ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and...

12
ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and Karin Siisser Four experiments examined the effects of happiness on the tendency to use stereotypes in social judgment. In each experiment, individuals who had been induced to feel happy rendered more stereotypic judgments than did those in a neutral mood. Experiment 1 demonstrated this phenome- non with a mood induction procedure that involved recalling life experiences. Experiments 2 and 3 suggested that the greater reliance on stereotypes evident in the judgments of happy individuals was not attributable to cognitive capacity deficits created by intrusive happy thoughts or by cognitively disruptive excitement or energetic arousal that may accompany the experience of happiness. In Experiment 4, happy individuals again were found to render more stereotypic judgments, except under conditions in which they had been told that they would be held accountable for their judg- ments. These results suggest that although happy people's tendency to engage in stereotypic thinking may be pervasive, they are quite capable of avoiding the influence of stereotypes in their judgments when situational factors provide a motivational impetus for such effort. Discovering the conditions under which group stereotypes are likely to be applied in forming impressions of and making judgments about individuals has been an issue of perennial in- terest in social psychology. Factors such as information overload (Pratto & Bargh, 1991; Stangor & Duan, 1991) and task diffi- culty (Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein, 1987), for example, have been shown to increase the social perceiver's reliance on stereo- typic preconceptions (for a review, see Hamilton & Sherman, in press). In the present research, we investigated the role of emotion, specifically happiness, in the application of stereo- types during social information processing. Does being happy have any impact on the likelihood of stereotyping others? If so, what is the mechanism involved? It was these questions that we sought to address. Interest in the relationship between emotion and stereotyping is certainly not new. However, previous attempts to understand the role of affective experience in prejudice and stereotyping have focused almost exclusively on the impact of negative emo- tions. Conventional wisdom indicates that it is during times of stress, anxiety, or hostility that prejudice and stereotypes are most likely to emerge and exert their influence on social percep- tion. Psychological research lends credence to the idea that an- ger, conflict, frustration, and anxiety are indeed associated with Galen V. Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University; Geoffrey P. Kramer, Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Indiana University-Kokomo; Karin Siisser, Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University. We are grateful to Linda Jackson and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on a previous version of this report. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Galen V. Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824-1117. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. the tendency to see the world through a stereotypic lens (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Dol- lard, Miller, Doob, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939; Greenberg et al., 1990; Greenberg, Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel, 1992; Peak, Muney, & Clay, 1960; Sherif & Sherif, 1953; Ste- phan & Stephan, 1985; Wills, 1981). From the standpoint of the various theoretical ideas that are represented by the array of research addressing the link between various negative emotions and the phenomena of prejudice and stereotyping, negative affect often serves as a motivational impetus for these processes. Whether by displacement, projection, rationalization, or some other defensive process, individuals in negative states may de- rive some measure of relief or gratification by expressing their prejudices and stereotypes toward outgroups. According to these approaches, then, negative affect is the fuel for the fire of prejudice and stereotyping. By implication, the experience of happiness or contentment should provide little motivational impetus to view others in stereotypic terms. Perhaps it is during times of happiness that we are most likely to look beyond our gross generalizations about outgroups. Contemporary research on stereotyping has often deempha- sized the motivational aspects of the phenomenon that were so central to many earlier conceptualizations. Instead, the focus has been on stereotyping as a mundane cognitive function that provides simplification and structure to our subjective experi- ence of the complex social milieu in which we live (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Hamilton, 1979;Rothbart, 1981). Several stud- ies suggest that stereotypes operate as heuristic cues in social information processing, providing a basis for a quick response to members of outgroups that may suffice whenever social per- ceivers cannot, or prefer not to, engage in a more thoughtful analysis of the unique personal qualities of specific outgroup members (Bodenhausen, 1988, 1990; Bodenhausen & Lich- tenstein, 1987; Bodenhausen & Wyer, 1985; see also Chaiken, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, Vol. 66, No. 4, 621-632 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/94/S3.00 621

Transcript of ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and...

Page 1: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION

Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment

Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and Karin Siisser

Four experiments examined the effects of happiness on the tendency to use stereotypes in socialjudgment. In each experiment, individuals who had been induced to feel happy rendered morestereotypic judgments than did those in a neutral mood. Experiment 1 demonstrated this phenome-non with a mood induction procedure that involved recalling life experiences. Experiments 2 and 3suggested that the greater reliance on stereotypes evident in the judgments of happy individuals wasnot attributable to cognitive capacity deficits created by intrusive happy thoughts or by cognitivelydisruptive excitement or energetic arousal that may accompany the experience of happiness. InExperiment 4, happy individuals again were found to render more stereotypic judgments, exceptunder conditions in which they had been told that they would be held accountable for their judg-ments. These results suggest that although happy people's tendency to engage in stereotypic thinkingmay be pervasive, they are quite capable of avoiding the influence of stereotypes in their judgmentswhen situational factors provide a motivational impetus for such effort.

Discovering the conditions under which group stereotypesare likely to be applied in forming impressions of and makingjudgments about individuals has been an issue of perennial in-terest in social psychology. Factors such as information overload(Pratto & Bargh, 1991; Stangor & Duan, 1991) and task diffi-culty (Bodenhausen & Lichtenstein, 1987), for example, havebeen shown to increase the social perceiver's reliance on stereo-typic preconceptions (for a review, see Hamilton & Sherman,in press). In the present research, we investigated the role ofemotion, specifically happiness, in the application of stereo-types during social information processing. Does being happyhave any impact on the likelihood of stereotyping others? If so,what is the mechanism involved? It was these questions that wesought to address.

Interest in the relationship between emotion and stereotypingis certainly not new. However, previous attempts to understandthe role of affective experience in prejudice and stereotypinghave focused almost exclusively on the impact of negative emo-tions. Conventional wisdom indicates that it is during times ofstress, anxiety, or hostility that prejudice and stereotypes aremost likely to emerge and exert their influence on social percep-tion. Psychological research lends credence to the idea that an-ger, conflict, frustration, and anxiety are indeed associated with

Galen V. Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Michigan StateUniversity; Geoffrey P. Kramer, Department of Social and BehavioralSciences, Indiana University-Kokomo; Karin Siisser, Department ofPsychology, Vanderbilt University.

We are grateful to Linda Jackson and the anonymous reviewers fortheir helpful comments on a previous version of this report.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to GalenV. Bodenhausen, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University,East Lansing, Michigan 48824-1117. Electronic mail may be sent [email protected].

the tendency to see the world through a stereotypic lens(Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Dol-lard, Miller, Doob, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939; Greenberg et al.,1990; Greenberg, Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel,1992; Peak, Muney, & Clay, 1960; Sherif & Sherif, 1953; Ste-phan & Stephan, 1985; Wills, 1981). From the standpoint of thevarious theoretical ideas that are represented by the array ofresearch addressing the link between various negative emotionsand the phenomena of prejudice and stereotyping, negativeaffect often serves as a motivational impetus for these processes.Whether by displacement, projection, rationalization, or someother defensive process, individuals in negative states may de-rive some measure of relief or gratification by expressing theirprejudices and stereotypes toward outgroups. According tothese approaches, then, negative affect is the fuel for the fire ofprejudice and stereotyping. By implication, the experience ofhappiness or contentment should provide little motivationalimpetus to view others in stereotypic terms. Perhaps it is duringtimes of happiness that we are most likely to look beyond ourgross generalizations about outgroups.

Contemporary research on stereotyping has often deempha-sized the motivational aspects of the phenomenon that were socentral to many earlier conceptualizations. Instead, the focushas been on stereotyping as a mundane cognitive function thatprovides simplification and structure to our subjective experi-ence of the complex social milieu in which we live (Fiske &Neuberg, 1990; Hamilton, 1979;Rothbart, 1981). Several stud-ies suggest that stereotypes operate as heuristic cues in socialinformation processing, providing a basis for a quick responseto members of outgroups that may suffice whenever social per-ceivers cannot, or prefer not to, engage in a more thoughtfulanalysis of the unique personal qualities of specific outgroupmembers (Bodenhausen, 1988, 1990; Bodenhausen & Lich-tenstein, 1987; Bodenhausen & Wyer, 1985; see also Chaiken,

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, Vol. 66, No. 4, 621-632Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/94/S3.00

621

Page 2: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

622 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Gilbert &Hixon, 1991). From this perspective, stereotyping is not moti-vated by a need to cope with negative affective experience somuch as a need or desire to avoid extensive cognitive work. Inessence, stereotyping represents a functional shortcut strategyfor social information-processing tasks, a strategy that in factfrees up cognitive resources for use on other tasks (Macrae,Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994).

