Adaptation to Peripheral Flicker - University of … to Peripheral Flicker STUART ANSTIS Received...

7
Pergamon PII: visionk’s,, Vol. 36, No. 21, pp. 3479–3485, 1996 Copyright01996 Elsevier Science Ltd.Allrightsreserved S0042-6989(96)00016-8 PrintedinGreat Britain 0042-6989/96 $15,00 + 0,00 Adaptation to Peripheral Flicker STUART ANSTIS Received 9 August 1995; in revisedform 29 December 1995 With strict fixation, a flickering disk presented in the peripheral retina rapidly appeared to lose contrast and stop flickering, owing to adaptation. Subjects measured this adaptation by continually adjusting the flicker amplitude of a peripherally viewed disk to hold itjust at threshold. Results: (1) The contrast threshold for flicker increased logarithmically over time. (2) The slope of the temporal decay function increased with eccentricity (1–16 deg) and with decreasing disk size (8 deg- 3.6 min arc). (3) M-scaling the stimulus size could abolish the dependence upon eccentricity for small disks, but not completely for large disks. (4) The temporal decay rate increased with flicker rate (3-15 Hz), as though each cycle of flicker elevated contrast threshold equally. Copyright 0 1996 Elsevier Science Ltd. Adaptation Flicker Corticalmagnification factor Peripheral vision INTRODUCTION If a novel target pops up in the peripheralvisual field, an observer normally makes a saccade to bring the target into the fovea. As a result of this efficient saccadic fixation reflex, we rarely let an object dwell at a fixed location in the peripheral retina. When this is done in the laboratory such an object tends to fade from view rather rapidly. This subjective fading was first noted for stationary objects by Troxler (1804). However, even moving or twinkling objects are apt to fade. This makes sense ecologically because such novel, attention-getting peripheral targets provoke a fixation reflex that promptly removes them from the periphery to the fovea, so they seldom dwell in the periphery long to need much long-term or sustained processing.We shall briefly review studies of subjective fading of flickering peripheral stimuli and then describe our own experi- ments. Peripheral flicker There have been at least four published investigations of peripherally viewed flicker. Frome et al. (1981) showed that threshold measurements of flashed periph- eral test spots were affected by continuingpresentation. With repeated presentations of a 50 msec flash every 0.5 see, thresholds rose progressively,sometimes reach- ing more than ten times their initial values. This loss in sensitivitywas not simply due to retinal light adaptation. With 15 min of rapid flash presentation there was an Department of Psychology, UC, San Diego 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109,U.S.A. Tel: (619)-534-5456;Fax: (619)- 53:4-7190; Emai[: [email protected]. initial sharp recovery in sensitivity after which there remained some loss of sensitivity even after 35 min. Habituation occurred within both rod and cone systems and transferred between them. The effect was specific to the size and spatial frequency but not the orientation of the habituating stimulus. Schieting and Spillman (1987) studied flicker adapta- tion in the peripheralretina, as we have done. They noted that with strict fixation,a small flickeringspot presented in the peripheral retina rapidly appeared to lose contrast and stop flickering within 35 see, before fading away completely.The time requiredfor this adaptationto occur decreased with: 1. decreasing depth of modulation (97–9%); 2. decreasing stimulus diameter (2 deg–7 min arc); 3. increasing retinal eccentricity (20-50 deg); and 4. increasing flicker frequency (l–7 Hz). Adaptation was twice as fast in the temporal as in the nasal retina. When changes in retinal eccentricity were compensated for by taking into account the cortical magnification factor, the time needed for perceived flicker to disappear remained constant at all eccentri- cities.With dichopticstimulationinteroculartransferwas about 3570,suggesting a cortical contribution to flicker adaptation. Harris et al. (1990) confirmed that time to disappearance became shorter at higher temporal fre- quencies, but they enquired whether this was a true frequency dependenceor whether it reflectedthe amount above threshold of the adapting flicker, since threshold contrast varies with frequency. They measured time to disappearance at contrasts which were multiples of the contrast threshold or were matched across frequencies. They found that near thresholdall frequenciesadapted at 3479

Transcript of Adaptation to Peripheral Flicker - University of … to Peripheral Flicker STUART ANSTIS Received...

