

70

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

B/subjects agoraphobi

a

wks

tapered tx-

free f/up)

self-ratings

Bond et

al., 1995

Experimen

t (PC,

random

allocation)

B/subjects

23

(17F)

A:

40.8

Placeb

o: 38.5

Patients

Panic

disorder

with

agoraphobi

a

Alprazolam

(post 8wks

treatment;

O’Sullivan

et al)

Modified

TAP

Self-report

Limited generality

Ecological validity

Not double-blind

A increased agg

following provocation

(p<.01)

Decrease in hostility 4

Triazolam

Cherek et

al., 1991

Experimen

t (DB, PC)

W/subjects

5M 25-36 Healthy

volunteers

Triazolam

– 0.125,

0.25,

0.5mg/70k

g

PSAP Self-selection (response bias)

Limited generality

Ecological validity

Unreported data for 1P

T decreased agg in 2Ps

T at 2 lower doses

increased agg in 1P

T dose-dependently

increased agg in 1P

3

Berman Experimen 46 M 18-30 Undergrad Triazolam TAP Limited generality T increased agg 5

Page 71: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

71

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

& Taylor,

1995

t (DB, PC)

B/subjects

uates

Non-

smoking

– 0.25mg Self-selection (response bias)

No examination of T dose-

response curve

Ecological validity

responding (p < .01) &

increased no. of Level 10

shocks (p <.01)

compared to placebo

Threat did not influence

T-related agg responding

Oxazepam

Burgio et

al., 1992

Clinical

(SB)

B/subjects

21

(7F)

77.8(-) Gero-

psychiatry

inpatients

Dementia,

severe

behavioural

disturbance

Oxazepam

– FDS (10-

30mg o.d.)

4-26 days

tx

Behavioural

microanalysi

s

Single-blind method –

observer bias?

No non-treatment comparison

group

Treatment effects collapsed

over O & Haloperidol groups

No significant change

between baseline agg &

agg during treatment

Physical agg least

frequent of targets

3

Temazepam

Hammers Cross- 100 16-30 Single Temazepa Semi- Limited generality T unique contributor to 1

Page 72: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

72

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

ley &

Pearl,

1997

sectional (44%

F)

Homeless m structured

interviews

No control for polydrug use

Reliance on self-report

No temporal causality

violence

Lorazepam

Pietras et

al., 2005

Experimen

t

(PC)

W/subjects

8 M 20-

37(30.

4)

Parolees Lorazepam

– 1, 2, 4mg

PSAP

Self-report

questionnaire

Self-selection (response bias)

Blinding method unclear

30mins between dose & task

– sufficient?

Not double blind

L decreased agg for 7Ps

(p<.01)

1P displayed increased

agg (by 400%) compared

to placebo

No association between

psychometric &

behavioural agg

4

Combined Benzodiazepines

Bond &

Lader,

1988

Experimen

t (DB, PC,

random

45

(20F)

19-46 Healthy

volunteers

Oxazepam

– 15, 30mg

Lorazepam

Modified

TAP

Self-selection (response bias)

Ecological validity

L & O increased agg

compared to placebo

(p < .05)

3

Page 73: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

73

Source Design

Participant Data

BZD Aggression

Measure Limitations Outcomes Q.I.

N Age

M(SD) Details

allocation)

B/subjects

– 1.0,

2.0mg

L greater increase in agg

than O (p < .05)

High L dose showed

greatest increase in agg

Weisman

et al.,

1998

Experimen

t (DB, PC)

B/subjects

44 M 18-24 Undergrad

uates

Diazepam

– 10mg

O – 50mg

CL – 15mg

TAP

Limited generality

Self-selection (response bias)

O & CL no effectg

D increased agg at low

provocation compared to

placebo (p< .05)

5

Rothschil

d et al.,

2000

Cross-

sectional

323 18-82 Psychiatric

hospital

inpatients

Alprazolam

C

No BZD

Blind chart

review

Non-randomised

Limited generality

No sig difference

between A, C, or no BZD

groups on assaults

1

Notes. Q.I. = quality indicator; RCT = randomised control trial; BZD = benzodiazepine; A = alprazolam; D = diazepam; Fl = flunitrazepam; T =

temazepam; C = clonazepam; O = oxazepam; TZ = triazolam; CL = clorazepate; L = lorazepam; P = participants; DB = double-blind; PC = placebo-

controlled; PSAP = Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm; TAP = Taylor Aggression Paradigm; SOAS = Staff Observation Aggression Scale; RAGE

= Rating Scale for Aggressive Behavior in the Elderly; FDS = flexible dose schedule; agg = aggression; F = female, M = male; CJS = criminal justice

system; AOD = alcohol and drug; n.s. = non-significant; B/subjects = between subjects design; W/subjects = within-subjects design; f/up = follow up.

Page 74: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

74

TAP involves participants administering and receiving electrical shocks under a

competitive reaction time task, where the participant’s shock setting for a (fictional)

opponent gives a proxy measure of behavioural aggression. Reaction to provocation

is also measured, by providing participants with an indication of the shock set per

trial by their opponent. Modifications involve using noise instead of shocks (e.g.,

Bond and Lader, 1988). The PSAP provides participants with a choice between

escape responding, non-aggressive monetary-reinforced responding, and a proxy

measure of aggression involving the subtraction of money from a (fictional)

opponent, following provocation. It is noted that although laboratory proxy measures

of aggression have demonstrated some external (Anderson & Bushman, 1997), and

construct validity (e.g., Cherek, Lane, Dougherty, Moeller, & White, 2000; Giancola

& Parrott, 2008; Golomb, Perez, Jaworski, Mednick, & Dimsdale, 2007), debate

continues as to the empirical value and external validity of laboratory aggression

paradigms (e.g., Ferguson & Rueda, 2009; Ferguson, Smith, Miller-Stratton, Fritz, &

Heinrich, 2008). Eleven studies investigated undergraduate students or community

members (18-46 years old), and one study each explored a clinical (28-40 years;

Bond, Curran, Bruce, O’Sullivan, & Shine, 1995) and forensic (20-37 years; Pietras

et al., 2005) sample.

Variable effects were reported both within and across benzodiazepine types.

Diazepam was found to invariably increase responding indicative of behavioural

aggression in five studies and produce mixed responses in one study; two studies

demonstrated increased responding indicative of aggression following alprazolam

consumption; lorazepam, oxazepam, and triazolam each demonstrated varied results

between two studies; and one study demonstrated clorazepate to have no effect on

responding. Study methodology may have influenced these findings, as responding

Page 75: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

75

indicative of aggression generally increased in TAP studies and decreased in PSAP

studies post-benzodiazepine consumption. Studies which utilised the original or

modified TAP demonstrated enhanced aggressive responding following consumption

of triazolam (Berman & Taylor, 1995), lorazepam, oxazepam (15mg, 30mg; Bond &

Lader,1988), alprazolam (Bond et al., 1995), combined alcohol and alprazolam

(Bond & Silveira, 1993), and diazepam (Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002; Gantner &

Taylor, 1988; Wallace & Taylor, 2009; Weisman, Berman & Taylor, 1998;

Wilkinson, 1985), but not clorazepate or oxazepam (50mg; Weisman et al., 1998),

and self-reported ‘sedative’ use in healthy adults did not influence alcohol-related

aggressive responding (Giancola & Parrott, 2005). The variation concerning

oxazepam may reflect a dose effect or an insufficient absorption period by Weisman

and colleagues (1998) (90 minutes versus 4 hours), as oxazepam has been found to

have a later onset of action (Bond & Lader, 1988). Interestingly, Wilkinson (1985)

reported that aggressive responding was enhanced in low-trait anxious participants

compared to high-trait anxious participants. Comparatively, the PSAP studies

demonstrated decreased responding indicative of aggression in the majority of

participants following lorazepam (n = 8; Pietras et al., 2005), triazolam (n = 5;

Cherek, Spiga, Roache, & Cowan, 1991), and diazepam consumption (n = 9; Cherek

et al., 1990). Interestingly, response patterns in the latter study indicated increased

aggressive responding in high hostile participants (n = 2). Personality characteristics

were measured in a further four studies (Bond et al., 1995; Bond & Lader, 1988;

Bond & Silveira, 1993; Pietras et al., 2005), however none discussed whether

individual differences were related to patterns of responding.

The most common approximate DZM equivalent doses administered acutely

were 5mg and 10mg (DZM range = 2-20mg), and each produced variable results.

Page 76: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

76

Findings of two out of five studies using 5mg (Berman & Taylor, 1995; Bond &

Lader, 1988) and five out of ten studies using 10mg (Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002;

Bond & Lader, 1988; Gantner & Taylor, 1988; Wallace & Taylor, 2009; Wilkinson,

1985) suggested increased aggression. Higher doses either failed to influence

aggressive responding (DZM = 16.7mg; Weisman et al., 1998) or resulted in

decreased responding indicative of aggression (DZM = 20mg; Pietras et al., 2005),

and only one participant displayed increased aggressive responding following the

low equivalent dose of 2.5mg (Cherek et al., 1991). Only one study considered

chronic administration, where participants engaged in the laboratory task following

eight weeks of alprazolam treatment (DZM = 10-100mg) or placebo (Bond et al.,

1995). Although the findings indicated alprazolam use increased behaviour

indicative of aggression compared to placebo the flexible dosing schedule precludes

consideration of dose effects.

Overall, the experimental studies suggest that acute administration of certain

benzodiazepines (i.e., diazepam, alprazolam) can result in an aggressive response in

adults. However, the use of analogue representations of aggression greatly limits the

ecological validity of the findings. Moreover, the studies cannot inform our

understanding of chronic benzodiazepine use, and the reliance on healthy community

samples reduces their ability to inform clinically pertinent prescribing practices.

3.4.2 Clinical studies.

The review identified six clinical studies of varying methodological rigor. In

each instance, aggression following benzodiazepine use was a secondary

consideration. The clinical populations varied widely, including individuals with

panic disorder and agoraphobia (Noyes et al., 1988; O’Sullivan et al., 1994), chronic

schizophrenia (Karson, Weinberger, Bigelow, & Wyatt, 1982), borderline personality

Page 77: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

77

disorder (BPD; Gardner & Cowdry, 1985), hypoactive syndromes (Feldman, 1962),

and psychogeriatric patients (Burgio et al., 1992). Across the combined samples,

aggressive behaviour was reported following benzodiazepine consumption in less

than one percent (0.6%) of participants.

Only two of the clinical studies reviewed were RCTs (Noyes et al., 1988;

O’Sullivan et al., 1994). Both explored acceptance and side effects of alprazolam

treatment over eight weeks in individuals with panic disorder and agoraphobia (n =

525, Noyes et al., 1988; n = 154, O’Sullivan et al., 1994). Inspection of the data

indicates that one (0.19%; Noyes et al., 1988) and two (1.3%; O’Sullivan et al.,

1994) patients demonstrated elevated physical aggression during treatment, in one

case following a DZM equivalent dose of 40mg (Noyes et al., 1988). Further

interpretation of the role of alprazolam in these events is hampered by inadequate

detail regarding dosing schedules and the length of alprazolam treatment prior to the

aggressive incidents.

Two studies utilised double-blind and placebo-controlled methods in a

within-subjects design to explore the effect of clonazepam on individuals with

chronic schizophrenia (n = 13; Karson et al., 1982) and alprazolam on behavioural

dyscontrol in females with BPD (n = 16; Gardner & Cowdry, 1985). Each of the

studies reported aggressive responding in a single patient, following either an

equivalent dose of 80mg (Karson et al., 1982) or twenty days of treatment (Gardner

& Cowdry, 1985). Less rigorous designs and poor reporting characterised the final

two studies. An unclear number of individuals with hypoactive syndromes

demonstrated aggressive responding following an unreported dose of diazepam

(Feldman, 1962), and collapsed treatment effects across oxazepam and haloperidol

suggested no changes in aggressive behaviour during treatment (4-26 days) in a

Page 78: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

78

sample of psychogeriatric patients (n = 21; Burgio et al., 1992). It is interesting, and

potentially clinically pertinent, that the latter study utilised a substantially lower dose

schedule (DZM = 3.3-10mg) than the above studies (DZM = 10-100mg) and

reported no changes in aggression. However, any oxazepam-specific results are

obscured by the authors’ decision to collapse the treatment effects.

The above findings suggest that instances of aggressive responding to

benzodiazepines are rare in the clinical populations investigated. However, poor

reporting makes consideration of control methods and dose schedules difficult.

Moreover, although the results appear to suggest that higher doses may be more

risky, it is difficult to draw conclusions regarding chronic administration of

benzodiazepines, due to the large variation in treatment periods (i.e., four days to

three months; Burgio et al., 1992; Feldman, 1962, respectively), and the potential of

other drug interactions, with medication wash-out periods ranging from one (Burgio

et al., 1992; Gardner & Cowdry, 1985; Noyes et al., 1988) to two weeks (Karson et

al., 1982; O’Sullivan et al., 1994).

3.4.3 Prospective studies.

Only two prospective studies were identified, and both suggest an association

between benzodiazepine consumption and aggressive behaviour. However, due to the

associative nature of the studies, neither provide evidence of causality.

The association between benzodiazepine prescription and aggression lacked

robustness in both papers, as the associative strength varied according to the

operationalization of aggression. In a six month study of nursing home residents, the

finding of an association between increased benzodiazepine prescription and

heightened levels of aggression depended on the observation rating scale used to

classify participants as aggressive (Shah, Chiu, Ames, Harrigan, & McKenzie, 2000).

Page 79: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

79

As residents were assessed using both scales, this variation suggests some issues with

scale validity. The second prospective study followed a random sample of help-

seeking U.S. veterans with PTSD, of whom 23% were prescribed benzodiazepines,

over 5-12 months (Shin, Rosen, Greenbaum, & Jain, 2012). The findings

demonstrated that although benzodiazepine prescription did not explain overall

variance between baseline and follow-up aggression scores, benzodiazepine

prescription was related to increased aggressive behaviour in individuals who were

aggressive at baseline. However, as the authors operationalized aggression as the

sum of four items adapted from the Conflict Tactics Scale, where only one item

referred to other-directed physical aggression, it is unclear which aspect/s of

aggressive behaviour increased between baseline and follow-up. Combined, these

studies indicate how variable measurement of aggression can affect findings, and in

turn, our understanding of the relationship between benzodiazepine use and

subsequent aggression. Tentatively, however, individuals with higher baseline

aggression levels may be more likely to experience benzodiazepine-related

aggression.

Overall, both studies indicate some level of association between

benzodiazepine prescription and aggressive behaviour. However, as neither study can

suggest temporal order, the associations may reflect increased benzodiazepine

prescriptions to manage aggressive behaviour. Moreover, both studies investigated

highly specific samples, limiting generalizability, and neither study explored

variations in benzodiazepine type or dose, and therefore fail to inform clinical

practice.

Page 80: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

80

3.4.4 Cross-sectional studies.

The cross-sectional studies explored a range of sample types, including

clinical (i.e., psychiatric, addiction, hospital; n = 7), healthy or community (n = 7),

and forensic (i.e., criminal justice involvement or incidents, forensic psychiatric; n =

11). Per sample type, a large proportion of studies suggested an association between

benzodiazepine use and aggression (63.6-71.4%).

Overall, fourteen (56%) cross-sectional studies demonstrate positive

associations between benzodiazepine consumption and aggressive behaviour (Afifi,

Henriksen, Asmundson, & Sareen, 2012; Dåderman, Fredriksson, Kristiansson,

Nilsson, & Lidberg, 2002; Dåderman, Edman, Meurling, Levander, & Kristiansson,

2012; Forsyth, Khan, & Mckinlay, 2011; Hakansson, Schlyter, & Berglund, 2011;

Haller & Deluty, 1990; Hammersley & Pearl, 1997; Lundholm, Haggård, Möller,

Hallqvist, & Thiblin, 2013; Moore Glenmullen, & Furberg, 2010; Nabors, 2010;

Rouve et al., 2011; Ryden et al., 1999; Stalans & Ritchie, 2008; Whitty & O’Connor,

2006), and three studies provide mixed findings according to gender and level of

aggression (Mattson, O’Farrell, Lofgreen, Cunningham, & Murphy, 2012),

delinquent status (Friedman, Terras, & Glassman, 2003), and unclear differences in

cognitive assessment scores (Rosenfeld, 1987). The final eight studies suggest that

benzodiazepine use is not associated with physical aggression or violence (Bramness,

Skurtveit, & Mørland, 2006; Dåderman & Edman, 2001; Dåderman & Lidberg,

1999; Dawson, 1997; Feingold, Kerr, & Capaldi, 2008; Haggård-Grann, Hallqvist,

Långström, & Möller, 2006; Rothschild, Shindul-Rothschild, Viguera, Murray, &

Brewster, 2000; Voyer et al., 2005). The latter point may relate to similar

temperament and sensation seeking scores between benzodiazepine users and non-

users (Dåderman & Edman, 2001), however other cross-sectional findings suggest

Page 81: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

81

that certain personality traits assessed through empirically supported personality

inventories (i.e., boredom susceptibility, verbal aggression, Dåderman & Lidberg,

1999; antisocial psychopathy characteristics, Dåderman et al., 2012) and file-based

indicators of psychiatric vulnerability (e.g., anxiety, impulsivity; Dåderman et al.,

2002) may influence an aggressive response to flunitrazepam.

Operationalization and measurement of benzodiazepines and aggression

varied considerably across studies. Examination of specific benzodiazepines

suggested that aggressive behaviour was associated with temazepam (Hammersley &

Pearl, 1997) and diazepam (Forsyth et al., 2011), but not alprazolam (Rothschild et

al., 2000), with mixed results for flunitrazepam (Bramness et al., 2006; Dåderman et

al., 2002; Dåderman et al., 2012; Dåderman & Edman, 2001; Dåderman & Lidberg,

1999) and clonazepam (Rosenfeld et al., 1987; Rothschild et al., 2000). However,

nine studies utilised poorly defined terms such as sedatives or tranquilizers (e.g.,

Afifi et al., 2012; Dawson, 1997; Feingold et al., 2008; Friedman et al., 2003), or

grouped benzodiazepines with other substances (e.g., opiates, GHB; Mattson et al.,

2012; Nabors, 2010; Stalans & Ritchie, 2008), reducing the specificity of results, and

attenuating the associative or predictive strength of the findings (e.g., Dawson,

1997). Furthermore, many studies failed to statistically control for concurrent

substance use (e.g., Dåderman et al., 2002; Dåderman & Lidberg, 1999; Hammersley

& Pearl, 1997). Similarly, although some studies clearly defined aggression (i.e.,

violent crime including manslaughter, assault; Dåderman et al., 2002; Dåderman et

al., 2012; Dåderman & Edman, 2001; Dåderman & Lidberg, 1999; Haggård-Grann et

al., 2006; Lundholm et al., 2013; Moore et al., 2010), only two studies utilised the

same questionnaire (Conflict Tactics Scale-Revised; Afifi et al., 2012; Mattson et al.,

2012), and five studies inferred aggression from endorsement of one (Hakansson et

Page 82: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

82

al., 2011; Hammersley & Pearl, 1997; Nabors, 2010; Stalans & Ritchie, 2008) or two

(Dawson, 1997) items, reducing the interpretability of findings. Such variable

operationalization limits the ability to compare findings and form accurate

conclusions about the benzodiazepine-aggression association.

Nonetheless, a tentative conclusion may be drawn regarding the role of dose

with reference to the findings of two similar forensic studies. Both case-crossover

designs, the studies operationalized violence as violent interpersonal crime (i.e.,

manslaughter, assault, murder), and employed structured interviews with convicted

male violent offenders undergoing forensic psychiatric evaluation (n = 133;

Haggård-Grann et al., 2006), and remand prisoners suspected of violent crime (n =

194; Lundholm et al., 2013). The studies demonstrated that benzodiazepines

consumed alone (i.e., without alcohol) and in regular doses were associated with a

reduced risk of violence (Håggard-Grann et al., 2006; Lundholm et al., 2013), but an

increased risk of violence was associated with unusually high intake of

benzodiazepines (Lundholm et al., 2013). However, these findings must be

considered in light of the following limitations. That is, they rely on ambiguous

definitions of ‘regular’ or ‘unusually high’ doses, and on uncorroborated

retrospective self-report from forensic samples.

3.5 Discussion

The literature review demonstrated a relative paucity of published papers

explicitly examining the benzodiazepine-aggression relationship given its clinical

importance. Of those papers which met inclusion criteria, the majority of findings

pertain to experimental studies of acute low doses of benzodiazepines, or cross-

sectional examination of self-report data. According to the reviewed papers,

benzodiazepine use is moderately associated with subsequent aggressive behaviour.

Page 83: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

83

Nine (69.2%) of the experimental studies reported a significant increase in behaviour

indicative of aggression. However, there are clear limitations in generalising from

simulated responses in contrived experimental settings to interpersonal aggression.

Indeed, the clinical studies reported aggressive responding in less than one percent of

participants (0.6%) during benzodiazepine treatment. However, as aggressive

responding was not a focus of these studies, many lacked detail which limited the

ability to draw conclusions to inform clinical prescribing practices. Although an

association between benzodiazepine use and aggressive behaviour was indicated in

the majority of the cross-sectional studies (n = 17) and both prospective studies, the

varied operationalization of aggression and benzodiazepines impedes consideration

of the proportion of individuals reporting benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Moreover, as the correlational nature of these studies precludes analysis of temporal

causality, their findings may reflect increased benzodiazepine use in order to manage

aggressive tendencies. The discrepancies within the literature regarding the

benzodiazepine-aggression relationship likely relate to differing methodologies,

samples, and benzodiazepines tested.

3.5.1 Sample.

The most methodologically rigorous studies (Levels 4 and 5) were conducted

primarily on non-clinical samples (71%) such as undergraduates or social drinkers,

with only two high level studies exploring a clinical or forensic sample. The lack of

high-quality evidence pertaining to these latter populations greatly hinders our ability

to understand the clinical and legal implications arising from the benzodiazepine-

aggression relationship.

Page 84: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

84

3.5.2 Benzodiazepine type.

Diazepam was the most commonly examined benzodiazepine, and was associated

with increased responding indicative of behavioural aggression in five out of six

methodologically strong experimental studies. Unfortunately, some benzodiazepines

are examined in fewer than two studies, limiting our understanding of whether all

benzodiazepines promote a risk of aggressive responding.

3.5.3 Benzodiazepine dose.

The inconsistent nature of the clinical and experimental dose-related findings

suggests that a dose effect does not adequately explain benzodiazepine-related

aggression. Aggression, or behavioural responses indicative of aggression, can result

after both acute (single dose) and chronic (continuous treatment) administration of

benzodiazepines. Examination of approximate DZM equivalent doses suggested that

therapeutic doses (i.e., 5-10mg) may be more likely to be associated with aggressive

responding when administered acutely, whereas higher doses (i.e., 40mg, 80mg) may

be more risky following chronic administration.

3.5.4 Personality.

Four cross-sectional studies explicitly examined personality, however the use

of targeted sampling (i.e., flunitrazepam using violent offenders; Dåderman et al.,

2002; Dåderman & Lidberg, 1999) and the lack of non-violent control groups

(Dåderman et al., 2012; Dåderman & Edman, 2001) precludes definitive conclusions

that personality influences flunitrazepam-related violence. Controlled laboratory

testing also demonstrated that diazepam-enhanced aggressive responding was

associated with low levels of trait anxiety (Wilkinson, 1985) and high levels of

hostility (Cherek et al., 1990). These findings suggest that trait levels of anxiety and

hostility may indicate a vulnerability to the experience of benzodiazepine-related

Page 85: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

85

aggression, however further research is needed to corroborate and clarify the role of

interpersonal vulnerabilities in this response.

3.5.5 Limitations.

Due to the inclusion criteria, only the studies which explicitly examined the

relationship between benzodiazepine consumption and subsequent aggressive

behaviour were reviewed, and therefore clinical drug trials where aggressive

behaviour was observed, but not explicitly measured or discussed in the design were

not identified. Furthermore, the review was limited to English studies and published

data, which can limit the generalizability of the data (Cole and Kando, 1993).

Operationalization of aggression in the cross-sectional studies was considerably

inconsistent, and studies often failed to control for concurrent substance use, use

control or comparison groups, or clearly report methodological or statistical

techniques. Overall, the quality of the included studies was poor, indicating a need

for higher quality studies exploring the benzodiazepine-aggression relationship. Of

concern, none of the clinical studies considered benzodiazepine-related aggression a

primary focus. Furthermore, investigation of forensic participants and individual

differences was rare, reducing our ability to draw conclusions regarding the forensic

implications of this review. Moreover, the majority of the experimental studies

examined male-only samples (Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002; Berman & Taylor, 1995;

Cherek et al., 1990; Cherek et al., 1991; Pietras et al., 2005; Wallace & Taylor, 2009;

Weisman et al., 1998; Wilkinson, 1985), limiting generalizability to females, who

have been demonstrated to use benzodiazepines for non-medical reasons at a high

rate (DCPC, 2007; Loxley, 2007; McGregor, Gately & Fleming, 2011).