What are the implications of happiness for stereotyping,when viewed from the framework of contemporary social-cog-nitive research? Although some recent research has begun toinvestigate the impact of happiness on memory for and judg-ments about social groups (e.g., Hamilton, Stroessner, &Mackie, 1993; Stroessner, Hamilton, & Mackie, 1992; Stroess-ner & Mackie, 1992, 1993; see Hamilton & Sherman, in press,for a review), there has been very little direct evidence abouthow happiness might affect the use of stereotypes in judgmentsof individual group members. Two lines of research can be iden-tified that have some relevance to an understanding of how hap-piness and stereotyping might be related in this context. One ofthese is research on the mood congruency effect in social judg-ment (e.g., Bower, 1991; Forgas & Moylan, 1987; Isen, 1987).Essentially, this effect involves rendering judgments that are bi-ased in the direction of one's prevailing mood. Happy peoplethus would be expected to make more positive, favorable judg-ments. Various models have been proposed to account for whythis would happen. The mood-as-information approach takenby Schwarz and Clore (1983, 1988) proposes that when peopleare asked to make a judgment about some object (X), they oftenuse their gut reaction, asking themselves "How do I feel aboutX?" As long as it is plausible to them that their current feelings(whatever their actual source) are at least partly a reaction toX, they will tend to make judgments that are colored by theirmomentary mood. At least insofar as negative stereotypes areconcerned, this implies that happy people may set aside theirunfavorable stereotypes of outgroups and reach more positiveassessments of members of these groups than do those in moreneutral moods. This approach is similar to the historical moti-vational approaches in predicting less use of negative stereo-types among those who are feeling happy.

A second line of contemporary research has different im-plications. A growing body of evidence addressing the relation-ship between emotional experience and social information pro-cessing suggests that the type of strategy people use in perform-ing a social judgment task may be determined in part by theirmomentary emotional state (for reviews, see Bodenhausen,1993; Clark & Williamson, 1989; Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, inpress; Forgas, 1992a, 1992b; Schwarz, 1990; Sinclair & Mark,1992). Interestingly, the state of happiness has been associatedrepeatedly with the use of more superficial or cursory styles ofthinking. Some of the most compelling evidence supporting thisproposition has emerged from research on mood and persua-sion (Mackie & Worth, 1989, 1991; Schwarz, Bless, & Bohner,1991; Worth & Mackie, 1987). These studies document thatwhen people are made happy prior to the presentation of a per-suasive message, they are less affected by variations in argumentquality. Presumably, systematic analysis of a persuasive messagewould result in the detection and rejection of specious or weak

arguments, but happy people appear to accept weak messagesjust as readily as those founded on stronger, more valid argu-ments. Moreover, happy people seem to be more attuned thanthose in a neutral or sad mood to simple heuristic cues presentin the persuasion situation; such cues permit a relatively quickand easy response to the persuasive appeal (cf. Chaiken, 1980,1987; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Credibility cues such as ex-pertise and trustworthiness are classic examples of cues thatform one basis for responding to a persuasive communicationthat may largely circumvent cognitive elaboration of thecontent of the message (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Thus, it ap-pears that happy people often prefer to base their reactions to apersuasive message more on simple cues such as the communi-cator's putative expertise rather than engaging in close scrutinyof the message content (see also Petty, Gleicher, & Baker, 1991).

The implications of this research for predictions about hap-piness and stereotyping are provocative. It may be that happypeople, who show a preference for simple heuristic cues in per-suasion situations, may also tend to rely more on simplistic ste-reotypes in other social judgment situations. This prediction isat odds with both conventional wisdom about the relationshipbetween mood and stereotyping and the implications of the tra-ditional motivational theories of prejudice and stereotyping. Italso runs against the implications of the mood congruencyeffect. According to previous theory and research, then, it issomewhat unclear whether happiness will promote or inhibitstereotypic thinking in the social perception of individuals whohappen to be members of stereotyped social groups. There areprecedents for expecting both outcomes. The initial experimentreported in this article was intended to provide further evidenceon this issue. After documenting whether stereotyping was aug-mented or reduced among happy people, a series of follow-upstudies was planned to examine mechanisms that might ac-count for the observed effects.

Experiment 1

Concern about the impact of social stereotypes on judgmentprocesses has often focused on particularly consequential kindsof judgment situations, such as decision making in organiza-tional personnel contexts (e.g., Terborg & Ilgen, 1975) and crim-inal justice contexts (e.g., Bodenhausen & Kramer, in press). Itis, of course, a matter of considerable importance to determinewhen and how members of stereotyped social groups are disad-vantaged when it comes to perceptions of their guilt, their de-servingness of parole, their likelihood of recidivism, their cred-ibility as witnesses, and countless other perceptions relevant todecision making in justice contexts. In the research reported inthis article, we focused on stereotyping in a situation that wasanalogous to a criminal justice setting. The experiments we un-dertook examined the role of happiness and stereotyping in stu-dents' perceptions of and judgments about their fellow students'alleged misconduct.

Method

Overview and Design

We asked participants to engage in two ostensibly unrelated tasks.The first task, which involved recalling prior experiences, was designed

Page 3: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

HAPPINESS AND STEREOTYPIC THINKING 623

to induce a positive mood in approximately one half of the participants.Subjects then completed a social judgment task in which they read asynopsis of a disciplinary hearing and made judgments about the case.There were two different scenarios, one involving a case of alleged as-sault and one involving a case of alleged cheating. The identity of thestudent accused of each offense was varied so that he was either a mem-ber of a group stereotypically associated with the offense or was not.Thus, the experiment was a 2 (affect: happy vs. neutral) X 2 (stereotype:present vs. absent) between-subjects design that was tested within twodifferent scenarios and stereotypes.

Subjects

Participants were 94 undergraduates (21 men and 73 women) whoreceived course credit in return for their participation.

Materials and Procedures

Subjects participated in groups of approximately 8. Upon arrival atthe laboratory, participants were greeted by an experimenter who toldthem that they would be involved in two different experiments, oneentitled "Mood and Memories" and one entitled "Students' Court."They were seated at individual cubicles, the basic nature of the first taskwas explained, and subjects signed a consent form.

Mood induction. One half of the experimental sessions were ran-domly assigned to the happy condition. Subjects in this condition weretold that the researchers were interested in the relationship betweenemotion and memory, and that today they wanted to understand the"psychological structure" of happy memories. To do this, subjects wereasked to recall, reexperience, and write about an event that had madethem particularly happy. Following Strack, Schwarz, and Gschneidinger(1985), the instructions emphasized focusing on concrete, vivid, expe-riential aspects of the event rather than on an abstract or objective as-sessment of it. Strack et al. found this procedure to be most successfulin inducing a state of happiness. Subjects in the other condition weretold that the research involved the psychological structure of everydaymemory and were asked to recall and describe the mundane events ofthe previous day. Approximately 12 min were allotted for the comple-tion of this task. After subjects had finished writing, the experimenterthanked them, collected their forms, and left.

Social judgment task. A new experimenter introduced the secondtask, which was described as a study of legal socialization. The experi-menter explained that some institutions of higher learning had adoptedadjudicatory systems in which students took responsibility for disciplin-ary proceedings. To examine how this kind of system might work ontheir own campus, the subjects were asked to take on the role of a stu-dent member of a peer disciplinary review panel. They were asked toread a case allegedly taken from another college campus and make de-cisions about the appropriate response to the case.

