ARTICLE IN PRESS - Boston University Medical Campus€¦ · Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,...

11
Age-related increase of sI AHP in prefrontal pyramidal cells of monkeys: relationship to cognition J.I. Luebke*, J.M. Amatrudo Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 72 East Concord St, Boston, MA 02118, USA Received 13 May 2010; received in revised form 23 June 2010; accepted 5 July 2010 Abstract Reduced excitability, due to an increase in the slow afterhyperpolarization (and its underlying current sI AHP ), occurs in CA1 pyramidal cells in aged cognitively-impaired, but not cognitively-unimpaired, rodents. We sought to determine whether similar age-related changes in the sI AHP occur in pyramidal cells in the rhesus monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were obtained from layer 3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells in dlPFC slices prepared from young (9.6 0.7 years old) and aged (22.3 0.7 years old) behaviorally characterized subjects. The amplitude of the sI AHP was significantly greater in layer 3 (but not layer 5) cells from aged-impaired compared with both aged-unimpaired and young monkeys, which did not differ. Aged layer 3, but not layer 5, cells exhibited significantly increased action potential firing rates, but there was no relationship between sI AHP and firing rate. Thus, in monkey dlPFC layer 3 cells, an increase in sI AHP is associated with age-related cognitive decline; however, this increase is not associated with a reduction in excitability. © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Slice; Patch-clamp; Voltage-clamp; Potassium channels; Excitability 1. Introduction Working memory, which is largely mediated by the dor- solateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), declines significantly during normal aging in a large proportion of rhesus mon- keys (Bartus et al., 1979; Lai et al., 1995; Moore et al., 2003, 2006; Rapp, 1990; Steere and Arnsten, 1997). It is increasingly evident that there is not a single causative factor; rather, a multitude of changes in neuronal structure and function occur with normal aging, which may together underlie cognitive decline (Dickstein et al., 2007). Rela- tively little is known about age effects on the functional electrophysiological properties of monkey neurons (Chang et al., 2005; Luebke et al., 2004; 2010); by contrast, the effects of age on rodent hippocampal pyramidal cells have been extensively studied with in vitro slice recordings (for review: Barnes, 2003; Burke and Barnes, 2010; Thibault et al., 2007). These studies have shown that there is an age- related decrease in action potential firing rates of CA1 pyramidal cells, with a concomitant increase in the ampli- tude of the calcium-dependent slow afterhyperpolarization (sAHP) responsible for spike frequency adaptation (Dister- hoft et al., 1996; Landfield and Pitler, 1984; Power et al., 2002; for review: Faber and Sah, 2003; Thibault et al., 1998). Other studies have demonstrated that the age-related increase in magnitude of the sAHP is inversely related to performance on tasks mediated largely by the hippocampus, with aged-impaired subjects having significantly higher sAHP amplitudes than both aged-unimpaired and young subjects, which do not differ (Matthews et al., 2009; Moyer et al., 2000; Tombaugh et al., 2005). These in vitro slice studies have led to a widely held view that age-related decline in hippocampal function can be attributed to reduced excitability and reduced synaptic plas- ticity of CA1 pyramidal neurons, secondary to increased calcium influx and amplitude of calcium-dependent sAHPs (for review: Faber and Sah, 2003; Foster, 2007; Thibault et al., 2007). While some in vivo single unit studies have reported an age-related decrease in CA1 pyramidal cell * Corresponding author. Tel.: 1 617 638 4930; fax: 1 617 638 5954. E-mail address: [email protected] (J.I. Luebke). Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/neuaging 0197-4580/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.002 ARTICLE IN PRESS

Transcript of ARTICLE IN PRESS - Boston University Medical Campus€¦ · Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,...

  • A

    ctooas3e

    K

    1

    sdk2ifauteeebra

    Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    0d

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Age-related increase of sIAHP in prefrontal pyramidal cells of monkeys:relationship to cognition

    J.I. Luebke*, J.M. AmatrudoDepartment of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 72 East Concord St, Boston, MA 02118, USA

    Received 13 May 2010; received in revised form 23 June 2010; accepted 5 July 2010

    bstract

    Reduced excitability, due to an increase in the slow afterhyperpolarization (and its underlying current sIAHP), occurs in CA1 pyramidalells in aged cognitively-impaired, but not cognitively-unimpaired, rodents. We sought to determine whether similar age-related changes inhe sIAHP occur in pyramidal cells in the rhesus monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings werebtained from layer 3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells in dlPFC slices prepared from young (9.6 0.7 years old) and aged (22.3 0.7 yearsld) behaviorally characterized subjects. The amplitude of the sIAHP was significantly greater in layer 3 (but not layer 5) cells fromged-impaired compared with both aged-unimpaired and young monkeys, which did not differ. Aged layer 3, but not layer 5, cells exhibitedignificantly increased action potential firing rates, but there was no relationship between sIAHP and firing rate. Thus, in monkey dlPFC layercells, an increase in sIAHP is associated with age-related cognitive decline; however, this increase is not associated with a reduction in

    xcitability.2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    eywords: Slice; Patch-clamp; Voltage-clamp; Potassium channels; Excitability

    www.elsevier.com/locate/neuaging

    rpt(h21ipwsse

    tatc(a

    . Introduction

    Working memory, which is largely mediated by the dor-olateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), declines significantlyuring normal aging in a large proportion of rhesus mon-eys (Bartus et al., 1979; Lai et al., 1995; Moore et al.,003, 2006; Rapp, 1990; Steere and Arnsten, 1997). It isncreasingly evident that there is not a single causativeactor; rather, a multitude of changes in neuronal structurend function occur with normal aging, which may togethernderlie cognitive decline (Dickstein et al., 2007). Rela-ively little is known about age effects on the functionallectrophysiological properties of monkey neurons (Changt al., 2005; Luebke et al., 2004; 2010); by contrast, theffects of age on rodent hippocampal pyramidal cells haveeen extensively studied with in vitro slice recordings (foreview: Barnes, 2003; Burke and Barnes, 2010; Thibault etl., 2007). These studies have shown that there is an age-

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: 1 617 638 4930; fax: 1 617 638 5954.

    rE-mail address: [email protected] (J.I. Luebke).

    197-4580/$ see front matter 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.oi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.002

    elated decrease in action potential firing rates of CA1yramidal cells, with a concomitant increase in the ampli-ude of the calcium-dependent slow afterhyperpolarizationsAHP) responsible for spike frequency adaptation (Dister-oft et al., 1996; Landfield and Pitler, 1984; Power et al.,002; for review: Faber and Sah, 2003; Thibault et al.,998). Other studies have demonstrated that the age-relatedncrease in magnitude of the sAHP is inversely related toerformance on tasks mediated largely by the hippocampus,ith aged-impaired subjects having significantly higher

    AHP amplitudes than both aged-unimpaired and youngubjects, which do not differ (Matthews et al., 2009; Moyert al., 2000; Tombaugh et al., 2005).