Page 86: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

86

3.6 Conclusions

The benzodiazepine-aggression response is an urgent clinical issue with

serious clinical and forensic implications. Although aggression has been noted to

follow benzodiazepine use in a number of experimental and non-causational studies,

inconsistency within the literature means that the circumstances under which

aggressive behaviour is likely to follow benzodiazepine consumption remain poorly

understood. The evidence base requires high quality and systematic investigation of

the various benzodiazepines and doses. Jones and colleagues (2011) outline

recommendations to reduce benzodiazepine-related harms when prescribing;

however there is limited evidence to further inform practice policy or legal defences.

Such examination is especially pertinent with increasing evidence that amnesia is a

common consequence of benzodiazepine use (e.g., Chavant, Favrelière, Lafay-

Chebassier, Plazanet, & Pérault-Pochat. 2011; Dåderman et al., 2003; Tannenbaum,

Paquette, Hilmer, Holroyd-Leduc, & Carnahan, 2012), which, when coupled with

their afore-mentioned disinhibitory properties, may lead to unprecedented use of

benzodiazepine-related legal defences (i.e., impaired responsibility, consideration of

mitigating circumstances) in cases involving interpersonal violence. Advances in the

literature are pertinent at a time when Australia has recently re-scheduled alprazolam

to a controlled drug and considers similarly up-scheduling other benzodiazepines to

Schedule 8.

3.7 Chapter Summary

A systematic literature review was conducted to explore the benzodiazepine-

aggression relationship. Results of this review indicated that the related literature

base is flawed and has poor explanatory power. Alprazolam and diazepam are the

most commonly researched benzodiazepines, presumably due to their preferential

Page 87: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

87

use within community, clinical and forensic populations, and as such have been most

closely associated with subsequent episodes of aggressive behaviour. One area which

has been understudied is the role of intrapersonal factors in benzodiazepine-related

aggression, despite arguments that such factors are highly important in understanding

this response (Lion et al., 1975; Hoaken & Stewart, 2003). The following two

original studies were designed in order to further our understanding of the role of

intrapersonal factors such as personality, motivational traits, and mental health

functioning in this response.

Page 88: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

88

Chapter Four: Motivational drive and alprazolam misuse: A recipe for

aggression?

Please note, the following chapter presents an expanded version of an original

manuscript submitted for review to Psychiatry Research (May, 2015), due to

copyright reasons. Details of the submitted article have been appended for your

consideration (Appendix C).

4.1 Introduction

Benzodiazepines are commonly used to manage anxiety or agitated behaviour

(Ashton, 2002). However, for an estimated 1-20% of users, benzodiazepine use is

followed by an aggressive response (Lader, 2011). The somewhat paradoxical nature

of this response, coupled with the high medical, financial and personal costs

associated with aggressive behaviour, suggests that changes to prescribing policies

and regulatory strategies may be required to reduce the likelihood of benzodiazepine-

related aggression from occurring. However, surprisingly little attention has been

paid to understanding the psychological processes associated with benzodiazepine-

related aggression. Controlled laboratory studies have demonstrated that alprazolam

and diazepam often result in an increased aggressive response in some participants

(e.g., Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002; Bond et al., 1995; Bond & Silveira, 1993; Wallace

& Taylor, 2009), and animal studies have alluded to the possible influence of

concurrent alcohol use (de Almeida, Saft, Rosa, & Miczek, 2010) and pre-existing

aggressive tendencies (Ferrari, Parmigiani, Rodgers, & Palanza, 1997; Weerts,

Miller, Hood, & Miczek, 2010) in benzodiazepine-related aggression. Yet, few

human studies have examined potential contributory factors (i.e., dose, other

substance use, psychological or intrapersonal factors, situation; see Albrecht et al.,

2014, for systematic review). Of note, irrespective of a long-standing proposal that

Page 89: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

89

intrapersonal factors are important in understanding this response (Hoaken &

Stewart, 2003; Lion et al., 1975), only a handful of studies have investigated the role

of various personality characteristics in benzodiazepine-related aggression (i.e., trait

anxiety, hostility; Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002; Cherek et al., 1990; Dåderman et al.,

2002; Wilkinson, 1985). The absence of a clear theoretical framework with which to

explore benzodiazepine-related aggression impacts our ability to develop

meaningful, and testable, hypotheses and intervention strategies. We argue that

current models of approach and avoidance motivational tendencies may be able to

inform our understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Motivational systems are theorised to underlie a number of human

behaviours, including violent and aggressive behaviour. Gray’s (1982)

Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory and its’ recent revision (rRST; Gray &

McNaughton, 2003) purports to explain behavioural output and emotional expression

based on three separate but interacting motivational systems. The behavioural

approach system (BAS) promotes movement towards incentives and rewards, often

involving goal-directed behaviour and impulsive action. The fight-flight-freeze

system (FFFS) promotes fearful avoidance of a threat, and over-activation clinically

presents as phobia or panic (Corr & Perkins, 2006). The behavioural inhibition

system (BIS) promotes risk assessment and conflict resolution (Corr, 2008), and is

stimulated by simultaneous and similar activation of the other two systems

(Pickering & Corr, 2008). As demonstrated by prior research, the independent and

interactive effects of these motivational systems have informed our understanding of

aggressive behaviour. It is therefore expected that the application of this theory to

benzodiazepine-related aggression will provide meaningful insight into the response,

on which intervention strategies can be based.

Page 90: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

90

Aggression appears to involve a strong approach motivational component

(i.e., BAS mediated action such as antagonism; Smits & Kuppens, 2005). That is,

aggression may result due to a strong motivation towards desired goals or rewards.

Indeed, studies with university students have reported that high levels of BAS are

associated with anger and aggressive behaviour (Harmon-Jones, 2003; Harmon-

Jones & Peterson, 2008). However, theoretical understanding of approach motivation

(BAS) suggests that it involves multiple aspects, including behavioural restraint,

planning and goal-directed behaviour (Segarra, Poy, López, & Moltó, 2014), and the

use of a broad, unidimensional measure of BAS in the above studies fails to account

for such complexity. Instead, greater specificity is afforded through the use of a

multidimensional measure of approach motivation. The BIS/BAS scales (Carver &

White, 1994) were designed to account for the dynamic and multifaceted nature of

the BAS. The division of BAS into three subscales (fun seeking, drive, and reward

responsiveness) has allowed for discrete exploration of how these motivational

tendencies differentially relate to behavioural outcomes, including the expression of

anger and aggression.

Empirical evidence suggests that Drive is the most important facet in our

understanding of aggressive behaviour. Drive (BAS-Dr) involves persistent goal

pursuit and functional impulsivity; whilst fun seeking involves dysfunctional

impulsivity, with minimal thought to consequences; and reward responsiveness

involves positive energy and affect in response to reward cues (Tull, Gratz, Latzman,

Kimbre, & Lejuez, 2010). BAS-Dr has been positively associated with the

experience of anger (Cooper, Gomez, & Buck, 2008; Harmon-Jones, 2003; Smits &

Kuppens, 2005), anger arousal, displaced aggression, the tendency to not suppress

angry feelings or prevent the expression of anger (Cooper et al., 2008), self-reported

Page 91: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

91

physical aggression (Harmon-Jones, 2003), relational aggression (Miller, Zeichner,

& Wilson, 2012), and laboratory proxies of aggressive behaviour (Seibert, Miller,

Pryor, Reidy, & Zeichner, 2010). In addition, Beaver, Lawrence, Passamonti, and

Calder (2008) identified that with increasing BAS-Dr, neural structures and

dopaminergic pathways are activated in a similar pattern to that observed in relation

to both reward processing and aggression, providing some explanation as to why

BAS-Dr is so important in understanding aggression. Further conceptual

understanding of the link between BAS-Dr and aggressive behaviour is afforded

through the concept of frustrative non-reward, which is experienced when the

expected reward is higher than the actual reward (Corr, 2002). Indeed, although

predominantly associated with the experience of positive affect (i.e., through goal

attainment), BAS-Dr has also been associated with the experience of negative affect,

especially sadness, frustration and anger, experienced in the context of blocked or

challenged reward attainment (Carver, 2004). Such scenarios may influence an

aggressive response. Following such theorising, aggression may be especially likely

if the individual also experiences sensitivity to cues of punishment or threat (i.e.,

avoidance motivational tendencies). Essentially, aggressive behaviour may involve a

facilitative interplay between appetitive and aversive motivational systems, where

aggression is more likely when an individual with high BAS-Dr also experiences

strong avoidance tendencies.

Such an interaction may be especially important in the understanding of

benzodiazepine-related aggression. Evidence suggests that benzodiazepines

selectively interfere with the conflict resolution system (BIS) by making approach

behaviour more likely (Pickering & Corr, 2008). Such movement away from risk

averse behaviour, coupled with a strong BAS-Dr, may increase the likelihood an

Page 92: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

92

individual engages in aggressive behaviour following benzodiazepine use. In

addition, BIS has been associated with reactive aggression (Miller et al., 2012),

potentially reflecting the frustration response, and at extremely high levels of BIS

activation, the related emotional disturbance may increase aggression risk (Hatfield

& Dula, 2014). As yet no studies have attempted to explore the role of these

motivational tendencies, or their interactions, in substance-related aggression,

particularly benzodiazepine-related aggression. Such investigation of the rRST

motivational systems and their interactive effects should provide further insight into

this relationship.

4.2 The Current Study

The current study aims to test the premise that motivational factors are

important in understanding benzodiazepine-related aggression, through the

application of the rRST to this response. Due to their prevalence, the current study

explicitly focuses on diazepam and alprazolam in relation to both general aggressive

behaviour (i.e., anger, hostility, verbal aggression) and specifically physical

aggression. In order to ascertain a holistic picture of the participants, data regarding

their substance use, mental health, and criminal history, and recent psychological

functioning are also gathered. It is predicted that:

1. Motivational factors will significantly predict engagement in benzodiazepine-

related aggressive behaviour over and above benzodiazepine type or use (e.g.,

Hoaken & Stewart, 2003; Lion et al., 1975).

2. BAS-Dr, or the tendency to persistently pursue appetitive goals, will significantly

predict general aggression and physical aggression.

Page 93: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

93

3. BAS-Dr will moderate the relationship between BIS and aggressive outcomes.

Specifically, it is predicted that the relationship between BIS and aggressive

behaviour will be stronger for individuals with high levels of BAS-Dr.

The current study is the first to explore the role of BAS-Dr in substance-

related aggression, particularly benzodiazepine-related aggression. It is hoped that

this study will provide a deeper, theoretically driven, conceptualisation of this

response than that currently afforded by the literature base.

4.3 Method

4.3.1 Design and procedure.

Participants were recruited via a purposeful sampling method, which utilised

an online electronic questionnaire and paper based questionnaires located at health

services. Online participants were recruited through electronic, newspaper, and paper

flyer advertising, and health clinic participants were recruited via flyers posted at two

health clinics, or through discussion with their treating clinician. Each recruitment

method included a brief summary of the research, the plain language statement

(PLS), and questionnaire. The PLS detailed the expected use of the data, and

participants’ freedom to end the questionnaire at any point, but the inability of

researchers to remove their data once submitted due to the anonymity of the

responses. Consent was implied by completion of the questionnaire. After

completing the questionnaire, participants were invited to enter a draw for one of six

shopping vouchers and to provide feedback on forms which were kept separate from

their responses. At the completion of data collection, all draw entries were collated

and the winning participants were notified and sent the vouchers.

Inclusion criteria were age of 18 years or older and use of benzodiazepines in

the past 12 months. There were no additional exclusion criteria. The study was

Page 94: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

94

approved by the Deakin University Human Ethics Advisory Group (HEAG-H

123_2012) and the Eastern Health Human Research Ethics Committee (E42-1213).

4.3.2 Materials.

Screening item. An item assessed whether participants had used

benzodiazepines in the previous 12 months. Those who responded ‘no’ were not

eligible for participation.

Demographics. A brief self-report questionnaire assessed participant age,

gender, country of origin, student status and education, employment and occupation,

whether they were currently taking prescribed medication, treatment history for drug,

alcohol, and mental health issues, and whether they had been charged or convicted

with an alcohol or drug offence or with physical crimes against another person.

Participants were instructed to select the most appropriate answer or provide brief

written responses.

Substance and benzodiazepine use. A modified version of the Alcohol,

Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST), an eight-item

questionnaire designed to detect psychoactive substance use and related problems

(WHO ASSIST Working Group [WHO], 2002), was used to assess lifetime

substance involvement and benzodiazepine-related dependency and harms. The

ASSIST usually covers frequency of use and associated problems for 10 substances

(tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, amphetamines type stimulants, inhalants,

sedatives, hallucinogens, opioids, and ‘other drugs’), though the majority of the items

were altered to refer only to the benzodiazepine the respondent selected as their

preferred type for non-medically prescribed use (see below). Specific substance

involvement scores (SSI) were therefore calculated in relation to the preferred

benzodiazepine. Moderate-high to high test-retest coefficients have been established

Page 95: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

95

over one to three days (WHO, 2002), and both internal consistency (α = 0.77-.94)

and convergent and concurrent validity has been established (Humeniuk et al., 2008).

A 15-item self-report questionnaire was constructed to examine prescribed

and non-medically prescribed benzodiazepine use. The literature utilises various

definitions to distinguish benzodiazepine use for medical reasons from abuse, and

confusing terms (e.g., medical versus non-medical) may result in inaccurate

responding (e.g., self-medication, even at a high rate, may be interpreted as medical

use). Therefore, the definition utilised in research needs to clearly differentiate

between what constitutes appropriate consumption of benzodiazepines and abuse, or

over-consumption. In the current study, medically prescribed use is when an

individual takes benzodiazepines in the amount and frequency prescribed by their

doctor. Non-medically prescribed use (NMP) is when benzodiazepines are not

prescribed by their doctor or are taken more frequently or at higher doses than their

doctor has prescribed. NMP benzodiazepine use can include to feel better, get high,

have fun, or to substitute a usual drug of choice. By separating medically-prescribed

and NMP use, we were able to isolate data regarding the misuse of benzodiazepines,

and specifically explore aggressive responses following such use. We focussed on

NMP use in order to further our understanding of the sequelae of benzodiazepine use

which may not be readily observed or monitored by prescribers or other health

professionals (e.g., due to the frequent diversion of benzodiazepines onto the black

market; Best et al., 2013), and therefore only these participants completed the

aggression measure. Additional items regarding benzodiazepine and substance use

patterns were developed through consultation with academics and clinicians within

the alcohol and other drugs field, specifically regarding item wording, relevance and

Page 96: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

96

exhaustiveness. Respondents were instructed to select the most appropriate answer/s

or provide brief written responses.

BIS/BAS. The Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural Activation

System Scales (BIS/BAS; Carver & White, 1994) is a 24-item self-report

questionnaire designed to assess Gray’s (1982) reward and punishment sensitivities.

A five factor model was used to reflect the revised theory; the three BAS factors,

BIS, and FFFS (Heym, Ferguson, & Lawrence, 2008). Items are rated on a 4-point

Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 4 = strongly disagree) with four filler items (i.e., not

included in the analysis). Moderate test-retest coefficients have been demonstrated

over an eight week period (Carver & White, 1994), and each subscale has

demonstrated moderate to high internal reliability (α = .54-82), although BIS-FFFS

demonstrates low to high coefficients between various populations (α = 0.17-0.73;

Dissabandara et al., 2012; Heym et al., 2008). Convergent and discriminant validity

has also been established (Carver & White, 1994; Heym et al., 2008). In the current

study, moderate to high internal consistency was demonstrated per subscale (α = .67

- .82), and the revised five-factor model was confirmed (see Appendix B).

Mood state. The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales (DASS-21;

Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item self-report questionnaire assessing state

levels of depression, anxiety, and stress, with seven items per factor. Participants rate

the degree to which each statement applied to them over the past week on a 4-point

Likert scale (0 = did not apply to me at all, 3 = applied to me very much). Scale

scores range from 0 to 21, and Australian norms are available (Crawford, Cayley,

Lovibond, Wilson, & Hartley, 2011). High internal consistency has been

demonstrated for each subscale and the total score (α = 0.79-.94; Crawford et al.,

2011; Henry & Crawford, 2005; Osman, Wong, Bagge, Freedenthal, Gutierrez, &

Page 97: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

97

Lozano, 2012; Sinclair, Siefert, Slavin-Mulford, Stein, Renna, & Blais, 2012).

Reliability coefficients were also high for each subscale in the current study (α =

.853-.928). Convergent (Henry & Crawford, 2005; Osman et al., 2012) and

concurrent validity has been established (Antony, Bieling, Cox, Enns, & Swinson,

1995).

Aggression. The Aggression Questionnaire (AQ; Buss & Perry, 1992) is a

29-item self-report questionnaire which measures various components of aggression;

physical aggression (9 items), verbal aggression (5 items), anger (7 items), and

hostility (8 items). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = extremely

uncharacteristic of me, 5 = extremely characteristic of me). A total aggression score

can be calculated by summing all subscale scores. The AQ has demonstrated

moderately high to high internal consistency (α = .70-.92; Buss & Perry, 1992;

Harris, 1997; O’Connor, Archer, & Wu, 2001), and temporal stability over nine

weeks (Buss & Perry, 1992) and seven months (Harris, 1997). Moreover, convergent

(Harris, 1997; McMurran, 2009; O’Connor et al., 2001) and concurrent validity have

been established (García-León, Reyes, Vila, Pérez, Robles, & Ramos, 2002). The

instructions were altered to prompt participants to respond to the questionnaire in

relation to the last time they had used NMP benzodiazepines as defined above, on an

occasion when they were not consuming other drugs or alcohol. Internal consistency

for the current study was high for the total score (α = .946) and across all factors (α =

.838-.880).

4.3.3 Statistical analyses.

The data were cleaned and analysed using SPSS PASW Statistics 18. Where

possible, parametric analyses were conducted, and means, standard deviations, and

95% confidence intervals (CI) are reported. Non-parametric chi-square tests of

Page 98: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

98

independence are used where assumptions are violated and with categorical

variables. Hierarchical multiple regressions were used to investigate the primary

research questions. The dependent variables pertaining to aggressive behaviour

reflect the AQ total score (i.e., a measure of general aggression) or the physical

aggression factor score (i.e., a measure of physical aggression specifically). A total

of 297 participants were recruited, though eight cases were removed due to the

absence of benzodiazepine use detail, and 85 cases reported never using

benzodiazepines for NMP reasons and therefore did not complete the AQ. The final

sample size was therefore 204 participants. Preliminary screening of the data

indicated a number of random item responses across the standardised questionnaires

were missing (0.34%), and each item’s sample median was imputed to fill this

missing data. In order to retain the originality of the data, due to its uniqueness in the

literature base, violations of normality were either dealt with by pulling in extreme

outliers, or by recoding into categorical variables. The variables used to compose the

interaction term (BAS-Dr, BIS-Anx) were centered prior to forming the interaction

term, in order to reduce multicollinearity, and categorical variables included in the

regression analyses were standardized using dummy-coding.

4.3.3.1 Model specification and invariance testing. Model specification was

purely conceptual, and based on understandings of the aggression and rRST

literature. Therefore, age, gender, previous drug and alcohol use, and prior violent

convictions were statistically controlled. However, due to the use of two recruitment

methods, it was necessary to determine whether the data could be pooled without

deleterious effects on the main analyses. It was determined through bivariate

analyses that the internet and health centre recruitment subsamples differed on a

number of demographic variables, including age (t(286) = -8.041, p = .000, 95% CI:

Page 99: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

99

-13.19 to -8.01, Cohen’s d = -1.21), education (χ2(2) = 39.131, p = .000, Cramer’s V

= .37), employment status (χ2(1) = 26.885, p = .000, Phi = .32), previous violent

(χ2(1) = 17.789, p = .000, Phi = -.27) and AOD convictions (χ2(1) = 26.048, p = .000,

Phi = -.31), and prior engagement in alcohol (χ2(1) = 112.281, p = .000, Phi = -.65)

or drug treatment (χ2(1) = 50.662, p = .000, Phi = -44), but not gender (χ2(1) = .000,

p = .989, Phi = .01) or mental health treatment history (χ2(1) = 2.635, p = .105, Phi =

-.11). Therefore, invariance analyses using the Chow test (Demaris, 2004) were

conducted, at the p < .05 standard. Based on the outcome of these tests, the

prediction of both general aggression (∆χ2 (15) = 9310.373, p < .05) and physical

aggression (∆χ2 (15) = 1368.56, p < .05) varied according to whether the data was

pooled or separated into recruitment samples. However, due to the size of the health

centre subsample relevant for the aggression analyses (i.e., NMP use of

benzodiazepines with AQ scores; n = 30), it is likely that the lack of power in the

health centre subsample may have artificially led to a significant difference from the

pooled sample model. The planned model on such a small subsample would likely

inflate the chance of Type 2 error, due to the lack of statistical power available. It

was therefore decided to pool the data and include a sample recruitment variable in

the analyses.

4.4 Results

4.4.1 Participant characteristics.

The final sample consisted of 204 adult community members (62.7% male)

who regularly use benzodiazepines, recruited via the internet (n = 174; 63.2% male)

and health services (n = 30; 60.0% male), aged between 18 and 51 years old (M =

27.12, SD = 8.21). Participant demographic characteristics are displayed in Table 1.

Page 100: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

100

The sample reported moderate to high scores across the BIS/BAS scales, and

moderate levels of psychological distress (see Table 2). The sample reported high

rates of poly-substance use, with the two most commonly used substances in the

month prior to reporting (other than tobacco) being alcohol and cannabis (see Table

1). Less than a third of the sample admitted previously injecting an illicit substance

(n = 61; 29.9%), though almost half of these participants had done so in the last three

months (n = 27; 44.26%). In the three months prior to survey completion, half

(54.4%) of the sample reported drinking alcohol on at least a weekly basis, whilst

82.4% reported using illicit drugs on at least a weekly basis. Of these, the majority

reported using one or two types of illicit drugs per week (77.4%), with 17.9% using

three types, and only 4.8% (n = 8) using 4 or more types of illicit drugs per week.

Table 1

Participant demographic characteristics.

Characteristic N %

Gender Male 128 62.7

Female 76 37.3

Country of Origin

Australia 74 36.3

New Zealand 5 2.5

Asia 6 2.9

Europe/UK 21 10.3

USA/Canada 96 47.1

Other 2 1.0

Student Yes 89 43.6

Page 101: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

101

No 112 54.9

Education

Before year 12 31 15.2

Year 12 79 38.7

University/TAFE 94 46.1

Employed No 100 49.0

Yes 100 49.0

Ever Current

Treatment

Drug 86 (42.2) 42 (20.6)

Alcohol 31 (15.2) 23 (11.3)

Mental Health 140 (68.6) 83 (40.7)

Ever charged Ever convict

Criminal History AOD 66 (32.4) 39 (19.1)

Violence 16 (7.8) 10 (4.9)

Lifetime Month prior

Substance Use

Tobacco 183 (89.7) 124 (60.8)

Alcohol 194 (95.1) 156 (76.5)

Cannabis 189 (92.6) 121 (59.3)

Cocaine 122 (59.8) 27 (13.2)

Amphetamines 168 (82.4) 76 (37.3)

Inhalants 67 (32.8) 15 (7.4)

Hallucinogens 143 (70.1) 41 (20.1)

Opioids 148 (72.5) 85 (41.7)

Note. Unless where specified, sample percentages are enclosed in brackets.

Note. AOD = Alcohol and other drugs; convict = convicted.

Page 102: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

102

Benzodiazepine profile. On average, participants began using NMP benzodiazepines

at 20.46 years old (SD = 6.17). NMP benzodiazepines were most frequently acquired

through friends (53.4%) or the black market (34.3%), and only 13.7% reported to

have engaged in doctor shopping. Most participants (72.5%) reported using

benzodiazepines with other substances; especially alcohol (17.6%), cannabis

(11.3%), or both (8.3%). As expected, diazepam and alprazolam were most likely to

be used for NMP reasons (52.9% and 54.3% respectively; see Table 3), and were

explicitly preferred for NMP use by 23.5% and 39.7% of participants, respectively.

ASSIST scores generally reflected a moderate risk of dependence (see Table 2).

Although used relatively infrequently over the year prior to survey completion (Table

3), participants consumed diazepam and alprazolam at high average doses (see Table

4). Alprazolam was used at the highest average doses, and at a level considerably

higher than recognised prescribing guidelines1 (i.e., up to 33mgs; ‘Alprazolam’,

2013). Diazepam and alprazolam were mostly used to reduce anxiety and tension

(29.8%, 30.0% respectively) or to get high (14.5%, 24.6% respectively), as well as to

reduce withdrawal from other substances (11.3%) for diazepam and to assist sleep

(8.5%) for alprazolam.

4.4.2 Main analyses.

Bivariate correlations between the variables of interest demonstrated that both

general and physical aggression scores were significantly associated with higher risk

of alprazolam and diazepam dependence, psychological distress (DASS), BAS-

Drive, and having a violent conviction (see Table 5). Physical aggression was also

positively associated with having a substance-related conviction. Increased

alprazolam doses were significantly associated, albeit at a low strength, with weaker

1 Prescribing guidelines suggest a daily range of 0.5-4.0mg per day (‘Alprazolam’, 2013).

Page 103: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

103

Table 2

Standardised questionnaire score ranges, means and standard deviations.