After signing a consent form for the "Students' Court" study, all sub-jects received a booklet containing (a) a participants characteristicsquestionnaire, (b) a case description, (c) a brief questionnaire about thecase, and (d) a probe for suspicions about the purpose of the study. Thedemographic questionnaire requested information such as subjects' ageand sex, and asked them to rate themselves on several characteristics.Each rating involved circling a number from 0 (not at alt) to 7 (ex-tremely) that best reflected how applicable the characteristic was tothem at the moment. Embedded within fillers (e.g., conscientious,bored) was an item designed to assess the effectiveness of the mood ma-nipulation (happy).

Subjects received one of two cases, involving either an allegation ofassault (beating up a roommate) or cheating (on a mathematics exami-nation). The case summary consisted of approximately five or six sen-tences detailing the nature of the accusation and providing a mixture of

evidence, some implying innocence and some implying guilt. The iden-tity of the student accused in each case was manipulated so that, for halfthe subjects, he was identified as a member of a group stereotypicallyassociated with the alleged offense (stereotype condition), whereas forthe remainder he was not (no stereotype condition). Specifically, for thecase of assault, the student-defendant was given either an obviously His-panic name (Juan Garcia) or an ethnically nondescript name (JohnGarner); for the cheating case, in one half of the booklets the target'sname was followed by the phrase "a well-known track-and-field athleteon campus," whereas in the others, this phrase was omitted. A highprevalence of endorsement of these stereotypes within the targeted sub-ject population was established in previous research (see Bodenhausen,1990). Within sessions, subjects were randomly assigned to one of thefour possible combinations of offense type and stereotype activation.

After reading the case, subjects were asked to report the likelihood ofthe accused student's guilt on an 11-point scale ranging from 0 (not atall likely) to 10 (extremely likely). They were also asked to completesome filler questions designed to match the alleged purpose of the ex-periment (e.g., To what extent could a peer discipline system work onour campus?). Upon completion of the case questionnaire, subjectswere probed for suspicions about the research, then they were given aneducational debriefing and dismissed.

Results and Discussion

Manipulation Check

Effectiveness of the mood manipulation was assessed by com-paring subjects' self-ratings of happiness in the happy and neu-tral mood conditions. As intended, subjects in the happy condi-tion rated themselves as significantly happier than their neutralmood counterparts (Ms = 4.29 and 3.72, respectively), t(92) =2.]4,p< .025, one-tailed.

Perceived Guilt

If traditional motivational approaches to stereotyping andprejudice are correct in asserting that these phenomena aredriven by a need to cope with negative states, then we mightexpect those who are feeling happy to be particularly unlikelyto show evidence of stereotypic thinking. On the other hand, ifhappy people are more inclined to use mental shortcuts, it maybe that happy people stereotype more than those in a neutralmood. Data relevant to this issue are depicted in Figure 1. The

HAPPY NEUTRALAFFECT CONDITION

I STEREOTYPE H NO STEREOTYPE

Figure 1. Mean perceived guilt as a function of stereotype activationand emotional state (Experiment 1).

Page 4: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

624 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

data were collapsed across offense type, as this variable exhib-ited no interactions with other independent variables, ps > .25.Inspection of Figure 1 reveals that, among neutral mood sub-jects, stereotyped targets were treated no differently than non-stereotyped targets, suggesting that these subjects based theirjudgments on the implications of the specific case evidence pro-vided. However, happy mood subjects'judgments of the stereo-typed target were significantly more negative than their judg-ments of the nonstereotyped target, p < .01. Overall, the in-teraction of mood and stereotype activation was marginallysignificant, F(l, 90) = 2.88, p < .09.

Consistent with the persuasion literature described pre-viously, these data show a greater reliance on stereotypes amonghappy subjects. Conventional wisdom notwithstanding, itseems that members of stereotyped social groups can be disad-vantaged as a result of the positive moods of social perceivers. Itis unclear, however, exactly why this is so. The remaining exper-iments were undertaken with the goal of better understandingthe process producing more stereotypic judgments among thosewho are feeling happy.

Experiment 2

Why might happiness induce a greater tendency to stereo-type, or to use any other kind of cognitive shortcut for thatmatter? Explanations for social perceivers' use of heuristic pro-cessing strategies typically focus on limitations of momentarycognitive capacity or motivation (Chaiken et al., 1989; Fiske &Neuberg, 1990; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). One possibility thatseems particularly plausible in accounting for the results of Ex-periment 1 might be called the cognitive distraction hypothesis.According to this view (cf. Mackie & Worth, 1989), happy peo-ple have less cognitive capacity for social information process-ing tasks because a portion of their cognitive resources is eatenup by rumination on happy thoughts and the life events thathave evoked one's current happiness. As a result of diminishedcapacity for more extensive processing, perceivers rely to agreater extent on mental shortcuts, when available, to guidetheir reactions to social stimulus input. This view seems partic-ularly plausible in accounting for the findings reported in Ex-periment 1 because the mood induction required subjects to filltheir minds with happy thoughts. It is quite likely that thesehappy thoughts are retained longer in short-term memory (the"workspace") than the neutral, mundane thoughts elicitedamong control group subjects, and these persisting thoughtsmay well distract the social perceiver from devoting full atten-tion to other social perception tasks at hand.

In our first conceptual replication of Experiment 1, we soughtto determine the validity of this capacity-based argument as anexplanation for happy subjects' greater reliance on social ste-reotypes. Experiment 2 sought specifically to address the im-portance of distracting thoughts in producing the tendencyamong happy people to exhibit more stereotypic judgments. Inthis experiment, we used a mood induction procedure that doesnot require the imposition of any cognitive load. Specifically, weused a facial feedback procedure to directly produce subjectivehappiness. Previous research shows that contraction of facialmuscles into poses associated with the expression of an emotioncan also induce the subjective experience of the particular emo-

tion (Adelman & Zajonc, 1989; Duclos et al., 1989; Ekman,Levenson, & Friesen, 1983), and this has been found even whenusing an elaborate and clever ruse to eliminate demand charac-teristics (Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988). If this procedure re-sults in levels of subjective happiness similar to those reportedby subjects who were made happy with the memory elicitationprocedure (Experiment 1), but it is not associated with anygreater level of stereotyping, this would provide support for thenotion that it is distracting happy thoughts that produce agreater reliance on stereotypes. This pattern would be consis-tent with the claim that only when happiness is accompaniedby rumination about specific happy circumstances does it pro-mote a tendency to stereotype others.

Method

Subjects and Design

We randomly assigned 51 participants (37 women and 14 men) to a 2(Affect: happy vs. neutral) X 2 (Stereotype: present vs. absent) between-subjects design. All subjects were undergraduates recruited from intro-ductory psychology classes. They received course credit in return fortheir participation.

Materials and Procedures

Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were greeted by an exper-imenter who described the purpose of the study, entitled "Cognitive-Motor Coordination." It was explained that the researchers were inter-ested in the extent to which the part of the brain that controls motorbehavior is independent of the part of the brain that controls higherthought processes. To study this, subjects would be asked to engage in acognitive and a motor task simultaneously. The "motor" task was actu-ally the mood induction procedure. Under happy conditions, subjectswere given the task of contracting the zygomaticus muscle (producing asmile). The term smile was never used; instead, subjects were individu-ally instructed in terms of specific muscle contractions until the posesuccessfully emulated a smile. In the control condition, subjects weresimply instructed to loosely contract their nondominant hand into afist. Once they had the proper pose, subjects were instructed to maintainthe pose while working on the "cognitive" task, which was the sameStudents' Court task used in Experiment 1. In this case, only the cheat-ing scenario was used.

Subjects completed the same questionnaires as in Experiment 1, in-cluding a mood manipulation check and case judgments, focusing pri-marily on assessments of the guilt of the student accused in the case. Inaddition, in line with the experimental cover story, they completed somequestions assessing how difficult it was for them to engage in both taskssimultaneously. We were particularly interested to know whether thefacial pose (happy condition) was any more difficult or distracting thanthe fist pose (neutral condition). Following completion of a probe forsuspicions about the purpose of the study, participants received an edu-cational debriefing and were dismissed.

