3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

download 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

of 8

Transcript of 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    1/18

    Review

    Electrophysiological measures as potential biomarkers in

    Huntington's disease: Review and future directions

    Lan Nguyena, John L. Bradshawa, Julie C. Stout a,Rodney J. Croft b, Nellie Georgiou-Karistianisa,⁎ 

    aSchool of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia

    bDepartment of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Northfields Ave, Wollongong 2522, Australia

    A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

     Article history:

    Accepted 29 March 2010

    Available online 8 April 2010

    Neuroimaging is fundamental to identifying quantifiable and objective biomarkers in

    symptomatic and pre-diagnostic Huntington's disease (HD). However, the challenge

    remains to find reliable biomarkers with high sensitivity and specificity that can be used

    to track the functional decline over time and test efficacy of therapeutic intervention. While

    many recent studies have focused on neuroimaging techniques based on brain

    hemodynamic activity, comparatively fewer have utilized electroencephalography (EEG)

    and event-related potentials (ERPs). This review aims to summarise and integrate key

    electroencephalographical findings from the last two decades in symptomatic and pre-

    diagnostic HD, in context with recent neuroimaging data, and to use this information toidentify promising candidate markers for future research and clinical consideration.

    © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    Keywords:

    Huntington's disease

    Pre-diagnostic Huntington's disease

    Electroencephalography

    Event-related potentialsBiomarkers

    Contents

    1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 178

    2. Use of EEG/ERP in identifying potential biomarkers of dysfunction and disease progression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 178

    3. Quantitative EEG markers of Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180

    3.1. Sleep EEG in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181

    4. Sensory ERP markers of Huntington's disease. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181

    4.1. Auditory ERPs in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181

    4.2. Visual ERPs in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1824.3. Somatosensory ERPs in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182

    5. Movement-related potentials in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183

    6. Long-latency potentials in Huntington's disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 184

    7. Animal studies of HD and electrophysiology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185

    8. Conclusions and future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186

    References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188

    B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

    ⁎   Corresponding author. Experimental Neuropsychology Research Unit, School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Monash University, Clayton,Victoria 3800, Australia. Fax: +61 3 9905 3948.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (N. Georgiou-Karistianis).

    0165-0173/$   – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.004

    a v a i l a b l e a t w w w . s c i e n c e d i r e c t . c o m

    w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / b r a i n r e s r e v

    mailto:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.004mailto:[email protected]

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    2/18

    1. Introduction

    Huntington's disease (HD) is a dominantly inherited neurode-

    generative condition classically characterized by motor dys-

    function, cognitive impairment and psychiatric disturbances

    (Hayden, 1981; Folstein, 1989). The symptoms do not manifest

    systematically and any of the three can predominate in agiven patient. Estimates of the worldwide prevalence of the

    disease are about 5 to 10 per 100,000, although there is

    considerable variation across geographical regions due to its

    hereditary nature (Conneally, 1984; Schilling and Borchelt,

    2003). The disease is caused by expansion of 36 or more

    trinucleotide (CAG) repeats on chromosome 4 (4p16.3), which

    leads to structural and functional alteration of the protein

    huntingtin (Harjes and Wanker, 2003; The Huntington's

    Disease Collaborative Research Group, 1993). The length of 

    CAG repeats correlates inversely with age of disease onset,

    with higher numbers associated with earlier symptom onset;

    onset of illness can occur from infancy to old age and mostly

    between the ages of 35 and 50. Upon diagnosis, symptoms are

    relentlessly progressive with death occurring on average

    within 15 to 20 years. Current pharmacological management

    can only alleviate symptoms as at present there is no effective

    treatment or cure (for a review, see Phillips et al., 2008).

    While the diagnosis of HD is based on the presence of 

    chorea, accumulating research supports a pre-diagnostic

    phase, during which symptoms and signs gradually appear 

    and progress, hereafter referred to as  “pre-HD”. Neuropsycho-

    logical studies indicate that subtle motor (Blekher et al., 2004;

    de Boo et al., 1997; Kirkwood et al., 1999, 2000a; Paulsen et al.,

    2008; Petit and Milbled, 1973; Siemers et al., 1996; Smith et al.,

    2000; Snowden et al., 2002; Verny et al., 2007), cognitive

    (Diamond et al., 1992; Hahn-Barma et al., 1998; Jason et al.,

    1997; Kirkwood et al., 2000b; McCusker et al., 2000; Paulsen et

    al., 2001b; Rosenberg et al., 1995; Verny et al., 2007) and

    psychiatric signs and/or symptoms (Berrios et al., 2001, 2002;

    Brandt et al., 2002; Duff et al., 2007; Kirkwood et al., 2002;

    Paulsen et al., 2001a) are present years before clinical

    diagnosis. However, changes documented in pre-HD are

    subtle and by definition do not fall in the clinically impaired

    range (Farrow et al., 2007; Paulsen et al., 2008; Solomon et al.,

    2007). Although several neuropsychological studies have

    failed to identify pre-HD changes (Blackmore et al., 1995;

    Brandtet al., 2002; Campodonico et al., 1996;de Booet al., 1999;

    Giordani et al., 1995; Gomez-Tortosa et al., 2001; Rothlind et al.,

    1993; Siemers et al., 1996; Strauss and Brandt, 1990; Witjes-Ane

    et al., 2003), neuroimaging confirms that structural changes

    commence a decade or more prior to diagnosis (for reviews,

    see Bohanna et al., 2008; Georgiou-Karistianis, 2009; Paulsen,

    2009). Improved understanding of the earliest manifestations

    of the illness offers the potential for early therapeutic

    intervention (Georgiou-Karistianis, 2009; Phillips et al., 2008).

    The identification of quantifiable and objective biomarkers

    of early and prodromal disease is pivotal to the acceleration of 

    possible neuroprotective therapies. However, the challenge

    remains to identify reliable biomarkers with high sensitivity

    and specificity that can track progressive functional decline.

    Many recent pre-HD studies have focused on neuroimaging 

    techniques based on brain hemodynamic activity such as

    positron emission tomography (PET) (Andrews and Brooks,

    1998; Ciarmiello et al., 2006; Feigin et al., 2001, 2006; Pavese et

    al., 2003), single photon emission tomography (SPET)

    (Andrews and Brooks, 1998), magnetic resonance imaging 

    (MRI) (Aylward, 2007; Aylward et al., 2000; Campodonico et al.,

    1998; Kassubek et al., 2004a,b; Paulsen et al., 2006; Peinemann

    et al., 2005; Starkstein et al., 1992; Thieben et al., 2002; Wolf et

    al., 2008a,b), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)(Georgiou-Karistianis et al., 2007; Paulsen et al., 2004; Reading 

    et al., 2004; Thiruvady et al., 2007; Wolf et al., 2007) and

    diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) (Reading et al., 2005; Rosas et

    al., 2006; Sritharan et al., 2010). Less has been done with

    electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potentials

    (ERPs), despite the increase of studies utilizing EEG and ERP to

    identify biomarkers in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive

    impairment ( Jackson and Snyder, 2008; Prichep, 2007). Renewed

    interest in EEG stems from significant technical advances and

    resultant improvements in the quality and quantification of 

    data. Electrophysiological data can be collected in high density

    recordings(from up to 256 leads or more), permitting imaging of 

    data topographically in three dimensions and localization

    analyses to identify sources of the EEG signals. This review

    aims to summarise and integrate key electroencephalographi-

    cal findings from the last two decades in symptomatic and pre-

    HD, in context with neuroimaging data, and to use this

    information to identify promising candidate markers for future

    research and clinical consideration.

    2. Use of EEG/ERP in identifying potential biomarkers of dysfunction and disease progression

    Biomarkers, generally defined as objectively measured char-

    acteristics, provide indications of normal biological processes,

    pathogenic processes or pharmacological responses to a

    therapeutic intervention (The Biomarkers Definitions Work-

    ing Group, 2001). In the context of HD, biomarkers could be

    used as quantifiable measures of manifest disease to define

    conversion from pre-HD to symptomatic disease. Biomarkers

    could also potentially be used as a tool to monitor the rate of 

    disease progression, in both the pre-HD and symptomatic

    phase, to better understand the pathogenesis of HD. Further-

    more, biomarkers would facilitate an accurate evaluation of 

    the effectiveness of new therapies and improve efficiency of 

    clinical trials. Due to the heterogeneity of HD symptoms, it is

    possible that a combination of clinical, behavioral and/or 

    biological biomarkers will be required to provide information

    about different aspects of the disease.

    EEG is a measurement of the electrical activity of the brain

    (Andreassi, 2000; Luck, 2005). It is a non-invasive technique

    that could potentially yield relatively low-cost biomarkers.

    EEG and ERPs are ideal tools to explore quantifiable and

    objective biomarkers of HD neuropathology. Surface EEG

    signals, recorded using electrodes applied to the scalp,

    represent a summation of billions of individual inhibitory

    and excitatory post-synaptic potentials influenced by shared

    activity between cortical and subcortical regions (see Fig. 1 for 

    a summary of EEG/ERP measurement and analyses). Data

    generated by these potentials offer millisecond temporal

    resolution, a thousand fold faster than hemodynamic

    178   B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    3/18

    measures, though with correspondingly poorer spatial reso-

    lution (Andreassi, 2000). ERPs are derived from the EEG signal

    by averaging a seriesof EEG segments that are time locked to a

    common class of events. As such, ERPs represent voltage

    changes in the brain associated with specific processes. There

    are several types of ERPs. Sensory ERPs refer to potentials

    produced by visual, auditory, somatosensory, and olfactory

    stimuli. Motor ERPs precede and accompany voluntary

    movement. In contrast to sensory and motor potentials,

    long-latency potentials refer to ERP components that occur 

    at 250 to 900 ms after an event and reflect more subjective

    aspects of processing, oftenlinked to cognitive and attentional

    functions. All of these potentials can vary in their amplitudes

    and latencies, reflecting strength of processes and/or effect of 

    particular experimental manipulations and the specific pat-

    terns of neural activity that these conditions evoke.

