Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved...

15
Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 25, Number 1—Winter 2011—Pages 209–222 D D uring the night of April 14, 1912, the RMS uring the night of April 14, 1912, the RMS Titanic collided with an iceberg collided with an iceberg on her maiden voyage. Two hours and 40 minutes later she sank, resulting on her maiden voyage. Two hours and 40 minutes later she sank, resulting in the loss of 1,501 lives—more than two-thirds of her 2,207 passengers in the loss of 1,501 lives—more than two-thirds of her 2,207 passengers and crew. This remains one of the deadliest peacetime maritime disasters in history and crew. This remains one of the deadliest peacetime maritime disasters in history and by far the most famous. The disaster came as a great shock because the vessel and by far the most famous. The disaster came as a great shock because the vessel was equipped with the most advanced technology at that time, had an experienced was equipped with the most advanced technology at that time, had an experienced crew, and was thought to be practically “unsinkable” (although the belief that the crew, and was thought to be practically “unsinkable” (although the belief that the ship had been widely believed to be truly unsinkable actually arose ship had been widely believed to be truly unsinkable actually arose after the sinking, the sinking, as explained in Howell, 1999). The as explained in Howell, 1999). The Titanic’s fame was enhanced by the considerable ’s fame was enhanced by the considerable number of films made about it: not including various made-for-television movies number of films made about it: not including various made-for-television movies and series, the list would include and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), (1912), Atlantic (1929), (1929), Titanic (1943 and 1953), (1943 and 1953), A Night to Remember (1958), (1958), Raise the Titanic! (1980), and of course the 1997 (1980), and of course the 1997 Titanic, directed by James Cameron and , directed by James Cameron and starring Leonardo DiCaprio and Kate Winslet. In 1985, a joint American–French starring Leonardo DiCaprio and Kate Winslet. In 1985, a joint American–French expedition, led by Jean-Louis Michel and Dr. Robert Ballard, located the wreckage expedition, led by Jean-Louis Michel and Dr. Robert Ballard, located the wreckage and collected approximately 6,000 artifacts, which were later shown in an exhibition and collected approximately 6,000 artifacts, which were later shown in an exhibition that toured the world. that toured the world. Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster Bruno S. Frey is Professor of Economics, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, Bruno S. Frey is Professor of Economics, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, University of Zurich, Switzerland, and Distinguished Professor of Behavioural Science, Warwick University of Zurich, Switzerland, and Distinguished Professor of Behavioural Science, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, U.K. David A. Savage is a Graduate Student, School Business School, University of Warwick, U.K. David A. Savage is a Graduate Student, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Benno of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Benno Torgler is Professor of Economics, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Torgler is Professor of Economics, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Frey and Torgler are also associated with CREMA–Center for Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Frey and Torgler are also associated with CREMA–Center for Research in Economics, Management and the Arts, Basel, Switzerland. Research in Economics, Management and the Arts, Basel, Switzerland. doi=10.1257/jep.25.1.209 Bruno S. Frey, David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler is article is substantially similar to: 1) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2010. “Interaction of Natural Survival Instincts and Internalized Social Norms Exploring the Titanic and Lusitania Disasters.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(11): 4862–65. 2) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2010. “Noblesse oblige? Determinants of Survival in a Life-and-Death Situation.” Journal of Economic Behavior and Organizations, 74(1–2): pp. 1–11. 3) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2011. “Who Perished on the Titanic? e Importance of Social Norms.” Rational- ity and Society, 23(1): 35–49. JEP regrets that these articles were not cited in the original publication.

Transcript of Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved...

Page 1: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 25, Number 1—Winter 2011—Pages 209–222

DD uring the night of April 14, 1912, the RMS uring the night of April 14, 1912, the RMS Titanic collided with an iceberg collided with an iceberg on her maiden voyage. Two hours and 40 minutes later she sank, resulting on her maiden voyage. Two hours and 40 minutes later she sank, resulting in the loss of 1,501 lives—more than two-thirds of her 2,207 passengers in the loss of 1,501 lives—more than two-thirds of her 2,207 passengers

and crew. This remains one of the deadliest peacetime maritime disasters in history and crew. This remains one of the deadliest peacetime maritime disasters in history and by far the most famous. The disaster came as a great shock because the vessel and by far the most famous. The disaster came as a great shock because the vessel was equipped with the most advanced technology at that time, had an experienced was equipped with the most advanced technology at that time, had an experienced crew, and was thought to be practically “unsinkable” (although the belief that the crew, and was thought to be practically “unsinkable” (although the belief that the ship had been widely believed to be truly unsinkable actually arose ship had been widely believed to be truly unsinkable actually arose after the sinking, the sinking, as explained in Howell, 1999). The as explained in Howell, 1999). The Titanic’s fame was enhanced by the considerable ’s fame was enhanced by the considerable number of fi lms made about it: not including various made-for-television movies number of fi lms made about it: not including various made-for-television movies and series, the list would include and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), (1912), Atlantic (1929), (1929), Titanic (1943 and 1953), (1943 and 1953), A Night to Remember (1958), (1958), Raise the Titanic! (1980), and of course the 1997 (1980), and of course the 1997 Titanic, directed by James Cameron and , directed by James Cameron and starring Leonardo DiCaprio and Kate Winslet. In 1985, a joint American–French starring Leonardo DiCaprio and Kate Winslet. In 1985, a joint American–French expedition, led by Jean-Louis Michel and Dr. Robert Ballard, located the wreckage expedition, led by Jean-Louis Michel and Dr. Robert Ballard, located the wreckage and collected approximately 6,000 artifacts, which were later shown in an exhibition and collected approximately 6,000 artifacts, which were later shown in an exhibition that toured the world.that toured the world.

Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster

■ ■ Bruno S. Frey is Professor of Economics, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, Bruno S. Frey is Professor of Economics, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, University of Zurich, Switzerland, and Distinguished Professor of Behavioural Science, Warwick University of Zurich, Switzerland, and Distinguished Professor of Behavioural Science, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, U.K. David A. Savage is a Graduate Student, School Business School, University of Warwick, U.K. David A. Savage is a Graduate Student, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Benno of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Benno Torgler is Professor of Economics, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Torgler is Professor of Economics, School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Frey and Torgler are also associated with CREMA–Center for Technology, Brisbane, Australia. Frey and Torgler are also associated with CREMA–Center for Research in Economics, Management and the Arts, Basel, Switzerland.Research in Economics, Management and the Arts, Basel, Switzerland.

doi=10.1257/jep.25.1.209

Bruno S. Frey, David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler

This article is substantially similar to:

1) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2010. “Interaction of Natural Survival Instincts and Internalized Social Norms Exploring the Titanic and Lusitania Disasters.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(11): 4862–65.

2) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2010. “Noblesse oblige? Determinants of Survival in a Life-and-Death Situation.” Journal of Economic Behavior and Organizations, 74(1–2): pp. 1–11.

3) Frey, Bruno S., David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler. 2011. “Who Perished on the Titanic? The Importance of Social Norms.” Rational-ity and Society, 23(1): 35–49.

JEP regrets that these articles were not cited in the original publication.