Results and Discussion

Manipulation Checks

Analysis of subjects' self-ratings of happiness, collected aspart of the participant characteristics questionnaire, revealedthat participants in the happy smile condition were indeed sig-nificantly happier than those in the neutral fist condition (Ms =4.62 vs. 3.76), r(49) = 1.96, p < .025, one-tailed.

Page 5: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

HAPPINESS AND STEREOTYPIC THINKING 625

Subjects were also asked to report how difficult it was to co-ordinate the cognitive and motor tasks. This provides one basisfor determining whether the two muscular contraction taskswere differentially distracting or taxing. Comparison of this rat-ing between subjects in the facial versus hand contraction taskrevealed that both tasks were rated as equally distracting, (F <1). Thus, the two tasks differed in how happy they made subjectsfeel, but they did not differ in how distracting they were.1

Perceived Guilt

If distracting mental content is the reason that happy peopleshowed greater reliance on stereotypes in Experiment 1, then itwas expected that people who are made happy by a means thatdoes not require filling the mind with happy ideas would notshow the same stereotyping tendency. Data relevant to this pre-diction are presented in Figure 2. Contrary to this hypothesis,people made happy by a facial feedback process showed thesame proclivity for stereotyping in their perceptions of the ac-cused student's guilt as did people made happy by a reminiscingprocedure. The interaction of mood and stereotype activationwas significant, F(l, 47) = 4.90, p < .05, with significantlygreater perceptions of guilt among the happy subjects who con-sidered a stereotyped target.

These findings cast doubt upon the cognitive-distraction in-terpretation, which holds that happy thoughts are distractingpeople in a good mood, constraining their capacity for system-atic thinking about other topics or persons. People in the smilepose condition, although happier than control subjects, were nomore distracted by their posing task, nor were they any morelikely to spontaneously generate random, distracting thoughts.Yet they were significantly more likely to render judgments inline with their stereotypic preconceptions. This implies thatsomething else about happiness must be responsible for the ten-dency to use stereotypes to a greater extent.

Revelle and Loftus (1992) have cautioned that so-calledmood effects are often actually arousal effects. Perhaps happi-ness is associated with some kind of physiological arousal thatdisrupts coordinated thought. According to this arousal-disrup-tion hypothesis, happiness may have some cortical concomi-tants that constrain the capacity of working memory or other-

HAPPY (SMILE) NEUTRAL (HAND)AFFECT CONDITION

wise disrupt the flow of thought, making the use of simple re-sponse strategies more appealing. Thayer (1989) has contrastedenergetic arousal with tense arousal. He noted that, whereastense arousal is strongly associated with negative affect, ener-getic arousal covaries strongly with positive affect. Obviously,the energetic arousal associated with being happy does notrender us incapable of maintaining a train of thought, but itmay provide some degree of disruption (for a more extensivereview of relevant evidence, see Bodenhausen, 1993), and pre-sumably the amount of such disruption would be in proportionto the amount of cortical arousal associated with the happystate. It is possible that in thinking back to an event that madethem extremely happy, subjects in Experiment 1 may have ex-perienced a measure of arousal that imposed some degree ofconstraint on their cognitive capacity. This is somewhat differ-ent from simply ruminating on happy thoughts, as it may in-volve a more physiological kind of capacity constraint. There isa body of evidence relating arousal, per se, to stereotyping (e.g.,Bodenhausen, 1990; Kim & Baron, 1988), so if happiness pro-duces its own kind of arousal, it may be by this means that itproduces a tendency to stereotype others. This arousal disrup-tion hypothesis is still viable in light of the data from Experi-ment 2, because it has been previously shown that facial feed-back mood induction procedures produce the same syndromeof physiological responses as a reminiscence-based procedure(Ekman et al., 1983; Levenson, Ekman, & Friesen, 1990). Ex-periment 3 directly examined the plausibility of the arousal dis-ruption hypothesis.

Experiment 3

The notion that there is some kind of physiological disruptionassociated with happiness implies that happy states that are lowin their level of energetic arousal (e.g., serenity or contentment)should not produce the same degree of heuristic (or stereotypic)thinking as happy states that are higher in this quality (e.g., ex-citement or exhilaration). If the hypothesis has merit, one wouldexpect to observe a greater degree of stereotyping in social judg-ments made by those experiencing greater levels of happyarousal. For Experiment 3, we wanted to find a mood inductionprocedure that would allow us to produce happiness that variedin its energetic arousal component, or more colloquially, in thelevel of excitement that accompanied the subjective happy state.Ideally, the overall amount of happiness would not differ be-tween conditions, but the level of excitement associated with thehappiness would differ. A musical mood induction seemed wellsuited for this purpose. As any music lover could testify, thereexists happy music that produces a surge of energy and excite-ment, but there also exists happy music that produces a sense oftranquility and calm. Through extensive pilot testing, we iden-tified musical selections that produced each of these desiredeffects. From the perspective of the arousal disruption hypothe-sis, happy states characterized by excitement and agitation

I STEREOTYPE • NO STEREOTYPE

Figure 2. Mean perceived guilt as a function of stereotype activationand type of muscle pose (Experiment 2).

1 In a pretest, we also asked subjects engaging in these two poses tolist any thoughts that came spontaneously to mind while contractingthe assigned muscle groups. Very few thoughts were listed, and the num-ber did not differ between the smile and the fist conditions.

Page 6: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

626 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

should produce greater disruption, or processing constraints,therefore leading to an increased use of stereotypes, when avail-able, as an efficient strategy for generating a response under theconditions of constraint. Calm, serene music, although produc-ing equivalent amounts of happiness per se, should have muchless disruptive influence on processing.

Method

Subjects

Fifty-three undergraduates participated in return for credit in theirintroductory psychology course. Of these, 28 were men and 25 werewomen.

Procedures and Materials

Music pretest. Several selections of music were pretested in terms oftheir influence on subjects' level of happiness and level of felt energeticarousal. Subjects sat in individual cubicles and listened to a single mu-sical selection. Each selection lasted approximately 10 min. Subjectswere instructed simply to close their eyes and listen to the music. Theywere told that they would be asked some questions about their reactionsto the music afterward. When the musical selection had finished, theywere given a questionnaire asking them to list the thoughts they hadwhile listening to the music and to rate their momentary levels of severalcharacteristics, including happy and energetic, on a 7-point scale rang-ing from 0 (not at all) to 6 (extremely). The questionnaire also includedseveral filler items concerning, for example, subjects' evaluations of var-ious styles of music.

Only data relevant to the chosen selections are described. For thehappy and excited (HE) music, we chose a section of music from Boro-din's Prince Igor. The selection comes from a vigorous dance sequence,and it is clearly very arousing and invigorating. For the happy and calm(HC) music, we chose a section of music from Delius'S/4 Village Romeoand Juliet ("The Walk Through Paradise Garden") that is particularlyrelaxing and serene. Both selections were instrumental only (i.e., noverbal content). For comparison purposes, we also had some subjectslisten to a selection of neutral (N) music ("Solar Winds" by Hykes) that,although clearly not aversive, seemed minimal in its impact on the ca-sual listener. Both the HE and HC musical selections produced higherlevels of happiness than did the N music (Ms: HE = 5.67, HC = 5.91, N= 4.50; ps < .05). However, the HE and HC selections did not differfrom each other in the amount of happiness they engendered in thelisteners, (p > .75). Analysis of the energetic ratings showed that the HCand N selections did not differ from each other (p > .50), but the HEselection produced significantly higher levels of excitement (ps < .05;Ms: HE = 5.33, HC = 3.46, N = 2.80). Finally, analysis of thoughtlisting data revealed no differences among the three music selections interms of the number of thoughts reported while listening to the music(p > .50). Thus, the HE and HC selections met the criteria we estab-lished in that they produced comparable levels of happiness but differedin the level of felt energetic arousal associated with that state.