    The ERP waveform consists of a sequence of positive and

    negative voltage fluctuations, referred to as peaks, waves, or 

    components (Andreassi, 2000; Luck, 2005). Components are

    labeled with a P (positive) or N (negative) followed by either 

    latency (e.g. P105 for a positive peak at 105 ms post-stimulus)

    or position of the peak within the waveform (e.g. P1 for the

    first major positive peak). The use of latencies can be

    misleading as component latencies vary as a function of the

    Fig. 1 –  Brief summary of EEG and ERP measurement and analyses. For a more detailed introduction to electrophysiology, see

    Luck, 2005.

    179B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    4/18

    stimulus and task. For example, the P300 component may

    peak as late as 1000 ms under some conditions. Three

    measurable aspects of the ERP waveform are amplitude,

    latency, and scalp distribution (Luck, 2005). Amplitude pro-

    vides an index of the magnitude and strength of neural

    activation. Latency refers to the point in time at which the

    peak occurs (or the onset of the deflection) and gives

    information on timing of the activation. Finally, scalpdistribution is the distribution of voltage over the scalp at

    anypoint of time andgives an indication of theoverall pattern

    of activated brain areas. The ERP waveforms are thought to

    reflect the sequence of neural and cognitive processes;

    however, components may be absent from the ERP waveform

    if the neural generators do not have the appropriate orienta-

    tion relative to the scalp (Luck, 2005). As such, the presence of 

    two sequentially occurring components in the ERP waveform

    does not necessarily indicate sequential stages of processing.

    Moreover, it is difficult to determine the exact locations of 

    generators of ERP components. However, the combination of 

    high density recordings and sophisticated spatial filtering 

    techniques can generally provide an indication of the corti-

    cally generated activity. Taken together, electrophysiology can

    detect sensory, motor, and cognitive processing deficits

    related to HD neuropathology. It is a relatively inexpensive

    technology that yields high temporal sensitivity also offering a

    range of measures as potential biomarkers of disease onset

    and progression.

    3. Quantitative EEG markers of Huntington's disease

    Quantitative EEG studies using power spectral analyses have

    documented changes across all frequency bands in HD

    (Bylsma et al., 1994; de Tommaso et al., 2003; Streletz et al,

    1990). The EEG of HD patients (in a resting state with eyes

    closed) was characterized by a marked reduction in raw and

    percent alpha power, a decrease in raw and percent theta in

    the medial frontal area, increases in percent delta and percent

    beta power, and decreased theta frequency (Bylsma et al.,

    1994). Conversely, Streletz et al. (1990) and de Tommaso et al.

    (2003)   both reported theta power to be increased in HD

    compared to controls. The most consistent and strongest

    electrophysiological abnormality in HD, compared to controls,

    is suppression of alpha activity (Bellotti et al., 2004; Bylsma et

    al., 1994; de Tommaso et al., 2003; Streletz et al., 1990). Using 

    artificial neural networks, the abnormality was sourced to

    dysfunction of the thalamus (Bellotti et al., 2004; de Tommaso

    et al., 2003). In line with these findings, MRI analyses revealed

    grey matter differences in the bilateral thalamus in symp-

    tomatic (Kassubek et al., 2005) and pre-HD (Wolf et al., 2009).

    The relationship between EEG changes and cognitive/

    neurological impairment in HD is not well understood.   De

    Tommaso et al. (2003)   reported that reduction in alpha

    correlated poorly with severity of illness and cognitive

    impairments in symptomatic and pre-HD. Similarly, both

    Bylsma et al. (1994) and Sishta et al. (1974) found that duration

    of disease was not correlated with any quantified EEG power 

    measure. However, Streletz et al. (1990) reported a significant

    correlation between increased theta and reduced alpha power 

    and clinical stage of dementia [using the Clinical Dementia

    Rating Scale (Burke et al., 1988)]. Bylsma et al. (1994) detailed a

    more complex relationship, reporting neurological impair-

    ment, including severity of oculomotor impairment, chorea,

    and voluntary motor impairment, was associated with higher 

    beta and delta frequencies, particularly at frontal areas,

    whereas poor cognitive performance was related to EEG

    abnormalities in frontal and temporal regions.Alpha activity abnormalities have been reported in pre-HD

    (de Tommaso et al., 2003; van der Hiele et al., 2007). Reduction

    of alpha activity (in a resting state) discriminated pre-HD from

    controls, despite the study comprising only a small number of 

    pre-HD participants (n= 7) (de Tommaso et al., 2003). The

    authors suggested that the degree of alpha reduction may

    identify those close to clinical symptom onset as correlational

    analyses indicated a negative linear relationship between

    expected time of clinical onset and alpha neural scores. On the

    contrary,   van der Hiele et al. (2007)   found absolute alpha

    power at rest to be similar between pre-HD and controls.

    However, during a working memory task, pre-HD had

    significantly less alpha power compared to controls, despite

    comparable memory performance. Drawing on research

    which suggests that alpha power decreases in response to

    mental challenge, the authors suggested that compensatory

    neuronal activity may keep memory performance intact.

    However,   Cooper et al. (2003)   showed that once sensory

    stimulation was controlled, alpha increased with difficulty

    level. In fact, a number of studies indicate that alpha activity

    involves a complex thalamo-cortical network and can index a

    range of possible functions while yet playing an active role in

    the inhibitory control and timing of cortical processes

    (Klimesch et al., 2006; 2007). Nevertheless, functional connec-

    tivity, based on correlations of fMRI blood-oxygen level-

    dependent signal responses between brain regions, has

    revealed dysfunction in prefrontal regions in both symptom-

    atic (Thiruvady et al., 2007; Wolf et al., 2009) and pre-HD (Wolf 

    et al., 2007, 2008a,b) during a working memory task. In line

    with van der Hiele et al. (2007), working memory performance

    did not significantly differ between pre-HD and controls (Wolf 

    et al., 2007, 2008a,b). Furthermore, a number of studies

    consistently find that pre-HD participants show increased

    functional activation in cortical brain regions during task

    performance despite comparable behavioral performance to

    controls (Paulsen et al., 2004; Reading et al., 2004; Wolf et al.,

    2007). Despite conflicting results, the findings of  de Tommaso

    et al. (2003) and van der Hiele et al. (2007)  are suggestive of 

    alpha changes in pre-HD. Further research is required to

    clarify the mechanisms of alpha at rest and during cognitive

    processing in pre-HD.

    In summary, several studies corroborate that alpha

    amplitude reduction is a discriminating feature in HD, with

    recent preliminary findings of changes in pre-HD (de

    Tommaso et al., 2003; van der Hiele et al., 2007 ). However,

    alpha oscillations have multiple functional correlates in-

    cluding sensory, motor, and memory processes (for a review,

    see  Basar et al., 1997; Klimesch et al., 2007 ), and there is

    limited understanding of their relationship with HD. Further 

    investigation in large-scale studies with both symptomatic

    and pre-HD is required to better understand the basis of 

    the electrophysiological abnormality, and longitudinal

    180   B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    5/18

    opportunities will enable examination of the reliability of 

    these measures in indexing disease progression.

    3.1. Sleep EEG in Huntington's disease

    Sleep disturbances including insomnia, nocturnal waking, and

    daytime sleepiness are commonly reported in HD (Taylor and

    Bramble, 1997); however, polysomnographic sleep studiesprovide mixed findings. The most consistently reported

    sleep abnormality in HD compared with controls is reduced

    sleep efficiency (Arnulf et al., 2008; Hansotia et al., 1985;

    Morton et al., 2005; Silvestri et al., 1995; Wiegand et al., 1991a,

    b). Other abnormalities include prolonged sleep latency

    (Cuturic et al., 2009; Wiegand et al., 1991a), reduced slow-

    wave sleep (Wiegand et al., 1991a; Silvestri et al., 1995), and

    increased density of sleep spindles (Emser et al., 1988;

    Wiegand et al., 1991a). Rapid eye movement abnormalities

    have also been suggested (Arnulf et al., 2008; Hansotia et al.,

    1985; Silvestri et al., 1995; Starr, 1967) although this account is

    not supported by a number of studies (Cuturic et al., 2009;

    Emseret al., 1988; Wiegand, et al., 1991a,b).In fact, a numberof 

    studies have found only one sleep variable to be abnormal in

    HD compared to controls. For example,  Cuturic et al. (2009)

    found that only sleep latency was significantly longer in the

    HD group.   Wiegand et al. (1991b)   found that only sleep

    efficiency was significantly reduced in HD compared to

    controls, although there was a trend for reduced slow-wave

    sleep and longer sleep onset latency. Emser et al. (1988) found

    no sleep abnormalities in HD, except for an increased spindle

    density. In an in vivo PET study, HD disturbances in the wake–

    sleep cycle were partly attributed to significant D2 receptor 

    loss and microglia activation in the hypothalamus (Politis et

    al., 2008). Variationin EEGresults maybe related to differences

    in study design including small sample sizes, single (Arnulf et

    al., 2008; Cuturic et al., 2009) or two night (Silvestri et al., 1995;

    Wiegand et al., 1991a,b; Emser et al., 1988) polysomnogram, as

    well as the variety of sleep variables measured. A number of 

    studies did not indicate medication status or control for the

    possible effect of medication on sleep (Arnulf et al., 2008;

    Emser et al., 1988; Silvestri et al., 1995; Starr, 1967). In

    conclusion, although a number of polysomnographic sleep

    abnormalities have been reported in HD, there is great

    variability in findings. Sleep disturbances can be affected by

    many extraneous variables such as medication, levels of 

    stress, and mood (Brotini and Gigli, 2004). To ascertain reliable

    sleep EEG abnormalities in HD as well as their reliability and

    sensitivity as measures of disease onset and progression, a

    well-controlled standardized study is required offering signif-

    icantly larger sample sizes than those conducted previously.

