Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effectICI=0 ms ICI=1 ms ICI=2 ms ICI=3 ms ICI=4...

2
General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Aug 20, 2021 Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effect Bianchi, Federica; Verhulst, Sarah; Dau, Torsten Publication date: 2012 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link back to DTU Orbit Citation (APA): Bianchi, F., Verhulst, S., & Dau, T. (2012). Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effect. Poster session presented at 35th MidWinter Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, San Diego, CA, United States.

Transcript of Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effectICI=0 ms ICI=1 ms ICI=2 ms ICI=3 ms ICI=4...

Page 1: Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effectICI=0 ms ICI=1 ms ICI=2 ms ICI=3 ms ICI=4 ms ICI=5 ms ICI=8 ms Increasing ICI Figure 7 ABRs recorded for subject KE for binaural

General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.

Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research.

You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain

You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Aug 20, 2021

Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effect

Bianchi, Federica; Verhulst, Sarah; Dau, Torsten

Publication date:2012

Document VersionPublisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link back to DTU Orbit

Citation (APA):Bianchi, F., Verhulst, S., & Dau, T. (2012). Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effect. Postersession presented at 35th MidWinter Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, San Diego, CA,United States.

Page 2: Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effectICI=0 ms ICI=1 ms ICI=2 ms ICI=3 ms ICI=4 ms ICI=5 ms ICI=8 ms Increasing ICI Figure 7 ABRs recorded for subject KE for binaural

0 2 4 6 8

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

ITD=300 us

ICI (ms)0 2 4 6 8

ITD=900 us

ICI (ms)

ABRsAim• Compare ABRs to monaural and binaural stimulation to

investigate whether binaural processes contribute to lag-suppression at brainstem level

StimuliDeviants with ICIs of 0-8 ms and ITDs of 300 and 900 μs were presented monaurally and binaurally. The ABRs were recorded using 4 surface electrodes: M1, M2, Fz (ground), Cz (reference).

Left ear

Right ear

Results

No evidence for a binaural contribution to lag-suppression at the brainstem level.

0 2 4 6 8

SV

KE

KG

JC

EC

NL

FB

AL

Mean

Echo threshold (ms)

Fusion

BinauralMonoRMonoL

Behavioral and objective measures of the precedence effectFederica Bianchi, Sarah Verhulst, Torsten Dau

Centre for Applied Hearing Research, Technical University of Denmark

AbstractThe precedence effect (PE) refers to the dominance of directional information carried by a direct sound (lead) over the spatial information contained in its multiple reflections (lags) in sound localization. Many studies investigated the PE at different stages along the auditory pathway, but it is still unclear whether this perceptual phenomenon originates from peripheral or central processes. The present study aimed at investigating peripheral correlates of the PE, by comparing psychoacoustical data to objective measures of lag-suppression. The psychoacoustical data showed that the time range in which the PE operated (precedence window) was between 1 and 5 ms. Click-evoked otoacoustic emissions (CEOAEs) were recorded to investigate monaural lag-suppression at a peripheral stage of the auditory pathway, and showed largest lag-suppression within the precedence window. Auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) were used to investigate both monaural and binaural lag-suppression at the brainstem level. The ABRs to monaural stimulation reflected the peripheral lag-suppression, while the binaural ABRs did not show any additional contribution of binaural processes to the monaural suppression. The findings of this study demonstrate a monaural and peripheral component to lag-suppression, reflecting basilar membrane (BM) lead-lag impulse response interactions, in a time range from 1 to 4 ms.

IntroductionThe precedence effectConsider a direct sound (lead) with a single reflection (lag). The lead-lag pair is reproduced in the free field by two clicks played over two loudspeakers at different locations. The perception of the lead-lag pair depends on the lead-lag delay and varies both in the number of perceived stimuli and in their location:

Three perceptual phenomena occur within the precedence window:• fusion• lead-localization dominance PRECEDENCE • lag-discrimination suppression EFFECT

Is the precedence effect a result of peripheral or central processes?

Aim of the studyTo investigate the contribution of peripheral processing

to the precedence effect by providing individual comparisons of objective and behavioral data.

