Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

download Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

of 7

Transcript of Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    1/7

    Accident Analysis and Prevention 34 (2002) 229235

    Anger and aggression among drivers in three European countriesDianne Parker a,*, Timo Lajunen b, Heikki Summala c

    a Department of Psychology, Uniersity of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UKb Department of Psychology, Middle East Technical Uniersity, Ankara, Turkey

    c Traffic Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Uniersity of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland

    Received in revised form 20 December 2000; accepted 28 December 2000

    Abstract

    Recent reports of road rage in the British media give the impression that driver aggression is escalating. In order to understandthis phenomenon we need to know what it is about driving that provokes motorists to feel anger and then to go on to express

    that anger in the form of aggression. A postal questionnaire survey of more than 2500 drivers was carried out in three European

    countries: Britain, Finland and the Netherlands. The study had three main aims: (a) to discover how angry, if at all, a range of

    situations on the road make drivers, (b) to find out how many drivers are likely to react aggressively to those situations, and (c)

    to investigate individual and/or cultural differences in terms of anger and/or aggressive responses among motorists. Results

    indicate that the same types of behaviour provoke anger and aggression in all three countries, and that traffic density may play

    a role. 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Driver anger; Driver aggression

    www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

    1. Introduction

    The media, at least in the UK, have become preoccu-

    pied in recent years with incidents of aggressive be-

    haviour occurring between ordinarily peaceful members

    of the public. There have been reports of, and specula-

    tion as to the causes of, a phenomenon that has been

    labelled trolley rage, and which involves aggression

    between supermarket shoppers. We have even been told

    about gym rage and office rage incidents in which

    people are prepared to use force against others to exact

    revenge or express their anger. However, by far the

    greatest level of attention has been paid to so-calledroad rage incidents in which drivers become so en-

    raged by the behaviour of another road user that they

    explode with rage and an ugly scene ensues. Sensa-

    tionalist reports of this phenomenon occur with alarm-

    ing regularity in the British media, and some surveys

    have reported worryingly high levels of aggressive driv-

    ing (Lex Report on Motoring, 1996; Sample Surveys

    Limited, 1996). In some quarters the problem of highly

    aggressive drivers is taken for granted and the focus is

    on ways in which the behaviour of such individualsmight be managed or remediated (Lowenstein, 1997;Deffenbacher et al., 2000). However, others have ques-tioned whether road rage actually exists as a distinc-tive phenomenon, and believe the term should bedropped in favour of something less emotive, e.g. driveraggression (Ward et al., 1998)

    One interesting question is what sort of behaviour onthe part of a driver is taken to be aggressive by others.One survey asked exactly this and reported that themanoeuvre most commonly felt to be aggressive wasclose following/tailgating, which 62% of the 529 mo-

    torists sampled reported having experienced in the pre-vious year (Joint, 1995). Headlight flashing and the useof obscene gestures were also seen as aggressive bymost, and had been experienced recently by 59% and48% of those sampled, respectively. It seems then, atleast on the roads of the UK, that drivers are regularlyinterpreting the behaviour of others as aggressive. In-deed, 60% admitted that they themselves had lost theirtemper while driving at least on occasion (Joint, 1995).Driver anger emerged as even more prevalent in a diarystudy reported by Underwood et al. (1999), with 85% ofthe 100 drivers taking part reporting having experi-enced anger while driving during a 2-week period.

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-161-2752570; fax: +44-161-

    275 2588.E-mail address: [email protected] (D. Parker).