Page 2: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

210 Journal of Economic Perspectives

For social scientists, evidence about how people behaved as the For social scientists, evidence about how people behaved as the Titanic sunk offers a quasi-natural fi eld experiment to explore behavior under extreme sunk offers a quasi-natural fi eld experiment to explore behavior under extreme conditions of life and death. A common assumption is that in such situations, conditions of life and death. A common assumption is that in such situations, self-interested reactions will predominate and social cohesion is expected to self-interested reactions will predominate and social cohesion is expected to disappear. For example, in an article called “The Human Being in Disasters: A disappear. For example, in an article called “The Human Being in Disasters: A Research Perspective,” Fritz and Williams (1957, p. 42) write: “(Human beings) Research Perspective,” Fritz and Williams (1957, p. 42) write: “(Human beings) . . . panic, trampling each other and losing all concern for their fellow human . . . panic, trampling each other and losing all concern for their fellow human beings. After panic has subsided—so the image indicates—they turn to looting beings. After panic has subsided—so the image indicates—they turn to looting and exploitation, while the community is rent with confl ict . . .” Other researchers and exploitation, while the community is rent with confl ict . . .” Other researchers like Gray (1988) and Mawson (2007) present a similar image, while movies, televi-like Gray (1988) and Mawson (2007) present a similar image, while movies, televi-sion and radio programs, novels, and journalistic reports of disasters often tend to sion and radio programs, novels, and journalistic reports of disasters often tend to reinforce this grim scenario. However, empirical evidence on the extent to which reinforce this grim scenario. However, empirical evidence on the extent to which people in the throes of a disaster react with self-regarding or with other-regarding people in the throes of a disaster react with self-regarding or with other-regarding behavior is scanty.behavior is scanty.

The sinking of the The sinking of the Titanic posed a life-or-death situation for its passengers. posed a life-or-death situation for its passengers.11 Failure to secure a seat in a lifeboat virtually guaranteed death because the average Failure to secure a seat in a lifeboat virtually guaranteed death because the average ocean temperature was about 2 degrees Celsius (35 degrees Fahrenheit). Only a ocean temperature was about 2 degrees Celsius (35 degrees Fahrenheit). Only a handful of swimmers were successfully rescued from the water (Subcommittee of handful of swimmers were successfully rescued from the water (Subcommittee of the Committee on Commerce, 1912). Moreover, lifeboats were in short supply. The the Committee on Commerce, 1912). Moreover, lifeboats were in short supply. The Titanic actually carried more lifeboats than required by the rules of the time, which actually carried more lifeboats than required by the rules of the time, which were set by the British Board of Trade in 1894. However, those rules determined were set by the British Board of Trade in 1894. However, those rules determined the number of lifeboats according to a ship’s tonnage, rather than the number the number of lifeboats according to a ship’s tonnage, rather than the number of persons aboard. As a result, the of persons aboard. As a result, the Titanic carried only 20 lifeboats, which could carried only 20 lifeboats, which could accommodate 1,178 people, or 52 percent of the people aboard. As the accommodate 1,178 people, or 52 percent of the people aboard. As the Titanic began to sink, deck offi cers exacerbated the shortage by launching lifeboats that began to sink, deck offi cers exacerbated the shortage by launching lifeboats that were partially empty.were partially empty.

We have collected individual-level data on the passengers and crew on the We have collected individual-level data on the passengers and crew on the Titanic, which allow us to analyze some specifi c questions: Did physical strength , which allow us to analyze some specifi c questions: Did physical strength (being male and in prime age) or social status (being a fi rst- or second-class (being male and in prime age) or social status (being a fi rst- or second-class passenger) raise the survival chance? Was it favorable for survival to travel alone passenger) raise the survival chance? Was it favorable for survival to travel alone or in company? Does one’s role or function (being a crew member or a passenger) or in company? Does one’s role or function (being a crew member or a passenger) affect the probability of survival? Do social norms, such as “Women and children affect the probability of survival? Do social norms, such as “Women and children fi rst!” have any effect? Does nationality affect the chance of survival? We also fi rst!” have any effect? Does nationality affect the chance of survival? We also explore whether the time from impact to sinking might matter by comparing the explore whether the time from impact to sinking might matter by comparing the sinking of the sinking of the Titanic over nearly three hours to the sinking of the over nearly three hours to the sinking of the Lusitania in in 1915, which took only 18 minutes from when the torpedo hit the ship. The answers 1915, which took only 18 minutes from when the torpedo hit the ship. The answers to these questions may help us to better understand human behavior in natural to these questions may help us to better understand human behavior in natural disasters such as hurricanes and tsunamis, as well as in man-made accidents and disasters such as hurricanes and tsunamis, as well as in man-made accidents and

1 For detailed accounts of the disaster, see, for example, Lord (1955, 1986), Eaton and Haas (1994), Quinn (1999), and Ruffman (1999), as well as the Encyclopedia Titanica (http://www.encyclopedia-titanica.org/), and the information provided by RMS Titanic, Inc. (2010)—which was granted “salvor-in-possession” rights to the wreck by the U.S. District Court for the Eastern District of Virginia—at their website (http://www.titanic-online.com).

Page 3: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Bruno S. Frey, David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler 211

terrorist attacks.terrorist attacks.22 Indeed, this kind of study can serve as a useful supplement Indeed, this kind of study can serve as a useful supplement to the fi ndings of laboratory and fi eld experiments (Levitt and List, 2009; List, to the fi ndings of laboratory and fi eld experiments (Levitt and List, 2009; List, 2008). Such studies have taught us much about, for example, the extent to which 2008). Such studies have taught us much about, for example, the extent to which individuals behave altruistically in helping each other in donating to charities, individuals behave altruistically in helping each other in donating to charities, along with issues like the role of reciprocity and endogenous punishment (for along with issues like the role of reciprocity and endogenous punishment (for example, Fehr, Falk, and Fischbacher, 2008; DellaVigna, 2009). But these studies example, Fehr, Falk, and Fischbacher, 2008; DellaVigna, 2009). But these studies seek to capture behavior under “normal” conditions, and it is not clear how or seek to capture behavior under “normal” conditions, and it is not clear how or whether they would apply on a sinking ship in the North Atlantic Ocean.whether they would apply on a sinking ship in the North Atlantic Ocean.

Who Was on the Who Was on the TitanicTitanic??

We have constructed a detailed dataset of 2,207 persons who were confi rmed We have constructed a detailed dataset of 2,207 persons who were confi rmed to be aboard the R.M.S. to be aboard the R.M.S. Titanic. The data were gathered from the Encyclopedia . The data were gathered from the Encyclopedia Titanica and crosschecked with other sources.Titanica and crosschecked with other sources.33 Summary statistics of the variables Summary statistics of the variables collected are reported in Table 1. This table also reports in the last column for collected are reported in Table 1. This table also reports in the last column for each category the fraction that survived. All of the means in the table are shares each category the fraction that survived. All of the means in the table are shares of the people on the of the people on the Titanic who fell into each category—except for age, which is who fell into each category—except for age, which is expressed in years.expressed in years.