Main study. Subjects were recruited for a study of music perception.When they reported to the laboratory in groups of four, they were toldthat because of the brevity of the music study, an additional, unrelatedstudy would take place during the session. After being seated at individ-ual cubicles, subjects were told that their task was simply to listen to aselection of music and then answer some questions about it. Approxi-mately one half of the subjects listened to the HE music (i.e., the Boro-din selection), and the remainder listened to the HC music (i.e., theDelius selection). After listening to the music, subjects made several bo-gus ratings of it and then went on to the second task, the Students' Court.The same materials were used again, under the same instructional con-

ditions as before. Subjects were randomly assigned within sessions toreceive either a stereotypic or a nonstereotypic case. They completedthe same dependent measures (case judgments, demographic profile) asdid subjects in the previously reported experiments. Again, the crucialdependent variable was subjects' ratings of the apparent guilt of the stu-dent accused in the case they read. After a probe for suspicions aboutthe purpose of the experiment, subjects received an educational de-briefing and were dismissed.

Results and Discussion

Support for the arousal disruption hypothesis would emergeif subjects in the happy, excited condition showed stronger evi-dence of stereotypic judgments than did happy, calm subjects.The means relevant to this prediction are shown in Figure 3.Contrary to the arousal disruption hypothesis, the data revealedonly a main effect of stereotype activation, F( 1, 49) = 4.91, p <.05. The size of the difference between ratings of stereotypedversus nonstereotyped targets was almost identical in both thehappy, excited and the happy, calm conditions. Thus, as before,happy people show evidence of stereotypic bias in their judg-ments, but the amount of this bias appears to be independent ofthe degree of excitement or arousal inherent in their happystate. Although they were significantly less excited after listen-ing to their musical selection, HC subjects were just as likely tostereotype as their HE counterparts.

Given these results, it becomes fairly implausible to assumethat there is any kind of disruptive arousal driving the stereo-typing effect consistently observed among happy people. It ap-pears that there is something about being in a pleasant state thatpromotes the tendency to stereotype, regardless of whether thisstate involves ruminating about happy ideas or feeling excitedor energized by the experience. What, then, can explain thistendency? A third hypothesis relies less on arguments aboutconstrained capacity and more on links between emotionalstates and patterns of cognitive motivation. According to theeffort conservation hypothesis, happy people may simply be lessmotivated to think very systematically about the external envi-ronment. Schwarz (1990) has articulated an evolutionary ratio-nale for this view (and provided empirical support for it; see,e.g., Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, & Strack, 1990; Schwarz & Bless,1991). Whereas negative moods signal the existence of a prob-

HAPPY-ENERGETIC HAPPY-CALMAFFECT CONDITION

I STEREOTYPE H NO STEREOTYPE

Figure 3. Mean perceived guilt as a function of stereotype activationand musical arousal level (Experiment 3).

Page 7: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

HAPPINESS AND STEREOTYPIC THINKING 627

lem that needs to be solved, happy moods signal satisfactionwith the current state. Happiness is a kind of safety signal, indi-cating that there is no current need for problem solving. If onemakes the plausible assumption that emotional states are asso-ciated with cognitive strategies that are most effective for thekinds of situations in which the emotion typically arises, it islogical to propose that unhappy people will think more deeplyabout their social environment (in an effort to solve their prob-lems; see Edwards & Weary, 1993; Weary, 1990) whereas happypeople can contentedly coast on cruise control, not bothering tothink very deeply about surrounding events unless they impingedirectly on their well-being. According to this view, happy peo-ple are not incapable of thinking systematically (because of con-straints on cognitive capacity); rather, they simply often choosenot to do so, unless their own outcomes are at stake (Forgas,1989) or they enjoy the cognitive task and thus have some in-trinsic motivation. Perhaps happy people, when asked to judgethe likelihood of a defendant's guilt, simply prefer not to exertmuch effort, especially if there is a viable shortcut or heuristiccue available that provides the basis for a less effortful response.

Regardless of the source or nature of a pleasant or happystate, it seems to signal a retreat from deeper, more systematicthinking, at least for tasks lower in interest and personal rele-vance. If a reduction in cognitive motivation is what is reallybehind the tendency for happy people to stereotype more, thena manipulation that makes task performance seem more conse-quential to subjects should be able to compensate for the moti-vational deficits characterizing happy people. This possibilitywas explored in a fourth and final experiment.

Experiment 4

Research in the domain of persuasion has shown that, what-ever the factors producing a tendency for happy people to pro-cess persuasive messages less systematically, happy people areindeed capable of responding appropriately to variations in ar-gument quality if they are explicitly instructed to attend to thearguments (Schwarz et al., 1991). This clearly indicates that, ifthere are any cognitive constraints on processing ability im-posed by the experience of happiness (as the cognitive distrac-tion and arousal disruption hypotheses would have it), theseconstraints are easily surmountable. This strongly suggests thatcognitive or epistemic motivation is what is really lacking inhappy people. In the final study, we examined the issue of happypeople's responsiveness to motivational cues in a stereotypingcontext.

When social perceivers are concerned about the accuracy oftheir impressions and evaluations of social stimuli, they shouldbe more attuned to the implications of specific available evi-dence and should rely less on simple cues or generalized precon-ceptions (Kruglanski, 1989). One way to induce this concern isto hold people accountable for their judgments and evaluations(Tetlock, 1983; Tetlock & Kim, 1987). Telling people that theywill be held accountable for the quality of their judgments, andthat they must be able to defend the decisions they make, shouldhave the effect of motivating more systematic thought. Whathappens when happy people are told that they will be held ac-countable for the quality of their judgments? One possibility isthat accountability cues will provide a sufficient motivational

impetus to overcome any resource-conserving tendency on thepart of happy people. If so, they may show a pattern of judg-ments that is quite similar to neutral mood subjects. That is, allsubjects may be evidence focused, and if they all have the sameevidence, they will all form similar judgments. If, on the otherhand, happy people still show a greater stereotyping tendencyeven in the presence of accountability cues, this would suggesteither that (a) their resource-conservation mode of thinking isnot easy to modify, or (b) perhaps there is some kind of basicconstraint that makes systematic thought more problematic forhappy people. The latter possibility seems unlikely, given theresults of our previous studies, and both possibilities seem un-likely given the results of Schwarz et al. (1991) in their persua-sion research. Thus, we expected that the stereotyping tendencyevidenced by happy people in the previous three studies wouldbe eliminated upon the activation of accountability cues.

Method

Subjects and Design

Participants were 131 undergraduates (93 women and 38 men) whoreceived credit in their introductory psychology course for participat-ing. Participants were randomly assigned to one of eight conditions de-fined by a 2 (Mood: happy vs. neutral) X 2 (Stereotype Activation: pres-ent vs. absent) X 2 (Accountability: low vs. high) between-subjects fac-torial design.

Materials and ProceduresThe same materials and procedures used in Experiment 1 were used

again. That is, subjects participated in two ostensibly unrelated experi-ments. The first was actually a mood induction task based on a reminis-cence procedure. The second was the Students' Court task. Under lowaccountability conditions, the methodology of Experiment 1 was repli-cated exactly. Subjects in the social perception task were simply askedto read the case they were given and make some judgments about it.At the end of the instructions for the social perception task, they werereminded that their judgments would be completely anonymous. In thehigh accountability conditions, however, the reminder about anonymitywas deleted, and in its place was the following statement, in bold type-face: "Bear in mind that you will be held accountable for your judg-ment, just as if you were a judge on a real peer discipline panel. That is,you will have to be able to justify the decisions that you make about thecase you read." After reading the instructions, subjects turned the pageto find a description of a case concerning either a stereotypic or a non-stereotypic offender and the usual questionnaire items. Only one newitem was added to evaluate the effectiveness of the accountability ma-nipulation. For this item, subjects were asked to rate how strongly theyfelt that it was necessary to be able to justify their judgments about thecase. This rating was made on a 10-point scale ranging from 0 (not atalt) to 10 (extremely). After completing the questionnaires and a probefor suspicions, subjects were debriefed and dismissed.