    These in vitro slice studies have led to a widely held viewhat age-related decline in hippocampal function can bettributed to reduced excitability and reduced synaptic plas-icity of CA1 pyramidal neurons, secondary to increasedalcium influx and amplitude of calcium-dependent sAHPsfor review: Faber and Sah, 2003; Foster, 2007; Thibault etl., 2007). While some in vivo single unit studies have

    eported an age-related decrease in CA1 pyramidal cell

    mailto:[email protected]

  • fispTM(ciasv(aaseZteasmtwet

    2

    2

    mpMmrwtPLIYUto

    2

    viMmRmm

    (tDaDCcDtcstay

    2

    tkkppwc1MPm1wsRNcwlFsHoeliI

    2

    wtfhPtt

    2 J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    ring rates (Sava and Markus, 2008; Shen et al., 1997),ome others have reported no change in firing rates of CA1yramidal cells (Barnes et al., 1983; Oler and Markus, 2000;anila et al., 1997) or of CA3 pyramidal cells (Oler andarkus, 2000; Tanila et al., 1997). Further, Wilson et al.

    2005) reported no change in firing rates of CA1 pyramidalells, but an increase in firing rates of CA3 pyramidal cellsn the aged rat. Age-related increases in firing rates havelso been reported for layer 3 pyramidal cells in in vitrolices of aged monkey dlPFC (Chang et al., 2005), and forisual cortical pyramidal cells in the aged monkey in vivoLeventhal et al., 2003; Schmolesky et al., 2000; Zhang etl., 2008). In each case where increased excitability withge has been seen, it has been associated with decline inensory or cognitive function (Chang et al., 2005; Leventhalt al., 2003; Schmolesky et al., 2000; Wilson et al., 2005;hang et al., 2008). While certainly functionally relevant,

    he cellular basis for age-related increases in neuronalxcitability is unknown, but one plausible mechanism isreduction in the amplitude of the current underlying the

    AHP (the sIAHP). This study was undertaken to deter-ine whether there are age-related changes in the ampli-

    ude of the sIAHP in monkey dlPFC pyramidal cells, andhether any such changes are related to alterations in

    xcitability of these cells and/or to cognitive decline inhe same monkeys.

    . Methods

    .1. Experimental subjects

    Rhesus monkeys obtained from the Yerkes National Pri-ate Research Center were used in this study, which was

    art of an ongoing program of studies of normal aging.onkeys were maintained at both the Yerkes National Pri-ate Research Center and at the Boston University Labo-

    atory Animal Science Center (LASC) in strict accordanceith animal care guidelines as outlined in the NIH Guide for

    he Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the USAublic Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use ofaboratory Animals. All procedures were approved by the

    nstitutional Animal Care and Use Committees of both theerkes National Primate Research Center and the Bostonniversity LASC. Both institutions are fully accredited by

    he Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Lab-ratory Animal Care.

    .2. Assessment of cognitive function

    Each monkey completed 69 months of testing on aariety of tasks designed to evaluate overall cognitive abil-ties. This testing consisted of the following: Delayed Non-

    atch to Sample (DNMS) basic (learning), DNMS perfor-ance at 2 and 10 minute delays, and the Delayedecognition Span Task (DRST), with spatial and objectodalities. Detailed descriptions of assessment and imple-

    entation of these tasks can be found in Herndon et al. E

    1997). Significant impairment on individual behavioralasks was defined as follows: greater than 210 errors for theNMS basic task, less than 78% correct for the DNMS 2

    nd 10 minute delay tasks, and a span of less than 2.5 for theRST spatial and object tasks (Herndon et al., 1997). Theognitive Impairment Index (CII) is a composite score,alculated as an average of the standardized scores on theNMS basic (10-second delay), DNMS 2-minute delay and

    he DRST spatial tasks using the guidance of a principalomponents analysis (Herndon et al., 1997). In the presenttudy, monkeys were considered cognitively impaired ifhey received a CII z-score of more than 2.5 standard devi-tions greater than the mean for a large cohort of healthy,oung monkeys.

    .3. Preparation of slices

    Following completion of behavioral testing and struc-ural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) brain scans, mon-eys were sacrificed. Monkeys were first tranquilized withetamine, 10 mg/mL, then deeply anesthetized with sodiumentobarbital (to effect, 15 mg/kg I.V.). A thoracotomy waserformed, and monkeys were killed by exsanguinationhile being perfused through the ascending aorta with ice-

    old Krebs buffer (concentrations, in mM: 6.4 Na2HPO4,.4 Na2PO4, 137 NaCl, 2.7 KCl, 5 Glucose, 0.3 CaCl2, 1gCl2; pH 7.4, chemicals from Sigma, St. Louis, MO).

    rior to perfusion, a craniotomy was performed, and, im-ediately following perfusion, the dura was opened and a

    0 mm3 block of the dlPFC (area 46) removed. The tissueas quickly mounted and cut into 400 m thick coronal

    lices with a vibrating microtome in ice-cold oxygenatedingers solution (concentrations, in mM: 26 NaHCO3, 124aCl, 2 KCl, 3 KH2PO4, 10 Glucose, 1.3 MgCl2; pH 7.4,

    hemicals from Sigma). Immediately after cutting, slicesere placed in oxygenated, room temperature Ringers so-

    ution, where they were allowed to equilibrate for 1 hour.ollowing this equilibration period, a slice was placed in aubmersion-type recording chamber (Harvard Apparatus,olliston, MA), held down by a nylon mesh, and continu-usly superfused with oxygenated, room temperature Ring-rs solution (at a rate of 22.5 ml/min). Chambers wereocated on the stages of Nikon E600 infrared-differentialnterference contrast (IRDIC) microscopes (MicroVideonstruments, Avon, MA).

    .4. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings

    Pyramidal cells from either layer 3 or layer 5 of area 46ere visually identified under IRDIC optics, and standard,

    ight-seal, whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were per-ormed. Pipettes were pulled from borosilicate glass on aorizontal Flaming and Brown micropipette puller (Model87, Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA). The internal solu-

    ion used in recording pipettes was as follows (concentra-ions, in mM): 100 potassium aspartate, 15 KCl, 3 MgCl2, 5

    GTA, 10 Na-HEPES, 0.3 NaGTP, and 2 MgATP (pH 7.4,

  • cowcqmriamt

    2a

    rspVitppmE

    2c

    iacttf1cnsc(miob

    2

    adPac

    cuagmDit

    3

    3i

    (pusibims-

    FcpitmbAp

    3J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    F1

    T1

    T2

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    hemicals from Fluka, NY), and electrodes had resistancesf 36 M in the external, Ringers solution. Experimentsere performed with either List EPC-9 or EPC-10 patch-

    lamp amplifiers using either Pulse or PatchMaster ac-uisition software (HEKA Elektronik, Lambrecht, Ger-any). Access resistance was monitored throughout, and

    ecordings were low-pass filtered at 10 kHz. To be includedn the analyses, cells were required to exhibit repetitivection potential firing on depolarization, have a restingembrane potential negative to 55 mV, an action poten-

    ial overshoot and stable access resistance.

    .5. Determination of intrinsic membrane properties andction potential firing rates

    To determine passive membrane properties, includingesting membrane potential (Vr) and input resistance (Rn), aeries of 200-ms current pulses (14 steps, 160 to 100A) were applied from a membrane potential of 70 mV.

    r was measured as the potential present with 0 currentnput and Rn was determined as the slope of the best fit linehrough the plotted VI data. To examine repetitive actionotential firing properties, 2000 ms depolarizing currentulses (30, 180 and 280 pA) were applied, also from aembrane potential of 70 mV. Data were analyzed using

    Pulse-Fit or FitMaster analysis software from HEKAlektronik.