    4. Sensory ERP markers of Huntington'sdisease

    4.1. Auditory ERPs in Huntington's disease

    ERPs have been utilized to investigate whether auditory

    processing is affected in HD. Studies of brainstem auditory

    evoked potential (BAEP) collectively indicate that early proces-

    sing of auditory stimuli (circa 0–12 ms) is normal in HD,

    signifying intact peripheral and central auditory pathways

    (Bollenet al., 1986; Ehle et al., 1984; Scott et al., 1972). However,

    HD patients showreduced amplitude and prolonged latency of 

    the P50 auditory ERP (AERP) (Uc et al., 2003). Abnormal

    manifestations of the P50 potential are suggestive of distur-

    bances in the control of states of arousal and sleep–wake

    regulation by the cholinergic arm of the reticular activating 

    system (Skinner et al., 2002). This is consistent with sleep

    deregulation in HD (Wiegand et al., 1991a). In the paired-clickparadigm, when twoclicks arepresented 500 ms apart,the P50

    response to the second of the clicks undergoes active

    inhibition from cholinergic hippocampal inputs (Uc et al.,

    2003); the reduction in P50 amplitude can index sensory

    gating. Using this paired-click paradigm, Uc et al. (2003) found

    that HD patients show reduced P50 sensory gating relative to

    normal controls. This is consistent with findings that HD

    patients failed to inhibit a normal reflex response to intense

    auditory and tactile stimuli (Swerdlow et al., 1995). Sensory

    gating impairment has beenassociated in other disorderswith

    reduced sustained attention and increased anxiety such as

    schizophrenia (Cullum et al., 1993; Geyer et al., 2001),

    obsessive compulsive disorder (Swerdlow et al., 1993), and

    Tourette's syndrome (Castellanos et al., 1996); Uc et al. (2003)

    suggested that sensory gating deficits may possibly underlie

    disordered attention (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1995) and/or 

    anxiety (Paulsen et al., 2001a) mechanisms in HD.

    In contrast to an expected global cognitive deterioration,

    HD patients displayed enhanced performance in auditory

    sensory memory, with lower error rates and shorter reaction

    time compared to pre-HDand controls (Beste et al., 2008a). The

    behavioral data were accompanied with higher amplitude of 

    the mismatch negativity (MMN); this ERP is evoked by rare

    (deviant) stimuli and is thought to reflect an automatic

    discrimination of stimulus change (Andreassi, 2000).   Beal

    and Ferrante (2004) indicated superior performance in HD may

    be due to increased receptiveness of voltage-dependent

    NMDA receptors to endogenous levels of glutamate. Increas-

    ing evidence suggests that excitotoxicity, excessive action of 

    glutamate receptors by excitatory amino acids leading to cell

    dysfunction/death, may play a role in the selective neuronal

    degeneration in HD (for a review, see   Ferrante, 2009).

    Excitotoxin-induced neuronal damage is inhibited by NMDA

    receptor antagonists (Ferrante et al., 1993). On the other hand,

    a number of studies collectively indicate that MMN is

    modulated by NMDA receptors since administration of 

    NMDA antagonists can abolish the MMN (for a review, see

    Kujala et al., 2007; Naatanen et al., 2007). Taken together and

    in line with suggestions by   Beste et al. (2008a), enhanced

    performance in sensory memory in symptomatic HD is likely

    to be related to sensitivity of MMN to NMDA receptors and

    increased receptiveness to glutamate in HD.

    Additional AERP abnormalities in HD include significant

    delays in N1, P2, and N2 components (circa 100–250 ms)

    (Goodin and Aminoff, 1986; Homberg et al., 1986). However,

    conflicting results have been reported, with other studies

    reporting significant differences only in N1 latency ( Josiassen

    et al., 1984), N2 latency (Filipovic et al., 1990), and P2 latency

    (Rosenberg et al., 1985). Furthermore,  Josiassen et al. (1984)

    and Homberg et al. (1986)   reported that the main AERP

    abnormality in HD was a general reduction in amplitude,

    rather than latency of components.

    181B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    6/18

    Taken overall, although these studies are suggestive of 

    auditory processing deficits in HD, the electrophysiological

    data are limited, with none published in pre-HD. Recent MRI

    data in early HD indicate significant thinning of primary

    auditory cortex and cortical areas adjacent (Rosas et al., 2008).

    Furthermore, recent fMRI data indicate an altered pattern of 

    auditory processing mechanisms, dependent on the progres-

    sion of neuronal dysfunction in both symptomatic andpre-HD(Saft et al., 2008).

    4.2. Visual ERPs in Huntington's disease

    Visual processing disturbances have been frequently de-

    scribed in symptomatic and pre-HD (Gomez-Tortosa et al.,

    1996; Lawrence et al., 2000; Mohr et al., 1991 ). For example,

    MRI data indicate that very early onset HD participants

    show cortical thinning of visual regions (Rosas et al., 2008).

    With disease progression, participants show more extensive

    thinning of the occipital cortex. Other changes include

    altered metabolism of the occipital cortex (Feigin et al.,

    2001; Jenkins et al., 2005) and significant atrophy of the

    occipital lobe (Lange et al., 1976). The early P1 and N1

    components of the visual ERP (VERP) have been shown to

    emanate from secondary visual areas, and these compo-

    nents provide the opportunity to examine time course of 

    neuronal alterations in early visual processing (Luck, 2005).

    HD patients are generally characterized by a non-specific

    amplitude reduction of VERP components in response to

    light flashes or checkerboard reversal stimulation (Ellenber-

    ger et al., 1978; Josiassen et al., 1984; Oepen et al., 1981), with

    abnormalities correlated to severity and duration of symp-

    toms (Ellenberger et al., 1978). However, a number of studies

    have also reported that VERPs in HD were not significantly

    different in amplitude (Ehle et al., 1984; Munte et al., 1997;

    Rosenberg et al., 1985; Scott et al., 1972 ) or latency (Ehle et

    al., 1984; Rosenberg et al., 1985; Scott et al., 1972 ) compared

    to controls. Recent VERP studies have utilized more complex

    task paradigms to delineate visual processing abnormalities

    in HD (Antal et al., 2003; Beste et al., 2008b).   Antal et al.

    (2003) found that HD patients showed reduced amplitude of 

    the N1 component for animal and non-animal images

    compared with controls and pre-HD, suggesting of disrup-

    tions in early perceptual categorization in HD. As difference

    in the N1 component was only found at the temporal

    electrode sites, the authors attributed likely pathological

    changes of the circuits between the basal ganglia and the

    temporal cortex, known to influence higher-order visual

    processing. In another study, the N1 VERP was used to

    examine processes of attention and response selection

    (Beste et al., 2008b). The flanker and target stimuli were

    separated by 100 ms; this enables a separate N1 to be elicited

    during occurrence of a flanker and on the occurrence of a

    target. Results indicated the N1 to the flankers, but not the

    target, was significantly attenuated in HD patients. These

    results suggest that HD patients attended less to the flanker 

    but normally to the target, supporting previous findings of a

    dysfunction in spatial visual attention in HD (Georgiou et al.,

    1995, 1996; Georgiou-Karistianis et al., 2002). The authors

    proposed that selective attenuation exclusively of the

    flanker N1 reflects an alternation of visuospatial attention

    or, more likely, a strategy by the patients to reduce

    distraction from the flankers so as to enhance performance

    on target presentation.

    VERP abnormalities in pre-HD are difficult to interpret as

    most studies were conducted prior to gene localization

    (Ellenberger et al., 1978; Josiassen et al., 1984; Oepen et al.,

    1981). Only two studies to date have explored visual proces-

    sing using ERPs in genetically confirmed pre-HD.  Antal et al.(2003)  found that although the N1 component to animal and

    non-animal stimuli was significantly reduced in amplitude in

    symptomatic HD, no differences were found in pre-HD

    compared to controls. However, Beste et al. (2008b) suggested

    that visual attentional processing may be affected electro-

    physiologically but not behaviorally in pre-HD. The pre-HD

    group produced a flanker N1 that was significantly reduced in

    amplitude compared to controls, despite no reaction time

    differences between the groups. The magnitude of reduction

    in pre-HD was not as strong compared to symptomatic HD,

    which suggests that visual attentional processing is only

    mildly affected and may decline as disease progresses.

    In summary, general visual processing deficits in HD, as

    indicated by ERP in response to light flashes or checkerboard

    reversal, are conflicting. However, the N1 VERP applied to

    specific tasks of visual processing (perceptual categorization

    and attention/response selection) has been shown to be

    valuable in separating pre-HD, symptomatic HD, and controls.