CEOAEsAim• Investigate monaural lag-suppression at a peripheral stage of

auditory processing

StimuliDeviants with ICIs of 0-8 ms and ITDs of 300 and 900 μs were presented using an interleaved technique. For each ICI and ITD condition, 1800 repetitions of the following three stimuli were presented:✦ Single Click (SC)✦ Double Click (DC)✦ Double Click Inverted (DCI)

Results

Lead-lag interactions at BM level provide a peripheral source of monaural lag-suppression occurring

for ICIs within the precedence window.

Lead Lag Lead

Lag

Lead Lag

SC

DiscussionPeripheral lag-suppression and echo-threshold✦ For all six test subjects, the suppression for ICIs below the echo-

threshold was significantly larger than the suppression above the echo-threshold.

✦ These results suggest that the lead-lag interactions on the BM level provide a peripheral source of lag-suppression occurring for ICIs within the precedence window.

Monaural ABRs and CEOAE resultsThe ABRs to monaural stimulation (blue and red dashed curves in Fig. 9) followed the trends of the CEOAE results (blue and red solid curves). However, the two curves were not expected to be identical:✦ CEOAE results: reflect the amplitude-reduction of the backward

traveling wave, which contains information of specific reflection sites along the basilar membrane [7, 8].

✦ ABR results: reflect the neural activity elicited by the forward traveling wave, which should hypothetically reflect the synchronous activity of the whole basilar membrane.

Even though both OAE and ABR results are expected to reflect monaural BM lead-lag interactions, the OAE only contains a subset of emission components present in the ABR. Thus, the shorter time range of suppression obtained in the ABR results might be due to the higher frequency content.

Monaural and binaural ABRs✦ For 4 out of 6 subjects, the lag-waveV reduction obtained for binaural

stimulation was not larger than the reduction obtained for either monaural stimulations.

✦ These results suggest that binaural processes did not add any substantial contribution to monaural and peripheral suppression at the brainstem level, in agreement with [2].

ConclusionThe results show that BM lead-lag interactions represent the main source of lag-suppression up to the brainstem. This suggests the existence of a monaural and peripheral component to the binaurally perceived PE.References[1] Litovsky RY (1999) The precedence effect. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106:1633-1654.[2] Damaschke J, Riedel H, Kollmeier B (2005) Neural correlates of the precedence effect in auditory evoked potentials. Hearing research 205:157-171.[3] Litovsky RY, Hawley ML, Colburn HS (1997) Measurement of precedence in monaural listeners. Meeting of the American Speech and Hearing Association, Boston, MA.[4] Kemp and Chum (1980) Properties of the generator of stimulated otoacoustic emissions. Hearing research 2: 213-232[5] Kapadia S, Lutman ME (2000) Nonlinear temporal interactions in click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Experimental data. Hearing research 146:89-100.[6] Verhulst S, Harte JM, Dau T (2011) Temporal suppression of the click-evoked otoacoustic emission level-curve. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 129:1452- 1463.[7] Shera CA, Guinan JJ, Jr. (1999) Evoked otoacoustic emissions arise by two fundamentally different mechanisms: a taxonomy for mammalian OAEs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 105: 782-798.[8] Moleti et al.(2008) Transient evoked otoacoustic emission latency and estimates of cochlear tuning in preterm neonates. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124: 2984-2994.

Observations:✦ Mean echo-threshold occurs for an ICI of 4.3 ms (binaural stimulation).✦ Similar thresholds for monaural and binaural stimulation.✦ Fusion mechanism NOT dependent on binaural processes, in

agreement with [3].

Figure 2 Individual and mean results of the fusion test. Echo-thresholds obtained for monaural and binaural stimulation.

Figure 3 Results of the lateralization and ITD-detection tests for subject KE. The markers indicate the lateralization of the lead-lag pair as a function of ICI and ITD. The green highlighted area depicts the region below the lateralization threshold, where the percept of the lead-lag pair is centered.

Observations:✦ Higher ITD-thresholds for an ICI between 1-5 ms indicate stronger lead-

dominance in this time window.✦ Precedence window between 1 and 4.3 ms, where lead-dominance, lag-

suppression and fusion occur.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

0

200

400

600

800

1000

ICI (ms)

ITD

(us)

LeftCenterCenter & CenterCenter & RightCenter & LeftCenter & Left & RightLateralization threshDetection thresh1/3 response2/3 responses3/3 responses

Figure 1 Perceived location of a lead-lag pair as a function of the lead-lag delay. The head schematics refers to a free field setup, where the leading speaker is towards the left and the lagging speaker towards the right. The time ranges are defined for click stimuli [1].