    .6. Characterization of the slow afterhyperpolarizationurrent sIAHP

    The sIAHP was evoked with a series of 200 ms depolar-zing prepulse steps in voltage clamp (eight steps, 50 to

    20 mV) from a holding potential of -55 mV (Fig. 1A). Themplitude of the sIAHP was measured as the peak outwardurrent 20 ms after cessation of the step, during the returno holding voltage (Fig. 1A). The calcium-dependence ofhe current was demonstrated by its increasing amplitudeollowing increasing amplitude (Fig. 1A) and duration (Fig.B) depolarizing prepulse voltage steps used to evoke cal-ium influx through high voltage-activated calcium chan-els. The voltage-independence of the current was demon-trated by a lack of change in the amplitude of the tailurrents at the offset of increasing holding potential stepsFig. 1C, arrow). Finally, the current was identified phar-acologically as being primarily comprised of the sIAHP as

    t was largely blocked by the noradrenergic -receptor ag-nist isoproterenol (Fig. 1D), but not significantly reducedy apamin (not shown).

    .7. Statistical analyses

    Both behavioral and electrophysiological data were an-lyzed for statistical significance using a two-tailed Stu-ents t-test. To investigate relationships, linear (Pearsonsroduct-Moment correlations) and quadratic regressionnalyses were performed. For statistical analyses, signifi-

    ance was defined as p 0.05. Monkeys within the aged k

    ohort were grouped into aged-impaired (AI) and aged-nimpaired (AU) groups based on their CII z-score. Inddition, for each behavioral task, aged monkeys wererouped into AI or AU groups based on their perfor-ance on the specific task (with the exception of theNMS 10-minute delay on which all aged animals were

    mpaired). All data are reported as the standard error ofhe mean.

    . Results

    .1. Most but not all aged monkeys were cognitivelympaired

    A total of 12 young (6.012.0 years old) and 16 aged17.627.0 years old) rhesus monkeys were used in theresent study. Table 1 provides information on the monkeyssed for layer 3 cell recordings and Table 2 provides theame information for monkeys used for layer 5 cell record-ngs. In four of the young and five of the aged monkeys,oth layer 3 and layer 5 cells were examined and thus theres overlap in the monkeys listed in Tables 1 and 2. Eachonkey successfully completed testing on the DNMS-ba-

    ic, -2 and -10 minute delay tasks and the DRST-spatial andobject tasks (Tables 1 and 2). As a group, the aged mon-

    ig. 1. Characterization of the sIAHP in representative layer 3 pyramidalells. (A) Response to increasing amplitude depolarizing 200-ms voltagerepulses. (B) Response of a representative layer 3 pyramidal cell toncreasing duration depolarizing prepulses. (C) Voltage-independence ofhe sIAHP tail current demonstrated by a consistent step to -5 mV for 100s, followed by a voltage step to 95 to 35 mV. (D) Near complete

    lock of the sIAHP by bath application of isoproterenol, 10 M. Scale bars: 35 mV, 100 pA/100 ms; B 30 mV, 20 pA/1 s; C 25 mV, 200

    A/500 ms; D 30 mV, 100 pA/200 ms.

    eys used for layer 3 recordings were significantly cog-

  • nsgzi0

    sPaMsb

    TL

    A

    AAAAAAAAMSAAAAAAAAAAAAMSp

    C % corr

    TL

    A

    AAAAAAAAMSAAAAAAAAAMSp

    C

    4 J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    itively impaired, as demonstrated by the mean CII z-core of 5.0 1.2 compared with 0.7 0.3 in the youngroup (p 0.005). In the young layer 3 cohort, the CII-score ranged from 0.03 to 2.3, with none consideredmpaired overall, while in the aged group the range was.55 to 11.9, with 8 of the 12 monkeys classified as

    able 1ayer 3 pyramidal cells experimental subjects

    m# Sex Age CII Trials Errors

    M204 M 6 0.03 446 85M230 M 7.5 0.27 220 66M198 F 7.8 0.41 439 104M255 F 9.5 0.12 100 29M199 F 10.6 1.61 732 168M214 F 10.7 2.30 759 168M254 F 11 0.97 329 99M197 F 11.3 0.02 361 46ean 9.3 0.70 423.3 95.6

    EM 0.74 0.32 86.6 19.4M257 F 17.6 1.66 420 98M253 F 18 0.55 329 99M256 F 20 2.20 883 202M200 F 21 4.95 1283 414M236 M 22.9 0.58 360 112M242 M 23 6.01 1600 431M234 F 23.5 3.48 1253 271M235 F 24 4.00 1140 375M243 M 24.4 11.9 3626 962M189 M 24.5 2.90 728 217M220 F 25.7 11.5 2880 948M181 F 27 9.63 2253 792ean 22.6 5.0 1396 410

    EM 0.88 1.22 314 96.3 0.0001 0.005 0.009 0.007

    II: z-score; DNMS basic: total number of trials or errors; DNMS delay:

    able 2ayer 5 pyramidal cells experimental subjects

    m# Sex Age CII Trials Errors

    M204 M 6 0.03 446 85M205 M 6.2 0.08 231 54M198 F 7.8 0.41 439 104M255 F 9.5 0.12 100 29M202 F 10.3 0.45 360 99M214 F 10.7 2.30 759 168M194 F 11.9 2.26 764 156M195 F 12 0.03 255 56ean 9.3 0.70 419.3 93.9

    EM 0.9 0.38 90.7 18.5M257 F 17.6 1.66 420 98M190 F 18 1.79 737 149M253 F 18 0.55 329 99M177 F 20.7 6.73 2060 518M208 M 22 1.09 300 74M234 F 23.5 3.48 1253 271M179 F 23.8 6.99 1559 505M189 M 24.5 2.90 728 217M220 F 25.7 11.5 2880 948ean 21.5 4.1 1141 319.9

    EM 1.1 1.3 313 102.2 0.0001 0.03 0.05 0.05

    II: z-score; DNMS basic: total number of trials or errors; DNMS delay: % corr

    ignificantly impaired and four as unimpaired (Table 1).erformance on every task was significantly worse in theged group of monkeys compared with young (Table 1).onkeys used for layer 5 recordings demonstrated a

    imilar pattern of age-related cognitive impairment (Ta-le 2).

    MS 2= delay DNMS 10= delay DRST spatial DRST object

    0.76 3.23 4.800.90 2.22 4.040.80 3.71 4.320.68 3.42 5.660.78 3.01 4.420.76 2.73 3.730.70 2.40 2.660.82 2.31 2.910.78 2.88 4.070.03 0.21 0.370.58 2.36 2.660.74 3.03 2.930.64 3.09 3.010.74 2.38 3.430.66 2.84 2.620.55 2.97 3.190.50 2.62 3.070.72 2.26 2.590.56 1.99 1.980.60 2.06 2.490.68 2.03 2.460.58 2.45 2.530.63 2.51 2.750.02 0.12 0.120.0003 0.05 0.0002

    ect; DRST: average total span.