PergamonPII:

visionk’s,, Vol. 36, No. 21, pp. 3479–3485, 1996Copyright01996 ElsevierScienceLtd.Allrightsreserved

S0042-6989(96)00016-8 Printedin GreatBritain0042-6989/96 $15,00 + 0,00

Adaptation to Peripheral FlickerSTUART ANSTIS

Received 9 August1995; in revisedform 29 December 1995

With strict fixation, a flickering disk presented in the peripheral retina rapidly appeared to losecontrast and stop flickering, owing to adaptation. Subjects measured this adaptation by continuallyadjusting the flicker amplitude of a peripherally viewed disk to hold itjust at threshold. Results: (1)The contrast threshold for flicker increased logarithmically over time. (2) The slope of the temporaldecay function increased with eccentricity (1–16 deg) and with decreasing disk size (8 deg-3.6 min arc). (3) M-scaling the stimulus size could abolish the dependence upon eccentricity forsmall disks, but not completely for large disks. (4) The temporal decay rate increased with flickerrate (3-15 Hz), as though each cycle of flicker elevated contrast threshold equally. Copyright 01996 Elsevier Science Ltd.

Adaptation Flicker Corticalmagnificationfactor Peripheralvision

INTRODUCTION

If a novel target pops up in the peripheralvisual field, anobserver normally makes a saccade to bring the targetinto the fovea. As a result of this efficient saccadicfixation reflex, we rarely let an object dwell at a fixedlocation in the peripheral retina. When this is done in thelaboratory such an object tends to fade from view ratherrapidly. This subjective fading was first noted forstationary objects by Troxler (1804). However, evenmoving or twinkling objects are apt to fade.

This makes sense ecologically because such novel,attention-getting peripheral targets provoke a fixationreflex that promptly removes them from the periphery tothe fovea, so they seldom dwell in the periphery long toneed much long-term or sustained processing.We shallbriefly review studies of subjective fading of flickeringperipheral stimuli and then describe our own experi-ments.

Peripheral flicker

There have been at least four published investigationsof peripherally viewed flicker. Frome et al. (1981)showed that threshold measurements of flashed periph-eral test spots were affected by continuingpresentation.With repeated presentations of a 50 msec flash every0.5 see, thresholds rose progressively,sometimes reach-ing more than ten times their initial values. This loss insensitivitywas not simply due to retinal light adaptation.With 15 min of rapid flash presentation there was an

Department of Psychology, UC, San Diego 9500 Gilman Drive, LaJolla, CA 92093-0109,U.S.A. Tel: (619)-534-5456;Fax: (619)-53:4-7190;Emai[: [email protected].

initial sharp recovery in sensitivity after which thereremained some loss of sensitivity even after 35 min.Habituation occurred within both rod and cone systemsand transferredbetween them. The effect was specifictothe size and spatial frequency but not the orientation ofthe habituatingstimulus.

Schieting and Spillman (1987) studied flicker adapta-tion in the peripheralretina, as we have done.They notedthat with strict fixation,a small flickeringspot presentedin the peripheral retina rapidly appeared to lose contrastand stop flickering within 35 see, before fading awaycompletely.The time requiredfor this adaptationto occurdecreased with:

1. decreasing depth of modulation(97–9%);2. decreasing stimulusdiameter (2 deg–7 min arc);3. increasingretinal eccentricity (20-50 deg); and4. increasingflicker frequency (l–7 Hz).

Adaptation was twice as fast in the temporal as in thenasal retina. When changes in retinal eccentricity werecompensated for by taking into account the corticalmagnification factor, the time needed for perceivedflicker to disappear remained constant at all eccentri-cities.With dichopticstimulationinteroculartransferwasabout 3570,suggesting a cortical contribution to flickeradaptation. Harris et al. (1990) confirmed that time todisappearance became shorter at higher temporal fre-quencies, but they enquired whether this was a truefrequencydependenceor whether it reflectedthe amountabove threshold of the adapting flicker, since thresholdcontrast varies with frequency. They measured time todisappearance at contrasts which were multiples of thecontrast threshold or were matched across frequencies.They found that near thresholdall frequenciesadapted at

3479

3480 S. ANSTIS

similar rates. However, at small multiples of thresholdhigher temporal frequencies had faster adaptation rates,confirmingSchietingand Spillman.They concludedthathigher temporal frequencies really are more adaptablethan low ones. Hammett and Smith (1990) measuredadaptation to counterphaseflickeringgratings instead ofto flickeringspots.They found that as temporalfrequencywas raised, adaptation time decreased in conditions ofconstant physical modulation depth but increased inconditionsof constantperceived modulationdepth.Theyconcluded that while adaptationtime was clearly relatedto modulation depth, its relation to temporal frequencywas ambiguous.