Results and Discussion

Manipulation Checks

Subjects' self-ratings of happiness confirmed that those in thehappy condition were significantly happier than those in theneutral mood condition (Ms = 6.76 and 6.06, respectively, p <.025, one-tailed). In addition, subjects in the high accountabil-ity condition reported feeling a greater need to be able to justify

Page 8: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

628 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

their judgments than those in the low accountability conditions(Ms = 5.90 vs. 5.20, respectively, p < .05, one-tailed). Thus,both the affect and the accountability manipulations wereeffective.

Social Judgment Task

The low accountability conditions constituted an empiricalreplication of Experiment 1, so we expected to find the samepattern of results (i.e., greater stereotyping in the social judg-ments of happy people). Under conditions of high accountabil-ity, however, we expected happy people to be no different fromneutral mood subjects because they would become more data-based in their social information processing. The data relevantto these predictions are presented in Table 1.

It is clear that the judgments rendered by subjects in the lowaccountability conditions show the same pattern as that ob-served in previous experiments: Happy subjects made more ste-reotypic judgments than did those in the neutral condition (p <.05). However, the judgments of the accountable subjectsshowed quite a different pattern. The interaction of affect con-dition, accountability, and stereotype activation was statisticallysignificant, F(l, 122) = 5.55, p < .025. Not only did the ac-countable happy people not show a greater stereotypic bias intheir judgments, they showed a trend toward reduced percep-tions of the guilt of the stereotyped target (p < . 15). In any case,it is clear that, as expected, happy subjects were quite capable ofavoiding stereotypic judgments when given some motivationalimpetus to do so.

General Discussion

In each experiment reported, happy people who were simplyasked to make some judgments about a case of alleged studentmisbehavior were significantly more likely to render harsherjudgments about a stereotyped judgment target than were peo-ple in a neutral mood. This finding is noteworthy in several re-spects. Whereas the bulk of prior research on affect and stereo-typing has emphasized the role of negative affect of one kind oranother in the elicitation of stereotypic responses to outgroupmembers (see Bodenhausen, Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994; Esses,Haddock, & Zanna, 1993), the present findings show that posi-tive affect can also elicit these responses. Although these find-ings in no way cast doubt on the possibility that negative statessuch as frustration or anxiety can produce increases in preju-dice and stereotyping, they do clearly show that negative statesare not necessary for this to occur, and indeed, positive moodscan (perhaps for very different reasons) produce similar sorts of

Table 1Perceived Guilt as a Function of Accountability, Mood, andStereotype Activation (Experiment 4)

Condition

StereotypeNo stereotype

Not accountable

Happy

6.945.69

Neutral

5.586.44

Accountable

Happy

5.846.78

Neutral

6.946.60

heightened stereotyping. Interestingly, Bodenhausen et al.(1994), using the same experimental paradigm as in the currentExperiment 1, found that angry subjects produced a pattern ofstereotypic judgments comparable to the happy subjects in thepresent experiments, whereas sad subjects did not. These find-ings collectively suggest that previous views about the connec-tions between affect and stereotyping have been overly simplis-tic. The general claim that heightened stereotyping is associatedwith negative affect fails to recognize that (a) not all negativestates seem to follow this pattern, and (b) positive states can alsoproduce the same pattern.

The findings reported also failed to match expectations de-rived from research on the mood congruency effect in socialjudgment. Although there were some mood congruency trendsfor judgments of the nonstereotyped target (i.e., happy subjectsjudged the nonstereotyped target somewhat more leniently thandid neutral mood subjects), happy subjects considering a ste-reotypic case certainly did not show more leniency toward thetarget of the case. This suggests that the tendency toward moodcongruency in social judgment is qualified by other factors, oneof which maybe happy people's preferential use of simple judg-ment heuristics, even if these have negative implications.

One implication of our main finding is that people who arein a good mood, for whatever reason, and who happen to hearabout the alleged stereotypic behavior of an outgroup member(e.g., in a news report, overheard gossip), are likely to jump tostereotypic conclusions about the matter. They may decide thatthe allegation has substance, irrespective of the availability orimplications of specific facts in the case. Such allegations,sufficiently documented in the imaginations of happy people,may constitute a type of mental confirmation of stereotypic be-liefs that is not distinguished in memory from actual, provenincidents (cf. Slusher & Anderson, 1987). Although the presentresearch examined only one judgment domain (perceptions ofmisbehavior), it is a particularly important one for social per-ception of many stigmatized outgroups. The most damaging so-cial stereotypes are those that associate outgroup membershipwith tendencies toward engaging in undesirable, socially unac-ceptable, or criminal practices. The present research shows thatfeeling good tends to increase the extent to which such stereo-types are applied in making judgments of individual membersof stigmatized outgroups.

Why do happy people produce more stereotypic judgments?These experiments provided little support for the idea that hap-piness promotes stereotypic thinking by constraining the per-ceiver's capacity for more systematic thought. Given that thesame results emerged even when the mood induction proceduredid not involve any particular cognitive content (e.g., the facialfeedback procedure of Experiment 2 or the music of Experi-ment 3) and did not involve any differential elicitation of ran-dom thoughts, it seems fairly unlikely that happy people arenecessarily too caught up in happy ruminations to think deeplyabout the social world. By the same token, the idea that happypeople experience some kind of disruptive arousal or excitementseems unlikely given that happy subjects do not stereotype to anygreater extent than do calm, happy subjects (Experiment 3).

The effort conservation hypothesis is compatible with thepatterns emerging across all four experiments. According to this

Page 9: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

HAPPINESS AND STEREOTYPIC THINKING 629

view, happy people simply are not motivated to engage in cog-nitive effort, unless the tasks requiring such effort have somedirect bearing on their own well-being or enjoyment. Experi-ment 4 showed that, if made to feel accountable, happy subjectsbecome quite unlikely to render stereotypic judgments. Pre-sumably, by making them feel that their own outcomes couldbe affected by the nature of their performance on the judgmenttask (e.g., if they could not justify a negative reaction and thusmight be suspected of prejudice, which would be an undesirableoutcome for most people), they became motivated to avoid ste-reotypic bias. Along similar lines, Forgas (1989) showed that themore heuristic or impulsive judgment strategies of happy peo-ple are only likely to emerge if their own outcomes were notcontingent upon the quality of their judgments. The costs ofeffort conservation may be considerable for the targets of socialstereotyping, but for the stereotyping perceiver, the costs of over-generalization may often be minimal, or they may be out-weighed by benefits such as preservation of cognitive resources(Macrae et al., 1994) or mood maintenance.

The fact that accountable subjects did not show the same ste-reotyping tendency as their nonaccountable counterparts mightalso be interpreted in other ways. For example, it is possible thatbeing told that one must justify one's judgments is somewhatthreatening and unpleasant, and as such it may counteract thepositive mood induction, eliminating its effects. Although plau-sible, it is unclear why this process would produce a tendency toavoid stereotypic thinking among the (formerly) happy subjects.Alternatively, the accountability manipulation may have simplyaroused social desirability concerns, leading to an avoidance ofstereotypic responses. This avoidance would therefore not nec-essarily be based on being more thoughtful or systematic inone's information processing as a result of the accountabilitymanipulation. This account is also plausible, but the reasoningwould seem to apply equally well to the neutral mood subjects.They, too, should be subject to social desirability concerns andshould avoid harsh judgments of the stereotyped target. Butthey did not. Their judgments were among the harshest ob-served across the four experiments. Perhaps happy people havea particular desire to protect their good mood, and as such, theyrespond especially strongly to social desirability cues. This pos-sibility remains to be examined in future research.

The claim that happiness is associated with less thoughtful-ness or cognitive effort has received a very interesting qualifi-cation from recent research by Martin, Ward, Achee, and Wyer(1993). Specifically, these researchers showed that good moodscan have very different motivational implications, depending ona person's orientation to a current task. If one asks oneself"Have I done enough?" then a good mood may imply anaffirmative answer, leading to a termination of cognitive effort.On the other hand, if one asks oneself "Am I still enjoying thistask?" then a good mood may imply an affirmative answer, lead-ing to continued effort. Task parameters and individual differ-ences may determine which kind of orientation one has, butthis research clearly implies that it is dangerous to generally as-sume that happiness implies a lack of cognitive motivation. Per-haps it is primarily for relatively uninvolving tasks that this istrue. Nevertheless, the outcomes of uninvolving tasks (e.g., pe-rusing evidence about the criminal culpability of a member of

a stereotyped group) can be consequential in building a socialperceiver's database of stereotypic knowledge.