    S 2= delay DNMS 10= delay DRST spatial DRST object

    0.76 3.23 4.800.88 3.24 6.320.80 3.71 4.320.68 3.42 5.660.64 2.90 4.150.76 2.73 3.730.66 2.43 2.990.68 3.07 5.110.73 3.09 4.640.03 0.15 0.400.58 2.36 2.660.72 2.33 2.810.74 3.03 2.930.42 2.37 2.790.60 2.66 3.660.50 2.62 3.070.67 1.84 1.820.60 2.06 2.490.68 2.03 2.460.61 2.37 2.740.04 0.13 0.180.02 0.002 0.001

    DN

    0.870.910.780.730.790.720.840.910.820.030.740.830.750.780.870.580.730.870.700.790.740.710.760.020.05

    DNM

    0.870.800.780.730.860.720.740.830.790.020.740.810.830.690.750.730.690.790.740.750.020.16

    ect; DRST: average total span.

  • 3

    o8a3m(patc

    3fi

    rpdatafslata

    3l

    sv(wmcis

    TE

    L

    F

    s

    L

    F

    s

    F3sabafiofimmpnb

    5J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    T3

    F2-F3

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    .2. Population of cells examined

    For layer 3 pyramidal cell analyses, recordings werebtained from a total of 48 cells in slices prepared fromyoung monkeys and 82 cells from 12 aged monkeys,

    nd for layer 5 analyses recordings were obtained from8 cells from 8 young monkeys and 33 cells from 9 agedonkeys (Tables 1 and 2). Passive membrane properties

    resting membrane potential and input resistance), actionotential firing rates evoked by depolarizing current stepsnd slow afterhyperpolarization current (sIAHP) proper-ies were assessed for all layer 3 and layer 5 pyramidalells (Table 3).

    .3. Passive membrane properties and action potentialring rates

    Neither the resting membrane potential (Vr) nor the inputesistance (Rn) were changed with age in layer 3 or layer 5yramidal cells (Table 3). Pyramidal cells responded toepolarizing current steps with trains of regular spikingction potentials that exhibited little spike frequency adap-ation. Layer 3 pyramidal cells from aged monkeys firedction potentials at significantly higher rates than did thoserom young subjects at the 180 and 280 pA currentteps (p 0.004 and 0.03, respectively; Table 3). Cells inayer 5, however, did not display an age-related change inction potential firing rate (Table 3). These data are consis-ent with those previously published by our group (Chang et

    able 3lectrophysiological properties of dlPFC pyramidal cells

    Young Aged p

    Mean SEM Mean SEM

    ayer 3Vr 67.9 0.58 67.1 0.49 nsRn (M) 114.5 7.29 118.0 4.36 ns

    iring rate (Hz)30-pA step 0.99 0.43 1.42 0.32 ns180-pA step 10.3 0.95 13.6 0.62 0.004280-pA step 13.5 0.87 15.9 0.69 0.03

    IAHP Amp (pA)10-mV step 45.1 5.07 57.3 5.27 ns0-mV step 78.3 7.64 95.7 7.84 ns10-mV step 84.7 9.09 112.4 8.90 0.0520-mV step 108.2 11.1 139.1 9.97 0.05

    ayer 5Vr 66.3 0.83 67.1 0.76 nsRn (M) 150.0 7.21 148.8 7.49 ns

    iring rate (Hz)30-pA step 1.7 0.53 3.0 0.64 ns180-pA step 13.4 0.78 16.0 1.14 ns280-pA step 16.9 0.66 18.9 1.14 ns

    IAHP Amp (pA)10-mV step 63.2 8.68 68.1 11.1 ns0-mV step 89.4 11.9 98.5 15.4 ns10-mV step 116.4 14.8 126.7 18.7 ns20-mV step 139.6 16.3 152.7 21.5 ns

    l., 2005; Luebke and Chang, 2007). p

    .4. sIAHP amplitude is significantly increased with age inayer 3 but not layer 5 cells

    Increasing depolarizing voltage steps led to increasedIAHP amplitude in all cells (Figs. 2A, C, 3A, C). At theoltage steps which activated the highest amplitude current10 and 20 mV) a significantly greater amplitude sIAHPas seen in layer 3 cells from aged compared with youngonkeys (p 0.05; Table 3). This contrasts with layer 5

    ells, which demonstrated no significant age-related changen sIAHP amplitude (Table 3). When the mean maximalIAHP amplitudes of layer 3 cells from each monkey were

    ig. 2. The amplitude of the sIAHP increases significantly with age in layerpyramidal cells. (A) sIAHPs evoked by increasing depolarizing voltage

    teps in representative cells from young (left), aged-unimpaired (middle)nd aged-impaired monkeys (right). (B) Significant positive correlationetween sIAHP amplitude and age in all monkeys (left) and those within theged cohort only (right). Additionally, a U-shaped quadratic equation wast to the data obtained from all monkeys, and a significant relationshipbserved (dashed line). (C) Line graph plotting mean sIAHP amplitude as aunction of voltage step for cells from young, aged-unimpaired and aged-mpaired monkeys. Inset: superimposed traces of sIAHPs evoked by 20

    V prepulses in cells from young, aged-unimpaired and aged-impairedonkeys. (D) Mean sIAHP amplitude vs firing rate evoked by a 180

    A 2-second current step. Linear regression line demonstrates no sig-ificant relationship between sIAHP amplitude and firing rate. Scalears: 50 pA/100 ms. *p 0.01; **p 0.001.

    lotted vs. age and a linear regression performed, a sig-

  • nardrmw0tgn(

    gy

    0nscee

    Fpsdbaaabaedfi

    FagdotcpLjfla4s(cc

    6 J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    F4

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    ificant linear relationship was seen-with increasingmplitude sIAHP associated with increased age (p 0.05, 0.494, d.f. 18; Fig. 2B, left). Interestingly, these

    ata were also well-fit by a quadratic function (p 0.01, 0.850, d.f. 18; Fig. 2B, left, dashed line). Theean sIAHP amplitude of layer 3 cells from monkeysithin only the aged cohort also correlated with age (p .01, r 0.780, d.f. 10; Fig. 2B, right), demonstratinghat the mean maximal sIAHP amplitude increases pro-ressively with advancing age. These relationships wereot observed for sIAHP amplitude in layer 5 cells vs. ageFig. 3B).