It is often forgotten that all visual patterns ultimatelyfade when the retinal image is stabilized (Sharpe, 1972).The involuntaryeye movementspreventthis stabilizationeffect, but their range (and other parameters) areoptimized for central vision and for seeing fine detail.People whose acuity is degraded in later life by maculardegenerationdevelopdifferenteye movementsto preventan equivalent of the “Troxler effect”. Thus they fixatenonfoveally, and the spatial precision of their eyemovementsis scaled to the eccentricityof their preferredfixationarea (White & Bedell, 1990).So in practice, it iseasy to show that the “peripheral events” fade rapidly,but when the secondary effects caused by “fading” areprevented by suitable experimentalcontrols, the rules ofpattern and movementdetectionare similar;Murrayet al.(1983) found that the spatial resolution for detecting apattern (P) was twice as fine as for its motion(M), as onewould expect from the Reichardt (1961) model ofmovement detection in which two adjacent outputs arerequired to signal movement. This P:M ratio wasconsistently 2 in central vision and for a wide range ofeccentricities.

Cortical magnificationfactorIt is not obvious whether peripheral disks rapidly

became invisible because flicker sensitivity is worse inthe periphery,or whether because the cortical representa-tion for a disk of fixed size falls off rapidly witheccentricity. This might make a more eccentric diskeffectively smaller so far as the visual system wasconcerned, since there would be fewer cortical neuronsavailable to analyze its properties.Many forms of visualsensitivity functions decrease monotonically with in-creasing eccentricity when measured with the samestimuli at different retinal positions. But these tasksbecome independent of visual field location when thedecrease in the density of retinal ganglion cells and theincrease in their receptive-field size toward the retinalperiphery are compensated for by increasing stimulusarea in inverse proportion to the human corticalmagnification factor squared (M-scaling). When thestimuli are normalized in size in this way so that theircalculated cortical representationsbecome equivalent atdifferent eccentricities, the visual sensitivity functionsbecome similar at all eccentricities (Pointer, 1986;

Watson, 1987).This normalizationhas proved effectivefor tasks that include:

It

acuity for letters (Anstis, 1974);vernier acuity (Levi, Klein & Aitsebaomo, 1985);judgments of visual numerosity (Parth & Ren-tschler, 1984);wavelength discrimination(Van Esch et al., 1984);binocular rivalry (Blake et al., 1992)motion and displacement thresholds for oscillatinggratings (Johnston & Wright, 1985; Wright &Johnston, 1985); and various kinds of temporalmodulation from O to 25 Hz (movement, counter-phase flicker, and on-off flicker) and differentthreshold tasks (detection, orientation discrimina-tion, and discrimination of movement direction),independentlyof the subjective appearances of thegratings at threshold (Virsu et al., 1982). Thestimulus gratings were also normalized in area,spatial frequency, and translationvelocity;photopic critical flicker frequency (CFF) (Rovamo& Raninen, 1984: Raninen & Rovamo, 1986). Itwas also necessary to reduce stimulusluminance ininverse proportion to Ricco’s area (F-scaling).

is true that some deviationsfrom perfect M-scalinghave been reported for thresholds for grating motionthresholds(Wesemann& Norcia, 1992),letter identifica-tion (Strasburgeret al., 1991) and visibility of gaussianblurred circular disks (Bijl et al., 1992), in some acuitytasks (Virsu et al., 1987),and in phase discriminationforf + 3f compound gratings (Stephenson et al., 1991).Overall, however, the evidence shows that central andperipheral vision are qualitatively similar in spatiotem-poral visual performance. The quantitative differencesobserved without normalization were caused by thespatial sampling properties of retinal ganglion cells thatare directly related to the values of M used in thenormalization.

In this paper, we measured adaptation to a disk thatflickeredat low amplitude in square-waveat 5 Hz in theretinalperiphery.To anticipate,we found that such a diskwould rapidly fade from view if left alone, and to keep itvisible the observer had to increase the amplitude of itsflicker logarithmically over time. We also found thatsmaller or more peripheral spots faded most rapidly, sowe examined the relationship between size and eccen-tricity as affected by the cortical magnificationfactor.