    0001-4575/02/$ - see front matter 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    PII: S 0 0 0 1 - 4 5 7 5 ( 0 1 ) 0 0 0 1 8 - 5

    mailto:[email protected]
  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    2/7

    D. Parker et al./Accident Analysis and Preention 34 (2002) 229235230

    A preliminary investigation of the factors that give

    rise to such aggressive driving was reported by Lajunen

    et al. (1999). A self-report questionnaire, completed by

    a volunteer sample of 270 British drivers, included a

    measure of driving anger adapted from a scale origi-

    nally developed by Deffenbacher et al. (1994) in the

    USA. This scale covered a range of driving situations

    that might provoke anger, and required respondents to

    indicate how angry they felt they would get if theyfound themselves in that situation. Factor analysis of

    the 27-item scale indicated that the situations could be

    grouped in terms of three underlying factors. These

    related to anger at having your progress impeded, anger

    at the reckless driving of another, and anger provoked

    by the direct hostility of another driver. Younger driv-

    ers, and those driving a relatively low exposure had

    relatively high scores on all three factors, but there were

    no gender differences in scale scores. It was also found

    that there were significant zero-order correlations be-

    tween scores on the three factor scales and the commis-

    sion of driving violations, as measured by the 28-itemextended Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire

    (Lawton et al., 1997) which includes a measure of

    drivers own self-reported violating behaviour (with the

    exception of the correlation between anger at anothers

    reckless driving and the ordinary violations sub-scale,

    which was non-significant at 0.07). Further analysis

    revealed that the associations of anger at progress

    impeded and anger at direct hostility with self-reported

    violations were both mediated by scores on the safety-

    skills sub-scale of the Driver Skill Inventory (DSI) of

    Lajunen and Summala (1995). The safety-skills sub-scale consists of 16 items requiring ratings of a range of

    safety skills in driving, so that those who obtain a high

    score rate themselves as safety minded. The relationship

    between anger at impeded progress, or at the direct

    hostility of another driver and the commission of driv-

    ing violations was reduced in those with a relatively

    high level of reported safety skills.

    The study of Lajunen et al. (1997) went some way

    towards establishing the factors that provoke anger in

    British drivers. However the study had both practical

    and methodological limitations. First, although respon-

    dents indicated how angry a range of driving situationsmade them, no information was provided as to whether

    that anger caused them to react in any way. Second, the

    respondents had all responded to a media appeal for

    volunteers, and so may have had a particular interest in

    the issue of aggressive driving. The study reported here

    enlarges and improves upon the one reported by La-

    junen et al. (1997) in three ways. First it uses a sample

    of drivers who have not volunteered specifically to take

    part in research on road rage. Second it extends the

    application of the Driver Anger Questionnaire by as-

    sessing the relationship between level of anger pro-

    voked by each driving situation and likelihood of an

    overt aggressive reaction. Third, it allows for the inves-

    tigation of cultural differences in driving anger and

    aggression, by covering drivers in three European coun-

    tries: the UK, Finland and the Netherlands.

    2. Method

    The questionnaire consisted of several sections, onlysome of which will be reported here. In the first section,

    demographic information about the respondent was

    collected. This included age, gender, annual exposure,

    accident and traffic offence history and preferred speed

    on motorways and residential roads. The DBQ section

    listed 28 driving behaviours, including eight lapses,

    eight errors, and 12 violations, of which six were ordi-

    naryviolations and six interpersonally aggressive viola-

    tions. With respect to each item respondents were

    required to indicate How often, if at all, this kind of

    thing has happened to you, using a 6-point frequency

    scale where 0=Never, 1=Hardly ever, 2=Occasion-ally, 3=Quite often, 4=Frequently and 5=Nearly all

    the time.

    The DAQ section listed 22 potentially anger-provok-

    ing situations, and asked respondents to indicate How

    much each of these situations would make you angry

    using a 5-point response scale where 0=Not at all

    angry, 1=A little angry, 2=Fairly angry, 3=Very

    angry and 4=Extremely angry. For each of the listed

    situations they were also asked to indicate their most

    likely reaction to the situation, using an 8-point scale

    labelled as follows: 0=No reaction, 1=Try to escapefrom the situation, 2=Beep horn and/or flash lights,

    3=Gesture at the other road user, 4=Swear at and/or

    verbally abuse the other road user, 5=Drive close

    to/follow the other road user in order to teach him/her

    a lesson, 6=Stop your vehicle and get out, ready to

    argue, and 7=Get out of your car, prepared to engage

    physically with the other road user.