    The maximal amplitude of the sIAHP was significantlyreater in layer 3 cells from AI compared with AU and

    ig. 3. The amplitude of the sIAHP is unaltered with age in layer 5yramidal cells. (A) sIAHPs evoked by increasing depolarizing voltageteps in representative cells from young (left), aged-unimpaired (mid-le) and aged-impaired monkeys (right). (B) No significant correlationetween sIAHP amplitude and age is seen in all monkeys (left) or in theged cohort only (right). (C) Line graph plotting mean sIAHP amplitudes a function of voltage step for cells from young, aged-unimpaired andged-impaired monkeys. Inset: superimposed traces of sIAHPs evokedy 20 mV prepulses in cells from young, aged-unimpaired andged-impaired monkeys. (D) Mean sIAHP amplitude vs. firing ratevoked by a 180 pA 2-second current step. Linear regression lineemonstrates no significant relationship between sIAHP amplitude andring rate. Scale bars: 50 pA/100 ms.

    oung monkeys (AI v. AU: p 0.001; AI v. young: p t

    .001; AU v. young: not significant; Figs. 2C, 4C). This wasot the case for layer 5 pyramidal cells, where there was noignificant difference in maximal sIAHP amplitude betweenells from AI, AU and young monkeys (Figs. 3C, 4D). Inter-stingly, when sIAHP amplitude was plotted versus firing ratevoked by a strong depolarizing current step in the same cells,

    ig. 4. Relationship of sIAHP amplitude to overall CII z-score. (A) Top,ll subjects: mean sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 pyramidal cells from aiven monkey vs the CII z-score for that monkey. Linear regression lineemonstrates a significant positive correlation. Bottom, aged subjectsnly: mean sIAHP amplitude for a given monkey vs. the CII z-score forhat monkey. Linear regression line demonstrates a significant positiveorrelation. (B) Top, all subjects: mean sIAHP amplitude in layer 5yramidal cells for a given monkey vs the CII z-score for that monkey.inear regression line demonstrates no correlation. Bottom, aged sub-

    ects only: mean sIAHP amplitude for a given monkey vs. the CII z-scoreor that monkey in the aged cohort of monkeys only. Linear regressionine demonstrates no correlation. (C) Bar graph giving mean sIAHPmplitudes in layer 3 cells from young (n 8), aged-unimpaired (n ) and aged-impaired monkeys (n 8). (D) Bar graph giving meanIAHP amplitudes in layer 5 cells from young (n 8), aged-unimpairedn 4) and aged-impaired monkeys (n 5). Dashed lines in A and Borrespond to a z-score of 2.5, subjects with z-scores above this line areonsidered significantly impaired. *p 0.001.

    here was no significant relationship between the two variables

  • i(

    35

    omlzaa0ammi4aptAm1dc

    stcottsorcbtct3ifmlDAmgiwcwi

    Ada

    b3wF6bamlDko

    amiqafepivtsp

    4

    4l

    iahSdtswsscsgaolr

    7J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    F5

    F6

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    n either layer 3 (r 0.01, d.f. 125; Fig. 2D) or layer 5 cellsr 0.085, d.f. 72; Fig. 3D).

    .5. The amplitude of the sIAHP in layer 3 (but not layer) cells is significantly related to cognitive performance

    The relationship between the amplitude of the sIAHP andverall cognitive impairment was assessed by plotting theean maximal amplitude of the current for all layer 3 or all

    ayer 5 cells from a given monkey vs. that monkeys CII-score (Fig. 4A, B). A linear regression was performed, andsignificant relationship was found between mean sIAHP

    mplitude in layer 3 cells and CII z-score (p 0.01, r .638, d.f. 18; Fig. 4A, top), but not between sIAHPmplitude in layer 5 cells and CII z-score (Fig. 4B, top). Theean sIAHP amplitude of layer 3 (but not layer 5) cells fromonkeys within the aged cohort only also correlated with

    ncreased CII z-score (p 0.05, r 0.650, d.f. 10; Fig.A, B, bottom), demonstrating that mean maximal sIAHPmplitude increases with increasing overall cognitive im-airment within the aged group. The mean amplitude ofhe sIAHP was significantly greater in layer 3 cells fromI than from AU and young monkeys with mean maxi-al amplitudes of 172 13 pA vs 96 12 pA and 108

    1 pA, respectively (p 0.001; Fig. 4C), but there was noifference in the mean amplitude of the current in layer 5ells from the three groups (Fig. 4D).

    The relationship between the maximal amplitude of theIAHP in layer 3 cells and impairment on each behavioralask was assessed by plotting the mean amplitude of theurrent for a given monkey vs that monkeys performancen the task. No relationship between layer 5 sIAHP ampli-ude and any behavioral task was found (CII: Fig. 4B; otherasks not shown). Performance on the DNMS basic task wasignificantly negatively correlated with the mean amplitudef the sIAHP in layer 3 cells, both in all monkeys (p 0.01, 0.661, d.f. 18; Fig. 5A, top) and within the aged

    ohort only (p 0.02, r 0.669, d.f. 10; Fig. 5A,ottom). By contrast, there was no significant linear rela-ionship between the mean amplitude of the sIAHP in layer 3ells and performance on the DNMS 2 or 10 minute delayasks (Fig. 5B, C). The mean amplitude of the sIAHP in layer

    cells from aged monkeys that were impaired on eachndividual task was compared with the amplitude in cellsrom those monkeys that were unimpaired on the task. Theean amplitude of the sIAHP was significantly greater in

    ayer 3 cells from aged animals that were impaired on theNMS basic task compared with those that were fromU or from young monkeys (p 0.001; Fig. 5D). Theean amplitude of the sIAHP was also significantly

    reater in layer 3 cells from aged animals that werempaired on the DNMS 2-minute delay task comparedith young monkeys (p 0.01; Fig. 5E); however, these

    ells did not differ from those from aged monkeys thatere unimpaired on this task. All aged animals were

    mpaired on the DNMS 10-minute delay task (thus, no l

    U data are presented in Fig. 5F) and a significantifference in the amplitude of the sIAHP between youngnd aged monkeys was seen (p 0.05).

    There was a trend toward a significant relationshipetween increased mean amplitude of the sIAHP in layercells and poorer performance on the DRST spatial taskithin the aged group (p 0.10, r 0.528, d.f. 10;ig. 6A, bottom), but not within the overall cohort (Fig.A, top). Finally, there was no significant relationshipetween the mean amplitude of the sIAHP in layer 3 cellsnd performance on the DRST object task (Fig. 6B). Theean amplitude of the sIAHP was significantly greater in

    ayer 3 cells from animals that were impaired on theRST spatial tasks compared with AU or to young mon-eys (p 0.01; Fig. 6C), but only between AI and youngn the DRST object task (p 0.02; Fig. 6D).

    Given that both CII and sIAHP significantly increase withge, a partial correlation analysis was performed to deter-ine if cognitive impairment per se is related to the increase

    n sIAHP, or if the increase is simply an unrelated conse-uence of the aging process. Partial correlation analysesllow for the examining of one variable while controllingor variance in another, in this case controlling for age whilexamining the relationship between sIAHP and CII. The-value approached but did not quite meet statistical signif-cance (p 0.059). It seems likely that this is due to theariance in this relatively small sample. Hence it is sugges-ive of an overall nonzero association (i.e. a relationship ofIAHP to CII, when age is controlled for) in the generalopulation of monkeys.

    . Discussion

    .1. Age-related increase in the amplitude of the sIAHP inayer 3 pyramidal cells

    One of the most consistent findings in the aging literatures of a significant increase in the amplitude of the sAHPnd/or its underlying current the sIAHP in aged rodentippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells (for review: Faber andah, 2003; Foster, 2007; Thibault et al., 2007). Here, weemonstrate that layer 3 pyramidal cells in in vitro slices ofhe primate dlPFC exhibit a similar age-related increase inIAHP amplitude. Thus, in contrast to neuronal excitability,hich has been reported to decrease, increase or remain the

    ame with age depending on brain area and species, theIAHP amplitude has consistently been demonstrated to in-rease with age across brain areas and species. However, ithould be noted that this phenomenon is not ubiquitous,iven that the amplitude of the sIAHP does not change withge in layer 5 pyramidal cells from the same monkeys. Thebservation of an age-related increase in sIAHP amplitude inayer 3 but not layer 5 cells is of interest given the differentoles of cells in the two cortical laminae; corticocortical

    ayer 3 cells are thought to play a key role in cognitive

  • fprd2

    osctl(ta

    hcceTiPvLrwf2

    Fcfabt*

    8 J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    unction and to be especially vulnerable during aging, whilerimarily subcortically projecting layer 5 cells play a lesserole in cognitive function and may be relatively spareduring aging (Morrison and Hof, 2002; Morrison and Hof,007).