METHODS

The stimulus was a small flickering disk on a monitorscreen, with the flicker amplitude under the subject’scontrol. The disk, which appeared on a computer-controlled monitor on a white background, was posi-tioned vertically above the fixation spot (to avoid theblind spot) at an eccentricity of 1,2,4,8, or 16 deg. Thespot initiallyalternatedbetween thewhite of the surroundand a lightgreywhich was 270darker.The flickeringspotwas alwayseither the same as or darker than the surround(a spatial decrement). During binocular viewing with

ADAPTATIONTO PERIPHERALFLICKER 3481

TABLE 1. Disk diameters in three experiments

Retinal eccentricity 10 20 4 “ 80 16“1. Constant-sizedisks 0.50 0.50 0.50 0.50 0.502. Large M-scaled disks 0.50 10 20 40 8“3. Small M-scaled disks — 0.060 0.12 “ 0.25 “ 0.50

strict fixation,subjectsreported that at firstthey couldseethe flicker but this faded out after a few secondsand thedisk became invisible. They were provided with twocomputer keys which increased or decreased the flickeramplitude, and they adjusted these over time to keep theflicker just visible. This process continued for 80 sec.Data were recorded and analyzed off-line later. Threeruns were taken in random order at each of the fiveeccentricities. Results were collected from four under-graduate subjects who were naive to the purpose of theexperiment.

The disk size was varied in three conditions,as shownin Table 1.

In Condition 1 (Constant-size disks) the diskdiameter was 0.5 deg at all eccentricities.In Condition 2 (Large M-scaled diameter) the diskdiameter was 0.5 deg at an eccentricity of 1 deg, asin condition 1, but it was expanded with increasingeccentricity to counteract the cortical magnificationfactor, reaching a diameter of 8 deg at an eccen-tricity of 16 deg. So the disk areas increasedaccording to M-scaling squared.In Condition 3 (Small M-scaled diameter) the diskdiameterwas 0.5 deg at an eccentricityof 16 deg, as

<co

in condition 1, but it was reduced with decreasingeccentricityto counteract the cortical magnificationfactor, reachinga diameterof 0.06 deg (3.6 min arc)at an eccentricityof 2 deg. This was the same ratioof disk diameter to eccentricity as Schieting andSpillman (1987) used. (Apparatus limitations pre-cluded our taking measurements at an eccentricityof 1 deg in this condition.)

● Note that the relative M-scaling ratios were thesame in conditions 2 and 3, as the disk diameterdoubled when the eccentricity doubled. However,the diameters of the disks were 16 times larger incondition 2 than in condition 3.

RESULTS

Results for Conditions 1–3 are shown in Fig. l(a)-(c)(means of four subjects x three readings). Figure 1 showsthat on every run in every condition, as time went on thesubject became progressively less sensitive to theperipheral flicker and his or her amplitude thresholdrose logarithmically with time (contrast threshold =m log t + c). The correlation coefficient r2 between thedataand the fittedlogarithmiccurveswas 0.96 or better inall cases. The steeper the time decay curves in Fig. 1, themore rapid the threshold elevation and the worse theflicker sensitivity.

In Condition1, when the diskshad a fixed diameter of0.5 deg regardless of eccentricity, sensitivity decreasedmuch more rapidlyfor the moreeccentricdisks.Note thatthe logarithmictime decay curvesat eccentricitiesof 1or2 deg-had shallow slopes, but as eccentricity increased

1]10

rI 05° constant / Eccentricity :

8

6

4

2

o~ ()~—~1 10 100 1 10 100

Time sec Time sec

a b

o~1 10 100

Time sec

c

FIGURE 1. Subjectsviewed a peripheral flickeringdisk and adjustedthe flicker amplitudeto hold it at threshold.Thresholdsincreased logarithmically over time as sensitivity decreased, giving straight lines on this log-time plot. Steeper time decaycurves indicate more rapid adaptation(mean of four observersx three readings).Vertical bars show 1 SE. (a) For a fixeddiskdiameter of 0.5 deg, eccentric disks disappearedmost rapidly.Disksat eccentricitiesof 1or 2 deg neededonlyslight amplitudeincreases to remain visible but disk at 16deg disappeared from view within 15sec. (b) M-scaling of large disks partlycompensatedfor eccentricity.Curvesare more closelybunchedthan in (a). Fullcompensationwouldhave superimposedall thecurves. (c) M-scaling of small disks overcompensatedfor eccentricity. Curves are now reversed in order, being lower (bettersensitivity) at 8 and 16deg than at 2 deg. Small disks were overall much harder to see, however:note that y scale is different

from (a) and (b).