    It is possible that visual processing deficits in HD are task

    specific, particularly in pre-HD. Future longitudinal research

    is required to validate reliability of N1 VERP in both pre-HD

    and HD.

    4.3. Somatosensory ERPs in Huntington's disease

    Although at the behavioral level sensory deficits are not a

    characteristic feature of HD symptomatology, somatosensory

    ERP (SERP) abnormalities are consistently reported following 

    electrical nerve stimulation (Abbruzzese et al., 1990; Beniczky

    et al., 2002; Bollen et al., 1985; Ehle et al., 1984; Huttunen et al.,

    1993; Josiassen et al., 1982; Kuwert et al., 1993; Lefaucheur et

    al., 2002, 2006; Noth et al., 1984; Oepen et al., 1981; Seiss et al.,

    2003; Thompson et al., 1988; Topper et al., 1993). The majority

    of studies report a reduction in amplitude of early SERP

    components, particularly the parietal N20 and P25 compo-

    nents, as well as in precentral frontal P22 and N30 compo-

    nents, despite varying methods and definitions of cortical

    components (Abbruzzese et al., 1990; Beniczky et al., 2002;

    Bollen et al., 1985; Ehle et al., 1984; Kuwert et al., 1993;

    Lefaucheur et al., 2002, 2006; Noth et al., 1984;Thompson et al.,

    1988; Topper et al., 1993). SERP changes in HD, particularly N20

    and N30, are thought to reflect abnormal transmission from

    the basal ganglia–thalamic complex to the sensory cortex

    (Abbruzzese and Berardelli, 2003). The parietal N20 peak

    corresponds to the primary somatosensory cortex receiving 

    cutaneous inputs via the lemniscal pathway, and the frontal

    N30has been proposed to reflect activity in thesupplementary

    motor areas (Rossini et al., 1989). Disruption of these neural

    circuits may interfere with sensory information integration,

    movement preparation, and control (Abbruzzese and Berar-

    delli, 2003). SERP components have been closely linked to HD

    pathology, with significant correlations to CAG repeat length

    182   B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    7/18

    (Beniczky et al., 2002) and Unified Huntington's Disease Rating 

    Scale (UHDRS) (Lefaucheur et al., 2002). Both Ehle et al. (1984)

    and Lefaucheur et al. (2006)   found that the N20 component

    progressively decreased in amplitude over a 2-year period and

    correlated with functional decline, suggesting it may be a

    suitable candidate marker for disease progression. Supporting 

    the ERP findings, significant neuronal loss has been found in

    the primary somatosensory cortex (Lange et al., 1976; Mann etal., 1993; Heinsen et al., 1994), and MRI data indicate cortical

    thinning of the primary sensory cortical regions in very early

    stages (Rosas et al., 2008). Moreover, PET revealed altered

    cortical activation on passive sensory stimulation in HD

    (Boecker et al., 1999).

    Limited SERP research has been conducted in pre-HD, but

    several early studies reported reduction of SERP amplitudes in

    individuals at risk ( Josiassen et al., 1982; 1986; Kuwert et al.,

    1993; Oepen et al., 1981). Surprisingly, only one study to date

    has been conducted on genetically confirmed pre-HD and

    reported a significant reduction in amplitudeof the tibial SERP

    components, although only five participants were included

    (Beniczky et al., 2002). The authors indicated that the tibial

    SERP wasmore sensitivethan the medianSERP, which is more

    commonly investigated. These findings suggest that somato-

    sensory changes are highly specific prior to symptom onset,

    and both the median and the tibial nerve SERP need to be

    considered when investigating SERP in pre-HD.

    In summary, SERP studies in HD consistently show

    amplitude reductions of early components with significant

    correlations with CAG repeats and cognitive and behavioral

    symptoms. Based on preliminary data of early SERP changes

    prior to disease onset (Beniczky et al., 2002) and longitudinally

    (Ehle et al., 1984; Lefaucheur et al., 2006), SERP shows promise

    as a potential biomarker of bothdiseaseonsetand progression.

    5. Movement-related potentials inHuntington's disease

    Motor disturbance is a hallmark of HD, and analysis of 

    movement-related potentials (MRPs) enables the temporal

    breakdown of cortical activity involved in both movement

    preparation and execution (Colebatch, 2007). MRPs associated

    with movement preparation, performed, and imagined move-

    ments were significantly reduced in HD compared to controls in

    a simple sequential finger-tapping task guided by visual cues

    ( Johnson et al.,2001). No differences were foundwith movement

    execution, suggestive of normal primary motor cortex activity.

    Based on these results, the authors supported the proposal by

    Thompson et al.(1988) andPhillips et al.(1994) thatitmaybethe

    construction of the motor program for a particular movement

    ratherthan theproduction of theactual movement itself,which

    is affected in HD. Interestingly, Johnson et al. (2002) found that

    withan attentional strategyto internallytime andanticipatethe

    extinction of the cue, there wasa significant increase in the pre-

    movement activity in both HD and control groups. Without the

    attentional strategy, the control group, but not the HD group,

    stillproduced a rising pre-movement potential.The researchers

    suggested that HD patients may have deficient automatic

    control, reflected by a lack of pre-movement activity; the

    strategy may serve to place the task under attentional control.

    Georgiou et al. (1997) also found that a concurrent task, during 

    performance of a kinematic task, was likely to evoke a strategy

    which resulted in improved motor performance in HD com-

    pared to when there was no concurrenttask.On theotherhand,

    Beste et al. (2009a) found no differences in MRPs preceding the

    motor response in HD and controls, suggesting that neurophys-

    iological processes occurring during voluntary response execu-

    tion are normal in HD. However, MRPs after the responsediffered dramatically between the groups. After the motor 

    response, HD participants, but not controls, produced a second

    contralateral activation followed shortly by an ipsilateral

    activation over the motor area, which is normally inhibited.

    Only one study to date has investigated MRPs in pre-HD and

    found that despite absence of behavioral differences from

    controls, there was an increased inhibition of the ipsilateral

    hemisphere during right hand movements (Beste et al., 2009a).

    This positive MRP in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the effector 

    (in this case theright hand) is thought to reflectinhibition of the

    alternative response, mediated by transcallosal fibers targeting 

    GABAergic interneurons (Daskalakis et al., 2002; Ferbert et al.,

    1992). Pre-HD participants produced stronger inhibitory poten-

    tials than controls for the right dominant hand, suggesting that

    transcallosal inhibition and GABAergic neural transmission are

    increased in pre-HD (Beste et al., 2009a). Moreover, pre-HD

    individuals failed to exhibit a difference in reaction time

    between hands, whereas in controls the reaction time of the

    right hand was significantly shorter than that of the left hand

    (right-handedness scores were also comparable with controls).

    The authors proposed that compensatory processes in pre-HD,

    mediated via adenosine receptors, may help to maintain a

    normal level of task performance behaviorally. They explained

    that adenosine A2A  receptors are involved in the control of 

    movements (Chase et al., 2003; Svenningsson et al., 1999) and

    increased activity of the adenosine receptor system may

    stimulate GABAergic neurotransmission, leading to increased

    inhibition of the ipsilateral hemisphere and symmetrical

    pattern of reaction time. Indeed, several pieces of evidence

    indicate that adenosine A2A   receptors are implicated in HD

    pathogenesis (fora review, seeChenet al., 2007and Popoli et al.,

    2007). However, the roleof adenosine in compensatory process-

    es is not clear, and further studies are needed to clarify the

    complexities of adenosine A2A  receptor pharmacology and its

    relation to HD pathogenesis.

    Closely related to MRPs is the contingent negative variation

    (CNV),a steadypotential shift,thought to be related to sensory

    motor integration and planning or execution of voluntary

    movements (Andreassi, 2000). It is a slow negative potential

    occurring between two successive but mutually associated

    stimuli.  De Tommaso et al. (2007)  found that the early CNV

    was reduced in amplitude in HD compared with controls; this

    was accompanied by significantly prolonged reaction times.

    No differences were found in the late CNV. The early CNV is

    thought to be related more to attention and expectancy

    stimulus processing, while the late CNV is perhaps associated

    with cognition and motor preparation (Ikeda et al., 1997). The

    amplitude reduction of early CNV correlated significantly with

    delay in motor responses and bradykinesia. Based on these

    findings, the authors suggested that motor impairments in HD

    may be linked to a deficit in attention to external stimuli

    rather than a problem with motor preparation.

    183B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    8/18

    Dysfunction of motor regions and brain networks is

    confirmed with postmortem studies (de la Monte et al., 1988;

    Mann et al., 1993; Macdonald and Halliday, 2002; Diprospero et

    al., 2004), MRI (Rosas et al., 2008; Bigland et al., 2009;

    Peinemann et al., 2005), PET (Bartenstein et al., 1997) and

    TMS (Nardone et al., 2007; Schippling et al., 2009), with strong 

    evidence that degeneration occurs early and progressively

    increases with disease severity (Rosas et al., 2008). Althoughfewer in number, similar motor dysfunctions have been

    reported in pre-HD using fMRI (Kloppel et al., 2009), TMS

    (Schippling et al., 2009),PET(Feigin et al., 2006), and DTI (Rosas

    et al., 2006). MRPs indicate abnormalities prior ( Johnson et al.,

    2001) and subsequent to (Beste et al., 2009a) hand movements.