    The present study did not directly address the mechanismf the age-related increase in sIAHP. Perhaps the mosttraightforward explanation would be an age-related in-rease in the somatic membrane surface area, and thus in theotal number of channels underlying the sIAHP. However,ayer 3 pyramidal cell soma size does not change with agePeters, personal communication), and input resistance washe same in young and aged cells in the present study,

    ig. 5. Relationship of sIAHP amplitude to performance on Delayed Non-ells from a given monkey vs the monkeys performance on (A) DNMSrom all monkeys, bottom: data from aged monkeys only. Bar graphs dend aged monkeys that were either unimpaired or impaired on (D) DNMecause all aged monkeys were impaired on the DNMS 10-minute delayo the level above which (DNMS errors) or below which (DNMS 2 an*p 0.01; ***p 0.001.

    rguing against this idea. Studies in the rodent hippocampus a

    ave clearly demonstrated that age-related changes in cal-ium homeostasis can directly impact the amplitude of thisalcium-dependent current (Kumar and Foster, 2002; Norrist al., 1998; for review: Faber and Sah, 2003; Foster, 2007;hibault et al., 2007). There is increased calcium influx

    n CA1 pyramidal cells with aging (Moyer et al., 1992;ower et al., 2002) due to an increase in density of higholtage-activated L-type calcium channels (Thibault andandfield, 1996). There is also evidence for changes in

    yanodine sensitive calcium-dependent calcium releaseith age, although consensus on this is lacking (Clod-

    elter et al., 2002; Gant et al., 2006; Kumar and Foster,004; for review: Foster, 2007; Thibault et al., 2007). In

    to Sample behavioral tasks. Mean sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 pyramidal(B) DNMS 2-minute delay, and (C) DNMS 10-minute delay. Top: datamean sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 pyramidal cells from young monkeys

    c, (E) DNMS 2-minute delay, or (F) DNMS 10-minute delay. Note thatre is no aged-unimpaired group. Dashed lines in A, B and C correspond

    inute delay) a subject is considered significantly impaired. *p 0.05;

    Matchbasic,pictingS basi

    task thed 10 m

    ddition to being influenced by intracellular calcium lev-

  • etbMnRtaiatta

    4rs

    an2assdaspptiMteWscdr2mZfipasS

    efcfiFtartbfisbitfiipn

    FRcsmmaDcc

    9J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    ls, the sIAHP is modulated by norepinephrine and ace-ylcholine, which reduce the sIAHP open channel proba-ility and thus its amplitude (Sah and Isaacson, 1995).oore et al. (2005) reported significant reductions in

    oradrenergic markers, and Vannucchi and Goldman-akic (1991) reported a decreased affinity of M1 recep-

    ors for the muscarinic agonist carbachol in the dlPFC ofged monkeys. An age-related decline in these sIAHPnhibitors could result in a disinhibition of the current andn increase in its amplitude. Further studies are requiredo determine which, if any, of these mechanisms underliehe significant increase in amplitude of the sIAHP in the

    ig. 6. Relationship of sIAHP amplitude to performance on Delayedecognition Span Tasks. Mean sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 pyramidalells from a given monkey vs. the monkeys performance on (A) DRSTpatial and (B) DRST object behavioral tasks. Top: data from allonkeys, bottom: data from aged monkeys only. Bar graphs depictingean sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 pyramidal cells from young monkeys

    nd aged monkeys that were either unimpaired or impaired on (C)RST spatial or (D) DRST object tasks. Dashed lines in A and B

    orrespond to the level below which a subject is considered signifi-antly impaired. *p 0.02; **p 0.01.

    ged primate dlPFC. n

    .2. Functional electrophysiological implications: firingates of layer 3 pyramidal cells are not associated withIAHP amplitude

    Changes in neuronal excitability and synaptic plasticityre frequently proposed to underlie cognitive decline duringormal aging (for review: Barnes, 2003; Burke and Barnes,006, 2010; Faber and Sah, 2003; Foster, 2007; Thibault etl., 2007). Both excitability and synaptic plasticity can betrongly modulated by the sIAHP, which acts by increasingpike frequency adaptation in the first case, and by shuntingendritic synaptic currents in the second (for review: Fabernd Sah, 2003). Many previous in vitro slice studies haveuggested that reduced excitability of CA1 pyramidal cellslays an important role in age-related impairment in hip-ocampal function in rodents. It is worth noting, however,hat while some in vivo recording studies report a reductionn the firing rates of CA1 pyramidal cells with age (Sava and

    arkus, 2008; Shen et al., 1997), several others report thathe firing rates of these cells are unaltered with age (Barnest al., 1983; Oler and Markus, 2000; Tanila et al., 1997;ilson et al., 2005). Furthermore, several studies have

    hown increased excitability of aged neurons with re-ordings of pyramidal cells in in vitro slices of the monkeylPFC (Chang et al., 2005), and with in vivo single unitecordings of CA3 pyramidal cells in rats (Wilson et al.,005) and visual cortical pyramidal cells in the rhesusonkey (Leventhal et al., 2003; Schmolesky et al., 2000;hang et al., 2008). Importantly, in these studies, increasedring rates were associated with age-related cognitive im-airment in monkeys and rats (Chang et al., 2005; Wilson etl., 2005; respectively), and with degradation of visualtimulus selectivity in monkeys (Leventhal et al., 2003;chmolesky et al., 2000; Zhang et al., 2008).

    Age-related reduction in rodent CA1 pyramidal cellxcitability is due to increased action potential firingrequency adaptation likely related to an age-related in-rease in sAHP amplitude (Disterhoft et al., 1996; Land-eld and Pitler, 1984; Power et al., 2002; for review:aber and Sah, 2003; Thibault et al., 1998). A key ques-

    ion addressed by the present study was whether thege-related increase in excitability of layer 3 dlPFC py-amidal cells in the monkey could be due to a decrease inhe amplitude of the sIAHP. The lack of a relationshipetween the amplitude of this current and action potentialring rates of layer 3 and layer 5 pyramidal cells arguestrongly against this idea and suggests a dissociationetween sIAHP amplitude and action potential firing ratesn these cells, which exhibit little spike frequency adap-ation compared with hippocampal pyramidal cells. Ourndings indicate that the age-related sIAHP amplitude

    ncrease is not related to excitability changes in layer 3yramidal cells of the monkey dlPFC. Thus, the mecha-ism(s) underlying increased excitability of neurons with

    ormal aging remains an open question.