Derived suppressed response (DS)

Unsuppressed response (US)

Figure 5 Monaural and peripheral lag-suppression (dB) for subject KE (ITD=300 μs) as a function of the ICI. The dashed curve shows the monaural right lag-suppression, the solid curve the monaural left suppression.

100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

R

C

L

lead-lag delay (ms)

Perc

eive

d L

ocat

ion

Summingwindow

Precedence EchoEcho-thresholdwindow window

Lead Lag

Left ear

Right ear

Lead Lag

ICI

Reference

LeadLag

Left ear

Right ear

Lead Lag

ICI

ITD

Deviant

Lead - Lag

Figure 4 Procedure to derive the suppressed response from the recordings [4, 5], by removing the CEOAE component due to the leading click, while keeping the component due to the lagging click and the nonlinear component due to the ICI.

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 80123456789

1011

ICI (ms)

Lead Lag

ICI

ITDL

Recho-thresh

Lag- suppression = Lrms (US) − Lrms (DS)

Observations:✦ The peripheral and monaural lag-suppression is largest for ICIs within

the precedence window (in agreement with [5, 6]).✦ Considering the linear coherent reflection theory of generation of

CEOAEs [7], the peripheral lag-suppression reflects basilar membrane (BM) lead-lag impulse response interactions.

0

5

10

15

echo thr

ICI (ms)

dB S

uppr

(ITD

= 3

00 u

s)

MonoL

ICI (ms)

MonoR

ICI (ms)

Bin

0 2 4 6 8

0

5

10

15

echo thr

ICI (ms)

dB S

uppr

(ITD

= 9

00 u

s)

0 2 4 6 8ICI (ms)

0 2 4 6 8ICI (ms)

OAEABR

OAEABR

ABR Bin

0.30

0.3

ITD = 300 us ITD = 900 us

0.30

0.30.3

00.30.3

00.3

volta

ge (u

V)

0.30

0.30.3

00.3

5 10 15

0.30

0.3

latency (ms)5 10 15

latency (ms)

ICI=0 ms

ICI=1 ms

ICI=2 ms

ICI=3 ms

ICI=4 ms

ICI=5 ms

ICI=8 ms

Increasing ICIFigure 7 ABRs recorded for subject KE for binaural stimulation (left: ITD=300 μs; right: ITD=900 μs). The waves V elicited by lead and lag are indicated by downward-pointing triangles.

Observations:✦ Lag-wave V amplitude increases with increasing ICI (Fig. 7).✦ Lag-wave V amplitude reductions for ICIs up to 4 ms.✦ Binaural lag-wave V amplitude reduction does not show an additional

reduction to the monaural one.

Figure 8 Reduction of lag-wave V amplitude (dB) as a function of ICI (subject KE), for either monaural stimulations and for binaural stimulation (left: ITD=300 μs; right: ITD=900 μs)

Figure 9 Lag-suppression [dB] as a function of ICI, obtained from CEOAE amplitude-reduction (blue and red solid curves) and ABR wave V-reduction (blue and red dashed curves, black curves), for monaural and binaural stimulation (subject KE). The monaural and binaural echo-thresholds, obtained in the fusion test, are represented by an arrow.

Contact:Federica Bianchi, [email protected]

Sarah Verhulst, [email protected]

Psychoacoustical experimentsAim• Define the three windows of perception• Define the lateralization of the lead-lag pair for each window

Stimuli

ExperimentsSix normal hearing subjects performed the following 3 experiments:1. Fusion test: define the echo-threshold for monaural and binaural

stimulation (“1 click or 2 clicks?”).2. ITD-detection test: define the minimum interaural time difference (ITD)

for lateralization [2].3. ITD-lateralization test: specify the perception of the lead-lag pair as a

function of the inter-click interval (ICI).

Results

Figure 6 Stimuli used in the ABR recordings and head schematics showing the position of the electrodes M1, M2 and Cz.