    The data were collected via postal surveys. The sam-

    pling technique was similar, although not identical in

    the three countries involved. In the UK a sample of

    2000 adults was taken from the electoral register. In the

    Netherlands 2000 names were selected from the registerof telephone users, and in Finland, the names and

    addresses of 2000 holders of full current driving licenses

    were obtained from the Finnish register of car owners.

    Care was taken to ensure that the samples contacted

    were representative of the whole geographical area of

    the countries involved.

    The survey questionnaire was posted out to those

    sampled, together with a covering letter explaining the

    research and a Freepost return envelope. Confidential-

    ity was ensured, and respondents were not asked to

    state their names or addresses. The questionnaires were

    each given a code in order to assess who had responded

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    3/7

    D. Parker et al./Accident Analysis and Preention 34 (2002) 229235 231

    and 3 weeks later, those who had not responded were

    sent a reminder, a further copy of the questionnaire and

    another return envelope. A total of 2613 completed

    questionnaires were returned. Of those 831 were col-

    lected from the UK, 703 from the Netherlands and

    1123 from Finland, response rates of 42, 35 and 56%,

    respectively. The differential response rates probably

    occurred because in the UK or the Netherlands it was

    not possible to access a sampling frame consisting onlyof drivers. Therefore, some of those receiving the ques-

    tionnaire would not have been eligible, as licence hold-

    ers, to take part in the survey. This means that the true

    response rate, calculated on the basis of responses from

    those actually eligible to take part, was undoubtedly

    higher.

    3. Results

    3.1. Characteristics of the sample

    The characteristics of the sample are shown in Table

    1. In all three countries there were roughly equal num-

    bers of males and females, and a good spread of ages.

    The annual exposure of the UK sample, at 18 612 km

    per year was substantially less than that of the Finnish

    and Dutch samples, both of which had an average

    annual exposure of over 20 000 km. The driving experi-

    ence of those in the three samples ranged from under 1

    year to over 50 years, with an average of around 20

    years experience. Respondents were also asked how

    many accidents they had been involved in as a driverduring the previous 3 years, including accidents in

    which there was damage to one or more vehicles but no

    injuries. This is a wider definition of an accident than

    the one used in official statistics which typically ex-

    cludes property-damage only accidents. Therefore, the

    percentages of respondents classified as accident in-

    volved are relatively high, ranging from 22.5% for the

    Finnish sample to 31.3% for the UK sample. The

    Dutch sample differed in some important respects from

    the Finnish and UK samples, in that the Dutch were

    older and the sample contained a higher proportion of

    males. In subsequent analyses comparing the three sam-ples this was dealt with by including age, sex and

    driving exposure as covariates, so that their effects

    could be separated out from the cultural effects of

    interest.

    Initial analyses considered the three samples together,

    giving an overall sample size of 2657, of whom 1123

    were Finnish, 831 were from the UK and 703 were

    Dutch. Table 2 shows the mean scores of the whole

    sample on each of items in the level of anger scale in

    the Driver Anger Questionnaire. The occurrence thatprovoked the most anger, among drivers in all three

    countries considered together, was when another driver

    cuts in and takes a parking spot you have been waiting

    for. A mean score of 2.20 indicates that on average

    drivers get fairly angry when this happens. The distri-

    butional data showed that only 6% reported that this

    would not make them angry at all, while 39.6% would

    get very angry or extremely angry. Following this, most

    anger was generated by having someone drive behind

    you at night with bright lights on, having someone

    drive very close to your rear bumper and having some-

    one speed up as you try to pass them. The least angerwas provoked by being held up by a driver who was

    slow to park, or someone slow to move off from traffic

    lights. Only 2.5 and 2.4%, respectively, of drivers report

    that these behaviours would make them very angry or

    extremely angry.

    Factor analysis was used to reduce the data to cate-

    gories, a procedure that resulted in five categories. The

    category that each item relates to is shown in the

    left-hand column of Table 2. Scale scores were calcu-

    lated for each category by summing the individual item

    scores and taking their mean. The categories werelabelled impeded progress, fast, reckless driing, direct

    hostility, inconsiderate driing, and impatient driing.