  • 4c

    oaas11tcmmpaCo(iffbicltidj(TciwaiFsibmdTctBco2qitp

    D

    iP0

    A

    ifCd

    R

    B

    B

    B

    B

    B

    C

    C

    D

    D

    D

    F

    F

    G

    H

    L

    L

    10 J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    .3. Relationship of sIAHP amplitude to performance onognitive tasks

    It is well known that significant cognitive impairmentccurs with normal aging in many species, and that withinny cohort of aged animals or humans there are successful-gers that are not impaired and unsuccessful-agers that areignificantly impaired (Bartus et al., 1979; Herndon et al.,997; Lai et al., 1995; Moore et al., 2003, 2005, 2006; Rapp,990; Steere and Arnsten, 1997). Thus, the demonstrationhat most, but not all, aged monkeys were significantlyognitively impaired in the present study is consistent withany previous studies. The findings in the aged rhesusonkey are consistent with findings in aged rodent CA1

    yramidal cells in that sIAHP amplitude in layer 3 cells ofged monkey dlPFC correlated positively with increasedII z-score. Further, cells from monkeys that were impairedn the DNMS basic, DNMS 2-minute delay and DRSTboth spatial and object) tasks exhibited significantlyncreased mean sIAHP amplitude compared with thoserom young monkeys. The amplitude of the sIAHP in cellsrom aged monkeys that were unimpaired on these tasks,y contrast, did not differ from young. The key questionswhat discriminates between successful and unsuc-essful aging individuals at a single neuron or networkevel? The inverse relationship between the amplitude ofhe sAHP in hippocampal pyramidal neurons recorded inn vitro slices and performance on both hippocampal-ependent and -independent conditioning tasks by sub-ects from which slices were prepared is well-establishedDisterhoft et al., 1986, 1996; Matthews et al., 2009;hompson et al., 1996; Tombaugh et al., 2005). Thisonsistent finding has led to the hypothesis that reductionn sAHP amplitude may be a general mechanism byhich neuronal excitability is increased during learning,

    nd that an increase in sAHP amplitude leads to reductionn excitability and cognitive impairment in aging (e.g.aber and Sah, 2003). The question arises as to whetherimilar mechanisms hold true in other brain areas, includ-ng the dlPFC of the aged monkey. Given the dissociationetween sIAHP amplitude and firing rate in dlPFC pyra-idal cells shown in this study, a change in excitability

    ue to a change in sIAHP is not a plausible mechanism.he sIAHP could also impact cognition by shunting in-oming depolarizing synaptic potentials, thus raising thehreshold for induction of synaptic plasticity (Sah andekkers, 1996; Thibault et al., 2001). This mechanism isonsistent with the report of reduced synaptic excitationf these cells in aged monkey dlPFC slices (Luebke et al.,004). Further work is needed to address the importantuestion of precisely how the increase in sIAHP amplituden layer 3 pyramidal cells relates to the decline in func-ions mediated by the dlPFC with normal aging in the

    rimate.

    isclosure statement

    The authors report no actual or potential conflicts ofnterest. This work was supported by NIH/NIA grants no.01 AG00001 and R01 AG025062 and NIH/NCRR # RR-0165.

    cknowledgments

    The authors are grateful to Douglas Rosene for perform-ng perfusions of the monkeys and providing brain tissuerom which recordings were obtained. We thank Johannarimins, Anne Rocher and Yu-Ming Chang for help withata acquisition and careful reading of the manuscript.

    eferences

    arnes, C.A., 2003. Long-term potentiation and the ageing brain. Philos.Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 765772.

    arnes, C.A., McNaughton, B.L., OKeefe, J., 1983. Loss of place speci-ficity in hippocampal complex spike cells of senescent rat. Neurobiol.Aging 4, 113119.

    artus, J.M., Dean, R.L., Fleming, D., 1979. Aging in the rhesus monkey:effects on visual discrimination learning and reversal learning. J. Ger-ontol. 34, 209219.

    urke, S.N., Barnes, C.A., 2006. Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat.Rev. Neurosci. 7, 3040.

    urke, S.N., Barnes, C.A., 2010. Senescent synapses and hippocampalcircuit dynamics. Trends Neurosci. 33, 153161.

    hang, Y., Rosene, D.L., Killiany, R.J., Mangiamele, L.A., Luebke, J.L.,2005. Increased action potential firing rates of layer 2/3 pyramidal cellsin the prefrontal cortex are significantly related to cognitive perfor-mance in aged monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 15, 409418.

    lodfelter, G.V., Porter, N.M., Landfield, P.W., Thibault, O., 2002. Sus-tained Ca2-induced Ca2-release underlies the post-glutamate lethalCa2 plateau in older cultured hippocampal neurons. Eur. J. Pharma-col. 447, 189200.

    ickstein, D.L., Kabaso, D., Rocher, A.B., Luebke, J.I., Wearne, S.L., Hof,P.R., 2007. Changes in the structural complexity of the aged brain.Aging Cell 6, 275284.

    isterhoft, J.F., Coulter, D.A., Alkon, D.L., 1986. Conditioning-specificmembrane changes of rabbit hippocampal neurons measured in vitro.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 83, 27332737.

    isterhoft, J.F., Thompson, L.T., Moyer, J.R., Mogul, D.J., 1996. Calcium-dependent afterhyperpolarization and learning in young and aginghippocampus. Life Sci. 59, 413420.

    aber, E.S., Sah, P., 2003. Calcium-activated potassium channels: multiplecontributions to neuronal function. Neuroscientist 9, 181194.

    oster, T.C., 2007. Calcium homeostasis and modulation of synaptic plas-ticity in the aged brain. Aging Cell 6, 319325.

    ant, J.C., Sama, M.M., Landfield, P.W., Thibault, O., 2006. Early andsimultaneous emergence of multiple hippocampal biomarkers of agingis mediated by Ca2-induced Ca2 release. J. Neurosci. 26, 34823490.

    erndon, J.G., Moss, M.B., Rosene, D.L., Killiany, R.J., 1997. Patterns ofcognitive decline in aged rhesus monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 87,2535.

    ai, Z.C., Moss, M.B., Killiany, R.J., Rosene, D.L., Herndon, J.G., 1995.Executive system dysfunction in the aged monkey: spatial and objectreversal learning. Neurobiol. Aging 16, 947954.

    andfield, P.W., Pitler, T.A., 1984. Prolonged Ca2-dependent afterhyper-polarizations in hippocampal neurons of aged rats. Science 226, 1089

    1092.

    http://:AG025062

  • L

    L

    L

    L

    K

    K

    M

    M

    M

    M

    M

    M

    M

    M

    N

    O

    P

    R

    S

    S

    S

    S

    S

    S

    T

    T

    T

    T

    T

    T

    T

    V

    W

    Z

    11J.I. Luebke et al. / Neurobiology of Aging xx (2010) xxx

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    eventhal, A.G., Wang, Y., Pu, M., Zhou, Y., Ma, Y., 2003. GABA and itsagonists improved visual cortical function in senescent monkeys. Sci-ence 300, 812815.

    uebke, J.I., Barbas, H., Peters, A., 2010. Effects of normal aging onprefrontal area 46 in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. Rev. 62, 212232.

    uebke, J.I., Chang, Y., 2007. Effects of aging on the electrophysiologicalproperties of layer 5 pyramidal cells in the prefrontal cortex. Neuro-science 150, 556562.