Tool Total

Range M (n) SD (%)

ASSIST

SSI diazepam (n = 45) 2-36 12.76 10.20

Low risk 0-3 (9) (20.0)

Moderate risk 4-26 (30) (66.7)

High risk 27+ (6) (13.3)

SSI alprazolam (n =

77)

0-39 15.74 11.22

Low risk 0-3 (8) (10.4)

Moderate risk 4-26 (52) (67.5)

High risk 27+ (17) (22.1)

DASS-21

Depression 0-21 10.12 6.44

Anxiety 0-21 7.78 5.57

Stress 0-21 10.01 5.38

BIS/BAS

BAS-Dr 5-16 10.66 2.47

BAS-FS 6-16 12.11 2.37

BAS-RR 11-20 15.99 2.11

BIS-Anx 6-16 13.09 2.41

BIS-Fear 4-12 9.21 2.08

AQ Total 29-137 69.99 23.90

Physical 9-43 19.06 7.91

Note. ASSIST = Alcohol, Smoking and Substance Involvement Screening Test

(WHO, 2002); AQ = Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992); BIS/BAS =

Behavioural Inhibition System and Behavioural Activation System Scales (Carver &

White, 1994); BAS-Dr = Drive, BAS-RR = Reward Responsiveness; BAS-FS =Fun

Seeking; BIS-Anx = Anxiety; DASS-21 = Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales

(Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995); SSI = Specific Substance Involvement score.

BIS-related anxiety and fear, a history of a substance-related conviction, and a

tendency to use multiple other substances when consuming benzodiazepines. Higher

Page 104: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

104

diazepam doses were associated with having violent and substance-related

convictions. Increased risk of dependence to alprazolam and diazepam was

associated with increased psychological distress, though alprazolam risk was not

associated with DASS-anxiety.

Table 3

Lifetime NMP use per benzodiazepine, frequency of alprazolam and diazepam use in

past year.

Benzodiazepine Ever NMP Frequency of use

n (%) Alprazolama Diazepama

Alprazolam 157 (54.3) Never 33 (20.2) 36 (22.0)

Temazepam 61 (21.2) Once/ twice 49 (30.1) 61 (37.2)

Oxazepam 39 (13.5) Monthly 30 (18.4) 28 (17.1)

Lorazepam 71 (24.6) Weekly 32 (19.6) 14 (8.5)

Diazepam 153 (52.9) Daily 19 (11.7) 25 (15.2)

Clonazepam 70 (24.2)

Flunitrazepam 27 (9.3)

a Percentages reflect those reporting ever using selected benzodiazepine for NMP

reasons.

4.4.2.1 Predicting benzodiazepine-related aggression. Two hierarchical

multiple regressions were conducted, to explore whether BIS/BAS variables could

predict benzodiazepine-related aggression over and above control and

benzodiazepine variables. In both models, control variables were entered at Step 1,

diazepam and alprazolam use at Step 2, BIS/BAS main effects at Step 3, and the

interaction term at Step 4. Preliminary analyses were conducted to explore the

Page 105: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

105

assumptions of normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homscedasticity. Two

multivariate outliers were identified via Mahalonobis’ distance, and case summaries

indicated these cases placed substantial leverage on both aggression models (Field,

2009). These cases were therefore removed from the following analyses. For general

aggression, casewise diagnostics demonstrated that only 1.4% of cases had

standardised residuals of + 2.0, and only 0.9% of + 2.5, with none + 3.0 (Field,

2009). For physical aggression only one case (.05%) had a standardised residual of +

2.5 (Field, 2009). Examination of this specific case indicated that it was not having

undue influence on the model and it was retained in the analysis.

Table 4

Typical and maximum daily alprazolam and diazepam doses, with approximate

diazepam equivalent doses (DZM).

BZD

Typical Maximum

n Range M (SD) DZMa

(mgs) n Range M (SD)

DZM

(mgs)

Alpraz 119 0-15.0 3.88 (4.29) 38.8 118 0-33.0 8.12 (8.71) 81.2

Diaze 111 0-61.0 16.96

(15.71) 16.96 110 1-161.0

39.81

(41.97) 39.81

Note. BZD = benzodiazepine; Alpraz = alprazolam; Diaze = diazepam; DZM =

approximate diazepam equivalent dose; mgs = milligrams. a Approximate DZM computed using a 1:10 ratio for alprazolam, as suggested by

dosing conversion table outlined by Farinde (2014).

.

Page 106: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

106

Table 5

Bivariate correlations between factors of interest.

A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U

A -

B .21^

C .25^ .01

D .13 -.04 .91^

E .02 -.07 .35^ .40^

F -.04 -.07 .29* .37^ .83^

G .08 .01 .08 .10 .08 .05

H .04 .09 -.12 -.13 .16 .09 .58^

I .11 .11 -.03 -.05 .15 .09 .66^ .80^

J .06 .05 .08 .11 .12 .11 .03 .24^ .23^

K .00 -.06 .08 .18 .04 .11 .08 .07 .03 .48^

Page 107: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

107

A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U

L .00 .07 .05 .07 .03 -.02 -.16* .00 .03 .40^ .38^

M .04 .13 -.25^ -.28^ -.06 -.14 .24^ .29^ .36^ -.17^ -.19* .11

N -.01 .08 -.21* -.25^ .05 -.12 .30^ .30^ .36^ -.24^ -.32^ -.00 .54^

O .04 -.03 .09 .08 .14 .17 .33^ .33^ .46^ .21^ -.01 .05 .08 .08

P .04 -.06 .10 .10 .14 .16 .22^ .22^ .34^ .22^ .03 .05 -.04 -.07 .87^

Q .10 -.03 .07 .14 .16 .40^ .04 .12 .15* .28^ .18^ .08 -.05 -.23^ .17* .21^

R .25^ -.06 .24^ .28^ .19 .30^ .05 .11 .04 .13 .14* .04 -.11 -.15* .05 .14* .35^

S -.04 -.07 .25^ .25^ .15 .03 .10 .15* .14* .22^ .25^ .14* -.02 -.05 .13 .12 .14* .05

T .24* -.17 .38^ .41^ .50^ .50^ .27* .17 .31^ .16 .04 -.01 .02 -.12 .33^ .28* .21 .30^ .24*

U .33* .28 -.18 -.22 .39* .46^ .30* .43^ .54^ .28 -.05 .05 .25 .07 .53^ .48^ .26 .13 .11 a

V -.07 -.02 .18 .13 .02 -.06 .15 .05 .10 .12 .21^ .02 -.15 -.10 .14 .14 .03 .13 .22^ .23 -.14

Note. A = age; B = gender; C = alprazolam typical dose; D = alprazolam maximum dose; E = diazepam typical dose; F = diazepam maximum dose; G = DASS Depression; H = DASS Anxiety; I = DASS Stress; J = BAS-Drive; K = FAS-Fun Seeking; L = BAS-Reward Responsiveness; M = BIS-Anxiety; N = BIS-Fear; O = general aggression; P = physical aggression; Q = violent conviction; R = substance-related conviction; S = use of other substances

Page 108: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

108

when taking non-medically prescribed benzodiazepines; T = ASSIST score for those who prefer alprazolam; U = ASSIST score for those who prefer diazepam; V = number of substances regularly (at least weekly) used. a correlation unable to be computed as discrete subsamples based on preferential benzodiazepine used for non-medically prescribed reasons. * p < .01 ^ p < .001

Page 109: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

109

General aggression. Inclusion of the control variables at Step 1 explained

5.5% of the variance in general aggression (Fchange (6, 195) = 1.903, p = .082). Entry

of the benzodiazepine variables at Step 2 significantly improved the model,

explaining an additional 3.7% of the variance; Fchange (2, 193) = 3.910, p = .022.

Entry of the BIS/BAS main effects at Step 3 again significantly improved the model,

explaining an additional 6.7% of the variance; Fchange (5, 188) = 3.009, p = .012. The

inclusion of the interaction term at Step 4 did not significantly improve the model

(∆R2 = .008); Fchange (1, 187) = 1.769, p = .185. However, the final model, with all

the variables in the equation, was significant and accounted for 16.7% of the total

variance in general aggression; F(14, 187) = 2.682, p = .001. Alprazolam and

diazepam use, and BAS-Dr significantly attributed unique variance to general

aggression, whilst recruitment group and BAS-FS approached significance (see

Table 6). Inspection of the data indicates that BAS-Dr and alprazolam use made the

strongest unique contributions to general aggression. Combined, BAS-Dr (5.29%)

and alprazolam use (4.04%) contributed just under 10.0% towards the explanation of

variance in general aggression, as calculated from the part correlation coefficients

(Pallant, 2007).

It is notable that prior to the inclusion of the recruitment variable in the first

step, the pooled data model indicated a significant effect of the interaction term (p =

.044), accounting for 1.8% unique variance. Upon entering the recruitment variable

the influence of the interaction term fell just outside significance (p = .075), before

moving further from significance in the final model (i.e., following final case

removal). This suggests that an extremely weak, but potentially clinically meaningful

moderation of BAS-Dr on the BIS-Anx and general aggression relationship may

exist, and may benefit from further research.

Page 110: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

110

Table 6

Hierarchical multiple regression predicting benzodiazepine-related general

aggression.

B S.E Beta t p

95% C.I. for B Part

cor

coef Lower Upper

Constant 91.56 20.09 4.558 .000 51.93 131.19

Recruitment 8.96 4.10 .15 1.793 .075 -.90 18.82 .120

Age -.06 .20 -.03 -.316 .752 -.45 .33 -.021

Gender -1.13 3.42 -.02 -.331 .741 -7.88 5.62 -.022

Viol Conv 10.09 7.82 .10 1.290 .199 -5.34 25.52 .086

Reg Dr -2.42 4.71 -.04 -.513 .609 -11.70 6.87 -.034

Reg Alc 2.12 3.89 .05 .545 .586 -5.55 9.80 .036

Alprazolam 11.70 3.88 .25 3.012 .003 4.04 19.36 .201

Diazepam -8.83 3.73 -.19 -2.366 .019 -16.19 -1.47 -.158

BAS-FS -.166 .87 -.17 -1.905 .058 -3.38 .06 -.127

BAS-RR -.76 .90 -.07 -.849 .397 -2.53 1.01 -.057

BAS-Dr^ 2.86 .83 .30 3.451 .001 1.23 4.50 .230

BIS-Anx^ .10 .83 .01 .124 .901 -1.54 1.74 .008

BIS-Fear 1.03 .98 .09 1.056 .293 -.90 2.97 .070

AnxXDr ^ .35 .26 .09 1.330 .185 -.17 .86 .089

Note. Part cor coef = part correlation coefficient; Viol Conv = violent conviction;

Reg Drg = regular drug use (previous 3 months); Reg Alc = regular alcohol use

(previous 3 months); BAS-Dr = drive subscale, BAS-RR = reward responsiveness

subscale; BAS-FS = fun seeking subscale; BIS-Anx = anxiety subscale; BIS-Fear =

Page 111: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

111

fear subscale; AnxXDr = interaction term.

^ centered variables.

Due to the significant findings pertaining to diazepam and alprazolam, it was

explored whether general aggression differed according to benzodiazepine dose. Two

follow-up independent samples t-tests (two-tailed) indicated that levels of general

aggression did not differ between those using alprazolam within the prescribing

range or above (t(116) = -1.054, p = .294, 95% C.I. = -13.52 to 4.13, Cohen’s d = -

.20), or between those using diazepam within the prescribing range or above; t(108)

= -.849, p = .398, 95% C.I. = -14.25 to 5.71, Cohen’s d = -.18.

Physical aggression. Inclusion of the control variables at Step 1 explained

6.4% of the variance in physical aggression; Fchange(6, 195) = 2.209, p = .044. Entry

of the benzodiazepine variables at Step 2 did not significantly improve the model

(∆R2 = .015); Fchange (2, 193) = 1.542, p = .217. Entry of the BIS/BAS variables at

Step 3 did however significantly improve the model (∆R2 = .057); Fchange (5, 188) =

2.459, p = .035. The inclusion of the interaction term at Step 4 did not significantly

improve the model (∆R2 = .000); Fchange (1, 187) = .000, p = .991. However, the

model remained significant at each step, and the final model, with all the variables in

the equation, accounted for 13.5% of the total variance in physical aggression; F(14,

187) = 2.084, p = .014. Again, alprazolam and BAS-Dr significantly attributed

unique variance to physical aggression, although recruitment group and BAS-FS

approached significance (see Table 7). As calculated from the part correlation

coefficients (Pallant, 2007), BAS-Dr (4.6%) and alprazolam use (1.8%) contributed a

combined 6.4% towards the explanation of variance in physical aggression.

Page 112: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

112

Table 7

Hierarchical multiple regression predicting benzodiazepine-related physical

aggression.

B S.E Beta t p

95% C.I. of B Part

cor

coef Lower Upper

Constant 29.51 6.79 4.349 .000 16.12 42.89

Recruitment 3.15 1.69 .16 1.867 .063 -.18 6.48 .127

Age -.02 .07 -.03 -.319 .750 -.15 .11 -.022

Gender -.46 1.16 -.03 -.397 .692 -2.74 1.82 -.027

Viol Conv 3.79 2.64 .11 1.434 .153 -1.42 9.00 .098

Reg Drg -.06 1.59 -.00 -.037 .971 -3.20 3.08 -.003

Reg Alc .86 1.31 .06 .654 .514 -1.73 3.45 .044

Alprazolam 2.63 1.31 .17 2.004 .046 .04 5.22 .136

Diazepam -1.95 1.26 -.12 -1.546 .124 -4.44 .54 -.105

BAS-FS -.58 .29 -.18 -1.96 .052 -1.16 .01 -.133

BAS-RR -.19 .30 -.05 -.613 .541 -.78 .41 -.042

BAS-Dr^ .88 .28 .28 3.154 .002 .33 1.44 .215

BIS-Anx^ -.12 .28 -.03 -.419 .676 -.67 .44 -.028

BIS-Fear -.16 .33 -.04 -.47 .639 -.81 .50 -.032

AnxXDr ^ .00 .09 .00 .011 .991 -.17 .17 .001

Note. Part cor coef = part correlation coefficient; Viol Conv = violent conviction; Reg Drg = regular drug use (previous 3 months); Reg Alc = regular alcohol use (previous 3 months); BAS-Dr = drive subscale, BAS-RR = reward responsiveness subscale; BAS-FS = fun seeking subscale; BIS-Anx = anxiety subscale; BIS-Fear = fear subscale; AnxXDr = interaction term. ^ centered variables.

Page 113: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

113

A follow-up independent samples t-test (two-tailed) indicated that levels of

physical aggression did not differ between those using alprazolam within the

prescribing range or above (t(116) = -1.658, p = .100, 95% C.I. = -5.36 to .48,

Cohen’s d = -.31).

4.5 Discussion

Benzodiazepine-related aggression is poorly understood. In an effort to

understand this response in greater depth, the current study aimed to explore the role

of motivational tendencies in benzodiazepine-related aggression. It was proposed

that such characteristics would predict aggressive responding over and above

benzodiazepine type, and that BAS-Dr specifically would be important in

understanding this response. It was also predicted that BAS-Dr would moderate an

association between risk averse motivational tendencies and benzodiazepine-related

aggression. The data generally supported these predictions, however the moderation

effect was not observed.

4.5.1 Role of BAS-Dr in benzodiazepine-related aggression.

The tendency to pursue appetitive goals in a persistent manner (BAS-Dr) has

been consistently associated with aggression and related tendencies (i.e., anger;

Cooper et al., 2008; Miller et al., 2012; Seibert et al., 2010). The current study

extends this literature, by hypothesising that BAS-Dr is important in benzodiazepine-

related aggression. In support of this hypothesis, BAS-Dr was the strongest unique

predictor of both general aggression and physical aggression, over and above the

influence of benzodiazepine type (diazepam and alprazolam). Such outcomes align

with research indicating the importance of BAS-Dr in the prediction of aggressive

behaviour compared to other intrapersonal factors (Seibert et al., 2010), and

importantly, support the contention that our understanding of benzodiazepine-related

Page 114: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

114

aggression may be enhanced via recourse to intrapersonal differences (Hoaken &

Stewart, 2003; Lion et al., 1975).

Individuals with strong BAS-Dr have been described as antagonistic,

competitive, and willing to work hard to achieve goals, even if at the expense of

others (Segarra et al., 2014). Such individuals hold high expectations of rewards

following goal attainment (Harmon-Jones, 2003) and their experienced affect is

strongly reflective of their perceived progress towards their goal (Carver, 2004). For

example, in the context of challenged or blocked goal attainment, individuals with

high BAS-Dr may experience frustration or anger (Carver, 2004), such as frustrative

non-reward (Corr, 2002). Aggressive behaviour then becomes increasingly likely, as

during frustration, such individuals display reduced impulse control (Beaver et al.,

2008) and attention to risk or punishment cues (Avila, 2001). Their ability to respond

appropriately to stressors or frustrations may become further disinhibited in the

context of benzodiazepine use (Paton, 2002). Indeed, the current data indicate that

individuals with stronger BAS-Dr tendencies experienced greater anxiety and stress,

which may reflect difficulty attaining desired outcomes (Carver, 2004), and tended to

consume benzodiazepines in the context of other substances, which may further

impact their coping or self-regulation ability. However, the absence of appropriate

causal testing limits the conclusions able to be drawn from such associations, though

the findings do clearly support the role of persistent action towards desired goals in

benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour. As such, prescribers may benefit from

exploring patients’ ability to engage in effective impulse control and frustration

tolerance strategies prior to prescribing benzodiazepines. Such information may be

gleaned from knowledge of the patient’s psychosocial background, or by specifically

enquiring into their coping strategies, tendency to act without consideration of

Page 115: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

115

consequences, engagement in risky behaviour, and ability to respond to frustration in

a pro-social manner (i.e., delayed consult time, smaller prescription than requested).

Interestingly, the expected moderation effect between a strong appetitive and

strong aversive motivational system on benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour

failed to significantly influence either aggression model. Given that our sample

displayed only moderate levels of aggression, such an effect may be more likely in a

more violent sample, against clearly defined violent incidents. Furthermore,

aggression risk may be associated with extremely high levels of BIS activation

(Hatfield & Dula, 2014), whilst our sample displayed only moderately-high BIS

activation. The moderating effect may therefore only be relevant to investigations of

benzodiazepine-related aggression in individuals with more complex motivational

presentations than those observed in the current sample.

4.5.2 Role of benzodiazepine type in benzodiazepine-related aggression.

The current findings suggest that alprazolam poses a greater risk than

diazepam for subsequent aggressive behaviour. As described elsewhere (Horyniak,

Reddel, Quinn, & Dietze, 2012; Rintoul, Dobbin, Neilsen, Degenhardt, & Drummer,

2013), alprazolam is one of the most problematic benzodiazepines, and has recently

been rescheduled to a controlled substance within Australia, in order to mitigate the

risks associated with it. Given its short acting effects, association with poly-

substance use in the current sample, and the common goal to become intoxicated

(25.6%), alprazolam-related aggression may reflect the context of use, rather than the

physiological effects of alprazolam. Indeed, aggressive behaviour did not differ

according to alprazolam dose. When considered with our finding that intrapersonal

factors accounted for greater amounts of variance than alprazolam use alone, the

current data appear to support Lion and colleagues’ (1975) supposition that it is the

Page 116: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

116

interaction between benzodiazepine use, the situation and intrapersonal factors that

influences subsequent aggressive behaviour. However, further research using

urinalysis and an in-depth analysis of substance use patterns is required.

Interestingly, general aggression was significantly negatively predicted by

diazepam use. Within the current sample, diazepam is used more for alleviation of

negative emotional and physical states (i.e., anxiety, tension, or effects of

withdrawal; 41.1%), rather than to get high (14.5%), whereas alprazolam use is more

evenly attributed to both reasons. This differentiation further highlights the role that

approach motivations (rather than avoidance motivational tendencies) may have in

the experience of benzodiazepine-related aggression. Furthermore, unlike

alprazolam, diazepam appears to be infrequently used within the context of poly-

substance use, and therefore may be generally used in scenarios less conducive to

aggressive interactions. In addition, only when diazepam-preferring participants were

examined in isolation, of whom predominantly displayed a moderate or high risk of

dependence (i.e., difficulty managing diazepam use, experience of problematic

diazepam-related outcomes), did we find a positive association with aggressive

outcomes. Comparatively, the regression analyses were conducted with all

participants who had reported historically using diazepam for NMP reasons (i.e., not

necessarily frequent, ongoing, dependent use). Therefore, it could be concluded that

diazepam poses a risk for benzodiazepine-related aggression only in those who

display increasingly problematic patterns of diazepam use, rather than to the majority

who use diazepam on a less regular basis. This has important implications for the

continued prescription of diazepam, highlighting the importance of prescribers

carefully monitoring patient adherence to low dose, short term use, and the potential

benefits of prioritising non-medicinal approaches in assisting patients (Dobbin, 2014;

Page 117: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

117

Lader, 2014). Comparatively, alprazolam appears to be a risk for aggression at both

dependent (problematic) levels and for those with less frequent use, and may be best

prescribed only after exhausting other treatment options.

4.5.3 Limitations and strengths.

A number of limitations must be acknowledged. Due to the cross-sectional

nature of the study, causality cannot be implied, and data collection relied on

uncorroborated, retrospective self-report which may be vulnerable to attributional

biases and memory decay. In addition, the study did not involve a manipulation

check to ensure that the modification to the aggression questionnaire was successful,

meaning that conclusions should be considered with an element of caution,

especially given the lack of measurement of aggressive tendencies in the absence of

benzodiazepine use. The questionnaire was considered already burdensome (on

average more than 45 minutes to complete) that to also include such a measure

(possibly the AQ without the modified instructions) would result in increased

participant dropout. The temporal duration between benzodiazepine consumption and

the aggressive response was also not assessed, and therefore the possibility that the

response occurred during benzodiazepine withdrawal (Votava, Kršiak, Podhorná, &

Miczek, 2001) cannot be discounted. Benzodiazepine use was also assessed through

a non-standardised and non-piloted questionnaire. Participant feedback indicated that

the questionnaire was lengthy and failed to include an exhaustive list of street names

for various licit and illicit substances. Furthermore, the predictive models were

unable to be explored based on recruitment type, due to the insufficient sample size

of the health centre subsample. In addition, the findings are limited in generality, and

cannot be reliably applied to individuals who commit more severe violence.

Moreover, it cannot be discounted that other substances used in combination with

Page 118: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

118

benzodiazepines may have impacted the findings (Sweeney & Payne, 2012). Finally,

the current study did not permit direct examination of frustrative non-reward as this

can only be examined when reward pathways are activated.

Despite these caveats, the current study has a number of important and unique

strengths. First, specific benzodiazepines are explored, allowing greater specificity

than the majority of cross-sectional studies available. Greater specificity is also

permitted through the separate examination of general aggressive tendencies and

physical aggression specifically. Third, the sample is relatively large, with an almost

even gender split; the latter feature absent in a number of well-designed, though

male-only, examinations of benzodiazepine-related aggression (see Albrecht et al.,

2014 for a review). The most important contribution of the current study, however, is

that it is the first study to have a theoretically informed examination of

benzodiazepine-related aggression, thus offering a clear model against which such

behaviour can be more greatly understood, and interventions can be designed. The

application of the rRST at the facet level provides additional specificity in order to

inform such implications (Jones, Miller, & Lynam, 2011; Miller et al., 2012; Segarra

et al., 2014).

4.6 Implications and Conclusions

Lion and colleagues (1975) suggested that it is the interaction between

benzodiazepine use, intrapersonal factors, and context which can explain

benzodiazepine-related aggression. Our data alludes to the influence of goal-driven

tendencies and certain benzodiazepine use, though the situational context remains

unclear. Future investigations could therefore benefit from an exploration of the

context surrounding the aggressive act (i.e., presence of frustration or goal

challenge), as well as consideration of beliefs and attitudes regarding the

Page 119: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

119

acceptability of aggressive behaviour (i.e., aggressive scripts; Anderson & Bushman,

2002).

In lieu of these aspects receiving further attention, the current findings do

demonstrate the importance of intrapersonal factors in understanding

benzodiazepine-related aggression (Hoaken & Stewart, 2003; Lion et al., 1975).

Notably, benzodiazepine users may be more likely to engage in aggressive behaviour

if they exhibit persistent tendencies to pursue desired goals. In addition, general

diazepam use (i.e., not in the context of dependency) appears to reduce the risk of

general aggressive behaviour (i.e., anger, hostility, verbal aggression), whilst

alprazolam increases the risk of aggression, regardless of dose. Although further

work is necessary to confirm the mechanisms underlying the association between

motivational drive and benzodiazepine-related aggression, the findings highlight the

benefit of attending to frustration tolerance and aggression scripts in individuals with

high BAS-Dr, and support the rescheduling of alprazolam to a controlled substance.