The present findings may seem somewhat at odds with re-search indicating greater flexibility and creativity among happypersons (for reviews, see Isen, 1984, 1987). The work of Martinet al. (1993) provides one way of integrating these apparentlydisparate findings. If creativity-related tasks offer an opportu-nity for enjoyment or fun, then happy people may indeed bewilling to expend some effort on them, because their orientationto task may take the form, "Am I still enjoying this?" Creativitytasks may have particular intrinsic appeal to happy people be-cause they provide a means for enjoyment, and therefore formood maintenance or protection. On the other hand, a socialperception task like the one used here (and its real-life analogs)may provide little promise of enjoyment, and happy people maybe correspondingly unwilling to exert much effort on it (unless,of course, other motivational concerns arise, as with the highaccountability conditions). Happy people's use of heuristicshortcuts (e.g., Isen, Means, Patrick, & Nowicki, 1982) is prob-ably limited to task environments that offer little promise ofimproving their current mood. If thinking deeply about the taskmight actually undermine or dissipate their good mood, thenhappy people have another motive to rely on a quick responsestrategy rather than engage in more systematic thought.

Yet another interpretation of the present results is also possi-ble. If positive moods are associated with enhanced cognitiveaccessibility of positive self-related information (and perhapsalso negative information about outgroups, if this informationadds to one's sense of positive distinctiveness; see Tajfel &Turner, 1986), then a positive mood induction may selectivelyprime such associations. If so, feeling good may also mean feel-ing quite self-confident or even overconfident (but see Allwood& Bjorhag, 1991). As a consequence, people who are feelinggood may be more likely to disparage members of other groupsrather automatically, without undertaking any effort to correctstereotypic judgments because of this confidence. Relatedly,Devine (1989) has suggested that stereotyping occurs automat-ically, but is often followed by an effortful attempt to correctone's judgments to avoid stereotypic influences (at least amongthose who are low in prejudice). Perhaps happy people do notbother to engage in this effortful correction process (see Boden-hausen, 1993). Given that the motivation for this corrective ac-tivity seems to lie in unpleasant feelings of guilt and compunc-tion (Devine & Monteith, 1993; Devine, Monteith, Zuwerink,& Elliot, 1991), it may be that the happy mood induction un-dermines social perceivers' effort to avoid prejudice because itcounteracts the feelings of compunction that might otherwisearise. This possibility is currently being investigated.

Conclusions

What is the typical ecological relationship between emotionalstates and intergroup perception, judgment, and interaction?Bodenhausen (1993) recently argued that there are two differentkinds of contexts in which affect may enter into intergroup per-ceptions. The first, termed integral affect, involves emotionalreactions that are generated by thoughts and associations aboutthe relevant outgroup(s). For many outgroups, the affect that isintegral to people's thoughts about the group is decidedly nega-

Page 10: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

630 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

tive (e.g., Dijker, 1987; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Jackson &Sullivan, 1988; Stephan & Stephan, 1985; Wilder, 1993). If in-tegral emotions were the only affective factor affecting socialperception, it would be largely unnecessary to be concernedabout happy moods producing stereotypic judgments. But thereis a second context, the one of focal concern in the present re-search, which is termed incidental affect. Incidental affect con-sists of emotions elicited by factors beyond the intergroupcontext itself (e.g., the weather, professional accomplishments,one's love life). The present research demonstrates that whenone is feeling good, for whatever incidental reason, the likeli-hood of stereotypic judgments increases, at least for judgmentaltasks of minimal personal relevance.

It may seem somewhat ironic, given the results of the presentexperiments, that researchers have touted the importance of apositive affective climate for the development of favorable in-tergroup relations (e.g., Amir, 1976; Brewer & Miller, 1984). Ifone considers the probable impact of integral positive affect onintergroup perception, however, there may be little contradic-tion between the implications of our findings and the recom-mendations of previous researchers concerning the best climatefor intergroup contact. Because the accuracy of social percep-tions in an actual intergroup interaction setting is relevant toone's own outcomes or well-being, it seems likely that happypeople will be willing to expend the cognitive effort necessary tolook beyond their general preconceptions about the outgroup insuch settings (cf. Erber & Fiske, 1984). Incidental positive affect,then, may be a more likely culprit in the promotion of stereo-typic thinking than integral positive affect. Incidental positiveaffect is more likely to occur in contexts in which thinking su-perficially about outgroups incurs no appreciable, immediatecosts, whereas thinking more systematically seems to hold littlepromise of appreciable immediate benefits.

A number of interesting questions remain for future re-search. The present findings have several intriguing implica-tions. One is that people who have a chronic or dispositionaltendency to experience happy states may correspondingly bemore prone to stereotypic thinking. It also remains for futureresearch to identify the various situational factors that are likelyto moderate the impact of happiness on stereotypic thinking.Accountability cues seem to provide one basis for circumvent-ing the stereotyping tendencies of happy social perceivers. Otherfactors impinging on epistemic motivation would undoubtedlyhave similar effects. After many years of successfully analyzingthe cognitive bases of stereotyping and prejudice, it is becomingapparent that reintroducing affective and motivational issues,within the context of an information-processing framework,promises to yield valuable insights into the nature of intergroupprocesses (cf. Mackie & Hamilton, 1993).

References

Adelman, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Facial efference and the expe-rience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 249-280.

Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N.(1950). The authoritarian personality. New "Vbrk: Harper.

Allwood, C. M., & Bjorhag, C. G. (1991). Mood and realism of confi-dence in one's own answers to general knowledge questions. Scandi-navian Journal of Psychology, 32, 358-37J.

Amir, Y. (1976). The role of intergroup contact in change of prejudiceand ethnic relations. In P. Katz (Ed.), Towards the elimination of rac-ism (pp. 245-308). New York: Pergamon Press.

Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and per-suasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psy-chology Bulletin, 16, 331-345.

Bodenhausen, G. V. (1988). Stereotypic biases in social decision makingand memory: Testing process models of stereotype use. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 55, 726-737.

Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: Evi-dence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychological Sci-ence, 7,319-322.

Bodenhausen, G. V. (1993). Emotion, arousal, and stereotypic judg-ment: A heuristic model of affect and stereotyping. In D. Mackie &D. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactiveprocesses in intergroup perception (pp. 13-37). San Diego, CA: Aca-demic Press.

Bodenhausen, G. V., & Kramer, G. P. (in press). Social stereotypes andmemorial accuracy in criminal justice contexts. In M. McLeod &C. N. Macrae (Eds.), Stereotypes in the criminal justice system: Asocial psychological perspective. London: Falmer Press.

Bodenhausen, G. V., & Lichtenstein, M. (1987). Social stereotypes andinformation processing strategies: The impact of task complexity.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 871-880.

Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negativeaffect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sad-ness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45-62.

Bodenhausen, G. V., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1985). Effects of stereotypeson decision making and information-processing strategies. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 48, 262-282.

Bower, G. H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgment. In J. Forgas(Ed.), Emotion and social judgment (pp. 31-54). Oxford, England:Pergamon Press.

Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. (1984). Beyond the contact hypothesis: The-oretical perspectives on desegregation. In N. Miller & M. Brewer(Eds.), Groups in contact: The psychology of desegregation (pp. 281-302). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processingand the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766.

Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna,J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontariosymposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and sys-tematic information processing within and beyond the persuasioncontext. In J. Uleman & J. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp.212-252). New York: Guilford Press.

Clark, M. S., & Williamson, G. M. (1989). Moods and social judge-ments. In H. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social psy-chophysiology (pp. 347-370). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (in press). Affective causes andconsequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., &T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.

Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic andcontrolled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,56, 5-18.