    uebke, J.I., Chang, Y., Moore, T.L., Rosene, D.L., 2004. Normal agingresults in decreased synaptic excitation and increased synaptic inhibi-tion of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the monkey prefrontal cortex.Neuroscience 125, 277288.

    umar, A., Foster, T.C., 2002. 17beta-estradiol benzoate decreases theAHP amplitude in CA1 pyramidal neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 621626.

    umar, A., Foster, T.C., 2004. Enhanced long-term potentiation duringaging is masked by processes involving intracellular calcium stores. J.Neurophysiol. 91, 24372444.

    atthews, E.A., Linardakis, J.M., Disterhoft, J.F., 2009. The fast and slowafterhyperpolarizations are differentially modulated in hippocampalneurons by aging and learning. J. Neurosci. 29, 47504755.

    oore, T.L., Killiany, R.J., Herndon, J.G., Rosene, D.L., Moss, M.B.,2003. Impairment in abstraction and set shifting in aged rhesus mon-keys. Neurobiol. Aging 24, 125134.

    oore, T.L., Killiany, R.J., Herndon, J.G., Rosene, D.L., Moss, M.B.,2006. Executive system dysfunction occurs as early as middle-age inthe rhesus monkey. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 14841493.

    oore, T.L., Schettler, S.P., Killiany, R.J., Herndon, J.G., Luebke, J.I.,Moss, M.B., Rosene, D.L., 2005. Cognitive impairment in aged rhesusmonkeys associated with monoamine receptors in the prefrontal cortex.Behav. Brain Res. 160, 208221.

    orrison, J.H., Hof, P.R., 2002. Selective vulnerability of corticocorticaland hippocampal circuits in aging Alzheimers disease. Prof Brain.Resources 136, 467486.

    orrison, J.H., Hof, P.R., 2007. Life and death of neurons in the agingcerebral cortex. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 81, 4157.

    oyer, J.R., Thompson, L.T., Black, J.P., Disterhoft, J.F., 1992. Nimodip-ine increases excitability of rabbit CA1 pyramidal neurons in an age-and concentration-dependent manner. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 21002109.

    oyer, J.R., Power, J.M., Thompson, L.T., Disterhoft, J.F., 2000. In-creased excitability of aged rabbit CA1 neurons after trace eyeblinkconditioning. J. Neurosci. 20, 54765482.

    orris, C.M., Halpain, S., Foster, T.C., 1998. Reversal of age-relatedalterations in synaptic plasticity by blockade of L-type Ca2 channels.J. Neurosci. 18, 31713179.

    ler, J.A., Markus, E.J., 2000. Age-related deficits in the ability to encodecontextual change: a place cell analysis. Hippocampus 10, 338350.

    ower, J.M., Wu, W.W., Sametsky, E., Oh, M.M., Disterhoft, J.F., 2002.Age-related enhancement of the slow outward calcium-activated po-tassium current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in vitro. J.Neurosci. 22, 72347243.

    app, P.R., 1990. Visual discrimination and reversal learning in the aged

    monkey (Macaca mulatta). Behav. Neurosci. 104, 876884.

    ah, P., Bekkers, J.M., 1996. Apical dendritic location of slow afterhyper-polarization current in hippocampal pyramidal neurons: implicationsfor the integration of long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 16, 45374542.

    ah, P., Isaacson, J.S., 1995. Channels underlying the slow afterhyperpo-larization in hippocampal pyramidal neurons: neurotransmitters mod-ulate the open probability. Neuron 15, 435441.

    ava, S., Markus, E.J., 2008. Activation of the medial septum reversesage-related hippocampal encoding deficits: a place field analysis.J. Neurosci. 28, 18411853.

    chmolesky, M.T., Wang, Y., Pu, M., Leventhal, A.G., 2000. Degradationof stimulus selectivity of visual cortical cells in senescent rhesusmonkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 384390.

    hen, J., Barnes, C.A., McNaughton, B.L., Skaggs, W.E., Weaver, K.L.,1997. The effect of aging on experience-dependent plasticity of hip-pocampal place cells. J. Neurosci. 17, 67696782.

    teere, J.C., Arnsten, A.F., 1997. The a-2A noradrenergic receptor agonistguanfacine improves visual object discrimination reversal performancein aged rhesus monkeys. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 883891.

    anila, H., Sipil, P., Shapiro, M., Eichenbaum, H., 1997. Brain aging:impaired coding of novel environmental cues. J. Neurosci. 17, 51675174.

    hibault, O., Gant, J.C., Landfield, P.W., 2007. Expansion of the calciumhypothesis of brain aging and Alzheimers disease: minding the store.Aging Cell 6, 307317.

    hibault, O., Hadley, R., Landfield, P.W., 2001. Elevated postsynaptic[Ca2]I and L-type calcium channel activity in aged hippocampalneurons: relationship to impaired synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 21,97449756.

    hibault, O., Landfield, P.W., 1996. Increase in single L-type calciumchannels in hippocampal neurons during aging. Science 272, 10171020.

    hibault, O., Porter, N.M., Chen, K.-C., Blalock, E.M., Kaminker, P.G.,Clodfelter, G.V., Brewer, L.D., Landfield, P.W., 1998. Calcium dys-regulation in neuronal aging and Alzheimers disease: history and newdirections. Trends Neurosci. 24, 417433.

    hompson, L.T., Moyer, J.R., Disterhoft, J.F., 1996. Transient changes inexcitability of rabbit CA3 neurons with a time course appropriate tosupport memory consolidation. J. Neurophysiol. 56, 103110.

    ombaugh, G.C., Rowe, W.B., Rose, G.M., 2005. The slow afterhyperpo-larization in hippocampal CA1 neurons covaries with spatial learningability in aged Fisher 344 rats. J. Neurosci. 25, 26092616.

    annucchi, M.G., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1991. Age-dependent decrease inthe affinity of muscarinic M1 receptors in neocortex of rhesus mon-keys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88, 1147511479.

    ilson, I.A., Ikonen, S., Gallagher, M., Eichenbaum, H., Tanila, H., 2005.Age-associated alterations of hippocampal place cells are subregionspecific. J. Neurosci. 25, 68776886.

    hang, J., Wang, X., Wang, Y., Fu, Y., Liang, Z., Ma, Y., Leventhal, A.G.,2008. Spatial and temporal sensitivity degradation of primary visualcortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 201

    207.

    Age-related increase of sIAHP in prefrontal pyramidal cells of monkeys: relationship to cognitionIntroductionMethodsExperimental subjectsAssessment of cognitive functionPreparation of slicesWhole-cell patch-clamp recordingsDetermination of intrinsic membrane properties and action potential firing ratesCharacterization of the slow afterhyperpolarization current sIAHPStatistical analyses

    ResultsMost but not all aged monkeys were cognitively impairedPopulation of cells examinedPassive membrane properties and action potential firing ratessIAHP amplitude is significantly increased with age in layer 3 but not layer 5 cellsThe amplitude of the sIAHP in layer 3 (but not layer 5) cells is significantly related to cognitive performance

    DiscussionAge-related increase in the amplitude of the sIAHP in layer 3 pyramidal cellsFunctional electrophysiological implications: firing rates of layer 3 pyramidal cells are not associated with sIAHP amplitudeRelationship of sIAHP amplitude to performance on cognitive tasks

    Disclosure statementAcknowledgmentsReferences