4.7 Chapter Summary

The application of a theory-drive research approach has highlighted the

importance of intrapersonal factors in understanding benzodiazepine-related

aggression. Notably, persistent approach of desired goals was associated with

increased aggressive behaviour. In addition, alprazolam was highlighted as more

risky than diazepam in regards to self-reported physical aggression, aligning with

recent national and international concerns about the benzodiazepine. Community

based studies are important in the examination of this response, as they provide

insight into the population likely to access community-based health centres and drug

and alcohol clinics. However, the low base rate of aggressive behaviour in such

samples suggests that a clearer understanding of this response may be afforded by

Page 120: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

120

investigation of a violent criminal justice sample. As will be discussed in the

following chapter, such samples have received minimal attention in related literature,

despite the potential that such examination has to inform the use of prescribed

benzodiazepines in justice health contexts, and the development of appropriate

violent offender rehabilitation strategies.

Due to local restrictions on new research protocols with justice populations, the

following study involved analysis of a previously-acquired database, developed in

the absence of strong theoretical reasoning. It is hoped that the uniqueness of the

database (i.e., benzodiazepine-using offenders) provides valuable insight into this

response, and informs the development of future, more theoretically rigorous,

research protocols exploring benzodiazepine-related aggression in violent samples.

Page 121: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

121

Chapter Five: Violent crime: A complex interplay of benzodiazepine use,

psychological distress, and problematic impulsive behaviours.

Please note, this chapter presents an expanded version of an original

manuscript submitted for review to Journal of Substance Abuse (September, 2015),

due to copyright reasons. Details of the submitted article have been appended for

your consideration (Appendix D).

5.1 Introduction

Why any individual commits a violent act is a question that generally

frightens and confronts us. Years of research and investigation have shown that

violence involves a complex interplay of social, environmental, biological and

situational elements (Anderson & Bokor, 2012; Steinert & Whittington, 2013).

However, in spite of examinations of the construct from a broad array of disciplines,

including neurobiology, mental health, sociology, criminology, and addiction (for

reviews, see Anderson & Bokor, 2012; Chereji, Pintea, & David, 2012; Friedman,

1998; Schenk & Fremouw, 2012), a number of questions still remain. One of these

questions involves identifying the role of various substances in the commission of

violent behaviour. Despite knowing that pharmacological properties of substances

can have a profound influence on neurobiology, mental state, and behaviour, we

know relatively little about how this differs between substances to effect violent

behaviour (i.e., both stimulants and depressants have been associated with violence;

e.g., Lennings, Copeland, & Howard, 2003; McKetin et al., 2014). While the role of

alcohol in violent behavior has been studied at length, benzodiazepine use on the

other hand has been surprisingly under-researched. Given the increasing misuse of

benzodiazepines (ACC, 2014), particularly amongst those with mental health issues

Page 122: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

122

and forensic backgrounds (Ng & Macgregor, 2012; Stafford & Burns, 2013;

Sweeney & Payne, 2012), further research is warranted.

A recent systematic review (Albrecht et al., 2014) suggests that

benzodiazepines may play a role in aggressive behavior and violent crimes (e.g.,

French, 1989; Ben-Porath & Taylor, 2002; Lundholm, Haggård, Möller, Hallqvist, &

Thiblin, 2013; Moore, Glenmullen, & Furberg, 2010). For example, an estimated 1-

20% of benzodiazepine users report experiencing some form of increased anger or

aggression following use (Lader, 2011). While this may seem counter-intuitive given

the sedating effects of benzodiazepines, neurobiological theories suggest that such

paradoxical effects may arise due to disinhibition following benzodiazepine use

(Bond, 1998; Longo & Johnson, 2000). This unlikely effect has been repeatedly

demonstrated in experimental laboratory studies, where increased responding

indicative of aggression has followed consumption of acute doses of benzodiazepines

(e.g., alprazolam, diazepam) compared to placebo (e.g., Wilkinson, 1985; Bond,

Curran, Bruce, O’Sullivan, & Shine, 1995). However, the use of analogue

representations of aggression in highly controlled circumstances greatly limits the

ecological validity of such findings.

5.1.1 Benzodiazepines and violent crime.

Criminal justice samples have been found to misuse benzodiazepines more

than the general community (e.g., AIHW, 2011; Ng & Macgregor, 2012). Yet,

empirical literature examining the association between benzodiazepine use and

subsequent violent crime is rare (see Albrecht et al., 2014, for a review). Although

benzodiazepines, either alone or in combination with other substances, have been

empirically associated with criminal behaviour, including acquisitive and property

crime (Bradford & Payne, 2012; Darke & Ross, 1994; Darke, Ross, Mills, Teesson,

Page 123: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

123

Williamson, & Havard, 2010; Payne & Gaffney, 2012; Smith, Miller, O’Keefe, &

Fry, 2007), comparatively little attention has been paid to their association with

violent offences. To date, no research has explored the relationship between

benzodiazepine use and actual violent crime in an adult community criminal justice

sample. This is despite recent research demonstrating young community based

offenders (aged 16-20 years) attribute their violent crimes most commonly to

diazepam (often in conjunction with alcohol; Forsyth, Kahn, & McKinlay, 2011).

Furthermore, studies of remanded and incarcerated violent offenders report positive

associations between violent crime and higher doses of (unspecified)

benzodiazepines (and combined alcohol use; Haggård-Grann, Hallqvist, Långström,

& Möller, 2006; Lundholm et al., 2013). The focus of this study is to examine in

detail how community-based offenders engaged in violent crime differ from those

engaged in non-violent crimes across a range of mental health and substance abuse

behaviours with a particular focus on their use of benzodiazepines. Findings will

have particular relevance to law enforcement and addiction treatment services in the

community, at a time when recent rescheduling of alprazolam to a controlled

substance (Schedule 8) in Australia may impact the already increasing diversion of

benzodiazepines onto the black market (ACC, 2014).

Importantly, the available literature provides little insight into the specific

dose schedules or types of benzodiazepines which present increased risk of violence.

That is, the nature of the benzodiazepine-violence relationship appears to vary

according to administration and dosing schedules of specific benzodiazepines. For

example, methodologically diverse investigations have provided evidence both for

and against the role of alprazolam in violent behaviour (e.g., Bond et al., 1995; Bond

& Silveira, 1993; O’Sullivan et al., 1994). In addition, existing cross-sectional

Page 124: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

124

studies often group benzodiazepines together (ignoring concepts such as the half-life

or function of specific benzodiazepines), fail to consider dose, and at times do not

include non-violent controls (e.g., Dåderman, Fredriksson, Kristiansson, Nilsson, &

Lidberg, 2002). Such methodological limitations have hampered our understanding

of the relationship between benzodiazepine use and subsequent violent crime.

Improving our understanding of these relationships may have important medical and

legal implications for prescribing practices, especially when benzodiazepines are

commonly used to manage agitation (Ashton, 2002), often in public spaces such as

hospital emergency rooms.

5.1.2 The current study.

In order to address some of these gaps, the present study aims to examine the

relationship between benzodiazepine use and engagement in violent crime within a

benzodiazepine-using, community-based criminal justice sample. Previously

gathered data are re-analysed with this specific research question in mind. The

current study uses a non-violent offender comparison group, and uniquely explores

whether benzodiazepine type or dose level is more closely associated with violent

than non-violent crimes. Alprazolam and diazepam are specifically examined due to

their frequent misuse in forensic populations (McGregor, Gately, & Fleming, 2011;

Sweeney & Payne, 2012), and benzodiazepine use during the month and day

preceding a recent crime is explored. It is predicted that individuals engaged in

violent crime will report greater benzodiazepine use, and higher doses, than non-

violent offenders. In order to attend to the complexity of violent behaviour (DeWall,

Anderson, & Bushman, 2011), core factors understood to play a role in violent crime

are also assessed. Based on prior literature, it is expected that individuals engaged in

violence will exhibit a heightened level of impulsivity (Derefinko, DeWall, Metze,

Page 125: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

125

Walsh, & Lynam, 2011; Smith & Waterman, 2006), increased psychological distress

or disorder (e.g., Swogger, Walsh, Houston, Cashman-Brown, & Connor, 2010;

Umberson, Williams, & Anderson, 2002), poly-substance use (Friedman, 1998), and

prior criminality (Rice, Harris, & Lang, 2013).

5.2 Method

5.2.1 Participants.

Participants were recruited through purposive sampling techniques via drug

treatment programs and initiatives within the criminal justice system in Melbourne,

Australia, including the Court Integrated Services and other drug treatment services,

through liaison with service managers and forensic counsellors. Eligibility criteria

were (i) 18 years and over, (ii) committed a crime within six months of the interview,

and (iii) used benzodiazepines (prescribed or non-prescribed) at least once per month

in the last six months. The current study uses the term ‘index offence’ to refer to the

participants’ most recent crime, to which eligibility criteria (ii) relates. Participants

were divided into violent and non-violent groups based on this offence.

5.2.2 Design and Procedure.

The study involved a specifically developed semi-structured interview

protocol. Participants provided demographic information, mental health and

substance use history, and the context of their index charge (i.e., employment and

residential information). Interviews lasted 45-60 minutes and were conducted either

face-to-face or over the telephone, and were audio recorded and transcribed

verbatim. Analysis of the de-identified data was approved by the relevant ethics

committees.

Page 126: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

126

5.2.3 Measures.

Psychological distress. The Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K-10;

Kessler et al., 2002) is a 10-item questionnaire designed to measure self-reported

psychological distress over the most recent 4-week period, or in this case the month

before the crime. A score of 30 or more has been demonstrated to be the most

accurate indicator for a severe mental disorder (Andrews & Slade, 2001). The K-10

has demonstrated predictive validity in a national Australian sample (Furukawa,

Kessler, Slade, & Andrews, 2003).

Impulsivity. The UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale (UPPS; Whiteside &

Lynam, 2001) is a 45-item questionnaire measuring impulsivity across four sub-

scales2, with higher scores indicating greater levels of impulsivity. The subscales are

Urgency (12 items; e.g., “when I am upset I often act without thinking”), (lack of)

Premeditation (11 items; e.g., “I like to stop and think things over before I do them”),

(lack of) Perseverance (10 items; e.g., “I concentrate easily”), and Sensation Seeking

(12 items; e.g., “I’ll try anything once”). The four factor model has demonstrated

construct and differential validity (Miller, Flory, Lynam, & Leukefeld, 2003;

Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds, 2005), and concurrent validity within a

sample of alcohol-consuming college students (Magid & Colder, 2007). The

subscales have shown good internal consistency (α = 0.82-0.91; Whiteside & Lynam,

2001).

Substance use and dependence. The Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS;

Gossop & Darke, 1995) is a five-item questionnaire providing a score indicating the

degree of dependence to a certain substance. Data was collected regarding

dependence to benzodiazepines, in the month prior to the index offence. The items 2 The original study utilised a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all, 2 = sometimes, 4 = very much). It is recognised that this departs from the standardised application of the UPPS, which uses a 4-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 4 = strongly disagree).

Page 127: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

127

are scored on a 4-point scale (0-3) with a total score obtained by the sum of all item

ratings. A score of 7 or higher indicates benzodiazepine dependence (de las Cuevas,

Sanz, de la Fuente, Padilla, & Berenguer, 2000).

A 28-day timeline follow back method (Sobell & Sobell, 1992; Sobell,

Brown, Leo, & Sobell, 1996) was used to measure benzodiazepine, alcohol,

pharmaceutical opiate and illicit drug use in the month prior to the index crime.

Participants were asked about drug type and amount used, route of administration,

days of no use/withdrawal, days of increased use, and days of different drug

combinations or administration methods.

Criminality. The Criminality Index from the Opiate Treatment Index (OTI;

Darke, Hall, Wodak, Heather, & Ward, 1992) was used as a measure of criminal

behaviour in the month prior to the index offence. The Criminality Index explores

the frequency of recent property crime, drug dealing, fraud, and violent crime using a

5-point Likert scale (0 = no crime, 2 = once a week, 4 = daily). Scores are summed to

form a total score (0-16), with higher scores indicating greater criminal involvement.

The Criminality Index has demonstrated test-retest reliability over a one week period

(0.96), and construct validity with official criminal records and the Addiction

Severity Index (ASI) crime days measure, but limited internal reliability (α = 0.38;

Darke et al., 1992), potentially due to the nature of the items all referring to specific

and disparate crime types. The violent crime item has demonstrated agreement

between a person’s self-report and collateral report (94% agreement, kappa: -0.03)

and conviction records (89.2% agreement; kappa = .0713; Darke et al., 1992). The

tool has been successfully applied in a study exploring benzodiazepine use (Darke &

Ross, 1994).

3 Low kappa figures with a high agreement may reflect the low base rate of the behaviour under the agreement analysis (Viera & Garrett, 2005)

Page 128: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

128

5.2.4 Statistical analyses.

The data were analysed in SPSS PASW Statistics 18. Preliminary screening

informed the deletion of six cases due to no data, and indicated that there was a

proportion of incomplete data for the dose variables (nrange = 44-60; 53.70-73.17%).

Due to the unique nature of the data, statistical imputation was not conducted, though

pairwise case exclusion was applied to analyses. This may have attenuated the

statistical power available, and influenced the choice of statistical analyses that were

conducted. Where normality was violated, variables were recoded, including number

of mental of health diagnoses (zero; one; two or more diagnoses), OTI Violence

(violent vs non-violent), and benzodiazepine dose (within or above the standard

dosing range4 outlined in the MIMS; “Alprazolam”, 2013; “Diazepam”, 2013).

Inspection of bivariate correlations indicated an absence of multicollinearity. The

final sample size was 82.

Due to concerns of the veracity of the impulsivity data provided (i.e., pre-

computed scale scores), a research assistant not involved in the current research

study extracted the item-level impulsivity data from the raw data, into a de-identified

electronic spreadsheet. Impulsivity data for 76 cases were provided. Inspection of

this item-level data by the first author indicated that the scale scores provided in the

de-identified database were incorrect. In order to align the final scores with those

possible within the standardized version of the UPPS (i.e., zero scores are not

possible), 7.25% (n = 248) of the data points required re-scoring from zero to one.

Inspection of missing data indicated that less than 1% (n = 35) of the data points

were missing, and these were imputed with the sample median per item. Items were

reverse-scored, taking into consideration the already reversed scale direction (i.e., 4 =

4 Alprazolam: 0.5-4.0mg per day; Diazepam: 5-40mg per day.

Page 129: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

129

very much, whilst standardized UPPS uses 4 = strongly disagree)5, and scale totals

were calculated using SPSS Compute Variable. Only impulsivity data of 74 cases

were transferred into the main database, due to the prior deletion of two cases above.

All scale scores were normally distributed, with no univariate outliers, and the

assumption of multicollinearity was upheld.

Sample characteristics were explored using descriptive and frequency

analyses. Where possible, group differences were explored using independent

samples t-tests (two-tailed), and means, standard deviations, and 95% confidence

intervals (CI) are reported. For categorical variables, or when assumptions are

violated, non-parametric tests were conducted.

5.3 Results

5.3.1 Participant characteristics.

The final sample consisted of 82 individuals, aged between 21-56 years old

(M = 34.6, SD = 7.1; see Table 1 for sample demographics).

5.3.2 Crime profile.

Eleven (13.4%) participants were charged with a violent index offence, which

included assault, armed robbery, aggravated burglary, serious threats, and sexual

assault. In the month prior to the index offence, 64 (78.0%) participants reported

engaging in criminal activity of some kind, most commonly property crime (69.5%),

followed by drug dealing (36.6%), violent crime (22.0%), and fraud (20.7%). Total

frequency of crime (OTI total) was positively associated with urgency and a total

impulsivity score, as well as increased use of substances and benzodiazepines in the

12-24 hours prior to the index offence.

5 It is noted that the scale used may have conceptual implications both when impacting the participant’s answer, and for over-all interpretability (i.e., estimation of frequency versus degree the participant agreed with each statement).

Page 130: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

130

Table 1

Demographic characteristics of the sample population

Characteristic n % Characteristic n %

Gender Country of birth

Male 65 79.3 Australia 73 89.0

Female 17 20.7 Other 9 11.0

ATSI status Highest education

Aboriginal 10 12.2 Yr 9 or below 35 42.7

Torres Strait Islander 2 2.4 Yr 10-11 32 39.0

Neither 70 85.4 Yr 12 9 11.0

Diploma/Tafe 6 7.3

Main source of

incomea Housing statusa

FT/PT employment 4 4.8 Homeless 14 17.1

Temporary benefit 33 40.2 Rented house or flat 25 30.5

Pension 35 42.7 Privately owned

house/flat 4 4.9

Criminal activity 9 11.0 Boarding house/hostel 15 18.3

Other 1 1.2 AOD treatment

residence 2 2.4

Other 22 26.8

Page 131: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

131

Table 2

Participant substance use and mental health histories, and recent distress ratings.

Previous AOD

treatment Lifetime substance use

Opiate substitution 68 82.9 Cannabis 82 100.0

AOD counselling 67 81.7 Amphetamines 82 100.0

Inpatient detoxification 54 65.9 Tobacco 81 98.8

Self-help programs 38 46.3 Alcohol 81 98.8

Residential

rehabilitation 35 42.7 Heroin 79 96.3

Supported accomm 27 32.9 Ecstasy 69 84.1

Outreach worker

support 17 20.7 Hallucinogens 57 69.5

Unspecified/other 17 20.7 Cocaine 57 69.5

Self-reported diagnosis Psychological distress

Depression 51 62.2 Likely well 6 7.3

Anxiety 37 45.1 Mild mental disorder 11 13.4

Bipolar disorder 20 24.4 Mod mental disorder 17 20.7

Drug-induced

psychosis 18 22.0 Severe mental disorder 43 52.4

Panic disorder 18 22.0

Schizophrenia 13 15.9

Personality disorder 9 11.0

Note. ATSI = identify as Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander; FT/PT = full time or part time; AOD = alcohol and other drugs; accomm = accommodation’ mod = moderate. astatus refers to the month prior to index offence.

Page 132: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

132

5.3.3 Benzodiazepine use profile.

The most commonly used benzodiazepine over the lifetime (including

prescribed and non-prescribed) was diazepam (98.8%), followed by temazepam

(95.1%), alprazolam (93.9%), oxazepam (92.7%), and clonazepam (62.2%). More

than one third (39.0%) had used six types of benzodiazepines (including ‘other’),

whilst more than half (59.8%) regularly used four or more types. Nearly half (46.3%)

exhibited benzodiazepine dependence, and participants specifically referenced

difficulty with diazepam (39.0%) or alprazolam (37.8%) when responding to the

SDS. As shown in Table 3, benzodiazepine dependence was significantly, positively

associated with urgency, sensation seeking, psychological distress, and the number of

other substances used on a regular basis. Non-medical acquisition methods (i.e.,

without a prescription) were extremely common (91.5%), though 70.7% of the

sample reported using both non-medical and medical sources during their lifetime.

In the 12-24 hours immediately prior to the index offence, 84.1% of the

sample reported using a benzodiazepine, with high rates of diazepam (61.0%) and

alprazolam (57.3%) use. More than a third (36.6%) reported using two types of

benzodiazepines prior to the index crime. Examination of approximate diazepam

equivalent doses (DZM) indicates that alprazolam was consumed at the highest doses

both in the day and month prior to the offence, at levels more than twice, and triple,

national prescribing recommendations, respectively (“Alprazolam”, 2013; see Table

4).

5.3.4 Substance use profile.

As shown in Table 2, all participants reported lifetime use of cannabis and

amphetamines, while other drug use was also high. The entire sample had used five

or more drugs in their life (M = 17.9, SD = 3.5), and reported a large number of

Page 133: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

133

Table 3

Means (M), standard deviations (SD) and bivariate correlations between factors of interest

M (SD) Age OTI Total

SDS BZD K-10 UPPS

PRE UPPS URG

UPPS SS

UPPS PER

UPPS Total Sub Reg BZD

Reg Sub 12-

24

OTI Total 3.8 (3.3) -.16

SDS BZD 6.6 (4.3) .01 .02

K-10 31.5 (9.5) .06 .20 .51**

UPPS PRE 30.5 (7.0) .21 .20 -.20 -.04

UPPS URG 33.5 (8.0) .09 .31** .32** .53** .30**

UPPS SS 31.2 (8.8) -.11 .12 .37** .17 -.13 .12

UPPS PER 26.1 (5.4) -.10 .19 -.20 .06 .51** .26* -33**

Page 134: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

134

M (SD) Age OTI Total

SDS BZD K-10 UPPS

PRE UPPS URG

UPPS SS

UPPS PER

UPPS Total Sub Reg BZD

Reg Sub 12-

24

UPPS Total

121.3 (16.8) .04 .36** .19 .34** .66** .74** .42** .48**

Sub Reg 12.8 (3.9) .07 .17 .24* .15 -.13 .23 .05 -.09 .05

BZD Reg 3.8 (1.4) .03 .09 .27* .13 -.02 .07 .05 .06 .07 .79**

Sub 12-24 4.21 (1.29) .01 .25* .12 .177 -.07 .11 -.13 -.08 -.08 .35** .33**

BZD 12-24 1.22 (.69) -.02 .26* .20 .174 -.20 .10 -.14 -.15 -.17 .20 .21 .54**

Note. OTI Total frequency of crime in last month; SDS BZD total Severity of Dependence score for benzodiazepines; K-10 Kessler distress

scales total score; UPPS PRE (lack of) premeditation; UPPS URG urgency; UPPS SS sensation seeking; UPPS PER (lack of)

perseverance; UPPS Total total impulsivity score; MH DX number of mental health diagnoses; Sub Reg number of substances regularly

used; BZD Reg number of benzodiazepines regularly used; Sub 12-24 number of substance used 12-24 hours prior to index crime; BZD 12-

24 number of benzodiazepines used 12-24 hours prior to index crime. * p < .05 ** p < .01

Page 135: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

135

substances regularly used (M = 12.8, SD = 3.9). In addition, the majority of the

sample (96.3%) had previously sought treatment for alcohol and/or drug (AOD)

difficulties, predominantly in the form of opiate substitution programs (82.9%). In

the 12-24 hours immediately prior to the index offence, 98.8% of the sample reported

using a substance in addition to benzodiazepines.

5.3.5 Mental health profile.

Scores on the K-10 indicated that the sample was highly distressed, with more than

half of the sample (52.4%) scoring 30 or above (see Table 2). The majority of the

sample reported being diagnosed with a mental illness in their life (82.9%), with

64.6% reporting more than one mental health diagnosis (M = 2.2, SD = 1.7). The

most commonly reported diagnoses were depression (62.2%) and anxiety (45.1%),

followed by bipolar disorder (24.4%), drug induced psychosis (22.0%), panic

disorder (22.0%), schizophrenia (15.9%), and personality disorder (11.0%).

5.3.6 Group differences.

Benzodiazepine use. As shown in Table 5, the degree of benzodiazepine

dependence significantly differed between those who committed violent and non-

violent index offences. Individuals who committed a violent offence reported a

greater degree of benzodiazepine dependence than those who committed a non-

violent index offence (t(76) = -2.120, p = .037, 95% C.I.: -5.84 to -.18, Cohen’s d =

0.73). The use of non-prescribed (i.e., illicit) benzodiazepines did not differ between

violent and non-violent offenders (p = .186; OR = 0.27, 95% CI: .04-1.71).

Alprazolam use. Non-parametric Fisher’s exact tests (two-tailed) found that

individuals who committed a violent index offence were significantly more likely to

use alprazolam at doses above the SDR (90.0%) in the month prior to the index

offence, than individuals who committed a non-violent offence (54.0%; p = .040; OR

Page 136: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

136

= 7.67, 95% CI: .90-65.13). Indeed, violent offenders reported average alprazolam

doses substantially higher than the SDR (M = 26.60, SD = 31.13), compared to the

high, though less extreme, average doses reported by non-violent offenders (M =

9.54, SD = 14.92). However, although a similar trend was observed, alprazolam dose

used in the 12-24 hours prior to the index offence did not significantly differ between

violent (M = 14.57, SD = 16.56) and non-violent groups (M = 9.95, SD = 16.54); p =

.416; OR = 2.76, 95% CI: .48-15.95. Neither group was significantly more likely to

have used alprazolam in the month (p = .441, OR = 3.21, 95% CI = .38-26.91) or day

(p = .754, OR = 1.31, 95% CI: .35-4.90) prior to the index offence.

Table 4

Benzodiazepine use the month and day prior to the index offence

Month prior to offence 12-24 hours prior to

offence

N who

reported use

Est. frequen

cy (days)

Est. dose per use

(mgs)

Approx. DZMa (mgs)

N who

reported use

Est. dose

prior to crime (mgs)

Approx. DZM (mgs)

Benzodiazepine

N (%)

M (SD) M

(SD) M

N (%) M (SD) M

Diazepam 68

(82.9) 19.5

(10.7) 35.2

(50.3) 35.2

50 (61.0)

41.4 (53.2)

41.4

Alprazolam 63

(76.8) 14.2

(11.0) 12.4

(19.3) 124.0

47 (57.3)

10.6 (16.4)

106.0

Note. Approx DZM = approximate diazepam equivalent dose; Est = estimated; mgs

= milligrams. a Approximate DZM computed using a 1:10 ratio for alprazolam, as suggested by

dosing conversion table outlined by Farinde (2014).