3482 S. ANSTIS

the slopes became steeper, until a disk at 16 degeccentricity disappearedfrom view within 10-15 sec.

Results for the large M-scaled disks (Condition2) areshown in Fig. l(b). Once again contrast thresholds roselogarithmically with time. Notice that if the M-scalinghad compensated for retinal variations, all the data fordifferenteccentricitieswouldbe superimposedin a singlecurve, but this was clearly not the case. Performancestillfell off with increasing retinal eccentricity; the 1 degcurve was lowest (best), the 16 deg curve was highest,and other eccentricitieslay in between. It is true that theperformance gap between different eccentricities hasbeen narrowed, because the curves are more tightlybunched,but they are nowherenear being superimposed.So M-scaling compensated only partially for the effectsof eccentricity. We thought it possible that these largedisks might exceed the limits of spatial summation, inwhich case sensitivitymightfail to benefitfrom enlargingthe more peripheral disks. So we repeated the M-scaledexperimentusing a set of much smaller disks, ranging indiameter from 3.6 min arc (0.06 deg) at 2 deg eccentri-city to 27 min arc (0.46 deg)at 16 deg eccentricity.Thesesizeswere chosento match the ratio of size to eccentricityused by Schieting and Spillman (1987).

Results for the small M-scaled disks (Condition3) areshown in Fig. l(c). Overall the slopeswere much higherthan for the larger disks, showing that flicker was farharder to see in small than in large disks.Notice that theyscale in Fig. l(c) is different from Fig. l(a) and (b). Forthe constant size or large M-scaled disks in Fig. l(a) and(b) it took in the order of 80 sec for the contrast thresholdto approach 10%,but for the small M-scaleddisks in Fig.l(c) it took only about 20 sec for the contrast thresholdtoapproach 30Y0.In addition, M-scaling actually over-compensatedfor eccentricitywith these small disks.Thecurves are not merely bunched together but actuallyreversed in order, with the curve for 16 deg eccentricitybelow the 1 deg curve instead of above as it was in Fig.l(a) and (b).

These results are replotted in Fig. 2. Here the log slopeof each time decay curve (m, wherey = m log t +c, andyis thresholdcontrast and t is time) is plotted as a functionof eccentricity,so that each curve in Fig. 1 is reduced to asingle point in Fig. 2. For the constant-sized disks inCondition 1, the function relating log-slope to eccen-tricity in Fig. 2 sloped steeply up to the right, showingthat flicker perception got worse in the periphery, and itwas positively accelerated, showing that the loss wasgreatest as the eccentricity increased from 8 to 16 deg.The values of the constantc (not illustrated)were alwayssmall (< 1.7) and were not systematically related todifficultyof seeing flicker.

The effects of M-scaling depended upon the sizes ofthe disks.A horizontalfunctionin Fig. 2 would mean thatM-scalingcompensatedperfectly for eccentricity.In fact,the function obtained sloped slightly upwards for largedisks and slightly downwards for small disks in Fig. 2,showing that M-scaling undercompensatedfor the large

Diskdiameter.08 .12 .23 .48 SmallMscaled

.5,5 .5 .5 .5 Constantdiameter

.51 2 LargeMscaled15~8

19 I

o~o 4 8 12 16

Eccentricity(deg)

FIGURE2. Slope of each curve in Fig. 1 is replotted here as a singlepoint. For constant sized disks, slope rose steeply (disks disappearedsooner) with increasing eccentricity. For large M-scaled disks, theyrose only slowly with eccentricity, showing undercompensation.Forsmall M-scaled disks, they actually fell with eccentricity, showingovercompensation;however, the whole function is high up, showing

that small flickeringdisks were hard to see.

disks and overcompensatedfor the small disks.We shallreturn to this point in the Discussion.

For the constantdisksthe log-slopemat 16 deg was 7.8times larger (worse) than at 1 deg eccentricity,showingavery pronounced loss of flicker sensitivity with eccen-tricity.It was only3.0 timeshigherfor the large M-scaleddisks, showing a much smaller loss of flicker sensitivitywith eccentricity.Thus, M-scaling mitigated the fall-offin performancewith eccentricity,but it certainly did notfully compensatefor it. For the small M-scaled disks thelog-slopem was actually 1.3 times larger (worse)at 2 degthan at 16 deg, showingthat M-scalingovercompensatedfor eccentricity, since flicker sensitivity actually im-proved as one went further out from the fovea.