    The alpha reliabilities for these scales fell in the range

    0.730.89 and therefore are sufficiently homogeneous

    to be treated as categories. Multivariate analysis of

    variance was then used to investigate possible differ-

    ences in scores on each of the five scales between

    Finnish, UK and Dutch drivers. Gender, age and expo-

    sure were used as covariates as it was thought likely

    that these variables might account for some differences

    in levels of anger.

    Table 3 shows the mean scores and standard devia-tions, of drivers from each country, on each of the

    Table 1

    Characteristics of the sample

    Finland UK Holland

    Males 516 429 499

    607Females 402 204

    20 435 (1300 000) 24 637 (1500 000)Mean annual exposure in km (range) 18 612 (1241 000)

    45.90 (1887)Mean age (range) 37.52 (1879) 39.55 (1780)

    23.62 (164)Mean driving experience in years (range) 18.35 (160)16.93 (152)

    31.3People involved in an accident in the previous 3 years (%) 28.222.5

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    4/7

    D. Parker et al./Accident Analysis and Preention 34 (2002) 229235232

    Table 2

    Whole sample mean scores on the level of anger section of the Driver

    Anger Questionnairea

    Category Mean (S.D.)Item

    2.20 (1.15)Someone cuts in and takes the5

    parking spot you have been waiting

    for

    4 At night someone is driving behind 1.87 (1.06)

    you with bright lights onSomeone is driving very close to5 1.84 (1.12)

    your rear bumper

    Someone speeds up when you try4 1.81 (1.11)

    to pass them

    5 Someone cuts right in front of you 1.75 (1.09)

    on the motorway

    Someone coming towards you does4 1.70 (1.02)

    not dim their headlights at night

    A slow vehicle on a winding road 1.48 (1.07)1

    will not pull over and let people

    pass

    3 Someone shouts at you about your 1.40 (1.18)

    driving

    1 Someone is driving too slowly in 1.34 (0.93)the outside lane, and holding up

    the traffic

    3 Someone makes an obscene gesture 1.29 (1.15)

    towards you about your driving

    A cyclist is riding in the middle ofX 1.25 (0.99)

    the lane and slowing traffic

    Someone is weaving in and out ofX 1.21 (1.10)

    traffic

    Someone pulls out right in front ofX 1.20 (0.98)

    you when there is no-one behind

    you

    1 Someone is driving more slowly 1.18 (0.97)

    than is reasonable for the traffic

    flow2 Someone runs a red light or stop 1.16 (1.06)

    sign

    2 Someone is driving too fast for the 1.08 (0.96)

    road conditions

    3 Someone beeps at you about your 1.06 (1.04)

    driving

    Someone is driving well above the2 0.88 (1.07)

    speed limit

    Someone in front of you does not1 0.70 (0.75)

    move off straight away when the

    light turns to green

    1 0.64 (0.75)A pedestrian walks slowly across

    the middle of the street, slowing

    you down

    Someone is slow in parking and1 0.53 (0.75)

    holds up traffic

    a Items showing an X in the left-hand column, did not emerge from

    factor analysis as related to any of the five categories of behaviour

    identified.

    evoked by having their progress impeded by anothers

    slow driving, and by direct hostility from other drivers.

    Both were significantly more angered than Dutch driv-

    ers. There were significant differences among all three

    countries in terms of the level of anger generated by

    reckless driving, inconsiderate driving and impatient

    driving. In the case of reckless driving, the UK sample

    were most angered, followed by the Finnish sample and

    then the Dutch sample. Finnish drivers were signifi-cantly more angered than UK drivers by inconsiderate

    driving, who in turn were significantly more angered

    than Dutch drivers. In relation to impatient driving, the

    Finnish sample were again the most angered, followed

    this time by the Dutch drivers and then the UK drivers.