Page 137: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

137

Table 5

Independent t-tests pertaining to substance use and commission of a violent index

offence

Violent Index

M (SD)

Non-violent Index

M (SD)

df t

statistic

p (two-tailed)

Mean difference

95% C.I.

d Lower Upper

SDS BZD

9.20 (3.52)

6.19 (4.27)

76 -2.120 .037 -3.01 -5.84 -.18 .73

No. BZD Reg Used

4.45 (1.69)

3.73 (1.29)

80 -1.657 .101 -.72 -1.59 .14 .54

No. BZD 12-24

1.27 (.79)

1.21 (.68)

79 -.260 .796 -.06 -.51 .39 .08

No. Sub Reg Used

13.64 (5.39)

12.72 (3.70)

80 -.717 .476 -.92 -3.47 1.63 .24

No. Sub 12-24

3.73 (1.56)

4.29 (1.24)

79 1.340 .184 .56 -.27 1.39 .44

Note. C.I. = Confidence Interval; SDS = Severity of Dependence Scale total score

(benzodiazepines); BZD = benzodiazepine; No. Sub = number of substances; 12-24

= 12-24 hour period prior to the index offence.

Diazepam use. Non-parametric Fisher’s exact tests (two-tailed) failed to find

significant group differences relating to diazepam average dose in the month prior to

the offence (p = .616; OR = 1.75, 95% CI: .30-10.27) or in the 12-24 hours preceding

the offence (p = .369; OR = 2.33, 95% CI: .44-12.45). Neither group was

significantly more likely to have used diazepam in the month (p = 1.000, OR = .84,

95% CI = .16-4.42) or day (p = 1.000, OR = 1.06, 95% CI = .28-3.97) prior to the

index offence.

Page 138: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

138

Mental health. Although those who committed a violent index offence (M =

37.25, SD = 9.97) reported higher distress scores than those who committed a non-

violent index offence (M = 30.81, SD = 9.31), this difference failed to reach

statistical significance; t (75) = -1.839, p = .070 (two-tailed), 95% CI: -13.41 to .53,

Cohen’s d = 0.70. Fisher’s exact tests (two-tailed) found that individuals who

committed a violent offence were significantly more likely to be diagnosed with

depression (90.9% vs 57.7%; p = .045; OR = 7.32, 95% CI: .89-60.29) and

personality disorder (36.4% vs 7.0%; p = .016; OR = 7.54, 95% CI: 1.64-34.77) than

those who committed a non-violent index offence. There were no significant group

differences based on anxiety (p = .210; OR = 2.39, 95% CI: .64-8.91), panic disorder

(p = .246; OR = 2.33, 95% CI: .60-9.07), or bipolar (p = .449; OR = 2.29, 95% CI:

.51-7.57), although the analyses regarding schizophrenia (p = .068; OR = 3.94, 95%

CI: .96-16.18) and short-term psychosis (p = .058; OR = 3.72, 95% CI: .98-14.07)

approached significance.

Criminality. Frequency of reported crime involvement (OTI total) in the

month prior to the index offence did not significantly differ between those who

committed violent and non-violent index offences; t (79) = -.043, p = .966, 95% CI =

-2.19 to 2.10, Cohen’s d = .01. However, Fisher’s exact test (two-tailed) found that

individuals who committed a violent index offence were more likely to have engaged

in violence in the month prior (54.5%) than those who committed a non-violent

index offence (17.1%; p = .012; OR = 5.8, 95% CI: 1.52-22.15).

Impulsivity. Individuals who committed a violent index offence (M = 37.38,

SD = 7.54) displayed significantly higher sensation seeking tendencies than non-

violent offenders (M = 30.42, SD = 8.72); t (72) = -2.157, p = .034, 95% C.I.: -13.38

to -.53, Cohen’s d = .82. No further group differences were observed (see Table 6).

Page 139: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

139

Table 6

Independent t-tests pertaining to impulsivity and commission of a violent index

offence

Violent Index

M (SD)

Non-violent Index

M (SD)

df t

statistic

p (two-tailed)

Mean difference

95% C.I.

d Lower Upper

(lack of) Premed

27.00 (8.02)

30.97 (6.86)

72 1.519 .133 3.97 -1.24 9.18 .58

Sensation Seeking

37.38 (7.54)

30.42 (8.72)

72 -2.157 .034 -6.95 -13.38 -.53 .82

Urgency 34.63 (8.85)

33.32 (7.93)

72 -.435 .665 -1.31 -7.29 4.68 .17

(lack of) Persev

25.00 (5.63)

26.24 (5.41)

72 .611 .543 1.24 -2.81 5.30 .23

Note. C.I. = Confidence Interval; Premed = premeditation; Persev = perseverance.

5.4 Discussion

The relationship between benzodiazepine use and violent crime was explored

in a benzodiazepine using, community criminal justice sample. Consideration was

also given to factors understood to play a role in the commission of violent crime,

notably mental health, impulsivity, substance use, and prior criminal behaviour. Of

note, our sample was highly distressed, at levels substantially greater than that

reported by persons entering Australian custodial settings (AIHW, 2013), a national

sample of injecting drug users (Stafford & Burns, 2013), and the general Australian

population (AIHW, 2014). Taken with the sample’s high rate of poly-substance use

and strong tendency towards impulsive action, such complex presentations are

however common in community criminal justice and AOD services; indeed, nearly

all of the participants reported having sought AOD treatment in the past.

Page 140: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

140

In line with the violence literature, our findings demonstrate that violence is

multiply-determined, influenced by both unchanging and dynamic factors (DeWall,

Anderson, & Bushman, 2011). Individuals who committed a violent index offence

were significantly more likely to report benzodiazepine dependence, use alprazolam

doses above the recognized standard prescribing range, display strong sensation

seeking tendencies, report engaging in violent behaviour in the month prior to the

index offence, and report diagnoses of depression and personality disorder, than

those who committed a non-violent index offence. The use of non-prescribed (i.e.,

illicit) benzodiazepines or other substances did not differ between violent and non-

violent offenders. Importantly, it appears that a general tendency towards high dose

benzodiazepine use is insufficient to indicate violence risk; it is the constellation of

risk indicators which is important (Lion, Azcarate, & Koepke, 1975).

Individuals who committed a violent index offence exhibited significantly

higher levels of benzodiazepine dependence than those who committed a non-violent

offence. This indicates that violent offenders were more likely to report difficulty

controlling their use, and greater concern about their use of benzodiazepines. Such

difficulties may have reduced their ability to effectively negotiate other situational

stressors, increasing their likelihood of responding to stressors in an emotional, rash

manner (e.g., violence). Enhanced disinhibition following benzodiazepine use

(Paton, 2002) may underlie this response. However, this response may also be

influenced by a series of indirect mechanisms, as suggested by significant

correlations found with benzodiazepine dependence in our study, but which were

unable to be investigated due to insufficient statistical power. For example, impulsive

action in the context of negative emotions (urgency) may influence the relationship

between benzodiazepine dependence and violence, by increasing the likelihood that

Page 141: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

141

an individual who is experiencing distress and using high levels of benzodiazepines

may act spontaneously and violently without considering the consequences (see

Figure 2). Urgency was found to associate with general offending, though in the

context of problematic benzodiazepine use may heighten the risk of violent

behaviour. Such a model aligns with research associating urgency with negative

substance use outcomes (Coskunpinar, Dir, Cyders, 2013), and as influential in the

relationship between emotional lability and violence (Dvorak, Pearson, & Kuvaas,

2013). Testing of such models would improve our understanding of this response,

and offer individually tailored treatment options.

We also found that sensation seeking tendencies were associated with

increased benzodiazepine dependence and engagement in violent offending; however

given the cross-sectional nature of the data causality cannot be inferred.

Nevertheless, sensation seeking has been associated with violent behaviour in

samples of undergraduate students (Derefinko et al., 2011; Dvorak et al., 2013;

Miller, Zeichner, & Wilson, 2012) and violent prisoners (Shoham, Askenazy, Rahay,

Chard, & Addl, 1989), and is often an observed characteristic in substance-using

Figure 2. Proposed relationships between psychological distress, impulsive coping,

benzodiazepine dependence and violence.

Page 142: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

142

samples (e.g., Horvath, Millch, Lynam, Leukefeld, & Clayton, 2004; Knafo, Jaffee,

Quinn, & Harden, 2013). Such tendencies may predispose individuals to situations

which place them at a greater risk of violence, particularly when using drugs that can

further disinhibit behaviour. Further research would benefit from exploring the

combined influence of sensation seeking and benzodiazepine use on violent crime in

a more violent sample.

Further research is needed to clarify the key factors underlying the

benzodiazepine-violent crime relationship. Nonetheless, there is a growing body of

evidence suggesting that high doses of alprazolam in particular may play a

significant role in violent criminal behavior. Alprazolam is arguably the most

concerning benzodiazepine due to its short-acting nature and potential for harm

(Nicholas, Lee, & Roche, 2011; Rintoul, Dobbin, Nielsen, Degenhardt, & Drummer,

2013), and is frequently abused in clinical and forensic populations (Horyniak,

Reddel, Quinn, & Dietze, 2012; Sweeney & Payne, 2012). Notably, widespread

evidence of alprazolam’s harm potential has led to its recent rescheduling to a

controlled substance (Schedule 8) in Australia. Specifically pertaining to violence,

alprazolam has been associated with instances of increased aggression in clinical

studies (Gardner & Cowdry, 1985; Noyes et al., 1998; O’Sullivan et al., 1994),

typically involving high dose regimes. Our data extend these findings, as general use

of higher doses of alprazolam by non-clinical, criminal-justice involved individuals

also appear to be associated with a greater likelihood of engaging in violent

behaviour. Our findings also appear to tentatively support the idea that acute high

doses of alprazolam may pose a proximal risk of violence. The identified trend

suggests that, in contrast to laboratory findings (Bond & Silveria, 1993), higher acute

doses of alprazolam may pose a greater risk of violence than regularly prescribed

Page 143: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

143

doses. Indeed, although their case-crossover study did not specify benzodiazepine

type, Lundholm and colleagues (2013) identified an association between acute,

‘unusually high’ benzodiazepine doses and violent crime. It is emphasized, however,

that this latter conclusion is based on a non-significant trend in the data, and merely

identifies an area in need of further research. Moving forward, an additional

consideration is the accessibility of the various benzodiazepines, as increased

restrictions on alprazolam (in Australia) may create a domino effect where other

benzodiazepines (or substances) are substituted to harmful levels.

The diagnoses of personality disorder and depression may present further risk

indicators for benzodiazepine-related aggression. Personality disorders, especially

antisocial (ASPD) and borderline (BPD) types, have been consistently associated

with violent behaviour (Gillies & O’Brien, 2006; Latalova & Prasko, 2010; Yu,

Geddes, & Fazel, 2012), and there is some evidence to suggest that comorbid

substance abuse increases the risk of violence in people with such diagnoses

(Fountoulakis, Leucht, & Kaprinis, 2008). The association between violence and

personality disorders may be due to problematic enduring cognitive and affective

characteristics, such as aggression-related and maladaptive cognitions and anger

(Gilbert & Daffern, 2011), or a need for excitement (Howard, 2011), as well as the

presence of diagnostic traits such as a callous disregard for others’ rights and lack of

empathy (ASPD), and impulsive tendencies and emotional dysregulation (BPD;

American Psychiatric Association, 2014). The association between depression and

aggressive behaviour may reflect common underlying factors (e.g., low serotonin,

impaired attachment), violence resulting from depressive sequelae (e.g., reduced

social support, increased alcohol use, angry rumination, impaired self-regulation or

impulsivity; Dutton & Karakanta, 2013), or depression developing in response to

Page 144: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

144

guilt, rumination and regret following an aggressive act (Graham, Bernards, Flynn,

Tremblay, & Wells, 2012). The progression between depression and aggressive

behaviour may also be non-linear, reflecting a complex dynamic progression over

time, influenced by factors such as gender or violence type or severity (e.g., Angkaw,

Ross, Pittman, Kelada, Valencerina, & Baker, 2013; Graham et al., 2012; Sadeh,

Javdani, Finy, & Verona, 2011; Stith, Smith, Penn, Ward, & Tritt, 2004). Although

our measure of lifetime diagnoses cannot suggest that active symptoms of depression

were directly associated with violent offending, our data does suggest that a

predisposition towards emotional disturbance, disrupted interpersonal functioning,

and reduced executive functioning may be important in the assessment of violence

risk, especially in substance-using samples.

5.4.1 Limitations and strengths.

The study has a number of limitations. Of note, the small sample size with

some variables of interest having missing data (i.e., impulsivity characteristics, dose

information), as well as the low base rate of violent offending, greatly precluded the

types of analyses that were able to be performed, prompting reliance on non-

parametric analyses which have less stringent criteria. Such sampling issues may

have also attenuated the statistical power available. In addition, due to the unique

nature of the data, an a priori decision was made that non-normal variables would be

recoded into categorical variables, and no correction was made to protect against

Type 1 error. The measurement protocols used may have impacted our results,

including the non-standardised UPPS response scale, a non-specific substance use

variable which may have attenuated the effects of specific drugs (Sweeney & Payne,

2012), and a criminal involvement scale with limited scope (i.e., beyond one month).

Furthermore, data collection relied on retrospective self-report which is vulnerable to

Page 145: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

145

memory decay and potential reporter bias, without any method to corroborate

substance use (i.e., urinalysis), benzodiazepine dose, crime involvement (i.e., official

data), or the accuracy and currency of reported mental health diagnoses. Indeed, a

proportion of the sample neglected to provide crime information (i.e., non-specified

‘other’ crime). In addition, the findings may have limited generality to individuals

with less complex presentations, and greater insight may have been afforded through

the use of a non-benzodiazepine using control group. It is noted that the observed

relationship may reflect increased dependence to benzodiazepines arising from

efforts to effectively manage pre-existing violent tendencies, or the occurrence of

aggression and agitation during benzodiazepine withdrawal (Votava, Kršiak,

Podhorná, & Miczek, 2001). It is further noted that benzodiazepine dependence was

also significantly associated with poly-substance use, indicating that other substances

may have influenced the identified relationship between benzodiazepine dependence

and violent crime. The findings may have limited generality to individuals with less

complex presentations.

While acknowledging these limitations, the current study adds to the limited

literature regarding the relationship between benzodiazepine use and violence in a

criminal justice sample. First, our examination of a community-based criminal

justice sample with regular benzodiazepine use and recent offending is unique within

the literature. Previous research has predominantly focused on healthy community

samples, clinical or substance using samples, or forensic samples in custodial settings

(Albrecht et al., 2014), greatly reducing the ecological validity of their findings.

Second, the research protocol offered numerous avenues of exploration, by way of

the range of data that was collected. Notably, 84.1% of the sample reported using

benzodiazepines in the day prior to committing an offence, and our data permitted

Page 146: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

146

specific examination of the roles of diazepam and alprazolam in violent offending.

With a less constrained sample size, the study had the potential to explore numerous

points of interest.

5.4.2 Implications and conclusions.

Recent literature, including a letter written regarding the benzodiazepine

acquisition practices of those included in the current database (Best, Wilson, Reed,

Lloyd, Eade, & Lubman, 2013), has demonstrated that benzodiazepines are being

frequently sought through illegal, or non-medically prescribed, means (Nielsen et al.,

2013). Of interest, although the majority of the current sample reported such a

preference, every individual who engaged in violence in the month prior to the index

offence reported preferring non-medically sourced benzodiazepines. This suggests

that people at risk for violence may be less likely to be identified at the point of

prescription, and intervention efforts should focus on management of the black-

market trade in benzodiazepines, with prevention efforts tailored towards (1)

enhancing prescription selectivity, (2) reducing large prescriptions, and (3) strategies

to reduce the ease of doctor and pharmacy shopping. Essentially, a two-tiered policy

approach involving enhanced control over the prescription of benzodiazepines and

greater regulation around the diversion of benzodiazepines onto the black market is

required. The latter directive may involve further up-scheduling of all

benzodiazepines to controlled substances.

The findings highlight the importance of adhering to recognized prescribing

protocols (e.g., Jones, Nielsen, Bruno, Frei, & Lubman, 2011), and addressing

reasons for benzodiazepine use (i.e., to manage negative affect). Our findings also

promote the use of psychological approaches that target mental health and adaptive

coping strategies (e.g., emotion identification and regulation techniques,

Page 147: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

147

consequential thinking and impulse control strategies) in violent offenders. Such

strategies can hopefully reduce the association between benzodiazepine consumption

and being detained for violent offending (Australian Institute of Criminology, 2013).

5.5 Chapter Summary

Research exploring the benzodiazepine-violence relationship is rare in

community samples with a recent criminal justice and benzodiazepine use history.

Investigation of such a sample identified risk factors of benzodiazepine dependence,

high alprazolam doses (i.e., above 4mg daily), sensation seeking, previous violence,

depression, and personality disorder. The current findings detail a complex picture of

psychiatric and impulsive functioning as influencing the relationship between

benzodiazepine (notably alprazolam) use and subsequent involvement in violent

crime. This picture highlights the need to take a holistic, multifaceted approach to the

prevention and intervention of benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour. As will

be discussed in greater detail in the following chapter, concurrent attention to

limiting the widespread diversion of benzodiazepines, addressing reasons for

benzodiazepines misuse, and promoting psychological approaches that target mental

health and adaptive coping strategies among violent offenders is required in order to

effect change and reduce the continued occurrence of benzodiazepine-related

violence.

Page 148: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

148

Chapter Six: Outcomes, Implications, and Future Directions

Benzodiazepines are widely prescribed, used, and diverted through the black

market. Yet, benzodiazepine use has been associated with an increased risk of harm,

behavioural disinhibition, and aggressive behaviour. It is this latter response which is

poorly understood, and has prompted further exploration within this thesis using both

general community and criminal justice samples. As identified through a systematic

review, the currently available literature is flawed and has limited explanatory power,

though benzodiazepine-related aggressive responses appear to be linked most closely

with alprazolam and diazepam use.

In a study with a community sample, benzodiazepine-related aggression was

more likely to be experienced by those who used alprazolam and exhibited persistent

approach tendencies, or motivational drive. It was posited that frustration

(in)tolerance and difficulties regulating impulses when pursuing desired goals may

influence aggressive responding. Examination of general and physical aggression

allowed for the comparison of predictive models in this sample, and it was observed

that for the general community, diazepam (used not to excess) may inhibit general

aggressive tendencies such as anger, hostility or verbal aggression, but may not

impact physically aggressive behaviour. Problematic use of diazepam (and

alprazolam), however, was associated with increased general and physical

aggression, highlighting the concerning sequelae that can result from problematic

benzodiazepine use or dependence.

In a study with an offender population, this response was associated with a

complex picture of emotional, impulse control and interpersonal difficulties. Violent

offenders were significantly more likely to present with benzodiazepine dependence,

alprazolam use at higher doses, depression and personality diagnoses, sensation

Page 149: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

149

seeking, and a history of violent behaviour than non-violent offenders. The findings

highlighted the need to take a holistic, multifaceted approach to the prevention and

intervention of benzodiazepine-related aggressive behaviour, and consider the

reasons underlying benzodiazepine use whilst promoting adaptive coping strategies.

Taken together, the systematic review and two studies showed alprazolam use

to pose a greater risk of subsequent aggression than diazepam use, and demonstrated

that intrapersonal factors can further our understanding of benzodiazepine-related

aggression. Specifically, the two empirical studies highlighted the role of impulse

control and maladaptive coping responses to negative affect in this response.

Although the above findings do require replication and extension through further

research, a number of pertinent clinical and forensic implications can be drawn from

these outcomes.

6.1 Clinical Implications

6.1.1 Role of alprazolam.

Alprazolam misuse has consistently been associated with problematic

sequelae. The research conducted as part of this thesis supports national and

international concern about alprazolam (Horyniak et al., 2012; Isbister et al., 2004;

Rintoul et al., 2013), by highlighting its association with benzodiazepine-related

aggression. However, the currently available findings cannot specify that it is the

psychophysiological effects of alprazolam that result in aggressive behaviour.

Instead, the link between alprazolam use and benzodiazepine-related aggression may

be due to the context in which alprazolam is frequently used (i.e., poly-substance use,

personality factors, high doses, unknown situational factors). Nevertheless, a positive

association has been consistently observed between alprazolam and this response,

Page 150: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

150

suggesting that those who consume alprazolam may be at an increased risk of

experiencing benzodiazepine-related aggression.

Alprazolam has recently been up-scheduled in Australia to a controlled

substance (Schedule 8), in an effort to reduce alprazolam access, use, and related

harms. However, as noted by Islam and colleagues (2014), it cannot be discounted

that re-scheduling may result in increased black market trade in alprazolam, or a rise

in misuse of a substitute benzodiazepine (or other substance). This will likely have

implications for ambulance attendants, emergency departments and substance use

treatment clinics, where clinicians should be aware of a possible rebound effect in

benzodiazepine-related overdoses, health complications and/or mortality. Prescribers

would also benefit by attending to the medicinal requests of patients who had

previously received alprazolam prescriptions. In addition, acquisitive crime to fund

black-market alprazolam use may increase, and border controls may see an increase

in attempts to traffic alprazolam and/or a substitute benzodiazepine. Therefore,

whilst it is important to continue monitoring the use of alprazolam, restricting its use,

and engaging in further well-controlled examination of this response following

alprazolam use, it is also imperative that policy approaches are broadened to the

regulation and monitoring of all benzodiazepines, so as to reduce this substitution

effect (Islam et al., 2014).

6.1.2 Prescribing policy.

Across both studies, benzodiazepine dependence was associated with

aggressive (or violent) behaviour in a proportion of participants. As introduced in

Chapter 5, the most appropriate course of action may involve a two-tiered policy

system for the regulation of benzodiazepines. It is recommended that such a strategy

would involve targeting the point of prescription whilst also targeting the point of

Page 151: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

151

diversion. Such dual focus enhances the likelihood of accessing those who are

unlikely to use health care services to obtain benzodiazepines.

The first tier, targeting the point of prescription, is recommended to involve a

number of strategies. These include increasing the selectivity of benzodiazepine

prescriptions, which may involve a review of the indications appropriate for

benzodiazepine treatment, reducing available prescription size and dosing strength

(i.e., Ibañez et al., 2013), increased training regarding prescription protocols (and

common side effects and risks associated with benzodiazepines), and greater

consequences for off-label prescribing of benzodiazepines. Off-label prescribing to

individuals already at increased risk of aggressive behaviour (i.e., violent offenders)

or in contexts more conducive to interpersonal violence (i.e., prison) is especially

concerning. The high dependence potential of benzodiazepines (i.e., physiological

dependence; O’Brien, 2005) further warrants the development of national regulations

regarding the upskilling or continuing professional development for benzodiazepine

prescribers, and emphasises the importance of non-pharmacological (i.e.,

psychological) approaches to treatment (Dobbin, 2014; Lader, 2014). By targeting

the point of prescription, it is expected that fewer inappropriate prescriptions (in dose

or indication) will be made, resulting in reduced access to benzodiazepines for

personal misuse, as well as a reduced market of prescribed benzodiazepines (Ibañez

et al., 2013). Although some psychiatrists (n = 20) already consider both

benzodiazepine factors (i.e., abuse and dependence potential) and person factors (i.e.,

suspicion of who takes the medication, history of abuse, feeling manipulated) when

prescribing benzodiazepines (Marienfeld, Tek, Diaz, Schottenfeld, & Chawarski,

2012), it is apparent that greater awareness (through enhanced regulation or training)

is required.

Page 152: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

152

The second tier, targeting the point of diversion, aims to further decrease the

ease of which benzodiazepines are diverted onto the black market. Strategies could

involve further up-scheduling of benzodiazepines, a nation-wide database to provide

real-time information to pharmacies regarding the dispensing of benzodiazepines,

and increased ramifications for those caught trafficking benzodiazepines or in

possession of large quantities of benzodiazepines. Community health promotion

strategies may be helpful in reaching individuals who are less likely to be accessed

through health centres (i.e., lower income; Ibañez et al., 2013), and offer

rehabilitation opportunities without negative consequences (i.e., misdemeanours for

illicit drug use). It is noted that each of the above recommendations requires notable

financial and personnel resources, and further research as recommended below may

assist in garnering support for the provision of such resources.

6.1.3 Poly-substance use.

A complicating factor in the regulation of benzodiazepines, especially

alprazolam, is the high rate of poly-substance use associated with benzodiazepine

misuse. As demonstrated in Chapter 4, benzodiazepines are often used to enhance the

effects of another drug, or to attenuate the negative effects of substance withdrawal.

Therein, drug and alcohol interventions may involve concurrent treatment of

benzodiazepine use and other substance use, and rely strongly on a comprehensive

functional assessment as to why the individual (mis)uses benzodiazepines.

6.1.4 Chicken-egg intervention.

Due to the absence of well-controlled longitudinal studies of this response,

the progression between intrapersonal factors, benzodiazepine misuse, and the

development of aggressive behaviour is currently unable to be determined.