    However, motor impairments have also been attributed to

    dysfunction of attention (de Tommaso et al., 2007; Johnson et

    al., 2002). These results suggest that motor dysfunction in HD

    is complex and involves multiple facets, likely also to depend

    on stage of disease. In pre-HD, further cross-sectional and

    longitudinal MRP studies are needed.

    6. Long-latency potentials inHuntington's disease

    Long-latency potentials take more time to develop than sensory

    and motor potentials and are strongly influenced by subjective

    factors (Andreassi, 2000). One of the most commonly investi-

    gated long-latency potentials is the P3. The P3 potential has

    been associated with a variety of cognitive processes related to

    information processing, including decision making, signal

    probability, context updating, attention, discrimination, uncer-

    tain resolution, stimulus relevance, and information delivery

    (Andreassi, 2000). Using oddball paradigms, whereby a target

    stimulus is presented amongst more frequent standard back-

    ground stimuli, several studies have found HD patients were

    significantly delayed in P3 latencies to auditory (Filipovic et al.,

    1990; Goodin andAminoff,1986;Homberg et al.,1986; Rosenberg 

    etal., 1985)andvisual(Rosenberget al.,1985) stimuli,even in the

    absence of behavioral differences to controls (Rosenberg et al.,

    1985). As discussed earlier, although significant differences

    between HD and controls were found in AERP components,

    Homberg et al. (1986)  found that the difference between the

    groupswas most markedfor theP3. This suggests that cognitive

    processing of auditory stimuli is more strongly affected than

    sensory processing. Less consistent data are available for 

    correlations of P3 to HD symptomatology.   Homberg et al.

    (1986) found that P3 latency was significantly correlated with

    psychometric measures including selective attention, short-

    term memory, and vulnerability to distraction. On the contrary,

    Rosenberg et al. (1985) found that P3 latency did not correlate

    with cerebral or caudate atrophy or abnormalities in neuropsy-

    chologicaltesting.Movingaway fromoddball paradigms,Munte

    et al. (1997)   employed a visual search paradigm to better 

    correlate electrophysiological parameters with cognitive defi-

    cits known to be affected in HD. HD patients were slower and

    less accurate in responding to target stimuli and corresponding 

    electrophysiological data indicate the P3 for target stimuli were

    attenuated in HD. The researchers suggested that HD patients

    lack ability to move and engage the attentional spotlight across

    the visual field.

    The complexity of the P3 renders application to a variety of 

    parameters. Consistent with findings of olfactory impairment

    inHD(Bacon Moore et al., 1999; Bylsma et al., 1997;Hamiltonet

    al., 1999; Lazic et al., 2007;Moberg and Doty, 1997; Nordinet al.,

    1995; Pirogovsky et al., 2007),   Wetter et al. (2005)   found a

    significant delay of approximately 250 ms on the P3 compo-

    nent of the olfactory ERP (OERP) in HD compared with controls,

    confirming deficits in the ability to process and classifyolfactory stimuli. The delay of the P3 correlated with CAG

    repeat length and UHDRS motor scores. As well as being more

    robust than AERP latency, the P3 OERP latency delay was

    substantially larger in effect size and more accurate at

    classifying individuals with or without HD than psychophys-

    ical and cognitive measures. The authors suggested that

    compromise of the orbitofrontal cortex as the P3 OERP

    abnormality was strongest at frontal sites. However, they did

    not perform source localization techniques to confirm this.

    MRI data indicate that olfactory deficits in HD were signifi-

    cantly correlated with degeneration of the entorhinal cortex,

    thalamus, parahippocampal gyrus, and caudate nucleus

    (Barrios et al., 2007). These areas have been previously

    shown to be involved in olfaction in healthy individuals

    (Levy et al., 1997; Savic et al., 2000).

    Similar P3 OERP deficits were also reported in pre-HD

    patients who were on average 4.7 years from predicted likely

    onset of diagnosis (Wetter et al., 2006, as cited in Pirogovsky et

    al., 2007). Normal latencies were found for earlier sensory

    OERP components, suggesting that cognitive processing of 

    olfactory stimuli deteriorates before sensory processing of 

    odors. Consistent with findings in symptomatic HD, Wetter et

    al. (2006, as cited in Pirogovsky et al., 2007) found that OERP

    amplitudes were strongly correlated to CAG repeats, particu-

    larly at frontal electrode sites. The authors consequently

    suggested that the olfactory deficits in pre-HD may be related

    to dysfunction in the frontal cortex or in frontal–striatal

    circuits.

    The P3 has also been used to examine processes related to

    inhibition in Go/No-go paradigms (Beste et al., 2008c). The go/

    no-go paradigm involves a continuously presented series of 

    stimuli composed of frequent “go” cues to which participants

    respond to as rapidly as possible and infrequent  “no-go” cues

    to which participants do not respond. No-go stimuli elicit a

    fronto-central, negative–positive ERP complex, labeled as

    Nogo-N2 and Nogo-P3 (Bokura et al., 2001; Falkenstein et al.,

    1999). TheNogo-N2 is thought to be associated with pre-motor 

    inhibitory processes (Falkenstein et al., 1999) or conflict

    processes (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003) and the Nogo-P3 is likely

    to be related to evaluation of the inhibitory processes (Band

    and van Boxtel, 1999; Roche et al., 2005). In line with other 

    behavioral studies (Aron et al., 2003; Fielding et al., 2006), Beste

    et al. (2008c)   found HD participants to be significantly

    impaired in their ability to inhibit responses compared to

    controls. Corresponding electrophysiological data indicate

    there was a strong selective attenuation of the Nogo-P3, but

    notthe Nogo-N2, in HD compared to controls.Reduction of the

    Nogo-P3 was found to correlate with CAG index, with higher 

    CAG repeat length associated with stronger attenuation of the

    Nogo-P3. Consistent with reports of dysfunctional anterior 

    cingulate cortex (ACC) in HD (Bartenstein et al., 1997; Beste et

    al., 2006, 2007; Reading et al., 2004; van Dellen et al., 2005 ),

    184    B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    9/18

    source localization suggests significant hypo-activations in

    the prefrontal cortex and ACC (Beste et al., 2008c). As the ACC

    and basal ganglia are intimately interconnected (Chudasama

    and Robbins, 2006),  Beste et al. (2008c)  suggested that basal

    ganglia damage may result in partial dysfunction of neural

    circuits and subsequent impairment of ACC, leading to an

    attenuated Nogo-P3 and not Nogo-N2. This is the only ERP

    study addressing inhibitory processesin HD,and no studies todate have been conducted in pre-HD. Future electrophysio-

    logical research in pre-HD is encouraged, with recent data

    suggesting that pre-HD individuals may have a deficit in

    inhibitory control, being on average three times more likely

    than control participants to commit anticipation errors

    (Farrow et al., 2007). In an fMRI study, automatic inhibitory

    motor control in HD was related to significant modulation of 

    both the caudate and thalamus (Aron et al., 2003).

    Several studies have shown electrophysiological differ-

    ences related to processing of errors in HD compared to

    controls (Beste et al., 2006, 2008a,b). Error processing is

    reflected in the ERP error negativity (Ne) or error-related

    negativity (ERN) (Falkenstein et al., 1999), which occurs as a

    negative deflection just after an incorrect response. The Ne

    has been hypothesized to reflect error detection (Falkenstein

    et al., 1999; Leuthold and Sommer, 1999) and shown to depend

    on the dopamine (DA) system and medial prefrontal areas,

    particularly the ACC (for a review, see   Holroyd and Coles,

    2002).   Holroyd and Coles (2002)   propose that following an

    error, in a reaction time task, the mesencephalic dopamine

    system conveys a negative reinforcement learning signal to

    the frontal cortex, where it generates the ERN by disinhibiting 

    the apical dendrites of motor neurons in theACC. Accordingly,

    Schulz and colleagues have suggested that the responsesseen

    in the dopamine neurons in prediction of the goodness of 

    ongoing events might serve as error signals (for a review, see

    Schulz and Dickinson, 2000). Compared with controls and pre-

    HD, symptomatic HD patients exhibited a reduction in Ne

    amplitude for error trials, despite no significant differences in

    false reaction time or frequency of errors (Beste et al., 2006,

    2008d, 2009b). As HD is accompanied by alterations in the DA

    system with a reduction in D1 and D2 receptor density (Pavese

    et al., 2003) and as Ne is associated with the DA system, the

    authors suggested impaired functioning of striatal DA system

    to be a possible cause for Ne reduction in HD.

    Contrary to findings in symptomatic HD, no amplitude

    differences in Ne were found in pre-HD (Beste et al., 2009a).

    Instead, data in pre-HD suggest a selective impairment in Ne.

    The Ne consists of two subcomponents: error-specific moni-

    toring at the cognitive level and motor response monitoring,

    expressed via the delta frequency band and the theta frequent

    band, respectively (Yordanova et al., 2004). Time-frequency

    decomposition of the Ne revealed that the pre-HD group

    showed a significant selective increase in power of the delta

    band of the Ne compared to controls (Beste et al., 2007). No

    differences were seen in the theta band. These results suggest

    that pre-HD differed with respect to behavioral/cognitive

    monitoring, but not in motor response monitoring compared

    to controls.In line with studies from Predict-HD (Paulsen et al.,

    2008), the authors found the increase in power in the delta

    band of the Ne was stronger in those pre-HD participants with

    an earlier estimated age of disease onset. Although the Ne has

    been consistently shown to be reduced in amplitude in

    symptomatic HD, no data are available on time-frequency

    decomposition of Ne to enable cross-sectional examination of 

    Ne changes with disease progression.