Out of 2,207 passengers and crewmembers, 1,501 people, or 68 percent, died. Out of 2,207 passengers and crewmembers, 1,501 people, or 68 percent, died. Based on the records, we were able to gather information about the gender, age, Based on the records, we were able to gather information about the gender, age, nationality, port where people boarded the nationality, port where people boarded the Titanic, and ticket price, which tells us , and ticket price, which tells us whether the passenger traveled fi rst-, second-, or third-class. In addition, we were whether the passenger traveled fi rst-, second-, or third-class. In addition, we were able to generate individual information related to travel plans and companions. able to generate individual information related to travel plans and companions. Limited information was available with regard to the cabin allocation: we were Limited information was available with regard to the cabin allocation: we were only able to fi nd this information for 15.2 percent of passengers, and it is based on only able to fi nd this information for 15.2 percent of passengers, and it is based on information provided by survivors, which means it is likely to include some bias. information provided by survivors, which means it is likely to include some bias. (In addition, because the (In addition, because the Titanic hit the iceberg shortly before midnight, some of hit the iceberg shortly before midnight, some of the passengers were not yet in their cabins, but elsewhere on the ship). Of the the passengers were not yet in their cabins, but elsewhere on the ship). Of the 2,207 persons onboard, the age of all but 21 individuals (four crewmembers and 2,207 persons onboard, the age of all but 21 individuals (four crewmembers and 17 passengers) is known. Thus, using age in the regression reduces the number of 17 passengers) is known. Thus, using age in the regression reduces the number of observations to 2,186 persons. Out of the 2,186 people, 1,300 were passengers and observations to 2,186 persons. Out of the 2,186 people, 1,300 were passengers and 886 were crew members. Among the passengers, 43 were servants. Additionally, of 886 were crew members. Among the passengers, 43 were servants. Additionally, of

2 In the economic literature, behavior under extreme conditions is rarely treated. Exceptions are Hirshleifer’s (1987) “behavior under adversity,” as well as studies dealing with specifi c events occurring in markets (Barro, 2006). Natural disasters and terrorism have found some attention, especially after Hurricane Katrina (Tavares 2004; Shugart, 2006; Kenny, 2009). Post-disaster effects have been treated more often (Dacy and Kunreuther, 1969; De Alessi, 1967; Kunreuther, 1967; Kunreuther and Slovic, 1978; Skidmore and Toya, 2002).3 While there is some anecdotal conjecture that other people may have been aboard the Titanic as stow-aways, all of the survivors were on the “offi cial” passenger lists. The cross-checked resources include Beavis (2002), Bryceson (1997), Committee on Commerce (1912), Eaton and Haas (1994), Geller (1998), Howell (1999), Lord (1955), Lord (1986), NSARM (2008), Quinn (1999), Ruffman (1999), U.S. National Archives (2008), and Wreck Commissioner’s Court (1912).

Page 4: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

212 Journal of Economic Perspectives

the 2,186 aboard, 1,704 were male (78 percent), and 460 of the 1,300 passengers the 2,186 aboard, 1,704 were male (78 percent), and 460 of the 1,300 passengers were female (35 percent).were female (35 percent).

We use the United Nations standard for age, which classifi es children as being We use the United Nations standard for age, which classifi es children as being less than 16 years of age. Thus, among the 2,186 people aboard, 124 were children less than 16 years of age. Thus, among the 2,186 people aboard, 124 were children (65 girls and 59 boys). Whether a passenger was in the company of friends and (65 girls and 59 boys). Whether a passenger was in the company of friends and family or traveled alone has been identifi ed by anecdotal evidence taken from family or traveled alone has been identifi ed by anecdotal evidence taken from family histories and known travel arrangements, ticket numbers, and cabin allo-family histories and known travel arrangements, ticket numbers, and cabin allo-cations; those passengers for whom there is no clear or known evidence were cations; those passengers for whom there is no clear or known evidence were assumed to be traveling alone and were assigned as “single.” We have complete assumed to be traveling alone and were assigned as “single.” We have complete information on each person’s country of residence (nationality). From this, we information on each person’s country of residence (nationality). From this, we have been able to generate several variables to investigate the effects of nation-have been able to generate several variables to investigate the effects of nation-ality. The largest national group (53 percent) was from England, followed by the ality. The largest national group (53 percent) was from England, followed by the United States (19 percent).United States (19 percent).

Table 1Summary Statistics for the Titanic

VariablesProportion of those on board Fraction survived

Survived 0.320Females 0.220 0.724Males 0.780 0.206Females without children 0.203 0.705Females with children 0.017 0.947Age < 16 (children) 0.052 0.478Age 16–50 0.891 0.309First-class passengers 0.147 0.617Second-class passengers 0.129 0.404Third-class passengers 0.321 0.253Crew 0.403 0.238Traveling alone 0.217 0.240Traveling with a group 0.380 0.342England 0.527 0.253Ireland 0.052 0.342Sweden 0.048 0.255United States 0.192 0.491Other nationalities 0.181 0.346

Mean age 30.044

Sources: The Encyclopedia Titanica (2008) has been used as the primary source, which was crosschecked across the following resources: Beavis (2002), Bryceson (1997), Subcommittee of the Committee on Commerce (1912), Eaton and Haas (1994), Geller (1998), Howell (1999), Lord (1955), Lord (1986), NSARM (2010), Quinn (1999), Ruffman (1999), U.S. National Archives (2010), and Wreck Commissioner’s Court (1912).Notes: The number of observations is 2,207, except for age, where it is 2,186. “Traveling with a group” includes couples with and without children and/or servants; singles with children and/or servants; and extended groups also covering friends.

Page 5: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster 213

Who Survived?Who Survived?

Before undertaking this study, we would have predicted that certain groups Before undertaking this study, we would have predicted that certain groups would be more likely to survive the sinking of the would be more likely to survive the sinking of the Titanic: prime-age men, fi rst-class : prime-age men, fi rst-class passengers, those traveling in company, crew members, women with children, and passengers, those traveling in company, crew members, women with children, and British subjects (because the British subjects (because the Titanic was perceived to be a British ship). But only was perceived to be a British ship). But only some of these hypotheses turned out to be correct. The descriptive statistics in some of these hypotheses turned out to be correct. The descriptive statistics in Table 1 indicate high survival rates for females, fi rst- and second-class passengers, Table 1 indicate high survival rates for females, fi rst- and second-class passengers, children, and a low survival rate for individuals traveling alone or with English and children, and a low survival rate for individuals traveling alone or with English and Swedish nationality. (Recall that the overall survival rate was only 0.320 percent, Swedish nationality. (Recall that the overall survival rate was only 0.320 percent, so a survival rate greater than 0.320 is considered “high.”) However, this purely so a survival rate greater than 0.320 is considered “high.”) However, this purely descriptive analysis gives information about the raw effects and not the partial descriptive analysis gives information about the raw effects and not the partial effects. Multiple regressions help us to better disentangle the single effects. Table 2 effects. Multiple regressions help us to better disentangle the single effects. Table 2 presents estimates from probit regressionspresents estimates from probit regressions44 in which the dependent variable in our in which the dependent variable in our analysis will be whether someone survived (analysis will be whether someone survived (==1) or not (1) or not (== 0). For each independent 0). For each independent variable in each row, the top fi gure is the estimated coeffi cient, and below is statis-variable in each row, the top fi gure is the estimated coeffi cient, and below is statis-tical signifi cance (tical signifi cance (z-value). Coeffi cients are not always easy to interpret in a probit -value). Coeffi cients are not always easy to interpret in a probit model, so we also present estimates of the marginal effects. For instance, looking model, so we also present estimates of the marginal effects. For instance, looking at the marginal effect, we see that a marginal change in age from the average of at the marginal effect, we see that a marginal change in age from the average of 30 years is associated with a 0.7 percent decrease in survival.30 years is associated with a 0.7 percent decrease in survival.55 The estimates include The estimates include an increasing number of determinants to show the extent to which the estimated an increasing number of determinants to show the extent to which the estimated coeffi cients are stable across different estimates.coeffi cients are stable across different estimates.