Presumably at some intermediate disk size the M-scaling would exactly compensatefor eccentricity.

As onewould expect, the curve for the constant0.5 degdiameterdisks in Fig. 2 intersectedwith the curve for thelarge M-scaled disk at 1 deg eccentricity, where bothdisks were 0.5 deg, and it sloped upwards to intersectwith the curve for the small M-scaled disk at 16 degeccentricity, where both disks were also 0.5 deg. Thusresultswere consistentacross conditions.

Effects of flicker r-ate

We measured the adaptation to different flicker rates,namely 3, 5, 8, 12, and 15 Hz, all for a 2 deg disk at aneccentricityof 4 deg. (The 5 Hz conditionwas the sameas the 4 deg eccentricity condition in Condition 2.)Results are shown in Fig. 3(a). Figure 3(a) shows thatflicker thresholds rose logarithmically with time, asbefore. The correlation coefficient r2 between data andfitted logarithmiccurves was 0.94 or better in all cases.Performance fell off monotonically with flicker fre-quency. The threshold contrast for seeing 3 Hz flicker

ADAPTATIONTO PERIPHERALFLICKER 3483

Frequency8]

‘ $“z a~

“1 10 100 1 10 100 1000Timesec #of cycles

a b

FIGURE3. (a) Thresholdcontrastswere higher,and time decaycurves steeper,with increasingflickerfrequency(meanof fourobserversx three readings). Vertical bars show 1 SE. (b) Same curves are replotted as a function of total number of flickercycles. This tends to superimposeall the curves, showing that total number of elapsed cycles determines the time course of

flicker sensitivity.

never went above 596,but it rose to above 770for 15 Hzflicker after 60 sec exposure.

Since sensitivity fell off more rapidly for highertemporal frequencies, we wondered whether it fell by afixedamountfor each cycle of flicker,so we replottedthesame data in Fig. 3(b) as a function of the number ofelapsed cycles. For this plot the x axis is changed fromseconds to numbers of cycles and the 3 Hz curve iseffectively stretched horizontally threefold, the 15 Hzcurve 15-fold,and so on. If the hypothesiswere true andthe number of cycles were the relevant variable, then allthe datum points for different frequencieswould now liealong, the same time decay curve. Although this is notentirely true, it is clear that the curves are much moretightly bunched in Fig. 3(b) than in Fig. 3(a). This isfurther brought out in Fig. 4, in which each curve in Fig.3(a) and (b) is reduced to a singlepoint. The open circles

3

ars~2g

1

0 {o 5 10 15

Hz

FIGURE4. Slope of each curve in Fig. 3 is replotted here as a singlepoint. 0, Slope of time decay curves rises, showing more rapidadaptation, as flicker frequency increases. ●, Horizontal functionshowsthat adaptationis a constantas a functionof total elapsedflicker

cycles.

in Fig. 4 show the log slope for each time decay curve,taken from Fig. 3(a) (m, where y = m log t +c), as afunction of its frequency, and the filled circles show thelog slope for each time decay curve taken from Fig. 3(b)as a function of the number of elapsed cycles. In Fig. 4the frequency data (open circles) show a monotonicincrease of log slope with frequency, and the log slopewas three times greater (worse) for 15 Hz than for 3 Hz.The number-of-cyclesdata (filled circles), on the otherhand, lay more nearly on a horizontal line, with the logslope being only 1.27 times greater (worse) for 15 Hzthan for 3 Hz. We conclude that to a first approximation,each doubling of the number of elapsed flicker cycles,rather than the temporal frequency per se, elevates thecontrastthresholdby a fixed amount.This disagreeswithSchieting and Spillmann’s finding that time-to-disap-pearance of a flickering spot became shorter as thefrequency increased, but became longer when thesefrequencies were converted into the number of elapsedcycles.

DISCUSSION

Our results confirm those of Schieting and Spillman(1987), Harris et al. (1990), and Hammett and Smith(1990).We confirmedthat smaller, more eccentric disksflickeringat higherfrequenciesare most apt to disappear.We differ from Schieting and Spillmann in a few minorrespects; they found that M-scaling compensated foreccentricity for small disks, whereas we found that it itundercompensatedfor large disks and overcompensatedfor small disks. They found that neither frequency nortotal numberof cycles fitted their resultsfor the effect offlickerrate on disappearancerate, whereas we found thattotal number of cycles gave a good fit.