    The type of driving that elicited most anger among

    the Finns was inconsiderate driving, the scale that

    included items related to being dazzled by anothers

    headlights. Both UK and Dutch drivers were most

    angered by impatient driving. Across all three countries

    the type of driving that gave rise to least anger was fast

    or reckless driving. The Dutch drivers were least an-

    gered of the three countries in relation to four of the

    five types of driving considered. The exception being

    impatient driving, which elicited least anger among

    Finnish drivers.

    The multivariate significance tests showed that there

    were also differences between men and women in rela-

    tion to level of anger elicited by four of the five types of

    driving, the exception being inconsiderate driving, and

    that there were also effects of age in relation to the

    anger evoked by all five types of driving. However,

    there were no effects of exposure on anger. Analysis ofvariance indicated that men got more angry than

    women about impeded progress, but that women got

    more angered than men about fast, reckless driving,

    direct hostility and impatient driving. Considering age

    in three categories (1730, 3145, 46 and over), it was

    Table 3

    Level of driving anger by country, with age, sex and exposure as

    covariatesa

    DutchUKFinnishCategory Fvalue

    label

    1.00a (0.61)Progress 0.81b (0.51)1.06a (0.70) 21.68b

    impeded

    1.19b (0.85)Reckless 0.84c (0.70)1.06a (0.87) 25.03b

    driving

    Direct 1.44a (1.09) 1.34a (0.97) 0.82b (0.81) 42.83b

    hostility

    Inconsiderate 1.98a (0.96) 1.72b (0.86) 1.56c (0.84) 30.22b

    driving

    1.93c (0.86)2.09b (0.89) 34.28b1.82a (0.93)Impatient

    driving

    a Across rows, means that do not share a superscript differ signifi-

    cantly.b P0.001.

    five level of anger scales. There were significant differ-

    ences on all five. The multivariate significance tests

    showed that there were significant effects for all three

    covariates, and for country. There was no significant

    difference between Finnish and UK drivers in the anger

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    5/7

    D. Parker et al./Accident Analysis and Preention 34 (2002) 229235 233

    Table 4

    The Driver Reaction Scale: mean scores (and S.D.s) and percentage who would not react at alla

    Category Mean (S.D.) Would not react at

    all (%)

    1 0.29 (0.75)Someone is slow in parking and holds up traffic 81.1

    0.39 (0.82)2 74.7Someone is driving well above the speed limit

    0.55 (0.93)A pedestrian walks slowly across the middle of the street, slowing you down 68.71

    Someone runs a red light or stop sign2 0.60 (0.97) 65.9

    0.56 (0.96)X 63.4Someone is weaving in and out of traffic0.57 (0.92)Someone is driving too fast for the road conditions 61.52

    Someone beeps at you about your driving3 0.93 (1.27) 53.6

    Someone is driving more slowly than is reasonable for the traffic flow1 0.85 (1.21) 52.3

    0.97 (1.05)Someone in front of you does not move off straight away when the light turns to green 50.41

    Someone makes an obscene gesture towards you about your driving3 1.24 (1.49) 48.4

    0.92 (1.14)X 47.6Someone pulls out right in front of you when there is no-one behind you

    1.36 (1.68)Someone shouts at you about your driving 43.83

    A slow vehicle on a winding road will not pull over and let people pass1 1.10 (1.27) 43.6

    Someone is driving too slowly in the outside lane, and holding up the traffic1 1.17 (1.29) 40.5

    1.22 (1.11)A cyclist is riding in the middle of the lane and slowing traffic 34.0X

    Someone is driving very close to your rear bumper5 1.23 (1.29) 33.0

    Someone cuts right in front of you on the motorway5 1.42 (1.29) 30.2

    1.35 (1.33)Someone speeds up when you try to pass them 28.94

    2.27 (2.02)5 25.7Someone cuts in and takes the parking spot you have been waiting for1.47 (1.21)At night someone is driving behind you with bright lights on 23.54

    1.68 (0.85) 15.14 Someone coming towards you does not dim their headlights at night

    a Items showing an X in the left-hand column, did not emerge from factor analysis as important to any of the five categories of behaviour

    identified.

    found that there were linear decreases in anger at

    impeded progress, at direct hostility, at inconsiderate

    driving and at impatient driving with age. However, the

    opposite was true of anger at fast, reckless driving which

    increased with age.