Table 2 shows that we are working with a large set of dummy variables. For Table 2 shows that we are working with a large set of dummy variables. For example, in the equation in column 1 we report results of two dummy variables example, in the equation in column 1 we report results of two dummy variables that describe the social status (ticket class) of the passengers: fi rst class and second that describe the social status (ticket class) of the passengers: fi rst class and second class. Each of these is coded 1 or 0 depending on whether or not the person is class. Each of these is coded 1 or 0 depending on whether or not the person is in that particular status or not. There are actually three categories of ticket status in that particular status or not. There are actually three categories of ticket status that are mutually exclusive and exhaustive: fi rst class, second class, and third class. that are mutually exclusive and exhaustive: fi rst class, second class, and third class.

4 Pr( y = 1 | x1, x2, . . . xk ) = Φ(α + β1 x1 + β2 x2 + . . . + βk xk ). Here y is a dummy variable indicating whether the passenger survived ( y = 1) or not ( y = 0); the variables (x1, x2, . . . xk ) are explanatory vari-ables such as gender, age, etc.; (α, β1, β2, . . . βk ) are parameters to estimate; and Φ is the cumulative standard normal distribution function. The role of Φ, which is increasing in its argument, is to keep the probability Pr( y = 1) in the zero to one interval. A linear probability model would generate fi tted probabilities that can be less than zero or greater than one. Each passenger contributes one observation on ( y, x1, x2, . . . xk ). From a sample of such observations, assumed independent, the parameters can be estimated by maximum likelihood. This is a standard probit model. 5 As usual in a probit model, the marginal effect of a continuous explanatory variable xj is interpreted

here through the partial derivative ∂Pr( y = 1 | x1, x2, . . . xk ) __

∂xj = βj ϕ(α + β1x1 + β2 x2 + . . . + βk xk ),

evaluated at the means, where ϕ is the standard normal density function. Since ϕ > 0, the sign of the marginal effect is the same as the sign of coeffi cients βj. For a discrete xj , a difference rather than a derivative is used in place of (1) (change of the dummy variable from 0 to 1). To get, for example, the marginal effect for age in the fi rst equation in Table 2, we multiply the coeffi cient – 0.018 with the stan-dard normal density function, which leads to a marginal effect of – 0.018 × 0.370 = – 0.0067. In this case we fi rst calculate the z-score for the stated probability of surviving (Pr(success) = 0.348) which is – 0.391. Next we calculate the probability density function value for that z-score, which gives us the value of 0.370.

Page 6: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

214 Journal of Economic Perspectives

Table 2Determinants of the Probability of Survival on the Titanic(dependent variable: survived = 1, not = 0)

Dependent variable: Survival of:

Independent variable Passenger Passenger Crew PassengerAll (passengers

and crew) Passenger

Female 1.462*** 1.468*** 1.858*** 1.456*** 1.471*** z-value 17.26 17.44 5.50 16.77 17.52 marginal effect 0.528 0.530 0.640 0.526 0.535Male ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. groupFemale with Children 2.368*** z-value 6.25 marginal effect 0.647Female, No Children 1.401*** z-value 15.87 marginal effect 0.512Age – 0.018*** z-value – 5.24 marginal effect – 0.007Age < 16 (Children) 0.382*** 0.807*** 0.743*** 0.835*** z-value 2.83 3.93 3.74 4.00 marginal effect 0.148 0.313 0.077 0.323Age 16–50 ref. group 0.470*** 0.418*** 0.492*** z-value 2.99 2.85 3.07 marginal effect 0.161 0.131 0.168Age > 50 ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. groupFirst-class Passengers 1.303*** 1.066*** 1.140*** 1.117*** 1.111*** z-value 11.34 10.62 10.75 10.55 10.39 marginal effect 0.485 0.403 0.429 0.420 0.420Second-class Passengers 0.462*** 0.387*** 0.407*** 0.482*** 0.488*** z-value 4.37 3.74 3.90 4.41 4.37 marginal effect 0.177 0.148 0.155 0.18 0.187Third-class Passengers ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. group ref. groupTraveling Alone – 0.057 – 0.064 – 0.051 z-value – 0.62 – 0.70 – 0.55 marginal effect – 0.021 – 0.022 – 0.019Traveling with a Group ref. group ref. group ref. groupEngland – 0.226** – 0.256** z-value – 2.48 – 2.45 marginal effect – 0.079 – 0.092Not from England ref. group ref. groupCrew   0.671*** z-value 5.80 marginal effect       0.237  

Observations 1,300 1,300 886 1,300 2,186 1,300Probability > χ2 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000Pseudo R 2 0.292 0.280 0.041 0.286 0.211 0.295

Notes: Dependent variable: Survival (value = 1). The reference groups are Male; Age > 50; Third-class Passenger; Traveling with a Group (couples with and without children and/or servants, singles with children and/or servants, and extended group also covering friends); and Not from England.*, **, *** represent statistical signifi cance at the 10, 5, and 1% levels, respectively.

Page 7: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Bruno S. Frey, David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler 215

The excluded category (third class) is the reference group, and each of the coef-The excluded category (third class) is the reference group, and each of the coef-fi cients is a comparison between an included category and the reference category. fi cients is a comparison between an included category and the reference category. To increase table readability we report all the reference groups in Table 2.To increase table readability we report all the reference groups in Table 2.

Prime-Age MenPrime-Age MenIn the situation of a large excess demand for places in the lifeboats, people with In the situation of a large excess demand for places in the lifeboats, people with

greater physical strength, namely people in their prime age and men, would have a greater physical strength, namely people in their prime age and men, would have a physical strength advantage over older people and women in the fi ght for survival.physical strength advantage over older people and women in the fi ght for survival.

The equations in columns 1 to 3 of Table 2 suggest that women both as passen-The equations in columns 1 to 3 of Table 2 suggest that women both as passen-gers and crew members had a higher probability of survival. Women passengers gers and crew members had a higher probability of survival. Women passengers had, compared to men, with age and traveling class held constant, a 53 percent had, compared to men, with age and traveling class held constant, a 53 percent higher chance of survival; it was even 64 percent higher for female crew members. higher chance of survival; it was even 64 percent higher for female crew members. The fourth column reveals that persons in prime age (16 to 50) had a 16 percent The fourth column reveals that persons in prime age (16 to 50) had a 16 percent higher chance to survive than older persons, which is consistent with the thesis that higher chance to survive than older persons, which is consistent with the thesis that physical strength was important in getting to the lifeboats.physical strength was important in getting to the lifeboats.