Devine, P. G., & Monteith, M. J. (1993). The role of discrepancy-asso-ciated affect in prejudice reduction. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamil-ton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes ingroup perception (pp. 317-344). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Devine, P. G., Monteith, M. J., Zuwerink, J. R., & Elliot, A. J. (1991).Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 60, 817-830.

Page 11: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

HAPPINESS AND STEREOTYPIC THINKING 631

Dijker, A. J. M. (1987). Emotional reactions to ethnic minorities. Eu-ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 305-325.

Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, Q H., & Sears, R. R.(1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale UniversityPress.

Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stem, L., & VanLighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions andpostures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 57, 100-108.

Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Ft. Worth,TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. (1993). Depression and the impression-formation continuum: Piecemeal processing despite the availabilityof category information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 64, 636-645.

Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic ner-vous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221,1208-1210.

Erber, R., & Fiske, S. T. (1984). Outcome dependency and attention toinconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 54, 709-726.

Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). The role of mood inthe expression of intergroup stereotypes. In M. P. Zanna & J. M.Olson (Eds.), The psychology of prejudice: The Ontario symposium(Vol. 7, pp. 77-101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fiske, S. T, & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression for-mation from category-based to individuating processes: Influences ofinformation and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P.Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp.1-74). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Forgas, J. P. (1989). Mood effects on decision-making strategies. Austra-lian Journal of Psychology, 41, 197-214.

Forgas, J. P. (1992a). Affect and social perception: Research evidenceand an integrative theory. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Eu-ropean review of social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 183-223). Chichester,England: Wiley.

Forgas, J. P. (1992b). Affect in social judgments and decisions: Amultiprocess model. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 227-275). San Diego, CA: AcademicPress.

Forgas, J. P., & Moylan, S. J. (1987). After the movies: The effects oftransient mood states on social judgments. Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, IS, 478-489.

Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. InJ. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, andracism (pp. 61-89). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, J. G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activa-tion and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 60, 509-517.

Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T, Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M.,Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror managementtheory: II. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those whothreaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 58, 308-318.

Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T, Solomon, S., & Chatel, D.(1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality saliencealways intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one'sworldview? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212-220.

Hamilton, D. L. (1979). A cognitive-attributional analysis of stereotyp-ing. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-ogy (Vol. 12, pp. 53-84). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, J. W. (in press). Stereotypes. In R. S.

Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed.).Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hamilton, D. L., Stroessner, S. J., & Mackie, D. M. (1993). The influ-ence of affect on stereotyping: The case of illusory correlations. InD. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereo-typing: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 39-61). SanDiego, CA: Academic Press.

Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition.In R. S. Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition(Vol. 3, pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behav-ior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol. 20, pp. 203-253). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Isen, A. M., Means, B., Patrick, R., & Nowicki, G. (1982). Some factorsinfluencing decision-making strategy and risk taking. In M. S. Clark& S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition: The seventeenth Carnegiesymposium on cognition (pp. 243-261). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Jackson, L. A., & Sullivan, L. A. (1988). Cognition and affect in evalu-ations of stereotyped group members. Journal of Social Psychology,129, 659-672.

Kim, H., & Baron, R. S. (1988). Exercise and illusory correlation: Doesarousal heighten stereotypic processes? Journal of Experimental So-cial Psychology, 24, 366-380.

Kruglanski, A. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge. New\brk: Plenum.

Levenson, R. W, Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facialaction generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity.Psychophysiology, 27, 363-384.

Mackie, D. M., & Hamilton, D. L. (Eds.). (1993). Affect, cognition, andstereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception. San Diego,CA: Academic Press.

Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. (1989). Processing deficits and the media-tion of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 57, 27-40.

Mackie, D. M., & Worth, L. (1991). Feeling good, but not thinkingstraight: The impact of positive mood on persuasion. In J. P. Forgas(Ed.), Emotion and social judgment (pp. 201-220). Oxford, England:Pergamon Press.

Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypesas energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 66, yi-Al.

Martin, L. L., Ward, D. W, Achee, J. W., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1993).Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implicationsof their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64,317-326.

Peak, H., Muney, B., & Clay, M. (1960). Opposites structures, defenses,and attitudes. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,74(8), Whole No. 495, 1-25.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood modelof persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental socialpsychology (Vol. 19, pp. 124-203). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Petty, R. E., Gleicher, F, & Baker, S. M. (1991). Multiple roles for affectin persuasion. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments(pp. 181-200). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.

Pratto, E, & Bargh, J. A. (1991). Stereotyping based on apparently in-dividuating information: Trait and global components of sex stereo-types under attention overload. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-chology, 27, 26-47.

Revelle, W., & Loftus, D. A. (1992). The implications of arousal effectsfor the study of affect and memory. In S.-A. Christianson (Ed.), Hand-book of emotion and memory (pp. 113-149). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rothbart, M. (1981). Memory processes and social beliefs. In D. L.Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroupbehavior (pp. 145-181). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 12: ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and ......ATTITUDES AND SOCIAL COGNITION Happiness and Stereotypic Thinking in Social Judgment Galen V. Bodenhausen, Geoffrey P. Kramer, and

632 G. BODENHAUSEN, G. KRAMER, AND K. SUSSER

Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and moti-vational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorren-tino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 527-561). New York: Guilford Press.

Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad andsmart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In J. For-gas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 55-71). Oxford, En-gland: Pergamon Press.

Schwarz, N., Bless, H., & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion:Affective states influence the processing of persuasive communica-tions. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-ogy {\o\. 24, pp. 161-199). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judg-ments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affectivestates. Journal ojPersonality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523.

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? The infor-mative function of affective states. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.),Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 44-62). Toronto, Ontario,Canada: Hogrefe.

Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1953). Groups in harmony and tension. New\ork: Harper & Row.

Sinclair, R. C, & Mark, M. M. (1992). The influence of mood stateon judgment and action: Effects on persuasion, categorization, socialjustice, person perception, and judgmental accuracy. In L. L. Martin& A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 165-193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Slusher, M. P., & Anderson, C. A. (1987). When reality-monitoring fails:The role of imagination in stereotype maintenance. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 52, 653-662.

Stangor, C, & Duan, C. (1991). Effects of multiple task demands uponmemory about social groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-chology, 27, 357-378.

Stephan, W. C, & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journalof Social Issues, 41(3), 157-175.

Strack, F, Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitatingconditions of facial expressions: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feed-back hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,768-777.

Strack, F, Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and

reminiscing: The role of time perspective, mood, and mode of think-ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460-1469.

Stroessner, S. J., Hamilton, D. L., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). Affect andstereotyping: The effect of induced mood on distinctiveness-based il-lusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,564-576.

Stroessner, S. J., & Mackie, D. M. (1992). The impact of induced affecton the perception of variability in social groups. Personality and So-cial Psychology Bulletin, 18, 546-554.

Stroessner, S. J., & Mackie, D. M. (1993). Affect and perceived groupvariability: Implications for stereotyping and prejudice. In D. M.Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping:Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 63-86). San Diego, CA:Academic Press.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of in-tergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychologyof intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

Terborg, J. R., & Ilgen, D. R. (1975). A theoretical approach to sexdiscrimination in traditionally masculine occupations. Organiza-tional Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 352-376.

Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 74-83.

Tetlock, P. E., & Kim, J. I. (1987). Accountability and judgment pro-cesses in a personality prediction task. Journal of Personality and So-cial Psychology, 52, 700-709.

Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. NewYork: Oxford Press.

Weary, G. (1990). Depression and sensitivity to social information. InB. S. Moore & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Affect and social behavior (pp. 207-230). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Wilder, D. (1993). The role of anxiety in facilitating stereotypic judg-ments of outgroup behavior. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton(Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes ingroup perception (pp. 87-109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychol-ogy. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245-271.

Worth, L. T., & Mackie, D. M. (1987). Cognitive mediation of positiveaffect in persuasion. Social Cognition, 5, 76-94.

Received July 1, 1993Revision received October 19, 1993

Accepted October 25, 1993 •