    Table 4 shows the mean scores and S.D.s, across theentire sample, on each of the items in the Driver Reaction

    Scale section of the questionnaire.

    The behaviour that elicited the greatest reaction, on

    average, was when someone coming towards you does

    not dip their headlights. As shown in Table 5, only 15%

    of those sampled said they would not react at all to this

    happening. On the other hand, over 80% said they would

    not react at all if a slow driver held up traffic while trying

    to park. An overview of reactions to the behaviours

    related to each of the five categories of behaviour

    suggests that fast, reckless driving attracts the fewest

    reactions, while inconsiderate driving attracts the most.Multivariate analyses of variance were carried out to

    investigate possible cultural and demographic differences

    in reactions. In relation to each of the items of the Driver

    Reaction Scale a fair proportion of respondents indicated

    that they would not respond at all (see Table 4). In order

    to get a clearer picture of the factors that influence

    reactions among those who do react, the Manova anal-

    ysis was restricted to those in the sample indicating some

    sort of reaction. This reduced the total sample size to

    1284. Age, sex and country were used as independent

    variables, and scores on the five reaction scales as

    dependent variables.

    There were significant main effects of country on three

    of thefive scales. These were reaction to direct hostility,

    reaction to inconsiderate driving and reaction to impa-

    tient driving. The mean scores from all three countries

    on each of the five reaction scales are shown in Table 5.

    Inspection of the means shows that Finnish and UKdrivers reacted in a broadly similar way, and significantly

    more than Dutch drivers to the direct hostility of another

    motorist. In relation to inconsiderate driving, Finnish

    drivers reacted significantly more than Dutch

    Table 5

    Level of reaction to anger by country (excluding non-reactors)a

    Finnish UKCategory Dutch Fvalue

    label

    1.27a (0.79) 1.22a (0.59)Progress 1.18a (0.67) 1.39

    impeded

    0.690.70a (0.75)Reckless 0.78a (0.65) 0.79a (0.62)

    driving

    2.13a (1.36) 1.96a (1.24) 1.37b (1.25)Direct 22.69b

    hostility

    21.23b2.14a (0.86)Inconsiderate 1.77b (0.80) 1.93c (0.83)

    driving

    47.32b2.14a (1.09)Impatient 2.37b (0.96) 2.72c (1.05)

    driving

    a Across rows, means that do not share a superscript differ signifi-

    cantly.b P0.001.

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    6/7

  • 8/13/2019 Anger and Aggression Among Drivers

    7/7

    D. Parker et al./Accident Analysis and Preention 34 (2002) 229235 235

    the type most likely to prompt a reaction, while reckless

    driving was the type least likely to. It could be argued

    that this latterfinding reflects the fact that many drivers

    feel that there is nothing they can do that might affect

    a driver who is already taking dangerous risks with

    speed. On the other hand, the level of anger provoked

    by reckless driving in all the samples is relatively low,

    suggesting that behaviours such as speeding and run-

    ning traffic lights are simply not viewed especially nega-tively by most drivers, and that is why they do not lead

    to much in the way of reaction. If this explanation is

    correct, it is very worrying, because speed is known to

    be a risk factor in the majority of road traffic accidents

    (e.g. Maycock (1997)).