Social Status and Ticket ClassSocial Status and Ticket ClassThe 1,316 passengers (including maids) on the The 1,316 passengers (including maids) on the Titanic were traveling as were traveling as

mentioned before in three different classes: 325 in fi rst class, 285 in second class, mentioned before in three different classes: 325 in fi rst class, 285 in second class, and 706 in third class. It seems plausible that fi rst-class passengers would be more and 706 in third class. It seems plausible that fi rst-class passengers would be more able to secure a place on a lifeboat than people of lesser economic means. First-able to secure a place on a lifeboat than people of lesser economic means. First-class passengers were used to giving orders to the crew, and they were better able class passengers were used to giving orders to the crew, and they were better able to bargain—even offering fi nancial rewards. They were also in closer contact to bargain—even offering fi nancial rewards. They were also in closer contact with the upper echelon crewmembers: in particular, First Offi cer Murdoch, who with the upper echelon crewmembers: in particular, First Offi cer Murdoch, who commanded the loading of lifeboats on the starboard side, and Second Offi cer commanded the loading of lifeboats on the starboard side, and Second Offi cer Lightoller, who did the same on the port side. Moreover, the fi rst-class passengers Lightoller, who did the same on the port side. Moreover, the fi rst-class passengers had better access to information about the danger, and the lifeboats were located had better access to information about the danger, and the lifeboats were located close to the fi rst-class cabins. In contrast, most third-class passengers had little idea close to the fi rst-class cabins. In contrast, most third-class passengers had little idea where the lifeboats were located (safety drills for passengers were introduced only where the lifeboats were located (safety drills for passengers were introduced only after the after the Titanic disaster), and they did not know how to reach the upper decks disaster), and they did not know how to reach the upper decks where the lifeboats were stowed.where the lifeboats were stowed.

Thus, it seems plausible that fi rst-class passengers would have a higher prob-Thus, it seems plausible that fi rst-class passengers would have a higher prob-ability of survival than second-class passengers; in turn, second-class passengers ability of survival than second-class passengers; in turn, second-class passengers would have a higher probability of survival than third-class passengers. Results in the would have a higher probability of survival than third-class passengers. Results in the fi rst and second columns of Table 2 indicate that fi rst- and second-class passengers fi rst and second columns of Table 2 indicate that fi rst- and second-class passengers had a signifi cantly better chance to survive than passengers in third class. Passengers had a signifi cantly better chance to survive than passengers in third class. Passengers traveling fi rst class had a more than 40 percent higher chance, and those in second traveling fi rst class had a more than 40 percent higher chance, and those in second class about a 16 percent higher chance, to be saved than those in third class.class about a 16 percent higher chance, to be saved than those in third class.

Social EmbeddednessSocial EmbeddednessPassengers traveling in the company of family and friends may be expected to Passengers traveling in the company of family and friends may be expected to

have a better chance of survival in life and death situations because they are more have a better chance of survival in life and death situations because they are more likely to receive information indirectly and to obtain psychological and physical likely to receive information indirectly and to obtain psychological and physical support from others. However, equations in columns 4, 5, and 6 in Table 2 suggest support from others. However, equations in columns 4, 5, and 6 in Table 2 suggest

Page 8: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

216 Journal of Economic Perspectives

that it did not matter for survival whether one traveled alone or in the company of that it did not matter for survival whether one traveled alone or in the company of family and friends. It may be that those who were alone were able to focus more family and friends. It may be that those who were alone were able to focus more specifi cally on their own best interests, rather than being slowed by a group, and specifi cally on their own best interests, rather than being slowed by a group, and that this counterbalanced any benefi t of traveling with a group.that this counterbalanced any benefi t of traveling with a group.

FunctionFunctionOne would expect the experienced crew of 886 men and women to be better One would expect the experienced crew of 886 men and women to be better

prepared for a catastrophic event than the passengers, to be informed earlier and prepared for a catastrophic event than the passengers, to be informed earlier and better about the location of lifeboats and the danger of sinking, and to have closer better about the location of lifeboats and the danger of sinking, and to have closer personal contacts with the crewmembers in charge of loading the lifeboats. On the personal contacts with the crewmembers in charge of loading the lifeboats. On the other hand, the crew has a duty to help save passengers, and they are only supposed other hand, the crew has a duty to help save passengers, and they are only supposed to abandon a sinking ship when that task has been fulfi lled.to abandon a sinking ship when that task has been fulfi lled.

We expect that in life or death situations, such as that encountered on the We expect that in life or death situations, such as that encountered on the Titanic, selfi sh interests tend to dominate. Indeed, the equation in column 5 of , selfi sh interests tend to dominate. Indeed, the equation in column 5 of Table 2 suggests that the members of the crew had, ceteris paribus, a 24 percent Table 2 suggests that the members of the crew had, ceteris paribus, a 24 percent higher chance of saving themselves than did third-class passengers.higher chance of saving themselves than did third-class passengers.

Social NormsSocial NormsA key social norm under life and death conditions is that women and children A key social norm under life and death conditions is that women and children

are to be saved fi rst. This norm may operate either through the actions of male are to be saved fi rst. This norm may operate either through the actions of male passengers, or through the offi cers loading the lifeboats. Interestingly, no interna-passengers, or through the offi cers loading the lifeboats. Interestingly, no interna-tional maritime law requires that women and children be rescued fi rst. However, tional maritime law requires that women and children be rescued fi rst. However, similar norms can be found in other areas where people need to be evacuated. similar norms can be found in other areas where people need to be evacuated. Humanitarian agencies often evacuate “vulnerable” and “innocent” civilians, such Humanitarian agencies often evacuate “vulnerable” and “innocent” civilians, such as women, children, and elderly people fi rst. The Geneva Convention provides as women, children, and elderly people fi rst. The Geneva Convention provides special protection and evacuation priority for pregnant women and mothers of special protection and evacuation priority for pregnant women and mothers of young children (Carpenter, 2003).young children (Carpenter, 2003).

The results in column 6 of Table 2 suggest that this norm was indeed in force. The results in column 6 of Table 2 suggest that this norm was indeed in force. Women in the company of children had a 65 percent higher survival chance than Women in the company of children had a 65 percent higher survival chance than men, while females without children had a 51 percent higher survival chance. men, while females without children had a 51 percent higher survival chance. Focusing only on female passengers in a further specifi cation not reported in Focusing only on female passengers in a further specifi cation not reported in Table 2, we fi nd that women with children had a 19 percent higher chance of Table 2, we fi nd that women with children had a 19 percent higher chance of surviving than women without children.surviving than women without children.

NationalityNationalityThe The Titanic was built in Great Britain, operated by British subjects, and manned was built in Great Britain, operated by British subjects, and manned

by a British crew. Interestingly enough, the Ocean Steam Navigation Company, by a British crew. Interestingly enough, the Ocean Steam Navigation Company, popularly known as the “White Star” line because of the white star appearing on popularly known as the “White Star” line because of the white star appearing on the company fl ag, was under the management of the industrial giant, J.P. Morgan. the company fl ag, was under the management of the industrial giant, J.P. Morgan. Nevertheless, the public perceived the Nevertheless, the public perceived the Titanic as being a British ship. Thus, it might as being a British ship. Thus, it might be expected that the crew would give preference to British subjects, easily identifi ed be expected that the crew would give preference to British subjects, easily identifi ed by their language.by their language.

However, columns 4 and 5 of Table 2 reveal a quite different picture. Control-However, columns 4 and 5 of Table 2 reveal a quite different picture. Control-ling for all other infl uences considered above, British passengers had between an ling for all other infl uences considered above, British passengers had between an

Page 9: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster 217

8 or 9 percent lower chance of surviving the 8 or 9 percent lower chance of surviving the Titanic disaster than the passengers disaster than the passengers of other nationalities aboard. This may be attributed to the fact that the British of other nationalities aboard. This may be attributed to the fact that the British behaved with a “stiff upper lip,” or perhaps British subjects were less likely to believe behaved with a “stiff upper lip,” or perhaps British subjects were less likely to believe that the that the Titanic would sink. would sink.