A more important difference lay in our method.Whereas they measured the time at which a givenflickeringdisk disappeared,giving a single number as a

3484 S. ANSTIS

measure of flicker lability, we adjusted the flickercontinuously to measure the logarithmic decay of itsvisibility, giving a continuous curve over time. Weplotted the full biographyof flicker sensitivityinstead ofjust the date of its death, so to speak.Notice that our timedecay curves do not portray the pure time constantsof aneural integrator.Instead they delineateits activitywithina negative feedback loop, namely its response to beingcontinuouslyadapted by a threshold-levelstimulus.

Our resultsdo not showperfect M-scaling.Whitakeretal. (1992) measured the rate of decline with increasingeccentricity of several position and movement acuities.For each task, the decline of acuity with eccentricitycould be quantifiedby the parameterE2 which representsthe eccentricity at which stimulus size must double inorder to match foveal performance.All tasks were foundto obey the concept of spatial scaling in that perfor-mances at any two eccentricities could be matchedsimply by a change of scale. However, the rate at whichperformancedeterioratedwith eccentricityvaried over anenormous range (100:1) depending upon the task itself.Acuity fell off much more rapidlyfor static tasks(spatial-intervalor spatial-bisectionjudgments) than for dynamictasks (apparentmotion with or without a landmark).Theadvantage of such diverse peripheral gradients is clear,since it is more advantageous for survival to preservemovement detection than precise spatial judgments inthe periphery, but the mechanismsare still unknown.

Our own results for flicker in Fig. 2 do not fit thisspatial scaling model. Perfect M-scaling would givehorizontal straight lines in Fig. 2. In fact, the lines forlarge and small M-scaled disks slope slightly upwardsand downwards with increasing eccentricity. That is,although flicker was overall much harder to see in thesmall than the large M-scaleddisks,performanceactuallyimprovedwith eccentricityfor the smalldisksand the M-scaling overcompensated for the reduced flicker sensi-tivity at greater eccentricities.This might be an effect ofRicco’s area, which is known to vary with eccentricity(Rovamo & Raninen, 1984; Raninen & Rovamo, 1986).We speculate that F-scaling the stimuli, that is reducingtheir stimulusluminancein inverseproportionto Ricco’sarea, might bring the flicker data closer to perfect M-scaling. It would be interestingto collect data for a dark-adapted eye, which has larger spatial summationareas.

It is hoped that these studiesof peripheralsensitivitytoflicker in normal subjectsmay provide a baseline againstwhich to evaluate early visual losses in glaucoma, adisease which starts by attacking peripheral vision andsensitivityto flicker (Glovinskyet al., 1992;Katz et al.,1993). Such testing might help to provide the earlydiagnosis and prompt treatment which are the keys tosuccessful management of this disease (Kaufman &Mittag, 1994).

REFERENCES

Anstis, S. M. (1974). A chart demonstratingvariations in acuity withretinal position. VisionResearch, 14, 589–592.

Bijl, P., Koenderink,J. J. & Kappers, A. M. (1992). Deviationsfrom

strict M scaling. Journal of the Optical Sociery of America A, 9,1233–1239.

Blake, R., O’Shea, R. P. & Mueller, T. J. (1992). Spatial zones ofbinocular rivalry in central and peripheral vision. VisualNeurosciencej8, 469478.

Frome, F. S., MacLeod,D. L, Buck, S. L. & Williams, D. R. (1981).Large loss of visual sensitivity to flashed peripheral targets. VisionResearch, 21, 1323–1328.

Glovinsky,Y., Quigley,H. A., Drum, B., Bissett, R. A. & Jampel, H.D. (1992). A whole-field scotopic retinal sensitivity test for thedetection of early glaucoma damage.Archives in Ophthalmology,110, 486Jt90.

Hammett, S. T. & Smith, A. T. (1990). Flicker adaptation in theperiphery at constant perceived modulationdepth. Perception, 19,113–117.

Harris, J. P., Calvert, J. E. & Snelgar, R. S. (1990). Adaptation toperipheral flicker: Relationship to contrast detection thresholds.VisionResearch, 30, 381–386.

Johnston,A. & Wright, M. J. (1985). Lower thresholdsof motion forgratings as a functionof eccentricity and contrast. VisionResearch,25, 179–185.

Katz, J., Tielsch, J. M., Quigley,H. A., Javitt, J., Witt, K. & Sommer,A. (1993).Automatedsuprathresholdscreening for glaucoma: TheBaltimore Eye survey.Investigations in Ophthalmologyand VisualScience, 34, 3271–3312.