Therefore, intervention for an individual who has experienced benzodiazepine-

Page 153: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

153

related aggressive behaviour will likely depend on a comprehensive individual

assessment. Such an assessment should inform the manner in which treatment is

provided, and the prioritisation of the various components. For example, underlying

anxiety may be prioritised as it leads to problematic benzodiazepine use, or

aggressive or hostile attitudes may be targeted as this prompts benzodiazepine use

and disinhibited violence, or benzodiazepine and other substance misuse may be

targeted as the cycle of use increases distress and agitation, culminating in aggressive

outbursts. Although the progression is still unclear in the general literature, core high

risk intrapersonal factors requiring assessment have, however, been indicated by the

current research (i.e., violent histories, sensation seeking, antisocial or borderline

traits, psychiatric distress, and persistent pursuit of goals).

Intrapersonal factors have long been argued to be important in the

understanding of benzodiazepine-related aggression. The findings contained within

this thesis support this premise, and highlight the importance of considering

emotional identification and regulation, including identification of prosocial,

solution-focused problem solving and coping strategies; impulse control, frustration

tolerance, and consequential thinking training; and interpersonal conflict resolution

training, when developing interventions following benzodiazepine-related

aggression. Such factors can be considered regardless of whether the intervention is

targeting underlying aggressive tendencies, problematic substance use, or psychiatric

health, and may indicate responsivity issues which need to be addressed. It has been

argued that effective substance use treatment needs to consider individual personality

factors (Staiger, Kambouropoulos, & Dawe, 2007), and it is argued that this premise

should extend to consideration of other intrapersonal factors (i.e., motivational

tendencies) and to other forms of intervention (i.e., violent offender treatment).

Page 154: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

154

Indeed, Davison and Janca’s (2012) review of the relationship between personality

and criminal behaviour highlight that effective risk assessment, management, and

treatment involves the integration of personality traits, substance misuse, and other

intrapersonal factors such as attitudes, beliefs, anger and arousal.

6.2 Theoretical Implications

This thesis is the first to apply the rRST to substance-related aggressive

behaviour, and the first to apply the theory to benzodiazepine use. Therefore, in the

absence of similar studies, comparisons of the current data to the relevant literature

relies on studies which explore general aggressive behaviour only. A number of

important conclusions can be drawn from such comparisons. First, BAS-Dr is

important in the understanding of aggressive behaviour. As discussed in Chapter 4,

this may be due to frustrative non-reward, increasing neurological activation

mimicking that of aggressive behaviour, the role of attitudes condoning the use of

aggression in order to achieve desired goals, or a combination of these factors.

However, by aligning with other violence research using the BIS/BAS scales, the

role of BAS-Dr in aggression has been supported, indicating that even in the

presence of intoxicating substances, underlying intrapersonal factors are important in

the treatment of violent offenders. As such, interventions targeting this motivational

tendency may involve frustration tolerance training, including impulse control, and

solution-focused coping, as well as challenging any beliefs or attitudes condoning the

use of aggression. Mindfulness or acceptance and commitment based strategies may

also be involved, in order to assist the client to more effectively manage goal

challenges.

Reasoning regarding the role of frustrative non-reward (and aggression-based

attitudes) is based on untested hypotheses, and therefore should be considered

Page 155: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

155

cautiously. In the absence of follow-up research, the current findings can only

highlight potentially relevant treatment targets for assessing and treating clinicians. It

is notable, however, that such considerations do align with the frustration-aggression

hypothesis (Dollard et al., 1939), and later expansions to the theory, that frustrations

generate aggressive behaviour only to the degree to which they arouse negative

affect (Berkowitz, 1989). According to Carver (2004), frustrative non-reward is

dependent on the person’s perception of the imminence of goal attainment.

Unfortunately, empirical investigation of the frustration-aggression hypothesis

cannot currently provide further insight into benzodiazepine-related aggression, as

the limited recent research (i.e., since 2000) explores bullying among Japanese

school girls (Wai-ming Tam & Taki, 2007), aggression post sporting losses (Priks,

2010), and a socio-economic argument of aggression (de Gaay Fortman, 2005); with

no apparent application of the hypothesis to situations involving substance use.

The role of affect regulation and coping capacity was in fact inferred by both

empirical studies, despite their methodological differences. As noted above, the

community sample study, through application of the rRST, posited that

benzodiazepine-related aggression involves a problematic response to negative

affective experiences (i.e., aggression in response to frustration or anger). Similarly,

the findings from the criminal justice study prompted the argument that violent

offending reflected impulsive action (urgency) in response to negative symptoms of

distress. Although based on different measures and outcome variables, the similarity

in results suggests that benzodiazepine-related aggression may involve problematic

responding to negative affective experiences, highlighting the importance of

emotional regulation and coping strategies, including problem-focused strategies, in

both violent and substance use intervention. Difficulties regulating emotion have

Page 156: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

156

frequently been linked with both problematic substance use (e.g., Berking &

Wupperman, 2012) and violence (e.g., Roberton, Daffern, Bucks, 2015); however the

current findings suggest that enhanced awareness and prioritisation of such treatment

targets may reduce the risk of benzodiazepine-related aggression recurring.

Although problematic responses to negative affect appears to be consistent

across the two studies, it is notable that divergent findings were observed in relation

to sensation seeking. Fun seeking (BAS-FS) negatively influenced the prediction of

physical aggression in Chapter 4 (albeit non-significantly), whilst sensation seeking

was positively associated with violent crime in Chapter 5. It is acknowledged that the

studies used different measures of sensation seeking and aggression, and it can be

argued that BAS-FS is not a pure measure of sensation seeking, instead also tapping

into dysfunctional impulsivity (Leone & Russo, 2009; Segarra et al., 2014), thus

likely explaining the divergence. However, it is interesting that the direction of the

effect differs between the studies, especially as BAS-FS has been positively

associated with physically aggressive behaviour (Harmon-Jones, 2003; Smits &

Kuppens, 2005), and has predicted both direct and indirect aggression in a sample of

pharmacy workers (Cooper et al., 2008). Sensation seeking tendencies have also

been positively associated with aggression in both general community (Derefinko et

al., 2011; Dvorak et al., 2013; Miller, Zeichner, & Wilson, 2012) and violent

offender samples (Shoham, Askenasy, Rahav, Chard, & Addl, 1989). It is therefore

tempting to infer that benzodiazepine-related aggression operates differently to

general aggression, especially given that the findings relating to BAS-FS further

support the role of persistent, planned approach behaviour in benzodiazepine-related

aggression (in the community sample), and that rash impulsivity or sensation seeking

tendencies are only relevant in antisocial or violent (i.e., criminal justice) samples.

Page 157: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

157

However, given the methodological inconsistencies leading to this inference, further

research comparing violent offender and general community samples on this

response, using a standardised measure of sensation seeking, is required.

6.3 Forensic Implications

Disinhibition potentially due to benzodiazepine misuse has been previously

suggested to result in criminal justice system involvement (Redman, 1994). Indeed,

offenders have attributed their criminal, and violent, behaviour to the

psychopharmacological effects of benzodiazepines (Forsyth et al., 2011; Payne &

Gaffney, 2012), though limited corroboration has been provided for such

physiological effects. However, it is likely that the current, and future, academic

interest in benzodiazepine-related aggression will result in attempts to seek decreased

culpability for violent offending. It is emphasised that the aim of this research is not

to excuse violence, or to provide an avenue under which a violent offender escapes

punishment for a violent crime. Instead, the aim is to help inform whether the violent

offender requires targeted treatment relating to his or her use of benzodiazepines, and

whether there may be responsivity factors or intervention targets which may improve

the offender’s rehabilitative prognosis (i.e., improve offender wellbeing, reduce risk

of reoffending, improve community safety). Consideration of this research, indeed in

the context of diminished responsibility due to intoxication, may enhance the

awareness of the professionals receiving the violent offender pre- or post-sentence.

Such awareness may, for example, inform decisions regarding further prescription of

benzodiazepines, especially in the presence of related risk factors (i.e., psychological

distress, impulsivity).

Similar to the discussion above regarding clinical intervention planning, the

outcomes of this research may aid discussion and planning about the offender’s

Page 158: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

158

rehabilitation needs. This would hopefully include an assessment of whether

continuing benzodiazepine treatment is the most appropriate option for the offender,

and tailoring of intervention techniques to meet responsivity factors or treatment

targets. This will culminate in recommendations regarding the priority of violent

offender rehabilitation or substance use or mental health treatment. In addition, it is

acknowledged that such consideration may inform the inclusion of specific

conditions (i.e., excluding or enforcing certain intervention pathways) on high risk

violent offenders’ parole orders, where parole is successfully attained. It would be

highly recommended, based on findings of Chapter 5, that the prescription of

benzodiazepines to violent offenders, or within highly violent contexts (i.e.,

incarceration), be limited and follow only comprehensive assessment and exhaustion

of alternative treatment methods.

6.4 Future Research

Due to the current state of related literature, there are a number of

methodological improvements which would assist our understanding of

benzodiazepine-related aggression (e.g., Albrecht & Staiger, in press). Of note, well-

designed and controlled longitudinal research would prove valuable in determining

true contributors to this response. In addition, regardless of study design (i.e., cross-

sectional, experimental), the use of appropriate control groups would enhance our

ability to be confident in findings. For example, a four-group design using

violent/non-violent and benzodiazepine using/non-using groups would be optimal

(i.e., B+V+, B+V-, B-V-, B-V+). In addition, larger samples, with a focus on gender

equality, would improve the generality of the literature base, and provide greater

foundations on which to develop prescribing and regulatory policies. Larger samples

will also provide additional statistical power in which more comprehensive and

Page 159: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

159

rigorous statistical modelling can be used. Such modelling ability is imperative for

the testing of the various mechanisms posited throughout this thesis (i.e., the role of

frustrative non-reward, BIS/BAS interactions, or impulsive action in response to

depressive symptoms). As noted in Chapter 1, misuse of benzodiazepines is an

international concern, and researchers could endeavour to explore benzodiazepine-

related aggression cross-culturally.

A core limitation of the current research and prior studies is the ability to

effectively measure, and statistically control, other substance use. Future research

could benefit by systematically exploring the role of substances such as heroin,

marijuana, and alcohol (i.e., closely associated with benzodiazepine use) in

benzodiazepine-related aggression. Physiological research could also provide insight

into how the various substances in combination with benzodiazepines affect an

individual’s responding. In addition, systematic examination of the different

benzodiazepines and dosing schedules requires further attention. As noted in Chapter

1, and replicated in the current research, alprazolam and diazepam have received the

most empirical attention, likely due to their frequent use in both community, clinical

and forensic populations. Further examination of the less-frequently used

benzodiazepines is therefore required. Such examination would greatly benefit from

the use of appropriate methods to corroborate self-report (i.e., urinalysis, systematic

testing (using double blind methods) of various doses). Such techniques would

increase the level of control of related studies, and improve our confidence in relying

on research findings.

Building on Lion and colleagues’ (1975) premise, an aspect which has been

overlooked in the current literature is the role of context in understanding this

response. Aggression is by definition an interpersonal behaviour (Baron &

Page 160: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

160

Richardson, 1994), and is highly influenced by situational factors (Anderson &

Bushman, 2002). However, no research has explored the context or situational

factors associated with benzodiazepine-related aggression, or how situational factors

may interact with intrapersonal responding and benzodiazepines (and other

substance) use (Lion et al., 1975). Blending quantitative and qualitative approaches

would likely provide greater understanding of these factors.

Finally, there are a number of additional intrapersonal factors which have yet

to be investigated in the benzodiazepine-aggression literature. Based on the current

findings, and what is understood about violent behaviour in general (i.e., the general

aggression model; Anderson & Bushman, 2002), individual goals, expectations,

attitudes, and desires seem important. A number of questions could be asked,

especially regarding perceptions of how substance use may vary the acceptability of

aggressive behaviour, and tested through well-controlled experimental studies.

As demonstrated throughout this thesis, the benzodiazepine-aggression

literature base has a number of limitations, and the above areas for further research

are suggested in order to reduce such flaws. It is hoped that further research adhering

to the above points and following the above questions will improve the statistical and

methodological rigour of the related literature base, and increase the specificity and

explanatory power of related outcomes. This includes the replication and expansion

of the findings included in this thesis.

6.5 Conclusion

Much remains to be understood about benzodiazepine-related aggressive

behaviour. This thesis has highlighted the increased risk that alprazolam poses in the

commission of violent or aggressive behaviour, though it is currently unclear

whether this is due to the physiological effects of alprazolam, or the increased

Page 161: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

161

disinhibition which may result from using alprazolam in combination with other

substances. It is also important to consider whether individuals who engage in

aggression merely have a tendency to select alprazolam over other benzodiazepine

options, possibly for its fast-acting effects, or whether the aggressive response may

occur during the withdrawal phase of alprazolam abuse. This thesis has also

highlighted the importance of intrapersonal factors in understanding this response,

and will hopefully inspire further research into the proposed interaction between

benzodiazepine, intrapersonal, and situational factors (Lion et al., 1975). In order to

reduce the incidence of this response, it is necessary to increase the regulation of

benzodiazepine prescription and diversion onto the black market, and a two-tiered

prescribing policy has been suggested. Violent offender and substance use treatment

programs should explore notable intrapersonal factors in order to determine their

relevance as responsivity factors and/or treatment targets. Although risks may differ

between violent offenders and the general public, attention should be paid to

impulsive tendencies in response to frustration or negative affect, persistent

motivational tendencies, violent histories, problematic personality traits and

disrupted interpersonal functioning, and regular, heavy consumption of alprazolam,

especially in the context of poly-substance use. It is through attention to the above

risks and recommendations that the incidence of benzodiazepine-related aggression

may be reduced, reducing the financial and personal injury toll on the community,

and improving community safety.

Page 162: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

162

References

Adams, J. B., Heath, A. J., Young, S. E., Hewitt, J. K., Corley, R. P., & Stallings, M.

C. (2003). Relationships between personality and preferred substance and

motivations for use among adolescent substance abusers. American Journal

of Drug & Alcohol Abuse, 29(3), 691-712. doi: 10.1081/ADA-120023465

Afifi, T. O., Henriksen, C. A., Asmundson, G. J. G., & Sareen, J. (2012).

Victimization and perpetration of intimate partner violence and substance use

disorders in a nationally representative sample. The Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease, 200(8), 684-691. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182613f64

Albrecht, B., & Staiger, P. K. (in press). Response to ‘benzodiazepine use and

aggressive behaviour’. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry.

Albrecht, B., Staiger, P., Hall, K., Miller, P., Best, D., & Lubman, D. I. (2014).

Benzodiazepine use and aggressive behaviour: A systematic review.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 48(12), 1096-1114. doi:

10.1177/0004867414548902.

Alprazolam. (September, 2013). In MIMS. Retrieved from www.mims.com.au.

American Psychiatric Association (2014). Diagnostic and statistical manual of

mental disorders (Fifth Ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.

Anderson, P. D., & Bokor, G. (2012). Forensic aspects of drug-induced violence.

Journal of Pharmacy Practice, 25(1), 41-49. doi:

10.1177/0897190011431150

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (1997). External validity of “trivial”

experiments: The case of laboratory aggression. Review of General

Psychology, 1, 19-41.

Page 163: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

163

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of

Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.

Andrews, G., & Slade, T. (2001). Interpreting scores on the Kessler Psychological

Distress Scale (K10). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health,

26(6), 494-497.

Angkaw, A. C., Ross, B. S., Pittman, J. O. E., Kelada, A. M. Y., Valencerina, M. A.

M., & Baker, D. G. (2013). Post-traumatic stress disorder, depression, and

aggression in OEF/OIF veterans. Military Medicine, 178(10), 1044-1050. doi:

10.7205/MILMED-D-13-00061

Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1995).

Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the

depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample.

Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176-181.

Ashton, C. H. (2002). Benzodiazepines: How they work and how to withdraw.

Retrieved from http://www.benzo.org.au/manual/contents.htm

Australian Bureau of Statistics (February, 2014). 4519.0 - Recorded crime –

offenders, 2012-2013. Retrieved from

http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/by%20Subject/4519.0~201

2-13~Main%20Features~In%20this%20issue~2

Australian Crime Commission (2014). Illicit Drug Data Report 2012-2013.

Retrieved from

https://www.crimecommission.gov.au/publications/intelligence-

products/illicit-drug-data-report/illicit-drug-data-report-2012-13

Page 164: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

164

Australian Institute of Criminology. (May, 2013). Australian crime: Facts & figures:

2012. Canberra: Author.

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2011). 2010 National Drug Strategy

Household Survey report (Drug statistics series no. 25, Category no. PHE

145). Canberra: AIHW.

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2013a). Alcohol and other drug

treatment services in Australia 2011-12 (Drug Treatment Series No. 21).

Canberra, ACT: Author.

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2013b). The health of Australia’s

prisoners 2012. Canberra, ACT: Author.

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. (2014). National drug strategy household

survey detailed report 2013 (Drug Statistic Series No. 28). Canberra, ACT:

Author.

Avila,C. (2001). Distinguishing BIS-mediated and BAS-mediated disinhibition

mechanisms: A comparison of disinhibition models of Gray (1981, 1987) and

of Patterson and Newman (1993). Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 80(2), 311-324.

Baron, R. A., & Richardson, D. R. (1994). Human aggression (2nd Ed.). New York:

Plenum.

Beaver, J. D., Lawrence, A. D., Passamonti, L., & Calder, A. J. (2008).Appetitive

motivation predicts the neural response to facial signals of aggression. The

Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2719-2725. doi:

10.1523/jNEUROSCI.0033-08.2908

Page 165: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

165

Ben-Porath, D. D., & Taylor, S. P. (2002). The effects of diazepam (Valium) and

aggressive disposition on human aggression: An experimental investigation.

Addictive Behaviors, 27(2), 167-177. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00175-1

Bennett, T. H., Holloway, K. R., Brookman, F., Parry, O., & Gorden, C. (2014).

Explaining prescription drug misuse among students from a widening access

university: The role of techniques of neutralization. Drugs: Education,

Prevention & Policy, 21(3), 189-196. doi: 10.3109/09687637.2013.870980

Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health:

Recent finding, current challenges and future directions. Current Opinion in

Psyhaitry, 25(2), 128-134.

Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Gable, S. L. (2009). BIS, BAS, and response

conflict: Testing predictions of the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory.

Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 586-591. doi:

10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.015

Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and

reformulation. OPsychological Bulletin, 106(1), 59-73. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.106.1.59

Berman, M. E., & Taylor, S. (1995). The effects of triazolam on aggression in men.

Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3(4), 411-416.

Best, D., Wilson, A., Reed, M., & Harney, A. (2012). Alprazolam study:

Understanding the role of benzodiazepine use in crime. Unpublished

manuscript, Turning Point Alcohol & Drug Centre, Melbourne, Australia.

Best, D., Wilson, A., Reed, M., Lloyd, B., Eade, A., & Lubman, D. I. (2013).

Alprazolam and wider benzodiazepine misuse in Australia – Sources of

Page 166: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

166

supply and evidence of harms. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 47, 94-95. doi: 10.1177/0004867412453774

Binder, R. L. (1987). Three case reports of behavioral disinhibition with clonazepam.

General Hospital Psychiatry, 9, 151-153

Bisaga, A. (2008). Benzodiazepines and other sedatives and hypnotics. In M.

Galanter, & H. D. Kleber (Eds.), The American psychiatric publishing

textbook of substance abuse treatment (4th Ed.). Arlington, VA: American

Psychiatric Publishing.

Björkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal and indirect aggression: A

review of the research. Sex Roles, 30(3-4), 177-188.

Bjørner, T., Tvete, I. F., Aursnes, I., & Skomedal, T. (2013). Dispensing of

benzodiazepines and Z drugs by Norwegian pharmacies 2004-2012. Tidsskr

Nor Laegeforen, 133(20), 2149-2153. doi: 10.4045/tidsskr.11.0543

Bond, A. J. (1998). Drug-induced behavioural disinhibition: Incidence, mechanisms

and therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs, 9(1), 41-57.

Bond, A. J., Curran, H. V., Bruce, M. S, O’Sullivan, G., & Shine, P. (1995).

Behavioural aggression in panic disorder after 9 weeks’ treatment with

alprazolam. Journal of Affective Disorders, 35(3), 117-123.

Bond, A., & Lader, M. (1988). Differential effects of oxazepam and lorazepam on

aggressive responding. Psychopharmacology, 95, 368-373.

Bond, A. J., & Silveira, J. C. (1993). The combination of alprazolam and alcohol on

behavioral aggression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 54(5), 30-39.

Page 167: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

167

Booth, C., & Hasking, P. (2009). Social anxiety and alcohol consumption: the role of

alcohol expectancies and reward sensitivity. Addictive Behaviors, 34(9), 730-

736. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.04.010

Bradford, D., & Payne, J. (2012). Illicit drug use and property offending among

police detainees. Sydney: NSW Bureau of Crime Statistics and Research.

Bramness, J. G., Skurtveit, S., & Mørland, J. (2006). Flunitrazepam: Psychomotor

impairment, agitation and paradoxical reactions. Forensic Science

International, 159, 83-91. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2005.06.009

Breen, C. L., Degenhardt, L. J., Roxburgh, A. D., Bruno, R. B., Jenkinson, R. (2004).

The effects of restricting publicly subsidised temazepam capsules on

benzodiazepine use among injecting drug users in Australia. The Medical

Journal of Australia, 181(6), 300-304.

Brown, C. R. (1978). The use of benzodiazepines in prison populations. The Journal

of Clinical Psychiatry, 39(3), 219-222.

Burgio, L. D., Reynolds, C. F., Janosky, J. E., Perei, J., Thornton, J. E., & Hohman,

M. J. (1992). A behavioral microanalysis of the effects of haloperidol and

oxazepam in demented psychogeriatric inpatients. International Journal of

Geriatric Psychiatry, 7, 253-262.

Buss, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. New York: Wiley.

Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. P. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459.

Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system.

Emption, 4(1), 3-22. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.3

Page 168: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

168

Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation,

and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS

scales. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67(2), 319-333.

Chavant, F., Favrelière, S., Lafay-Chebassier, C., Plazanet, C., & Pérault-Pochat, M.

C. (2011). Memory disorders associated with consumption of drugs:

Updating through a case/noncase study in the French PharmacoVigilance

Database. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 72(6), 898-904. doi:

10.1111/j.1365-2125.2011.04009.x

Ch’ng, C. W., Fitzgerald, M., Gerostamoulos, J., Cameron, P., Bui D., Drummer, O.

H., … & Odell, M. (2007). Drug use in motor vehicle drivers presenting to an

Australian, adult major trauma centre. Emergency Medicine Australasia, 19,

359-365. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-6723.2007.00958.x

Chereji, S. V., Pintea, S., & David, D. (2012). The relationship of anger and

cognitive distortions with violence in violent offenders’ population: A meta-

analytic review. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to a Legal

Context, 4(1), 59-77.

Cherek, D. R., Spiga, R., Roache, J. D., & Cowan, K. A. (1991). Effects of triazolam

on human aggressive, escape and point-maintained responding.

Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 40(4), 835-839.

Cherek, D. R., Steinberg, J. L., & Kelly, T. H. (1987). Effects of diazepam on human

laboratory aggression: Correlations with alcohol effects and hostility

measures. NIDA Research Monograph, 76, 95-101.

Cherek, D. R., Steinberg, J. L., Kelly, T. H., Robinson, D. E., & Spiga, R. (1990).

Effects of acute administration of diazepam and d-amphetamine on

Page 169: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

169

aggressive and escape responding of normal male subjects.

Psychopharmacology, 100, 173-181.

Cherek, D. R., Lane, S. D., Dougherty, D. M., Moeller, F. G., & White, S. (2000).

Laboratory and questionnaire measures of aggression among female parolees

with violent and nonviolent histories. Aggressive Behavior, 26(4), 291-307.

doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(2000)

Cole, J. O., & Kando, J. C. (1993). Adverse behavioral events reported in patients

taking alprazolam and other benzodiazepines. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,

54(10), 49-63.

Cooper, A., Gomez, R., & Buck, E. (2008). The relationships between the BIS and

BAS, anger and responses to anger. Personality and Individual Differences,

44, 403-413. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.005

Coroners Court of Victoria. (2013). Coroners Prevention Unit – Submission to

Therapeutic Goods Association – Reschedule of benzodiazepines. Retrieved

from

http://www.coronerscourt.vic.gov.au/find/publications/coroners+prevention+

unit+submission+to+therapeutic+goods+association+reschedule+of+benzodi

azepines

Corr, P. J. (2002). J. A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative

nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding

stimuli. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1247-1253.

Corr, P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neuroscience

and Biobehavioural Reviews, 28, 317-332. doi:

10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.01.005

Page 170: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

170

Corr, P. J. (Ed.) (2008). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality.

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Corr, P. J. (2009). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. In P. J. Corr,

& G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology

(pp. 347-376). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Corr, P. J., & Perkins, A. M. (2006). The role of theory in the psychophysiology of

personality: From Ivan Pavlov to Jeffrey Gray. International Journal of

Psychophysiology, 62, 367-376. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.005

Coskunpinar, A., Dir, A. L., & Cyders, M. A. (2013). Multidimensionality in

impulsivity and alcohol use: A meta-analysis using the UPPS model of

impulsivity. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 37(9), 1441-

1450. doi: 10.1111/acer.12131

Cowdry, R., & Gardner, D. L. (1988). Pharmacotherapy of borderline personality

disorder. Alprazolam, carbamazepine, trifluoperazine, and tranylcypromine.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(2), 111-119.