The Infl uence of Time as a Disaster UnfoldsThe Infl uence of Time as a Disaster Unfolds

It took 2 hours and 40 minutes between the time the It took 2 hours and 40 minutes between the time the Titanic hit the iceberg hit the iceberg and the moment the ship sank to the bottom of the sea. It can be argued that and the moment the ship sank to the bottom of the sea. It can be argued that this left suffi cient time for socially determined patterns of behavior, such as letting this left suffi cient time for socially determined patterns of behavior, such as letting the passengers of the better classes, or women and children, to be saved fi rst. We the passengers of the better classes, or women and children, to be saved fi rst. We therefore compare the survival probabilities on the therefore compare the survival probabilities on the Titanic with those of another with those of another well-known shipping disaster, the sinking of the well-known shipping disaster, the sinking of the Lusitania only three years later, on only three years later, on May 7, 1915, as a result of a torpedo attack by a German U-boat, costing the lives May 7, 1915, as a result of a torpedo attack by a German U-boat, costing the lives of 1,313 people. It can be argued that on the of 1,313 people. It can be argued that on the Lusitania, selfi sh behavior prevailed, , selfi sh behavior prevailed, while on the while on the Titanic the adherence to social norms and social status dominated. This the adherence to social norms and social status dominated. This difference could be attributed to the fact that the difference could be attributed to the fact that the Lusitania sank in only 18 minutes, sank in only 18 minutes, creating a situation in which the short-run fl ight impulse dominates behavior.creating a situation in which the short-run fl ight impulse dominates behavior.

In addition, the In addition, the Lusitania was sunk by violence during a time of war, which was sunk by violence during a time of war, which may provoke different reactions. For example, before the sailing of the may provoke different reactions. For example, before the sailing of the Lusitania, warning notices had been printed in the leading newspapers reminding transat-warning notices had been printed in the leading newspapers reminding transat-lantic passengers that a state of war was in effect and that any vessel traveling under lantic passengers that a state of war was in effect and that any vessel traveling under the British fl ag was liable to come under attack and passengers sailed at their own the British fl ag was liable to come under attack and passengers sailed at their own risk. On the other hand, there are several reasonable suppositions supporting risk. On the other hand, there are several reasonable suppositions supporting the idea that the the idea that the Lusitania was not at severe risk, primarily that it was capable of was not at severe risk, primarily that it was capable of speeds fast enough to outrun enemy torpedoes. In addition, the speeds fast enough to outrun enemy torpedoes. In addition, the Lusitania was a was a vessel carrying civilian passengers, not a warship, and, it was carrying a considerable vessel carrying civilian passengers, not a warship, and, it was carrying a considerable number of neutral American civilians. Maritime law states that in wartime merchant number of neutral American civilians. Maritime law states that in wartime merchant vessels must be given a warning prior to attack, whereas warships should not expect vessels must be given a warning prior to attack, whereas warships should not expect any warning. The any warning. The Lusitania was never given such a warning by the attacking U-boat was never given such a warning by the attacking U-boat (Bailey, 1935).(Bailey, 1935).

We were able to collect data containing detailed information about gender, We were able to collect data containing detailed information about gender, age, and ticket price (and thus passenger-class status) for the age, and ticket price (and thus passenger-class status) for the Lusitania. Table 3 . Table 3 shows that the passenger structure with respect to the share of women, age, and shows that the passenger structure with respect to the share of women, age, and fi rst-class passengers on the fi rst-class passengers on the Titanic and the and the Lusitania was quite similar. In both disas- was quite similar. In both disas-ters, 32 percent of the persons aboard survived. On the other hand, the survival rate ters, 32 percent of the persons aboard survived. On the other hand, the survival rate with respect to gender and ticket class is substantially different. In the with respect to gender and ticket class is substantially different. In the Lusitania, the , the survival rates of women, fi rst-class, and second-class passengers are now lower than survival rates of women, fi rst-class, and second-class passengers are now lower than the average survival rate.the average survival rate.

We again carried out a probit regression analysis in which the dependent vari-We again carried out a probit regression analysis in which the dependent vari-able indicates whether an individual survived the disaster (able indicates whether an individual survived the disaster (==1) or did not survive 1) or did not survive ((= = 0). Table 40). Table 4 shows the estimated parameters, the signifi cance level (indicated by shows the estimated parameters, the signifi cance level (indicated by z-values), and the marginal effects for the -values), and the marginal effects for the Lusitania. These estimates reveal a smaller . These estimates reveal a smaller

Page 10: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

218 Journal of Economic Perspectives

number of statistically signifi cant determinants, suggesting that in the case of the number of statistically signifi cant determinants, suggesting that in the case of the sinking of the sinking of the Lusitania random elements played a larger role than in the case of random elements played a larger role than in the case of the the Titanic.

The results in Table 4 indeed indicate that people in their prime age (16–50) The results in Table 4 indeed indicate that people in their prime age (16–50) had a statistically signifi cantly higher survival rate than older people. Prime-age had a statistically signifi cantly higher survival rate than older people. Prime-age males had a 17 percent higher survival probability than other persons aboard. males had a 17 percent higher survival probability than other persons aboard. Women in their prime age had a 20 percent higher chance of survival, but not Women in their prime age had a 20 percent higher chance of survival, but not women in general. A comparison with the women in general. A comparison with the Titanic disaster suggests that when a disaster suggests that when a disaster is perceived to be imminent, individuals in their prime age have an advan-disaster is perceived to be imminent, individuals in their prime age have an advan-tage, but not men as such (the coeffi cient for female is not statistically signifi cant). tage, but not men as such (the coeffi cient for female is not statistically signifi cant). Unlike the situation on the Unlike the situation on the Titanic, fi rst- and second-class passengers did not have , fi rst- and second-class passengers did not have a signifi cantly higher chance of survival on the a signifi cantly higher chance of survival on the Lusitania; as shown in Table 4, fi rst-; as shown in Table 4, fi rst-class passengers fared even worse than those in third class.class passengers fared even worse than those in third class.

The empirical analysis is consistent with the view that the effects of status The empirical analysis is consistent with the view that the effects of status (passengers traveling in higher classes have a better chance of surviving) and social (passengers traveling in higher classes have a better chance of surviving) and social norms (such as saving women and children fi rst) depend on time. It seems that on norms (such as saving women and children fi rst) depend on time. It seems that on the more slowly sinking the more slowly sinking Titanic pro-social behavior played a larger role, while more pro-social behavior played a larger role, while more selfi sh conduct prevailed on the rapidly sinking selfi sh conduct prevailed on the rapidly sinking Lusitania. Of course, time may not . Of course, time may not be the only factor at work. Natural experiments based on naval disasters may well be the only factor at work. Natural experiments based on naval disasters may well have other factors for which control variables would be useful.have other factors for which control variables would be useful.