Kaufman, P. L. & Mittag, T. W. (Eds) (1994). Glaucoma. London:Mosby.

Levi, D. M., Klein, S. A. & Aitsebaomo,A. P. (1985).Vernier acuity,crowdingand cortical magnification.VisionResearch, 25, 963–977.

Murray, I., MacCana, F. & Krdikowski,J. J. (1983). Contributionoftwo movement detecting mechanisms to central and peripheralvision. VisionResearch, 23, 151–159.

Parth, P. & Rentschler,L (1984).Numerosityjudgments in peripheralvision. Limitations of the cortical magnification hypothesis.Behavioral Brain Research, 11, 241–248.

Pointer, J. S. (1986). Visual representation at the cerebral cortex:Qualitativeandcpranthativeaspects.Ophthalmic& PhysiologicalOptics, 2, 171-175.

Raninen, A. & Rovamo, J. (1986). Perimetry of critical flickerfrequencyin humanrodandconevision. VisionResearch,26,1249-1255.

Reichardt,W. (1961).Autocorrelation,a principlefor the evaluationofsensory informationby the central nervous system. In Rosenblith,W. (Ed.),Sensorycommunication.New York:MITPress andWiley.

Rovamo,J. & Raninen,A. (1984). Critical flicker frequency and M-scalingof stimulussize andretinal illuminance.VisionResearch,24,1127–1131.

Schieting, S. & Spillman, L. (1987). Flicker adaptation in theperipheral retina. VisionResearch, 27, 2717-3284.

Sharpe,C. R. (1972).The visibility and fading of thin lines visualizedby their controlled movement across the retina. Journal ofPhysiology,232, 113-134.

Stephenson, C. M., Knapp, A. J. & Braddick, O. J. (1991).Discrimination of spatial phase shows a qualitative differencebetween foveal and peripheral processing. Vision Research, 31,1315–1326.

Strasburger, H., Harvey, L. 0. & Rentschler, 1. (1991). Contrastthresholds for identification of numeric characters in direct andeccentric view. Perception& Psychophysics,49, 495–508.

Troxler, L P. V. (1804). Uber das Verschwinden gegebenerGegenstande innerhalb unseres Gesichtskreises. In Himly, J. andSchmidt,J. A. (Eds), Ophthalm.Biliothek (Vol. 2, pp. 51–53). Jena:Fromann.

Van Esch,J. A., Koldenhof,E. E., VanDoom,A. J. & Koenderink,J. J.(1984). Spectral sensitivity and wavelength discrimination of thehuman peripheral visual field. Journal of the Optical Socie~ ofAmerica A, 1, 443-450.

Virsu,V., Nasanen,R. & Osmoviita,K. (1987).Corticalmagnificationandperipheralvision.Journalof the OpticalSociety ofAmericaA, 8,156%1578.

Virsu, V., Rovamo,J., Laurinen,P. & Nasanen, R. (1982).Temporal

ADAPTATIONTO PERIPHERALFLICKER 3485

contrast sensitivityand cortical magnification.VisionResearch, 22,1211–1217.

Watson,A. B. (1987).Estimationof local spatial scale.Journal of theOptical Society of America A, 4, 1579–1582.

Wesermann,W. & Norcia, A. M. (1992). Contrast dependenceof theoscillatory motion threshold across the visual field. Journal of theOpticalSociety of America A, 10, 1663–1671.

Whitaker, D., Makelda, P., Rovamo, J. & Latham, K. (1992). Theinfluence of eccentricity on position and movement acuities asrevealed by spatial scaling. VisionResearch, 32, 1913–1930.

White, J. M. & Bedell, H. E. (1990). The oculomotor reference inhumanswith bilateral maculardisease.lrrvestigativeOphthalmologyand VisualScience, 31, 1149–1161.

Wright,M. J. & Johnston,A. (1985).The relationshipof displacementthresholds for oscillating gratings to cortical magnification,spatiotemporal frequency and contrast. VisionResearch, 25, 187–193.

AcknowledgementsSupported by NIH Grant EY 10241-3.Thanks toMike Benedetto, Joyce Chang, Sam Feroudeini, Corey Lehman,Heather O’Shea, Kathy Priebe, Ilan Shrira, and Marc Van Enk forassistance in data collection. I also thank the two anonymousrefereesfor helpful suggestions.