    Finally, the N400, an ERP related to memory processes,

    particularly in the establishment and retrieval of memory

    (Andreassi, 2000), is also affected in HD.  Munte et al. (1997)

    employed a word-recognition memory task in which partici-pants were required to decide whether the word was a first

    presentation (new word) or a second presentation (old word).

    HD participants showed recognition accuracy deficits as well

    as reduction of the N400 component, suggesting that they

    engaged in less semantic elaboration and integration. Another 

    closely related ERP is the P600, a measure of episodic memory

    encoding, which are found to be affected in early stages of 

    cognitive impairment ( Jackson and Snyder, 2008) but has not

    been investigated in HD.

    7. Animal studies of HD and electrophysiology

    Animal toxin and genetic models are crucial to the ongoing 

    investigation of HD pathogenesis as well as in the develop-

    ment and implementation of treatments and novel therapeu-

    tic strategies. Animal models include toxin models of HD, for 

    example, instrastriatal injection of quinolinic acid (QA) and

    genetic models such as R6/2 transgenic mice and knock-in

    mouse models (for a review, see Ferrante, 2009). EEG enables

    understanding of neuronal information processing in HD mice

    models, and there is ample published evidence of altered

    electrophysiological processing in animal models of HD. A

    complete review of EEG studies in animal studies is beyond

    the scope of this review; however, a few studies will be

    reviewed to highlightanother possible role of theutility of EEG

    in HD research.

    A number of studies usingR6/2 transgenic andHD knock-in

    (KI) line have identified deficits in information processing at

    the single-neuron and population levels. At the single-unit

    level, R6/2, but not KI mice, showed significantly elevated

    spontaneous firing rates and spike-train variability, an index

    of overall activation, compared to wild-type controls, in

    pyramidal neurons within the prefrontal cortex (Walker et

    al., 2008) and in medium size spiny neurons (MSSNs) (Miller et

    al., 2008). Reduced bursting, associated with information

    transmission and synaptic plasticity (Lisman, 1997; Izhikevich

    et al., 2003), was also noted in the R6/2 compared to wild-type

    controls (Miller et al., 2008; Walker et al., 2008) and is

    consistent with compromised cortical plasticity in HD (Cum-

    mings et al., 2006, 2007; Crupi et al., 2008; Mazarakis et al.,

    2005). The authors corroborate that abnormalities at the

    single-unit level may reflect symptom severity since differ-

    ences were only found in R6/2 mice, who exhibited robust

    behavioral signs, and not KI mice who express relatively mild

    behavioral signs (Ferrante, 2009). Both R6/2 and KI mice

    exhibited impaired activity at the neuronal population level,

    as indicated by reduced synchronous activity between simul-

    taneously recorded neurons (Walker et al., 2008) and reduced

    correlated spikes (Miller et al., 2008), suggesting reduced

    functional connectivity of networks. As abnormalities were

    consistent across both models, Miller et al. (2008) and Walker 

    185B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    10/18

    et al. (2008) suggested that activity at the population level is

    perhaps more reflectiveof the underlying cellular pathologyof 

    HD than firing rate or bursting at the single-neuron level.

    In line with the above electrophysiological abnormalities in

    mice models of HD, increasing evidence supports that

    dysfunction of MSSNs and corticostriatal inputs is likely to

    trigger onset and progression of the behavioral phenotype in

    HD (for a review, see Cepeda et al., 2007). Changes in neuronalpathways can be elucidated with EEG. One of the first signs of 

    corticostriatal dysfunction is that symptomatic R6/2 and

    Tg100 transgenic mice require a significant increase in

    stimulus intensity in order to evoke an excitatory post-

    synaptic potential in MSSNs (Klapstein et al., 2001; Laforet et

    al., 2001). In addition to alterations in evoked synaptic

    responses,   Cepeda et al. (2003)   also report a transient and

    progressive reduction in frequency of spontaneous excitatory

    post-synaptic currents in striatal MSSNs in slices from R6/2

    mice. The reduction was apparent with onset of overt

    symptoms (5–7 weeks) and most pronounced in older mice

    (11–15 weeks), when symptoms were severe. The authors

    suggested a progressive disconnection between the striatum

    and its cortical inputs (Cepeda et al., 2003).

    Corroborating EEG findings in patients with HD, cortical

    electrophysiological studies in the QA-lesioned rats report a

    marked reduction in voltage amplitude in the frontal cortex

    (Popoli et al., 1994). More recent reports indicate that

    intrastriatal QA has specific alterations in relative EEG power 

    distribution in therat frontal cortexsimilar to that observed in

    HD patients (Reggio et al., 1999). QA-lesioned rats showed

    significant reduction in voltage amplitude and in total EEG

    power, increased percent delta power, and decreased percent

    alpha power. Reduced amplitude of parietal and frontal

    somatosensory potentials has also been reported in QA-

    lesioned rats (Schwarz et al., 1992). Taken together, these

    findings support growing evidence that phenotypes from HD

    mouse models closely correlate with human symptomatology

    and offer new ways to validate biomarkers of disease onset,

    progression, and effectiveness of CNS drug therapies. Cur-

    rently very few affective and cognitive assessments have been

    validated in mouse models. EEG and ERPs offer additional

    measures of intracellular dynamics and neuronal and cortical

    information processing in both human and animal models

    which could help yield common biomarkers between species

    of both disease onset and progression, invaluable for future

    clinical drug trials.

    8. Conclusions and future directions

    Tables 1 and 2 report the key EEG and ERP measures and

    deficits in symptomatic and pre-HD, respectively. In sum-

    mary, EEG and ERP research to date has revealed sensory,

    motor, and cognitive abnormalities in both pre-HD and

    symptomatic HD. The majority of HD studies conducted

    prior to localization of the Huntington gene in 1993

    compared sensory ERPs and P3 potential using simple

    oddball paradigms. Although these studies suggest sensory

    and cognitive processing deficits in HD, the electrophysio-

    logical data are extremely weak and inconsistent. Since

    1993, most studies have utilized the sensitivity and speci-

    ficity of EEG/ERP and have tended to focus on processes

    known to be affected in HD. Consistent with what we know

    about the HD phenotype as well as abnormalities noted from

    neuroimaging studies, ERP data indicate auditory, visual,

    somatosensory, and motor electrophysiological deficits.

    Furthermore, specific cognitive dysfunction has been

    reported in sensory motor integration, inhibitory processes,

    processing of errors, and memory processes. Only eightstudies have been conducted to date in genetically con-

    firmed pre-HD, indicating visual, somatosensory, and motor 

    electrophysiological abnormalities. Moreover, cognitive def-

    icits have been reported in olfaction, error processing, and

    working memory. One of the largest limitations of EEG/ERP

    research in HD is that very few studies have replicated

    abnormal findings, limiting reliability and generalizability of 

    results, and only two have employed a longitudinal design

    (Ehle et al., 1984; Lefaucheur et al., 2006). Future studies need

    to do much more than simply show significant differences

    between groups; they should build and extend current

    knowledge and investigate longitudinally the most sensitive

    measures over sufficient time to track the utility of 

    electrophysiological measures as potential markers of dis-

    ease onset and progression. Currently, comparisons across

    studies are difficult due to lack of standardization in details

    of patient information including CAG repeat length, duration

    of disease, and medication status. Spectrum of the disease

    varies across studies and may account for the mixed

    findings. Severity (and/or stage) of disease must be clearly

    defined since the degenerative process can span many years.

    Moreover, EEG and ERP data are complex and often generate

    a wealth of data. As such, standardized reporting of 

    equipment type, electrophysiological and statistical analy-

    ses, and results are essential for comparison across studies.

    Few studies have reported effect sizes which would facilitate

    comparison among studies and help to identify robust and

    reliable markers, and small sample sizes limit statistical

    power. Better standardization of study protocol with much

    larger sample sizes is required to better understand electro-

    physiological abnormalities in pre-HD and symptomatic HD.

    Of the key measures listed in   Tables 1 and 2, several

    variables show substantial sensitivity to quantify manifest

    disease. We recommend that the following should be priori-

    tized for longitudinal research in pre-HD as these measures

    show potential as reliable and sensitive measures of disease

    onset and progression.

    1) Somatosensory ERPs. In line with MRI (Rosas et al., 2008)

    and PET (Boecker et al., 1999) studies, SERP studies

    consistently show amplitude reductions of early compo-

    nents which significantly correlate with CAG repeats and

    cognitive and behavioral symptoms. Preliminary studies in

    pre-HD individuals suggest early SERP changes prior to

    disease diagnosis.

    2) Olfactory P3. Olfactory deficits in HD have been confirmed

    with MRI (Barrios et al., 2007). The olfactory P3 had the

    largest effect size and was more robust than other 

    electrophysiological, psychophysical, and cognitive mea-

    sures(Wetter et al., 2005). Similar P3 OERP deficits were also

    reported in pre-HD (Wetter et al., 2006, as cited in

    Pirogovsky et al., 2007).

    186   B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    11/18

    3) Error negativity—

    Ne. A number of studies consistentlyshow reduced amplitude of the Ne potential in HD during 

    processing of errors (Beste et al., 2006, 2008a, 2009b). Subtle

    deficits have also beendemonstrated in pre-HD individuals

    (Beste et al., 2009a).