Crawford, J., Cayley, C., Lovibond, P. F., Wilson, P. H., & Hartley, C. (2011).

Percentile norms and accompanying interval estimates from an Australian

general adult population sample for self-report mood scales (BAI, BDI,

CRSD, CES-D, DASS, DASS-21, STAI-X, STAI-Y, SRDS, and SRAS).

Australian Psychologist, 46, 3-14. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-9544.2010.00003.x

Dåderman, A. M., & Edman, G. (2001). Flunitrazepam abuse and personality

characteristics in male forensic psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research,

103(1), 27-42.

Dåderman, A. M., Fredriksson, B., Kristiansson, M., Nilsson, L., & Lidberg, L.

(2002). Violent behavior, impulsive decision-making, and anterograde

Page 171: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

171

amnesia while intoxicated with flunitrazepam and alcohol or other drugs: A

case study in forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of the American Academy

of Psychiatry and the Law, 30(2), 238-251.

Dåderman, A. M., Edman, G., Meurling, A. W., Levander, S., & Kristiansson, M.

(2012). Flunitrazepam intake in male offenders. Nordic Journal of

Psychiatry, 66, 131-140. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2010.522730

Dåderman, A. M., & Lidberg, L. (1999). Flunitrazepam (Rohypnol) abuse in

combination with alcohol causes premeditated, grievous violence in male

juvenile offenders. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and

the Law, 27(1), 83-99.

Dåderman, A. M., Stridlund, H., Wiklund, N., Fredriksen, S., & Lidberg, L. (2003).

The importance of a urine sample in persons intoxicated with flunitrazepam-

legal issues in a forensic psychiatric case study of a serial murder. Forensic

Science International, 137, 21-27. doi: 10.1016/S0379-0738(03)00273-1

Darke, S., Hall, W., Wodak, A., Heather, N., & Ward, J. (1992). Development and

validation of a multi-dimensional instrument for assessing outcome of

treatment among opiate users: the Opiate Treatment Index. British Journal of

Addiction, 87, 733-742.

Darke, S., & Ross, J. (1994). The use of benzodiazepines among regular

amphetamine users. Addiction, 89, 1683-1690.

Darke, S., Ross, J., Mills, K., Teesson, M., Williamson, A., & Havard, A. (2010).

Benzodiazepine use among heroin users: Baseline use, current use, and

clinical outcome. Drug and Alcohol Review, 29, 250-255. doi: 10.111/j.1465-

3362.2009.00101.x

Page 172: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

172

Dassanayake, T., Michie, P., Carter, G., & Jones, A. (2011). Effects of

benzodiazepines, antidepressants and opioids on driving. Drug Safety, 34(2),

125-156.

Davison, S. & Janca, A. (2012). Personality disorder and criminal behaviour: What is

the nature of the relationship? Current Opinion in Psychaitry, 25(1), 39-45.

doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834d18f0

Dawson, D. (1997). Alcohol, drugs, fighting and suicide attempt/ideation. Addiction

Research, 5(6), 451-472.

de Almeida, R. M. M., Saft, D. M., Rosa, M. M., & Miczek, K. A. (2010).

Flunitrazepam in combination with alcohol engenders high levels of

aggression in mice and rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 95(3),

292-297. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.02.004

de Gaay Fortman, B. (2005). Violence among peoples in the light of human

frustration and aggression. European Journal of Pharmacology, 526(1-3), 2-

8.

de las Cuevas, C., Sanz, E. J., de la Fuente, J. A., Padilla, J., Berenguer, J. C. (2000).

The severity of dependence scale (SDS) as screening test for benzodiazepine

dependence: SDS validation study. Addiction, 95(2), 242-250. doi:

10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.95224511.x

DeMaris, A. (2004). Regression with social data: Modeling continuous and limited

response variables. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.

Derefinko, K., DeWall, C. N., Metze, A. V., Walsh, E. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2011).

Do different facets of impulsivity predict different types of aggression?

Aggressive Behavior, 37, 223-233. doi: 10.1002/ab.20387

Page 173: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

173

DeWall, C. N., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). The general aggression

model: Theoretical extensions to violence. Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 245-

258. doi: 10.1037/a0023842

Diazepam. (March, 2013). In MIMS. Retrieved from www.mims.com.au.

Dissabandara, L. O., Loxton, N. J., Dias, S. R., Daglish, M., & Stadlin, A. (2012).

Testing the fear and anxiety distinction in the BIS/BAS scales in community

and heroin-dependent samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 52,

888-892. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.023

Dobbin, M. (2014). Pharmaceutical drug misuse in Australia. Australian Prescriber,

37(3), 79-81.

Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N., Mowrer, H., & Sears, R. (1939). Frustration and

aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Drugs and Crime Prevention Committee (2007). Inquiry into the misuse/abuse of

benzodiazepines and other forms of pharmaceutical drugs in Victoria: Final

report. Retrieved from

http://www.parliament.vic.gov.au/dcpc/inquires/inquiry/233

Dutton, D., & White, K. (2012). Attachment insecurity and intimate partner violence.

Aggression & Violent Behavior, 17(5), 475-481.

Dvorak, R. D., Pearson, M. R., & Kuvaas, N. J. (2013). The five-factor model of

impulsivity-like traits and emotional lability in aggressive behavior.

Aggressive Behavior, 39(3), 222-228. doi: 10.1002/ab.21474

Essman, W. B. (1978). Benzodiazepines and aggressive behavior. Modern Problems

of Pharmacopsychiatry, 13, 13-28.

Page 174: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

174

Evans, D. (2003). Hierarchy of evidence: a framework for ranking evidence

evaluating healthcare interventions. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 12(1), 77-

84. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2702.2003.00662.x

Farinde, A. (May, 2014). Benzodiazepine equivalency table. Retrieved from

http://emedicine.medscape.com/article/2172250-overview

Fatséas, M., Lavie, E., Denis, C., & Auriacombe, M. (2009). Self-percieved

motivation for benzodiazepine use and behaviour related to benzodiazepine

use in opiate-dependent patients. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment,

37(4), 407-411. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2009.03.006

Feingold, A., Kerr, D. C. R., & Capaldi, D. M. (2008). Associations of substance use

problems with intimate partner violence for at-risk men in long-term

relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(3), 429-438. doi:

10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.429

Feldman, P. E. (1962). An analysis of the efficacy of diazepam. Journal of

Neuropsychiatry, 3, 62-67.

Ferguson, C. J., & Rueda, S. M. (2009). Examining the validity of the modified

Taylor competitive reaction time test of aggression. Journal of Experimental

Criminology, 5(2), 121-137. doi: 10.1007/s11292-009-9069-5

Ferguson, C. J., Smith, S., Miller-Stratton, S., Fritz, S., & Heinrich, E. (2008).

Aggression in the laboratory: Problems with the validity of the modified

Taylor competitive reaction time test as a measure of aggression in media

violence studies. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 17(1), 118-

132.

Page 175: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

175

Ferrari, P. F., Parmigiani, S., Rodgers, R. J., Palanza, P. (1997). Differential effects

of chlordiazepoxide on aggressive behavior in male mice: the influence of

social factors. Psychopharmacology, 134(3), 258-266.

Field, A. P. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS: and sex and drugs and rock

‘n’ roll (3rd Ed.). London: Sage publications.

Forsyth, A. J. M., Khan, F., & Mckinlay, B. (2011). Diazepam, alcohol use and

violence among male young offenders: ‘The devil’s mixture.’ Drugs,

Education, Prevention & Policy, 18(6), 468-476.

French, A. P. (1989). Dangerously aggressive behavior as a side effect of alprazolam

(letter). American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 276.

Friedman, A. S. (1998). Substance use/abuse as a predictor to illegal and violent

behaviour: a review of the relevant literature. Aggression and Violent

Behaviour, 3(4), 339-355.

Friedman, A. S., Terras, A., & Glassman, K. (2003). The differential disinhibition

effect of marijuana use on violent behaviour: A comparison of this effect on a

conventional, non-delinquent group versus a delinquent or deviant group.

Journal of Addictive Diseases, 22(3), 63-77. doi: 10.1300/J069v22n03_06

Fry, C., Smith, B., Bruno, R., O’Keefe, B., & Miller, P. (2007). Benzodiazepine and

pharmaceutical opioid misuse and their relationship to crime: An

examination of illicit prescription drug markets in Melbourne, Hobart and

Darwin (Monograph series no. 21). Hobart, Tasmania: National Drug Law

Enforcement Research Fund.

Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2003). The performance

of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the

Page 176: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

176

Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. Psychological

Medicine, 33(2), 357-362. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006700

Gantner, A. B., & Taylor, S. P. (1988). Human physical aggression as a function of

diazepam. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14(3), 479-484.

García-León, A., Reyes, G. A., Vila, J., Pérez, N., Robles, H., & Ramos, M. M.

(2002). The Aggression Questionnaire: A validation study in student samples.

The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 5(1), 45-53.

Gardner, D. L., & Cowdry, R. (1985). Alprazolam-induced dyscontrol in borderline

personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 98-100.

Gardos, G. (1980). Disinhibition of behavior by antianxiety drugs. Psychosomatics,

21(12), 1025-1026.

Giancola, P. R., & Parrott, D. J. (2005). Differential effects of past-year stimulant

and sedative drug use on alcohol-related aggression. Addictive Behaviors, 30,

1535-1554. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.02.011

Gilbert, F. & Daffern, M. (2011). Illuminating the relationship between personality

disorder and violence: Contributions of the general aggression model.

Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 230-244. doi: 10.1037/a0024089

Gillies, D., & O’Brien, L. (2006). Interpersonal violence and mental illness: A

literature review. Contemporary Nurse, 21(2), 277-286. doi:

10.5172.conu.2006.21.2.277

Golomb, B. A., Cortez-Perez, M., Jaworski, B. A., Mednick, S., & Dimsdale, J.

(2007). Point subtraction aggression paradigm: Validity of a brief schedule of

use. Violence and Victims, 22(1), 95-103.

Page 177: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

177

Gossop, M., & Darke, S. (1995). The severity of dependence scale (SDS):

Psychometric properties of the SDS in English and Australian samples of

heroin, cocaine and amphetamine users. Addiction, 90(5), 607-614.

Gourley, S. L., DeBold, J. F., Yin, W., Cook, J., & Mizek, K .A. (2005).

Benzodiazepines and heightened aggressive behavior in rates: reduction by

GABA A/a1 receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology, 178(2), 232-240.

doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1987-3

Graham, K., Bernards, S., Flynn, A., Tremblay, P. F., & Wells, S. (2012). Does the

relationship between depression and intimiate partner aggression vary by

gender, victim-perpetrator role, and aggression severity? Violence and

Victims, 27(5), 730-743. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.27.5.730

Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of

the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gray J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2003). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry

into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Grych, J., & Swan, S. (2012). Towards a more comprehensive understanding of

interpersonal violence: Introduction to the special issue on interconnections

among different types of violence. Psychology of Violence, 2(2), 105-110.

doi: 10.1037/a0027616

Haggård-Grann, U., Hallqvist, J., Långström, N., & Möller, J. (2006). The role of

alcohol and drugs in triggering criminal violence: A case-crossover study.

Addiction, 101, 100-108. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01293.x

Page 178: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

178

Hakansson, A., Schlyter, F., & Berglund, M. (2011). Associations between

polysubstance use and psychiatric problems in a criminal justice population

in Sweden. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 118, 5-11. doi:

10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.02.014

Haller, R. M., & Deluty, R. H. (1990). Characteristics of psychiatric inpatients who

assault staff severely. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178(8),

536-537.

Hamad, A. E., Al-Ghadban, A., Carvounis, C. P., Soliman, E., & Coritsidis, G. N.

(2000). Predicting the need for medical intensive care monitoring in drug-

overdosed patients. Journal of Intensive Care Medicine, 15(6), 321-328.

Hammersley, R., & Pearl, S. (1997). Temazepam misuse, violence and disorder.

Addiction Research, 5(3), 213-222.

Harmon-Jones, E. (2003). Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personality

and Individual Differences, 35, 995-1005. doi: 10.1016/S0191-

8869(02)00313-6

Harmon-Jones, E., & Peterson, C. K. (2008). Effect of trait and state approach

motivation on aggressive inclinations. Journal of Research in Personality, 42,

1381-1385. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.05.001

Harris, J. A. (1997). A further evaluation of the Aggression Questionnaire: Issues of

validity and reliability. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35(11), 1047-1053.

Hatfield, J., & Dula, C. S. (2014). Impulsivity and physical aggression: Examining

the moderating role of anxiety. American Journal of Psychology, 127(2), 233-

243. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0233

Page 179: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

179

Havens, J. R., Walker, R., & Leukefeld, C. G. (2010). Benzodiazepine use among

rural prescription opioids users in a community-based study. Journal of

Addiction Medicine, 4(3), 137-139. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181c4bfd3

Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the depression

anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a

large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 227-

239. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

Heym, N., Ferguson, E., & Lawrence, C. (2008). An evaluation of the relationship

between Gray’s revised RST and Eysenck’s PEN: Distinguishing BIS and

FFFS in Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual

Differences, 45(8), 709-715. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.013

Hoaken, P. N. S., & Stewart, S. H. (2003). Drugs of abuse and the elicitation of

human aggressive behaviour. Addictive Behaviors, 28(9), 1533-1554. doi:

10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.033

Hollingworth S. A., & Siskind, D. J. (2010). Anxiolytic, hypnotic and sedative

medication use in Australia. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Safety, 19(3), 280-

288.

Home Office Statistics. (2012). Drug misuse declared: Findings from the 2011/12

Crime Survey for England and Wales (2nd Ed.). London: Author.

Horvath, L. S., Millch, R., Lynam, D., Leukefeld, C., & Clayton, R. (2004).

Sensation seeking and substance use: A cross-lagged panel design. Individual

Differences Research, 2(3), 175-183.

Horyniak, D., Reddel, S., Quinn, B., & Dietze, P. (2012). The use of alprazolam by

people who inject drugs in Melbourne, Australia. Drug & Alcohol Review,

31(4), 585-590. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00381.x.

Page 180: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

180

Hosie, J., Gilbert, F., Simpson, K., & Daffern, M. (2014). An examination of the

relationship between personality and aggression using the general aggression

and five factor models. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 189-196. doi:

10.1002/ab.21510

Howard, R. C. (2011). The quest for excitement: a missing link between personality

disorder and violence? The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology,

22(5), 692-705. doi: 10.1080/14789949.2011.617540

Humeniuk, R., Ali, R., Babor, T. F., Farrell, M., Formigoni, M. L., Jittiwutikarn, J.,

… & Simon, S. (2008). Validation of the alcohol, smoking and substance

involvement screening test (ASSIST). Addiction, 103(6), 1039-1047. doi:

10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.02114.x

Ibañez, G. E., Levi-Minzi, M. A., Rigg, K. K., & Mooss A. D. (2013). Diversion of

benzodiazepines through healthcare sources. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs,

45(1), 48-56. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2013.764232

International Narcotics Control Board. (2014). Annual Report 2013. Vienna, Austria:

Author.

Isbister, G. K., O’Regan, L., Sibbritt, D., & Whyte, I. M. (2004). Alprazolam is

relatively more toxic than other benzodiazepines in overdose. British Journal

of Clinical Pharmacology, 58(1), 88-95. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-

2125.2004.02089.x

Islam, M. M., Conigrave, K. ., Day, C. A., Nguyen, Y., & Haber, P. S. (2014).

Twenty-year trends in benzodiazepine dispensing in the Australian

population. Internal Medicine Journal, 44(1), 57-64. doi: 10.1111/iml.12315

Page 181: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

181

Jackson, C. J. (2009). Jackson-5 scales of revised reinforcement sensitivity theory (r-

RST) and their application to dysfunctional real world outcomes. Journal of

Research in Personality, 43, 556-559. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.007

Jones, S. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2011). Personality, antisocial behaviour,

and aggression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 329-

337. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2011.03.004

Jones, J. D., Mogali, S., & Comer, S. D. (2012). Polydrug abuse: A review of opioid

and benzodiazepine combination use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 125(1-

2), 8-18. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.07.004

Jones, K. A., Nielson, R., Bruno, R., Frei, M., & Lubman, D. I. (2011).

Benzodiazepines: Their role in aggression and why GPs should prescribe

with caution. Australian Family Physician, 40(11), 862-865.

Kalachnik, J. E., Hanzel, T. E., Sevenich, R., Harder, S. R. (2002). Benzodiazepines

behavioral side effects: Review and implications for individuals with mental

retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107(5), 376-410. doi:

10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0376:BBSERA>2.0.CO;2

Kalachnik, J. E., Hanzel, T. E., Sevenich, R., & Harder, S. R. (2003). Brief report:

Clonazepam behavioral side effects with an individual with mental

retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 349-354.

Kambouropoulos, N., & Staiger, P. (2004). Personality and responses to appetitive

and aversive stimuli: The joint influence of behavioural approach and

behavioural inhibition systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 37,

1153-1165. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.11.019

Karch, F. E. (1979). Rage reaction associated with clorazepate dipotassium. Annals

of Internal Medicine, 91(1), 61-62.

Page 182: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

182

Karson, C. N., Weinberger, D. R., Bigelow, L., & Wyatt, R. J. (1982). Clonazepam

treatment of chronic schizophrenia: Negative results in a double-blind,

placebo-controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139(12), 1627-

1628.

Kerr, T., Kiatying-Angsulee, N., Fairbairn, N., Hayashi, K., Suwannawong, P.,

Kaplan, K., Lai, C., & Wood, E. (2010). High rates of midazolam injection

among drug users in Bangkok, Thailand. Harm Reduction Journal, 7, 1-7.

Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S. L.

T., et al. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and

trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32(6),

959-976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074

Klötz, F., Petersson, A., Isacson, D., & Thiblin, I. (2007). Violent crime and

substance abuse: A medico-legal comparison between deceased users of

anabolic androgenic steroids and abusers of illicit drugs. Forensic Science

International, 173(1), 57-63. doi: 10.1016/j.forsclint.2007.01.026

Knafo, A., Jaffee, S. R., Quinn, P. D., Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in

impulsivity and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance use from

adolescences into early adulthood. Development & Psychopathology, 25(1),

223-239. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000284

Kurzthaler, I., Wambacher, M., Golser, K., Sperner, G., Sperner-Unterweger, B.,

Haidekker, A., … & Fleischhacker, W. W. (2005). Alcohol and/or

benzodiazepine use: Different accidents – different impacts? Human

Psychopharmacology, 20, 583-589. doi: 10.1002/hup.736

Page 183: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

183

Lader, M. (2011). Benzodiazepines revisited – will we ever learn? Addiction,

106(12), 2086-2109. doi: 10.111/j.1360-0443.2011.03563.x

Lader, M. (2014). Benzodiazepine harm: how can it be reduced? British Journal of

Clinical Pharmacology, 77(2), 295-301. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-

2125.2012.04418.x

Latalova, K., & Prasko, J. (2010). Aggression in borderline personality disorder.

Psychiatry Quarterly, 81(3), 239-251. doi: 10.1007/s111126-010-9133-3.

Lee, G. & Gammie, S. C. (2010). GABAA receptor signalling in caudal

periaqueductal gray regulates maternal aggression and maternal care in mice.

Behavioural Brain Research, 213(2), 230-237. doi:

10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.001

Lennings, C. J., Copeland, J., & Howard, J. (2003). Substance use patterns of young

offenders and violent crime. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 414-422. doi:

10.1002/ab.10048

Leone, L., & Russo, P. M. (2009). Components of the behavioural activation system

and functional impulsivity: A test of discriminant hypotheses. Journal of

Research in Personality, 43(6), 1101-1104.

Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P.

A., … & Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic

reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health interventions:

Explanation and elaboration. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), 1-28. doi:

10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100

Lion, J. R., Azcarate, C. L., & Koepke, H. H. (1975). “Paradoxical rage reactions”

during psychotropic medication. Diseases of the Nervous System, 36(10),

557-558.

Page 184: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

184

Lloyd, B., Matthews, S., & Gao, C. X. (2014). Trends in alcohol and drug related

ambulance attendances in Victoria 2012/13. Fitzroy, Victoria: Turning Point.

Lloyd, B. K. & McElwee, P. R. (2011). Trends over time in characteristics of

pharmaceutical drug-related ambulance attendances in Melbourne. Drug and

Alcohol Review, 30, 271-280. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00292.x

Longo, M. C., Hunter, C. E., Lokan, R. J., White, J. M., White, M. A. (2000). The

prevalence of alcohol, cannabinoids, benzodiazepines and stimulants amongst

injured drivers and their role in driver culpability: Part I: The prevalence of

drug use in drivers, and characteristics of the drug-positive group. Accident

Analysis and Prevention, 32(5), 613-622. doi: 10.1016/S0001-

4575(99)00111-6

Longo, L. P., & Johnson, B. (2000). Addiction: Part I. Benzodiazepines – side

effects, abuse risks and alternatives. American Family Physician, 61(7),

2121-2128.

Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress

scales (2nd Ed.). Sydney, NSW: Psychology Foundation of Australia.

Loxley, W. (2007). Benzodiazepine use and harms among police detainees in

Australia (Trends and issues in crime and criminal justice no. 336). Canberra:

Australian Institute of Criminology.

Loxton, N. J., Wan, V. L.-N., Ho, A. M.-C., Cheung, B. K.-L., Tam, N., Leung, F. Y.

K., & Stadlin, A. (2008). Impulsivity in Hong Kong-Chinese club-drug users.

Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 95 (1-2), 81-89. doi:

10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.009

Page 185: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

185

Lundholm, L., Kall, K., Wallin, S., & Thiblin, I. (2010). Use of anabolic androgenic

steroids in substance abusers arrested for crime. Drug & Alcohol

Dependence, 111(3), 222-226. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.04.020

Lundholm, L., Haggård, U., Möller, J., Hallqvist, J., & Thiblin, I. (2013). The

triggering effect of alcohol and illicit drugs on violent crime in a remand

prison population: A case crossover study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,

129(1-2), 110-115. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.09.019

Magid, V., & Colder, C. R. (2007). The UPPS Impulsive Behaivor Scale: Factor

structure and associations with college drinking. Personality & Individual

Differences, 43(7), 1927-1937. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.013

Marienfeld, C. B., Tek, E., Diaz, E., Schottenfeld, R., & Chawarski, M. (2012).

Psychiatrist decision-making towards prescribing benzodiazepines: The

dilemma with substance abusers. Psychiatric Quarterly, 83(4), 521-529. doi:

10.1007/s11126-012-9220-8

Mattson, R. E., O’Farrell, T. J., Lofgreen, A. M., Cunningham, K., & Murphy, C. M.

(2012). The role of illicit substance use in a conceptual model of intimate

partner violence in men undergoing treatment for alcoholism. Psychology of

Addictive Behaviors, 26(2), 255-264. doi: 10.1037/a0025030

McGregor, C., Gately, N., & Fleming, J. (2011). Prescription drug use among

detainees: Prevalence, sources and links to crime (Trends and issues in crime

and criminal justice no. 423). Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.

McKetin, R., Lubman, D. I., Najman, J. M., Dawe, S., Butterworth, P., & Baker, A.

L. (2014). Does methamphetamine use increase violent behaviour? Evidence

Page 186: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

186

from a prospective longitudinal study. Addiction, 109(5), 798-806. doi:

10.111/add.12474

McMurran, M. (2009). The relationships between alcohol use, trait aggression, and

the alcohol-aggression outcome expectancy in male students. Journal of

Substance Use, 14(1), 1-9. doi: 10.1080/14659890802302686

Merlin, T., Weston, A., & Tooher, R. (2009). Extending an evidence hierarchy to

include topics other than treatment: Revising the Australian ‘levels of

evidence’. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 9, 34-41. doi:

10.1186/1471-2288-9-34

Miczek, K. A., Fish, E. W., & DeBold, J. F. (2003). Neurosteroids, GABAA

receptors, and escalated aggressive behavior. Hormones & Behaviour, 44(3),

242-258. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.04.002

Miczek, K. A., Weerts, E. M., & DeBold, J. F. (1993). Alcohol, benzodiazepine-

GABAA receptor complex and aggression: Ethological analysis of individual

differences in rodents and primates. Journal of Studies of Alcohol , Sup11,

170-179.

Miller, J., Flory, K., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). A test of the four-factor

model of impulsivity-related traits. Personality & Individual Differences,

34(8), 1403-1418. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00122-8

Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). Examining antisocial behavior

through the lens of the five factor model of personality. Aggressive Behavior,

29, 497-514. doi: 10.1002/ab.10064

Miller, J. D., Zeichner, A., & Wilson, L. F. (2012). Personality correlates of

aggression: Evidence from measures of the five-factor model, UPPS model of

Page 187: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

187

impulsivity, and BIS/BAS. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 27(14), 2903-

2919. doi: 10.1177/0886260512438279

Misuse of Drugs Act 1971 (UK)

Misuse of Drugs Act 1975 (NZ)

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altmann, D. G., & The PRISMA Group. (2009).

Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The

PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), 1-6. doi:

10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

Moore, T. J., Glenmullen, J., & Furberg, C. D. (2010). Prescription drugs associated

with reports of violence towards others. PLoS ONE, 5(12), 1-5.

Nabors, E. L. (2010). Drug use and intimate partner violence among college

students: An in-depth exploration. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25(6),

1043-1063. doi: 10.1177/0886260509340543

Ng, S., & Macgregor, S. (2012). Pharmaceutical drug use among police detainees

(Research in practice, no. 23). Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.

Nicholas, R. (2010). An environmental scan on alcohol and other drug issues facing

law enforcement in Australia 2010. Canberra: National Drug Law

Enforcement Research Fund.

Nicholas, R., Lee, N., & Roche, A. (2011). Pharmaceutical drug misuse problems in

Australia: Complex issues, balanced responses. Adelaide: National Centre

for Education and Training on Addiction (NCETA), Flinders University.

Nielsen, S., Bruno, R., Carruthers, S., Fischer, J., Lintzeris, N., & Stoove, M. (2008).

Investigation of pharmaceutical misuse amongst drug treatment clients.

Melbourne: Turning Point Alcohol and Drug Centre Inc.

Page 188: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

188

Nielsen, S., Bruno, R., Degenhardt, L., Stoove, M. A., Fischer, J. A., Carruthers, S.

J., & Lintzerls, N. (2013). The source of pharmaceuticals for problematic

users of benzodiazepines and prescription opioids. Medical Journal of

Australia, 199, 696-699. doi: 10.5694/mja13.11331

Noyes, R., DuPont, R. L., Pecknold, J. C., Rifkin, A., Rubin, R. T., Swinson, R. P.,

… & Burrows, G. D. (1988). Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia:

Results from a multicentre trial. II Patient acceptance, side effects, and safety.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 423-428.

O’Brien, C. P. (2005). Bnezodiazepine use, abuse, and dependence. Journal of

Clinical Psychiatry, 66, 28-33.

O’Connor, D. B., Archer, J., & Wu, F. W. C. (2001). Measuring aggression: Self-

reports, partner reports, and responses to provoking scenarios. Aggressive

Behavior, 27(2), 79-101.

O’Sullivan, G. H., Noshirvani, H., Başoğlu, M., Marks, I. M., Swinson, R., Kuch, K.,

& Kirby, M. (1994). Safety and side-effects of alprazolam. Controlled study

in agoraphobia with panic disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2),

79-86.

Office for National Statistics. (September, 2014). Deaths related to drug poisoning,

England and Wales – 2013. Retrieved from

http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/rel/subnational-health3/deaths-related-to-drug-

poisoning/england-and-wales---2013/index.html

Osman, A., Wong, J. L., Bagge, C. L., Freedenthal, S., Gutierrez, P. M., & Lozano,

G. (2012). The depression anxiety stress scales-21 (DASS-21): Further

Page 189: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

189

examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 68(12), 1322-1328. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21908

Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual (3rd Ed.). NSW: Allen & Unwin.

Paton, C. (2002). Benzodiazepine and disinhibition: A review. Psychiatric Bulletin,

26, 460-462. doi: 10.1192/pb.26.12.460

Paulozzi, L. J., Mack, K. A., & Hockenberry, J. M. (2014). Variation among states in

prescribing of opioid pain relievers and benzodiazepines - United States,

2012. Journal of Safety Research, 51, 125-129. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2014.09.001

Payne, J., & Gaffney, A. (2012). How much crime is drug or alcohol related? Self-

reported attributions of police detainees (Trends & issues in crime and

criminal justice No. 439). Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.

Peretti-Watel, P. (2003). Neutralisation theory and the denial of risk: Some evidence

from cannabis use among French adolescents. British Journal of Sociology,

54, 21-42.

Perkins, A. M., Kemp, S. E., & Corr, P. J. (2007). Fear and anxiety as separable

emotions: An investigation of the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory of

personality. Emotion, 7(2), 252-261. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.252

Perkins, A. M., Cooper, A., Abdelall, M., Smillie, L. D., & Corr, P. J. (2010).

Personality and defensive reactions: Fear, trait anxiety, and threat

magnification. Journal of Personality, 78(3), 1071-1090. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

6494.2010.00643.x

Pickering, A., & Corr, P. (2008). J. A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST)

of personality. In G.E. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H., Saklofske (Eds.),

Personality theories and models (Vol. 1) (pp. 239-256). London: SAGE

Publications.

Page 190: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

190

Pietras, C. J., Lieving, L. M., Cherek, D. R., Lane, S. D., Tcheremissine, O. V., &

Nouvion, S. (2005). Acute effects of lorazepam on laboratory measures of

aggressive and escape responses of adult male parolees. Behavioural

Pharmacology, 16(4), 243-251. doi: 10.1097/01.fbp.0000170910.53415.77

Priks, M. (2010). Does frustration lead to violence? Evidence from the Swedish

hooligan scene. Kyklos, 63(3), 450-460. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

6435.2010.00482.x

Puangkot, S., Laohasiriwong, W., Saengsuwan, J., & Chiawiriyabunya, I. (2011).

Prevalence of benzodiazepine misuse in Ubon Ratchathani province in

Thailand. Journal of the Medical Association Thailand, 94(1), 118-122. doi:

10.4103/0253-7178.78510

Redman, J. R. (1994). Long-term benzodiazepine use: Problems for the criminal

justice system. Psychiatry, Psychology, & Law, 1(1), 3-10. doi:

10.1080/13218719409524823

Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., & Lang, C. (2013). Validation of and revision of the

VRAG and SORAG: The Violence Risk Appraisal Guide-Revised (VRAG-

R). Psychological Assessment, 25(3), 951-965. doi: 10.1037/a0032878

Rigg, K. K., & Ibañez, G. E. (2010). Motivations for non-medical prescription drug

use: A mixed methods analysis. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment,

39(3), 236-247. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2010.06.004

Rintoul, A. C., Dobbin, M. D. H., Neilsen, S., Degenhardt, L., & Drummer, O. H.

(2013). Recent increase in detection of alprazolam in Victorian heroin-related

deaths. Medical Journal of Australia, 198(4), 2069-209. doi:

10.5694/mja12.10986

Page 191: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

191

Roberton, T., Daffern, M., & Bucks, R. S. (2015). Beyond anger control: Difficulty

attending to emotions also predicts aggression in offenders. Psychology of

Violence, 5(1), 74-83. doi: 10.1037/a0037214

Rockett, I. R.H, Putnam, S. L., Jia, H., & Smith, S. G. (2006). Declared and

undeclared substance use among emergency department patients: a

population-based study. Addiction, 101, 706-712. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-

0443.2006.01.397.x

Rosenbaum, J. F., Woods, S. W., Groves, J. E., & Klerman, G. I. (1984). Emergence

of hostility during alprazolam treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry,

141(6), 792-793.

Rosenfeld, W. E., Beniak, T. E., Lippmann, S. M., & Loewenson, R. B. (1987).

Adverse behavioral response to clonazepam as a function of verbal IQ-

performance IQ discrepancy. Epilepsy Research, 1, 347-356.

Rothschild, A. J., Shindul-Rothschild, J. A., Viguera, A., Murray, M., & Brewster, S.

(2000). Comparison of the frequency of behavioral disinhibition on

alprazolam, clonazepam, or no benzodiazepine in hospitalized psychiatric

patients. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20(1), 7-11.

Rouve, N., Bagheri, H., Telmon, N., Pathak, A., Franchitto, N., Schmitt, L., … &

The French Association of Regional PharmacoVigilance Centres. (2011).

Prescribed drugs and violence: A case/noncase study in the French

PharmacoVigilance Database. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology,

67, 1189-1198. doi: 10.1007/s00228-011-1067-7

Ryden, M. B., Feldt, K. S., Oh, H. L., Brand, K., Warne, M., Weber, E., … & Gross,

C. (1999). Relationships between aggressive behavior in cognitively impaired

Page 192: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

192

nursing home residents and use of restraints, psychoactive drugs, and secured

units. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 13(4), 170-178.

Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., Finy, M. S., & Verona, E. (2011). Gender differences in

emotional risk for self- and other-directed violence among externalizing

adults. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 79(1), 106-117. doi:

10.1037/a0022197

Sandberg, S. (2012). Is cannabis use normalized, celebrated or neutralized?

Analysing talk as action. Addiction Research and Theory, 20(5), 372-381.

doi: 10.3109/16066359.2011.638147

Schenk, A. M., & Fremouw, W. J. (2012). Individual characteristics related to prison

violence: A critical review of the literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior,

17(5), 430-442. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.05.005

Segarra, P., Poy, R., López, R., & Moltó, J. (2014). Characterizing Carver and

White’s BIS/BAS subscales using the five factor model of personality.

Personality and Individual Differences, 61-62, 18-23. doi:

10.1016/j.paid.2013.12.027

Seibert, L. A., Miller, J. D., Pryor, L. R., Reidy, D. E., & Zeichner, A. (2010).

Personality and laboratory-based aggression: Comparing the predictive power

of the five-factor model, BIS/BAS, and impulsivity across context. Journal of

Research in Personality, 44, 13-21. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.003

Shah, A., Chiu, E., Ames, D., Harrigan, S., & McKenzie, D. (2000). Characteristics

of aggressive subjects in Australian (Melbourne) nursing homes.

International Psychogeriatrics, 12(2), 145-161.

Page 193: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

193

Sheridan, J., Jones, S., & Aspden, T. (2012). Prescription drug misuse: Quantifying

the experiences of New Zealand GPs. Journal of Primary Health Care, 4(2),

106-112.

Sherman, L. W., Gottfredson, D. C., MacKenzie, D. L., Eck, J. E., Reuter, P., &

Bushway, S. D. (1998). Preventing crime: What works, what doesn’t, what’s

promising. Maryland: National Institute of Justice.

Shin, H. J., Rosen, C. S., Greenbaum, M. A., & Jain, S. (2012). Longitudinal

correlates of aggressive behavior in help-seeking U.S. veterans with PTSD.

Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25, 649-656. doi: 10.1002/jts.21761

Shoham, S. G., Askenasy, J. J., Rahav, G., Chard, F., & Addl, A. (1989). Personality

correlates of violent prisoners. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(2),

137-145.

Sinclair, S. J., Siefert, C. J., Slavin-Mulford, J. M., Stein, M. B., Renna, M., & Blais,

M. A. (2012). Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the depression,

anxiety, and stress scales-21 (DASS-21) in a nonclinical sample of U.S.

adults. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 35(3), 259-279. doi:

10.1177/0163278711424282

Smith, R. G., Jorna, P., Sweeney, J. & Fuller, G. (2014). Counting the costs of crime

in Australia: A 2011 estimate (Research and Public Policy Series No. 129).

Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.

Smith, B., Miller, P., O’Keefe, B., & Fry, C. (2007). Benzodiazepine and

pharmaceutical opioid misuse and their relationship to crime: Victorian

report (Monograph series No. 23). Hobart, Tasmania: National Drug Law

Enforcement Research Fund (NDLERF).

Page 194: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

194

Smith, P., & Waterman, M. (2006). Self-reported aggression and impulsivity in

forensic and non-forensic populations: the role of gender and experience.

Journal of Family Violence, 21, 425-437. doi: 10.1007/s10896-006-9039-x

Smits, D. J. M., & Kuppens, P. (2005). The relations between anger, coping with

anger, and aggression, and the BIS/BAS system. Personality and Individual

Differences, 39, 783-793. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.023

Sobell, L. C., Brown, J., Leo, G. I., Sobell, M. B. (1996). The reliability of the

Alcohol Timeline Followback when administered by telephone and by

computer. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 42, 49-54.

Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline follow-back: A technique for assessing

self-reported ethanol consumption. In: Allen, J., Litten, R. Z. (Eds.).

Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biological methods (pp.

41-72). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.

Stafford, A. C., Alswayan, M. S., & Tenni, P. C. (2011). Inappropriate prescribing in

older residents of Australian care homes. Journal of Clinical Pharmacy &

Therapeutics, 36(1), 33-44. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2710.2009.01151.x

Stafford, J., & Burns, L. (2012). Findings from the Illicit Drug Reporting System

(IDRS) (Australian drug trends series no. 73). Sydney: National Drug and

Alcohol Research Center, University of New South Wales.

Staiger, P. K., Kambouropoulos, N., & Dawe, S. (2007). Should personality traits be

considered when refining substance misuse treatment programs? Drug &

Alcohol Review, 26(1), 17-23. doi: 10.1080/09595230601036952

Stalans, L. J., & Ritchie, J. (2008). Relationship of substance use/abuse with

psychological and physical intimate partner violence: Variations across living

Page 195: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

195

situations. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 9-24. doi: 10.1007/s10896-007-

9125-8

Steinert, T., & Whittington, R. (2013). A bio-psycho-social model of violence related

to mental health problems. International Journal of Law & Psychiatry, 36,

168-175. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2013.01.009

Stith, S. M., Smith, D. B., Penn, C. E., Ward, D. B., & Tritt, D. (2004). Intimate

partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta-

analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10(1), 65-98. Doi:

10.1016/j.avb.2003.09.001

Stephenson, C. P., Karanges, E., & McGregor, I. S. (2013). Trends in the utilisation

of psychotropic medications in Australia from 2000 to 2011. Australia New

Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 47(1), 74-87. doi:

10.1177/0004867412466595

Straus, M. A., & Mickey, E. I. (2012). Reliability, validity and prevalence of partner

violence measured by the conflict tactics scales in male-dominant nations.

Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 463-474. doi:

10.1016/j.avb.2012.06.004

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (June 2, 2011). The

TEDS Report: Substance Abuse Treatment Admissions for Abuse of

Benzodiazepines. Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and

Quality, Author.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2012). 2010: National

estimates of drug-related emergency department visits (HHS Publication No.

(SMA) 12-4733, DAWN Series D-38). Rockville, MD: Author.

Page 196: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

196

Sweeney, J., & Payne, J. (2012). Drug use monitoring in Australia: 2009-10 report

on drug use among police detainees (AIC Monitoring Reports No. 17).

Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.

Swogger, M. T., Walsh, Z., Houston, R. J., Cashman-Brown, S., Connor, K. R.

(2010). Psychopathy and Axis I psychiatric disorders among criminal

offenders: Relationships to impulsive and proactive aggression. Aggressive

Behavior, 36, 45-53. doi: 10.1002/ab.20330

Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of

delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22(6), 664-670.

Tannenbaum, C., Paquette, A., Hilmer, S., Holroyd-Leduc, J., & Carnahan, R.

(2012). A systematic review of amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive

impairment induced by anticholinergic, antihistamine, GABAergic and opioid

drugs. Drugs & Aging, 29(8), 639-658. doi: 10.2165/11633250-00000000-

00000

The Health and Social Care Information Centre. (2011). Prescriptions dispensed in

the community: England statistics for 2001 to 2011. UK: Author.

The Health and Social Care Information Centre. (July, 2013). Prescriptions dispensed

in the community: England 2002-12. Retrieved from

http://www.hscic.gov.uk/catalogue/PUB11291/pres-disp-com-eng-2002-12-

rep.pdf

Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Moltó, J., & Caseras, X. (2001). The sensitivity to

punishment and sensitivity to reward (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s

anxiety and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(6), 837-

862. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5

Page 197: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

197

Tull, M. T., Gratz, K. L., Latzman, R. D., Kimbrel, N. A., & Lejuez, C. W. (2010).

Reinforcement sensitivity theory and emotion regulation difficulties: A

multimodal investigation. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 989-

994. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.010

Umberson, D., Williams, K., & Anderson, K. (2002). Violent behavior: A measure of

emotional upset? Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 43, 189-206.

van der Bijl, P., & Roelofse, J. A. (1991). Disinhibitory reactions to benzodiazepines:

A review. Journal of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery, 49(5), 519-523.

Viera, A. J., & Garrett, J. M. (2005). Understanding interobserver agreement: The

Kappa statistic. Family Medicine, 37(5), 360-363.

Votava, M., Kršiak, M., Podhorná, J., & Miczek, K. A. (2001). Alprazolam

withdrawal and tolerance measured in the social conflict test in mice.

Psychopharmacology, 157(2), 123-131. doi: 10.1007/s002130100784

Voyer, P., Verreault, R., Azizah, G. M., Desrosiers, J., Champoux, N., & Bédard, A.

(2005). Prevalence of physical and verbal aggressive behaviours and

associated factors among older adults in long-term care facilities. BMC

Geriatrics, 5, 13-26. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-5-13

Wai-ming Tam, F., & Taki, M. (2007). Bullying among girls in Japan and Hong

Kong: An examination of the frustration-aggression model. Educational

Research and Evaluation: An International Journal on Theory and Practice,

13(4), 373-399. doi: 10.1080/13803610701702894

Wallace, P. S., & Taylor, S. P. (2009). Reduction of appeasement-related affect as a

concomitant of diazepam-induced aggression: Evidence for a link between

Page 198: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

198

aggression and the expression of self-conscious emotions. Aggressive

Behavior, 35(2), 203-212. doi: 10.1002/ab.20294

Weerts, E. M., Miller, L. G., Hood, K. E., Miczek, K. A. (1992). Increased GABA-a

dependent chloride uptake in mice selectively bred for low aggressive

behaviour. Psychopharmacology, 108(1-2), 196-204.

Weisman, A. M., Berman, M. E., & Taylor, S. P. (1998). Effects of clorazepate,

diazepam and oxazepam on a laboratory measurement of aggression in men.

International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 13(4), 183-188.

Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity:

Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality

& Individual Differences, 30(4), 669-689. doi: 10.1016/S0191-

8869(00)00064-7

Whiteside, S. P., Lynam, D. R., Miller, J. D., & Reynolds, S. K. (2005). Validation

of the UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity.

European Journal of Personality, 19, 559-574. doi: 10.1002/per.556

Whitty, P., & O’Connor, J. J. (2006). Violence and aggression in the Drug Treatment

Centre Board. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 23(3), 89-91.

World Health Organisation ASSIST Working Group (2002). The Alcohol, Smoking

and Substance Involvement Screening Test (ASSIST): Development,

reliability, and feasibility. Addiction, 97(9), 1183-1194.

Wilkinson, C. J. (1985). Effects of diazepam (Valium) and trait anxiety on human

physical aggression and emotional state. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,

8(1), 101-114.

Page 199: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

199

Yu, R., Geddes, J. R., & Fazel, S. (2012). Personality disorders, violence, and

antisocial behavior: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis.

Journal of Personality Disorders, 26(5), 775-792.

Zador, D., Rome, A., Hutchinson, S., Hickman, M., Baldacchino, A., Fahey, T., ... &

Kidd, B. (2007). Differences between injectors and non-injectors and a high

prevalence of benzodiazepines among drug related deaths in Scotland 2003.

Addiction Research & Theory, 15(6), 651-662. doi:

10.1080/16066350701699080

Zisook, S., & DeVaul, R. A. (1977). Adverse behavioral effects of benzodiazepines.

The Journal of Family Practice, 5(6), 963-966.

Page 200: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

200

Appendices

Page 201: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Appendix A

Albrecht, B. A., Staiger, P. K., Hall, K., Miller, P., Best, D., & Lubman, D. I.

(2014). Benzodiazepine use and aggressive behaviour: A systematic review.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 48(12), 1096-1114.

DOI: 10/1177/0004867414548902

Page 202: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 203: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 204: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

unavailability, provided there is no evidence to suggest that the person would object to being named as author

7. Data storage

The original data for this project are stored in the following locations. (The locations must be within an appropriate institutional setting. If the executive author is a Deakin staff member and data are stored outside Deakin University, permission for this must be given by the Head of Academic Unit within which the executive author is based.)

Data format Storage Location Date lodged Name of custodian if other than the executive author

N/A

This form must be retained by the executive author, within the school or institute in which they are based.

If the publication is to be included as part of an HDR thesis, a copy of this form must be included in the thesis with the publication.

Page 205: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Appendix B

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of BIS/BAS scales

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to assess the

reliability of the BIS/BAS scales with the recently revised 2-factor BIS (i.e., into

fear- and anxiety-mediated avoidance motivation; Dissabandara et al., 2012;

Heym et al., 2008), in line with the revised RST. The analysis was conducted in

IBM SPSS AMOS 22, on 288 participants as one case did not have a complete

BIS/BAS scale, providing more than a 10:1 ratio of cases to unknowns. The CFA

was conducted only to assess whether the two-factor or one-factor BIS scale was

most appropriate for the current sample, not to modify the BIS scale, and

therefore the modification indices were not examined and no changes to the BIS

scale were made. Although neither model demonstrated great fit to the data, the

two-factor model (χ2(13) = 56.496, p <.001, CFI = .913, TLI = .860, RMSEA =

.108, standardised RMR = .062) was a significantly better fitting model than the

one-factor BIS (χ2(14) = 90.814, p <.001, CFI = .847, TLI = .770, RMSEA =

.138, standardised RMR = .072); Δχ2 (1) = 34.317, p < .05. Both BIS and FFFS

demonstrated adequate internal reliability (α =.73, .67, respectively). A moderate

association between the subscales was observed (r = .519, p < .001), supporting

the separation of the scales.

Page 206: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Appendix C

Albrecht, B., Staiger, P. K., Hall, K., Kambouropoulos, N., & Best, D.

Motivational drive and alprazolam misuse: A recipe for aggression?

Submitted to: Psychiatry Research

Submission date: 27 May 2015

Page 207: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 208: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 209: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Data format Storage Location Date lodged Name of custodian if other than the executive author

Paper-based questionnaires School of Psychology, Deakin University (locked cabinet)

2014

De-identified electronic database, password protected

Deakin University

2014

This form must be retained by the executive author, within the school or institute in which they are based.

If the publication is to be included as part of an HDR thesis, a copy of this form must be included in the thesis with the publication.

Page 210: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Abstract

Benzodiazepine-related aggression is understudied in the literature, in

particular little is known about the motivational factors which may contribute to the

development of this paradoxical response. The revised Reinforcement Sensitivity

Theory (rRST) provides a theoretical framework from which to understand the

relevant underlying motivational processes. The current study aimed to identify the

role of approach and avoidance motivational tendencies in relation to

benzodiazepine-related aggression. Data regarding benzodiazepine and other

substance use, approach and avoidance motivation, and general and physical

aggressive behaviour were collected via self-report questionnaires. Participants were

a convenience sample (n = 204) who reported using benzodiazepines in the previous

month. Participants were primarily male (62.7%), aged 18-51 years old. General and

physical aggression were predicted by alprazolam use and Drive, a facet of approach

motivation reflecting persistent goal-directed action. Overall, lower use of diazepam

significantly predicted higher levels of general aggression. However, when

diazepam-preferring participants were examined in isolation of the larger sample

(23.5% of sample), problematic (dependent) diazepam use was associated with

greater aggression scores, as was dependence risk for alprazolam-preferring

participants (39.7% of sample). The findings highlight the importance of

motivational factors and benzodiazepine use patterns in understanding

benzodiazepine-related aggression, with implications for violent offender

rehabilitation.

Keywords: benzodiazepines, aggressive behaviour, Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory

Page 211: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

Appendix D

Albrecht, B., Staiger, P. K., Best, D., Hall, K., Nielsen, S., Miller, P., & Lubman, D.

I.

Violent crime: Could benzodiazepine use be playing a role?

Submitted to: Journal of Substance Abuse

Submission date: September 2015

Page 212: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 213: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
sfol
Retracted Stamp
Page 214: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

7. Data storage

The original data for this project are stored in the following locations. (The locations must be within an appropriate institutional setting. If the executive author is a Deakin staff member and data are stored outside Deakin University, permission for this must be given by the Head of Academic Unit within which the executive author is based.)

Data format Storage Location Date lodged Name of custodian if other than the executive author

De-identified electronic database, password protected

Deakin University

This form must be retained by the executive author, within the school or institute in which they are based.

If the publication is to be included as part of an HDR thesis, a copy of this form must be included in the thesis with the publication.

Page 215: by Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Honsdro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30079711/albrecht-benzodiazepinerelate… · Bonnie Albrecht BSc (Psych) Hons Submitted in partial fulfilment of the

APPENDIX D: VIOLENT CRIME AND BENZODIAZEPINES

Abstract

Objective:

To examine the relationship between benzodiazepine use and violent crime in a

sample of offenders.

Methods:

Data regarding benzodiazepine and other substance use, mental health, personality

characteristics, and crime involvement was collected through semi-structured

interviews. Participants (n = 82, 79.3% male) were 21-56 years old, and, in the

previous six months, had been charged with a criminal offence and used

benzodiazepines at least monthly.

Results:

Individuals charged with violent offences were significantly more likely to use

higher average doses of alprazolam, exhibit benzodiazepine dependence and report

high levels of sensation seeking, have committed prior violence, and report the

diagnoses of depression and personality disorder, than individuals charged with non-

violent offences.

Conclusions:

The findings suggest the existence of a complex dynamic between mental health and

violent offending that may be influenced by benzodiazepine use (in particular

alprazolam). Implications for prescribing and continued efforts to reduce

benzodiazepine diversion are discussed.

Key words: benzodiazepines, alprazolam, violence, impulsive behaviour, mental

health