ConclusionsConclusions

The econometric estimates of the factors determining survival during the The econometric estimates of the factors determining survival during the sinking of the sinking of the Titanic produce a coherent story. However, this story is not neces- produce a coherent story. However, this story is not neces-sarily in line with the simple model of selfi sh sarily in line with the simple model of selfi sh homo economicus. While people in their While people in their prime were more likely to be saved, it was women—rather than men—who had a prime were more likely to be saved, it was women—rather than men—who had a

Table 3Passenger Structure on the Lusitania and the Titanic

Lusitania Titanic

Variables Mean Fraction survived Mean Fraction survived

Survived 0.326 0.320Females 0.261 0.280 0.220 0.720Males 0.739 0.343 0.780 0.206Age (years) 31.570 30.040First-class passengers 0.149 0.193 0.147 0.617Second-class passengers 0.307 0.295 0.129 0.404Third-class passengers 0.189 0.325 0.321 0.253

Sources: For the Lusitania data, The Lusitania Resource (2010) has been used as the primary source, which was crosschecked across Butler (2000), Lusitania Online (2010), O’Sullivan (2000), Preston (2002), Wreck Commissioner’s Court (1915). For sources of the data on the Titanic, refer to Table 1.

Page 11: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Bruno S. Frey, David A. Savage, and Benno Torgler 219

better chance of being saved. Children also had a higher chance of surviving. At the better chance of being saved. Children also had a higher chance of surviving. At the time of the disaster, the unwritten social norm of “saving women and children fi rst” time of the disaster, the unwritten social norm of “saving women and children fi rst” seems to have been enforced.seems to have been enforced.

However, we do fi nd evidence suggesting that effects predicted using the stan-However, we do fi nd evidence suggesting that effects predicted using the stan-dard dard homo economicus model are also important. People in their prime age drowned model are also important. People in their prime age drowned less often than older people. Passengers with high fi nancial means, traveling in less often than older people. Passengers with high fi nancial means, traveling in fi rst class, were better able to save themselves as were passengers in second class fi rst class, were better able to save themselves as were passengers in second class (compared to third class). Crew members who had access to better informational (compared to third class). Crew members who had access to better informational and relational resources managed to survive more often than others aboard. In and relational resources managed to survive more often than others aboard. In

Table 4Determinants of the Survival on the Lusitania(dependent variable: survived = 1; not = 0)

Female –0.064 z-value –0.33 marginal effect –0.022

Male ref. group

Age < 16 (Children) 0.312 z-value 1.5 marginal effect 0.111

Age > 50 ref. group

Female, age 16–50 0.556** z-value 2.59 marginal effect 0.199

Male, age 16–50 0.493** z-value 2.42 marginal effect 0.166

First-class Passengers –0.384*** z-value –3.33 marginal effect –0.123

Second-class Passengers 0.033 z-value 0.31 marginal effect 0.011

Third-class Passengers ref. group

Observations 933Probability > χ2 0Pseudo R 2 0.025

Notes: We carried out a probit regression analysis in which the dependent variable indicates whether an individual survived the disaster (=1) or did not survive (=0). For each variable, we show the estimated parameters, the signifi cance level (indicated by z-values) and the marginal effects. Reference groups are Male, Age > 50, and Third-class Passengers.*, **, *** represent statistical signifi cance at the 10, 5, and 1% levels, respectively.

Page 12: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

220 Journal of Economic Perspectives

contrast, the British passengers who were the same nationality as most of the crew contrast, the British passengers who were the same nationality as most of the crew members did not use this fact to their advantage. Differences in context are likely members did not use this fact to their advantage. Differences in context are likely to matter in life-or-death situations. The comparison between the to matter in life-or-death situations. The comparison between the Titanic and the and the Lusitania suggests that when time is scarce, individual self-interested fl ight behavior suggests that when time is scarce, individual self-interested fl ight behavior predominates, while altruism and social norms and power through social status predominates, while altruism and social norms and power through social status become more important if there is suffi cient time for them to evolve.become more important if there is suffi cient time for them to evolve.

The sinking of the The sinking of the Titanic represents a well-documented, dramatic, life-or-death represents a well-documented, dramatic, life-or-death situation. However, even under these extreme situations, the behavior of human situation. However, even under these extreme situations, the behavior of human beings is not random or inexplicable, but can be explored and, at least in part, beings is not random or inexplicable, but can be explored and, at least in part, explained by economic analysis.explained by economic analysis.

References

American Society for Reproductive Medicine (A.S.R.M.). 2003. “Reproductive Aging in Women.” Patient’s Fact Sheet. Chicago: American Society for Reproductive Medicine.

Bailey, Thomas A. 1935. “The Sinking of the Lusitania.” The American Historical Review, 41(1): 54–73.

Barro, Robert J. 2006. “Rare Disasters and Asset Markets in the Twentieth Century.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(3): 823–66.

Beavis, Debbie. 2002. Who Sailed on Titanic? The Defi nitive Passenger Lists. 1st ed. London: Ian Allen Publishing.

Bryceson, Dave. 1997. The Titanic Disaster: As Reported in the British National Press April–July 1912. New York: W.W. Norton & Company Inc.

Butler, Daniel Allen. 2000. The Lusitania: The Life, Loss and Legacy of an Ocean Legend. Mechanicsburg, PA: Stackpole Books.

Camerer, Colin F., and Ernst Fehr. 2006. “When Does ‘Economic Man’ Dominate Social Behavior?” Science, 311(5757): 47–52.

Carpenter, R. Charli. 2003. “Women and Children First: Gender, Norms and Humanitarian Evacuation in the Balkans 1991–95.” International Organization, 57(4): 661–94.

Dacy, Douglas, and Howard Kunreuther. 1969. The Economics of Natural Disasters: Implications for Federal Policy. New York: Free Press.

De Alessi, Louis. 1975. “Towards an Analysis of Postdisaster Cooperation.” American Economic Review, 65(1): 127–138.

DellaVigna, Stefano. 2009. “Psychology and

Economics: Evidence from the Field.” Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2): 315–72.

Eaton, John P., and Charles Haas. 1994. Titanic: Triumph and Tragedy, 1st ed. London: Patrick Stephens Ltd.

Elster, Jon. 2007. Explaining Social Behavior. More Nuts and Bolts for the Social Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Encyclopedia Titanica. 2010. Website “contains facts, opinion and media relating to RMS Titanic.” http://www.encyclopedia–titanica.org.

Fehr, Ernst, Armin Falk, and Urs Fischbacher. 2008. “Testing Theories of Fairness—Intentions Matter.” Games and Economic Behavior, 62(1): 287–303.

Fritz, Charles E., and Harry B. Williams. 1957. “The Human Being in Disasters: A Research Perspective.” The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 309(1): 42–51.

Geller, Judith B. 1998. Titanic: Women and Children First. New York: W.W. Norton.

Gneezy, Uri, and John A. List. 2006. “Putting Behavioral Economics to Work: Testing for Gift Exchange in Labor Markets Using Field Experi-ments.” Econometrica, 74(5): 1365–84.

Gray, Jeffrey A. 1988. The Psychology of Fear and Stress, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hirshleifer, David, and Tyler Shumway. 2003. “Good Day Sunshine: Stock Returns on the Weather.” Journal of Finance, 58(3): 1009–1032.

Hirshleifer, Jack. 1987. Economic Behaviour in Adversity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Page 13: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster 221

Howell, Richard. 1999. The Myth of the Titanic. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

Kenny, Charles. 2009. “Why Do People Die in Earthquakes? The Costs, Benefi ts and Institu-tions of Disaster Risk Reduction in Developing Countries.” World Bank Policy Research Working Paper 4823.

Kunreuther, Howard. 1967. “The Peculiar Economics of Disaster.” Papers on Non-Market Decision Making, 3(1): 67–83.

Kunreuther, Howard, and Paul Slovic. 1978. “Economics, Psychology, and Protective Behavior.” American Economic Review, 68(2): 64–69.