    4) Movement-related potentials and CNV. MRPs have been

    shown to be abnormal in both symptomatic ( Johnson et al.,

    2001; Beste et al., 2009a,b) and pre-HD(Beste et al., 2009a,b).

    Sensory motor integration via the CNV is also worth

    exploring to ascertain possible mechanisms of motor 

    dysfunction in HD.

    5) Quantitative EEG at rest and during cognitive processing.

    Abnormalities in QEEG (particular alpha power) are

    reported in both pre-HD and symptomatic HD (Bellotti etal., 2004; Bylsma et al., 1994; de Tommaso et al., 2003;

    Streletz et al., 1990), with suggestions of a compensatory

    network in pre-HD during a working memory task (van der 

    Hiele et al., 2007). Further research is needed to clarify the

    utility of EEG during manifestation and progression as well

    as its phenomenology during other cognitive, motor, and

    psychomotor functioning.

    Despite the recommendations, it should be noted that

    there are a number of other electrophysiological parameters

    yet to be explored in HD, such as for example time course of 

    emotional processing; this will help to clarify behavioralreports of impaired emotional recognition ( Johnson et al.,

    2007).

    The studies summarized in this review highlight for the

    first time the potential of EEG and ERP measures as possible

    biomarkers in HD.  Michell et al. (2004)  recommend a list of 

    ideal biomarker characteristics, for example, that the bio-

    marker should have first degree association with the clinical

    trait, that it should be sensitive to reflect small changes, and

    relatively cheap and non-invasive (for a comprehensive

    review, see Michell et al., 2004). In line with  Michell et al.'s

    (2004)   recommendations, EEG is a direct measure of brain

    activity, without reliance on intermediate variables, and

    minimizes risk of secondary variables. It has millisecondtemporal resolution which can sensitively reflect even small

    changes in disease phenotype. It is inexpensive, non-invasive,

    and available in many hospital and research centers world-

    wide. Importantly, in thecontextof HD,EEG andERP measures

    could serve as potential biological biomarkers to quantify

    onset of manifest disease and monitor rate of progression and

    clinical response to therapeutic intervention. Each of these

    will be discussed in turn. Firstly, in animal models, EEG can

    elucidate changes of intracellular dynamics at the single-

    neuron level, functional connectivity of networks and neuro-

    nal pathways. Further research into neuronal information

    Table 2 – Key EEG and ERP markers and findings in pre-diagnostic HD with references.

    EEG/ERP makers Key Findings Key references

    EEG Alpha power (resting state) Suppression of alpha activity   de Tommaso et al. (2003)

    Alpha power (during working memory) Reduction of alpha activity

    during working memory

    van der Hiele et al. (2007)

    Visual ERP N1 Reduced amplitude   Beste et al. (2008b)

    Somatosensory ERP Early SERP components Reduced amplitude of the

    tibial SERP components

    Beniczky et al. (2002)

    Movement-related potentials MRPs after the response Increased inhibition of the

    ipsilateral hemisphere

    Beste et al. (2009a,b)

    Long-latency potentials P3 (olfactory) Delay of approximately 250 ms   Beste et al. (2009a,b)

    Ne (error processing) Increase in power of the delta band   Beste et al. (2007)

    Table 1 – Key EEG and ERP markers and findings in symptomatic HD with references.

    EEG/ERP makers Key findings Key references

    EEG Alpha power (resting state) Suppression of alpha activity   Bellotti et al. (2004); Bylsma et al. (1994);

    de Tommaso et al. (2003); Streletz et al. (1990)

    Auditory ERP P50 potential Reduced amplitude and

    prolonged latency

    Uc et al. (2003)

    Visual ERP N1 Reduced amplitude   Antal et al. (2003); Beste et al. (2008b)

    Somatosensory ERP Early components Reduced amplitude   Ehle et al. (1984)⁎ Lefaucheur et al. (2002, 2006)⁎

    Movement-related potentials Movement preparation Reduced amplitude   Johnson et al. (2001)

    Potentials after the response Lack of inhibition   Beste et al. (2009a,b)

    Long-latency potentials Contingent negative variation Reduced amplitude   De Tommaso et al. (2007)

    Olfactory P3 Significant delay of  

    approximately 250 ms

    Wetter et al. (2005)

    No-go P3 (inhibitory processes) Reduced amplitude   Beste et al. (2008d)

    Ne (error processing) Reduced amplitude   Beste et al. (2006, 2008a, 2009)

    N400 (memory processes) Reduced amplitude   Munte et al. (1997)

    ⁎ Longitudinal study.

    187B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7 –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    12/18

    processing is essential as there is increasing evidence for 

    dysfunction of neurons and neuronal circuits that precede

    symptoms (Cepeda et al., 2007). In humans, ongoing EEG and

    averaged ERPs can provide millisecond data on changes in

    time course of information processing with onset of manifest

    disease. Importantly, like fMRI, both EEG and ERP can be used

    to elucidate processing specific to sensory, motor, and

    cognitive processes which may be more sensitive to theearliest changes in HD. Secondly, the powerful time resolution

    of EEG/ERP enables detection of subtle abnormalities in pre-

    HD which could be used as a tool to monitor rate of disease

    progression. Only two ERP studies have been conducted

    longitudinally; both corroborate that somatosensory ERPs

    progressively deteriorate over time and correlate with disease

    severity (Ehle et al., 1984; Lefaucheur et al., 2006). Finally,

    electrophysiological biomarkers can also be applied to mon-

    itor clinical responses to therapeutic intervention. For exam-

    ple, improvements of somatosensory ERPs have been found in

    small preliminary clinical drug trials (Bachoud-Levi et al.,

    2000; Bloch et al., 2004). These studies indicate that ERP

    measures change linearly with clinical stability of symptoms

    following treatment. Furthermore, increasing studies indicate

    that the loudness-dependent auditory ERP and resting state

    QEEG can help identify particular medications that are most

    likely to lead to a response and/or remission in major 

    depression (for a review, see Leuchter et al., 2009). However,

    before this is possible in HD, more studies are required to

    validate, clarify consistency, reliability, and replicability of 

    these markers in order to better understand the pattern of 

    electrophysiological deterioration over time and with treat-

    ment. At present, existing clinical drug trials in manifest HD

    disease (for reviews, see Mason and Barker, 2009; Mestre et al.,

    2009) are currently based on changes over time in the total

    functional capacity (TFC) score (Shoulson, 1981). We urgently

    require morereliableand sensitive biomarkers of disease onset

    and progression, and with much better statistical properties

    thanthe TFCscore.Currently,however, there areveryfew EEG/

    ERP longitudinal studies in manifest HD. This makes it

    challenging to recommend, or make an estimate, as to which

    measure/method, as described in this review, would yield the

    most promising properties that would make it more sensitive,

    less variable, and/or show greater magnitude of change over 

    time than the current TFC score. We do however recommend

    that in order to derive a better measure, future planning of 

    longitudinal study designs should, as the first requirement,

    perform effect size analyses on existing cross-sectional EEG

    data to determine which measure/s yields the highest effect

    size between groups; these measure/s could then be incorpo-

    rated into large-scale longitudinal studies to ascertain the

    most sensitive and reliable biomarker of progression.

    There is great variability in the expression of even the most

    characteristic features of HD. Specific clinical, behavioral, and

    biological biomarkers of disease phenotype with concomitant

    underlying neurophysiology may be required at all stages of 

    disease. For example, the temporal variability in activation

    patterns may indicate critical points during the pre-HD neuro-

    degenerative process, involving the onset or worsening of more

    than one pathological process (e.g., axon or myelin degenera-

    tion, neuronal dysfunction or death) that could potentially be

    captured with electrophysiological techniques (Bohanna et al.,

    2008; Georgiou-Karistianis, 2009). The wide range of ERP

    parameters is well suited to investigating subtle and specific

    dysfunctions in HD across a number of domains. A noteworthy

    advantage of EEG/ERP is its ability to be applied to both humans

    and animals, enabling parallel investigation across species.

    A common limitation of current EEG/ERP research is the

    difficulty determining the exact locations of cortical genera-

    tors from the scalp data. A new approach, which may help toovercome this limitation, is the combination of EEG/ERP

    simultaneously with fMRI (for a review, see   Mulert et al.,

    2008). Simultaneous acquisition of both EEG and fMRI is now

    gradually more accessible (Bregadze and Lavric, 2006; Debener 

    et al., 2005; Eichele et al., 2005); however, the acquisition of 

    ERPs concurrent with fMRI in cognitive paradigms is yet to be

    explored in HD. These methods will enable enhanced infor-

    mation on spatial localization of brain structures as well as

    precise time course of neural activity in HD. This will not only

    accelerate our understanding of mechanisms associated with

    disease onset and progression but could also identify sensitive

    biomarkers to test efficacy of therapeutic intervention.

    R E F E R E N C E S

    Abbruzzese, G., Berardelli, A., 2003. Sensorimotor integration inmovement disorders. Mov. Disord. 18, 231–240.

    Abbruzzese, G., Dall'Agata, D., Morena, M., Reni, L., Favale, E., 1990.Abnormalities of parietal and preolandic somatosensoryevoked potentials in Huntington's disease. Electroencephalogr.Clin. Neurophysiol. 77, 340–346.

    Andreassi, J.L., 2000. Psychophysiology: Human Behaviour &Physiological Response, Fourth Edition. Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates, London.

    Andrews, T.C., Brooks, D.J., 1998. Advances in the understanding 

    of early Huntington's disease using the functional imaging techniques of PET and SPET. Mol. Med. Today 4, 532–539.