    The finding that males react significantly more to

    three of the five sources of anger than women do is not

    a surprise. If anything, we might have expected this

    difference to emerge across all five categories of anger-

    provoking behaviour. However, there are some interest-

    ing age-related patterns in reactions. Across the whole

    data set, older drivers were less likely to react to the

    direct hostility of others, or to the inconsiderate or

    impatient driving of others. This accords with a view of

    the individual mellowing with age and becoming less

    inclined to want to set the world to rights. However,

    the pattern of mean scores in reaction to the reckless

    driving of others shows the opposite pattern. In this

    instance older drivers were more likely to react than

    their younger counterparts, perhaps by virtue of their

    experience realising that the type of driving included in

    the reckless driving sub-scale poses more of an actual

    threat to road users than some of the other types, whichmight be more irritating but are probably not going to

    cause an accident directly. This ties in with the finding

    of Underwood et al. (1999) that reports of near acci-

    dents were associated with the frequency with which

    anger is experienced. In their study near accidents were

    frequently found to provoke anger, especially where the

    driver felt that the incident was not their fault. The

    reckless driving category in the present study includes

    behaviour relatively likely to put other road users in

    danger, leading to traffic conflicts and perhaps to near

    misses and accidents.

    The overall conclusions to be drawn from this study

    are that the level of anger scale of the Driver Anger

    Questionnaire includes five categories of driving be-

    haviour across three European driving cultures, and

    that both the level of anger provoked by these types of

    behaviour and the reaction to them, measured in the

    Driver Reaction Scale items, are broadly similar across

    those cultures. Where differences exist tentative expla-

    nations in terms of the traffic situation prevailing in

    each particular country may be suggested. In general,

    many more similarities than differences emerged, and in

    all three countries, the behaviours most likely to pro-

    voke anger were also those most likely to give rise to a

    reaction. It could well be beneficial to alert drivers to

    the behaviours most likely to cause another motorist to

    become angry, and to provoke a reaction.

    Acknowledgements

    This research was supported by a grant from the

    European Commission (Marie Curie Fellowship con-

    tract no. ERBFMBICT972398). The authors wish to

    acknowledge the contribution of Jolieke Mesken, who

    translated the questionnaire into Dutch and also helped

    with the collection of data in the Netherlands. The

    comments of three anonymous referees, and especially

    those of Jim McKnight, were extremely helpful.

    References

    Deffenbacher, J.L., Oetting, E.R., Lynch, R.S., 1994. Development of

    a driving anger scale. Psychological Reports 71, 8391.

    Deffenbacher, J.L., Huff, M.E., Lynch, R.S., Oetting, E.R., Salva-

    tore, N.F., 2000. characteristics and treatment of high-anger

    drivers. Journal of Counselling Psychology 47, 517.

    Joint, M., 1995. Road Rage. Automobile Association, London.

    Lajunen, T., Summala, H., 1995. Driving experience, personality and

    skill- and safety-motive dimensions in drivers self-assessments.

    Personality and Individual Differences 19, 307318.

    Lajunen, T., Parker, D., Stradling, S.G., 1997. Dimensions of driver

    anger, aggressive and highway code violations and their mediationby safety orientation in UK drivers. Transportation Research Part

    F 1, 107121.

    Lajunen, T., Parker, D., Summala, H., 1999. Does traffic congestion

    increase driver aggression? Transportation Research Part F 2,

    225236.

    Lawton, R.L., Parker, D., Stradling, S.G., Manstead, A.S.R., 1997.

    The role of affect in predicting social behaviors: the case of road

    traffic violations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 27, 1258

    1276.

    Lex Motor Group, 1996. Lex Report on Motoring. Lex Motor

    Group, London.

    Lowenstein, L.F., 1997. Research into causes and manifestations of

    aggression in car driving. Police Journal 70, 263270.

    Maycock, G., 1997. Speed, speed choice and accidents. In: Grayson,

    G. (Ed.), Behavioural Research in Road Safety VII. TRL,

    Crowthorne.

    Sample Surveys Limited, 1996. Road Rage: A Study of Attitudes and

    Behaviour. West Malling, Kent, Sample Surveys Limited

    (Reprinted November 1996).

    Ward, N.J., Waterman, M., Joint, M., 1998. Rage and violence of

    driver aggression. In: Grayson, G. (Ed.), Behavioural Research in

    Road Safety VIII. TRL, Crowthorne.

    Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S., Crundall, D., 1999. Anger

    while driving. Transportation Research Part F 2, 5568.