Levitt, Steven D., and John A. List. 2009. “Field Experiments in Economics: The Past, the Present and the Future.” European Economic Review, 53(1): 1–18.

List, John A. 2008. “Homo experimentalis Evolves.” Science, 321(5886): 207–208.

Lord, Walter. 1955. A Night to Remember. New York: Bantam.

Lord, Walter. 1986. The Night Lives On. New York: Morrow.

Lusitania Online. 2010. Website is “The home port of RMS Lusitania. No. 1 for information on the ship and her last Master, Captain W. T. Turner.” http://www.lusitania.net. Last accessed 2009.

Lusitania Resource, The. 2010. Website “dedi-cated to the RMS Lusitania and her passengers and crew . . .” http://www.rmslusitania.info (last accessed 2009.)

Mawson, Anthony R. 2007. Mass Panic and Social Attachment. Surrey: Ashgate.

Nova Scotia Archives and Records Management (NSARM). 2010. “RMS Titanic: Full Electronic List of Bodies and Disposition of Same.” http://www.gov.ns.ca/nsarm/virtual/titanic/fulllist.asp.

O’Sullivan, Patrick. 2000. The Lusitania: Unrav-eling the Mysteries. Staplehurst, Ireland: Collins Press.

Polinsky, A. Mitchell, and Steven Shavell. 2000. “The Fairness of Sanctions: Some Implications for Optimal Enforcement Policy.” American Law and Economics Review, 2(2): 223–37.

Preston, Diana. 2002. Willful Murder. London: Transworld Publishers.

Quinn, Paul J. 1999. Dusk to Dawn: Survivor Accounts of the Last Night on the Titanic. USA: Fantail.

Reiley, David H., and John A. List. 2008. “Field Experiments in Economics.” In The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, edited by Steven N. Durlauf and Lawrence E. Blume. London: Palgrave Macmillian.

Ripley, Amanda. 2008. The Unthinkable: Who Survives When Disaster Strikes–and Why. New York: Crown.

Ruffman, Alan. 1999. Titanic Remembered: The Unsinkable Ship and Halifax. Halifax, NS, Canada: Formac Publishing Company.

RMS Titanic, Inc. 2010. Website. http://www.titanic-online.com.

Shughart, William. 2006. “Katrinanomics: The Politics and Economics of Disaster Relief.” Public Choice, 127(1): 31–53.

Skidmore, Mark, and Hideki Toya. 2002. Do Natural Disasters Promote Long-Run Growth? Economic Inquiry, 40(4): 664–87.

Subcommittee of the Committee on Commerce. 1912. Offi cial Transcript of the Causes Leading to the Wreck of the White Star Liner Titanic. U.S. Senate Inquiry.

Tavares, Jose. 2004. The Open Society Assesses Its Enemies: Shocks, Disasters and Terrorist Attacks. Journal of Monetary Economics, 51(5): 1039–70.

Tierney, Kathleen J. 2006. “Metaphors Matter: Disaster Myths, Media Frames, and Their Conse-quences in Hurricane Katrina.” The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 604(1): 57–81.

U.S. National Archives. 2010. “Partial List of Survivors of the Titanic who were taken aboard the Carpathia, which arrived at the Port of New York, NY, April 18, 1912.” Roll 1883, Vol. 4183. http://www.archives.gov.

Wallace, Anthony F. C. 1956. Tornado in Worcester. An Exploratory Study of Individual and Community Behavior in an Extreme Situation. Washington, DC: National Academy of Science, National Research Council.

Wreck Commissioner’s Court. 1912. Formal Investigation into the Loss of the S.S. Titanic: Evidence, Appendices and Index. Reprinted 1998. London: Public Records Offi ce.

Wreck Commissioner’s Court. 1915. Formal Investigation into the Loss of the R.M.S. Lusitania. London: Public Records Offi ce.

Page 14: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

222 Journal of Economic Perspectives

Page 15: Behavior under Extreme Conditions: The Titanic Disaster · and series, the list would include Saved from the Titanic (1912), In Nacht und Eis (1912), Atlantic (1929), Titanic (1943

This article has been cited by:

1. Tatsuya Nogami. What Behaviors We Think We Do When a Disaster Strikes: Misconceptions andRealities of Human Disaster Behavior 343-362. [Crossref]

2. Herbert S. Klein. 2017. The “Historical Turn” in the Social Sciences. The Journal of InterdisciplinaryHistory 48:3, 295-312. [Crossref]

3. Aakriti Singh, Shipra Saraswat, Neetu Faujdar. Analyzing Titanic disaster using machine learningalgorithms 406-411. [Crossref]

4. Shuo-Chun Weng, Yu-Chi Chen, Ching-Yu Chen, Yuan-Yang Cheng, Yih-Jing Tang, Shu-Hui Yang,Jwu-Rong Lin. 2017. Application of qualitative response models in a relevance study of older adults'health depreciation and medical care demand. Geriatrics & Gerontology International 17:4, 645-652.[Crossref]

5. D. A. Savage. 2016. Those left behind: Euthanasia, suicide and other regarding preferences.Rationality and Society 28:4, 439-452. [Crossref]

6. Mohammed Al-Taie, Siti Shamsuddin. Social Networks and Information Dissemination for DisasterRisk Management 385-437. [Crossref]

7. James R. Wible. 2016. Scientific misconduct and the responsible conduct of research in science andeconomics. Review of Social Economy 74:1, 7-32. [Crossref]

8. Ko Kuwabara. 2015. Do Reputation Systems Undermine Trust? Divergent Effects of EnforcementType on Generalized Trust and Trustworthiness1. American Journal of Sociology 120:5, 1390.[Crossref]

9. Clemens Kroneberg. 2014. Understanding Merton. KZfSS Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie undSozialpsychologie 66:4, 643-653. [Crossref]

10. Jiuh-Biing Sheu. 2014. Post-disaster relief–service centralized logistics distribution with survivorresilience maximization. Transportation Research Part B: Methodological 68, 288-314. [Crossref]

11. Edson Gandiwa, Sylvie Sprangers, Severine van Bommel, Ignas M.A. Heitkönig, Cees Leeuwis,Herbert H.T. Prins. 2014. Spill-over effect in media framing: Representations of wildlife conservationin Zimbabwean and international media, 1989–2010. Journal for Nature Conservation 22:5, 413-423.[Crossref]

12. Tatsuya Nogami, Fujio Yoshida. 2014. Disaster myths after the Great East Japan Disaster and theeffects of information sources on belief in such myths. Disasters 38:s2, s190-s205. [Crossref]

13. Hussein Soliman. 2013. The sinking of the Al-Salam Boccaccio 98 ferry in the Red Sea: Theintegration of disaster support system models and emergency management experience. InternationalJournal of Disaster Risk Reduction 4, 44-51. [Crossref]

14. M. Elinder, O. Erixson. 2012. Gender, social norms, and survival in maritime disasters. Proceedings ofthe National Academy of Sciences 109:33, 13220-13224. [Crossref]

15. Eiji Yamamura. 2012. Effect of Free Media on Views Regarding Nuclear Energy after the FukushimaAccident. Kyklos 65:1, 132-141. [Crossref]

16. Christoph Dobler, Matthias Kowald, Nadine Rieser-Schüssler, Kay W. Axhausen. 2012. Within-Day Replanning of Exceptional Events. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the TransportationResearch Board 2302:1, 138-147. [Crossref]