    Antal, A., Beniczky, S., Kincses, T.Z., Jakab, K., Benedek, G., Vecsei,L., 2003. Perceptual categorization is impaired in Huntington'sdisease: an electrophysiological study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn.Disord. 16, 187–192.

    Arnulf, I., Nielsen, J., Lohmann, E., Schieffer, J., Wild, E., Jennum, P.,Konofal, E., Walker, M., Oudiette, D., Tabrizi, S., Durr, A., 2008.Rapid eye movement sleep disturbances in Huntingtondisease. Arch. Neurol. 65, 482–488.

    Aron, A.R., Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 2003. Distractibilityduring selection-for-action: differential deficits inHuntington's disease and following frontal lobe damage.Neuropsychologia 41, 1137–1147.

    Aylward, E.H., 2007. Change in MRI striatal volumes as abiomarker in preclinical Huntington's disease. Brain Res. Bull.72, 152–158.

    Aylward, E.H., Codori, A., Rosenblatt, A., Sheer, M., Brandt, J., Stine,O.C., Barta, P.E., Pearlson, G.D., Ross, C.A., 2000. Rate of caudateatrophy in presymptomatic and symptomatic stages of Huntington's disease. Mov. Disord. 15, 552–560.

    Bachoud-Levi, A., Remy, P., Nguyen, J., Brugieres, P., Lafaucheur, J.,Bourdet, C., Baudic, S., Gaura, V., Maison, P., Haddad, B., Boisee,M., Grandmougin, T., Jeny, R., Bartolomeo, P., Barba, G.D.,Gegos, J., Lisovoski, F., Ergis, A., Pailhous, E., Cesaro, P.,Hantraye, P., Peschanski, M., 2000. Motor and cognitiveimprovements in patients with Huntington's disease after neural transplantation. Lancet 356, 1975–1979.

    Bacon Moore, A.S., Paulsen, J.S., Murphy, C., 1999. A test of odor fluency in patients with Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease.

     J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 21, 341–351.

    188   B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S 6 4 ( 2 0 1 0 ) 1 7 7  –  1 9 4

  • 8/9/2019 3 Caama+¦o Mayrena - psycho_1

    13/18

    Band, G.P., van Boxtel, G.J., 1999. Inhibitory motor control in stopparadigms: review and reinterpretation of neural mechanisms.Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 101, 179–211.

    Barrios, F.A., Gonzalez, L., Favila, R., Alonso, M.E., Salgado, P.M.,Diaz, R., Fernandez-Ruiz, J., 2007. Olfaction andneurodegeneration in HD. NeuroReport 18 (1), 73–76.

    Bartenstein, P., Weindl, A., Spiegel, S., Boecker, H., Wenzel, R.,Ceballos-Baumann, A.O., Minoshima, S., Conrad, B., 1997.

    Central motor processing in Huntington's disease. A PET study.Brain 120, 1553–1567.

    Basar, E., Schurmann, M., Basar-Eroglu, C., Karakas, S., 1997. Alphaoscillations in brain functioning: an integrative theory. Int. J.Psychophysiol. 26, 5–29.

    Beal, M.F., Ferrante, R.J., 2004. Experimental therapeutics intransgenic mouse models of Huntington's disease. Nat. Rev.Neurosci. 5, 373–384.

    Bellotti, R., De Carlo, F., Massagra, R., de Tommaso, M., Sciruicchio,V., 2004. Topographic classification of EEG patterns inHuntington's disease. Neurol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 37, 1–4.

    Beniczky, S., Keri, S., Antal, A., Jakab, K., Nagy, H., Benedek, G., Janka, Z., Vecsei, L., 2002. Somatosensory evoked potentialscorrelate with genetics in Huntington's disease. Neuroreport 3,2296–2298.

    Berrios, G.E., Wagle, A.C., Markova, I.S., Wagle, S.A., Ho, L.W.,Rubinsztein, D.C., Whittaker, J., Ffrench-Constant, C., Kershaw,A., Rosser, A., Bak, T., Hodges, J.R., 2001. Psychiatric symptomsand CAG repeats in neurologically asymptomatic Huntington'sdisease gene carriers. Psychiatry Res. 102, 217–225.

    Berrios, G.E., Wagle, A.C., Markova, I.S., Wagle, S.A., Rosser, A.,Hodges, J.R., 2002. Psychiatric symptoms in neurologicallyclinical Huntington's disease gene carriers: a comparison withgene negative at risk subjects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 105,224–230.

    Beste, C., Saft, C., Konrad, C., Andrich, J., Gold, R., Falkenstein, M.,2006. Error processing in Huntington's disease.PLoS ONE1, 1–5.

    Beste, C., Saft, C., Yordanova, J., Andrich, J., Gold, R., Falkenstein,M., Kolev, V., 2007. Functional compensation or pathology incortico-subcortical interactions in preclinical Huntington's

    disease. Neuropsychologia 45, 2922–2930.Beste, C., Saft, C., Gunturkun, O., Falkenstein, M., 2008a. Increase

    cognitive functioning in symptomatic Huntington's disease asrevealed by behavioral and event-related potentials indices of auditory sensory memory and attention. J. Neurosci. 28,11695–11702.

    Beste, C., Saft, C., Konrad, C., Andrich, J., Gold, R., Falkenstein, M.,2008b. Stimulus-response compatibility in Huntington'sdisease: a cognitive-neurophysiological analysis

     J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1213–1223.Beste, C., Saft, C., Konrad, C., Andrich, J., Gold, R., Falkenstein, M.,

    2008c. Response inhibition in Huntington's disease—a studyusing ERPs and sLORETA. Neuropsychologia 46, 1290–1297.

    Beste, C., Saft, C., Konrad, C., Andrich, J., Habbel, A., Schepers, I., Jansen, A., Pfleiderer, B., Falkenstein, M., 2008d. Levels of error processing in Huntington's disease: a combined study using event-related potentials and voxel-based morphometry. Hum.Brain Mapp. 29, 121–130.

    Beste, C., Konrad, C., Saft, C., Ukas, T., Andrich, J., Pfeiderer, B.,Hausmann, M., Falkenstein, M., 2009a. Alterations in voluntarymovement executionin Huntington's disease are related to thedominant motor system—evidence from event-relatedpotentials. Exp. Neurol. 216, 148–157.

    Beste, C., Willemssen, R., Saft, C., Falkenstein, M., 2009b. Error processing in normal aging and in basal ganglia disorders.Neuroscience 159, 143–149.

    Bigland, K.M., Ross, C.A., Langbehn, D.R., Aylward, E.H., Stout, J.C.,Queller, S., Carlossi, N.E., Duff, K., Beglinger, L.J., Paulsen, J.S.,2009. Motor abnormalities in premanifest persons withHuntington's disease: the PREDICT-HD study. Mov. Disord. 24,1763–1772.

    Biomarkers Definitions Working Group, 2001. Biomarkers andsurrogate endpoints: preferred definitions and conceptualframework. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 69, 89–95.

    Blackmore, L., Simpson, S.A., Crawford, J.R., 1995. Cognitiveperformance in UK sample of presymptomatic people carrying the gene for Huntington's disease. J. Med. Genet. 32, 358–362.

    Blekher, T.M., Yee, R.D., Kirkwood, S.C., Hake, A.M., Stout, J.C.,Weaver, M.R., Foroud, T.M., 2004. Oculomotor control in

    asymptomatic and recently diagnosed individuals with thegenetic marker for Huntington's disease. Vis. Res. 44,2729–2736.

    Bloch, J., Bachoud-Levi, A.C., Deglon, N., Lefaucheur, J.P., Winkel,L., Palfi, S., Nguyen, J.P., Bourdet, C., Gaura, V., Remy, P.,Brugieres, P., Boisse, M.F., Baudic, S., Cesaro, P., Hantraye, P.,Aebischer, P., Peschanski, M., 2004. Neuroprotective genetherapy for Huntington's disease using polymer-encapsulatedcells engineered to secrete human ciliary neurotrophic factor:results of a phase I study. Hum. Gene Ther. 15, 968–975.

    Boecker, H., Ceballos-Baumann, A., Bartenstein, P., Weindl, A.,Siebner, H.R., Fassbender, T., Munz, F., Schwaiger, M., Conrad,B., 1999. Sensory processing in Parkinson's and Huntington'sdisease: investigations with 3D H(2)(15)O-PET. Brain 122,1651–1665.

    Bohanna, I., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Hannan, A.J., Egan, G.F.,2008. Magnetic resonance imaging as an approach towardsidentifying neuropathological biomarkers for Huntington'sdisease. Brain Res. Rev. 58, 209–225.

    Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., Kobayashi, S., 2001. Electrophysiologicalcorrelates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin.Neurophysiol. 112, 2224–2232.

    Bollen, E.L., Arts, R.J., Ross, R.A., van der Velde, E.A., Buruma, O.J.,1985. Somatosensory evoked potentials in Huntington'schorea. Electroencephologr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 62, 235–240.

    Bollen, E., Arts, R., Ross, R., van der Velde, E.A., Buruma, O.J., 1986.Brain reflexes and brainstem auditory evoked responses inHuntington's chorea. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.Psychiatry 49, 313–315.

    Brandt, J., Shpritz, B., Codori, A.M., Margolis, R., Rosenblatt, A.,2002. Neuropsychological manifestations of the genetic

    mutation for Huntington's disease in presymptomaticindividuals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 918–924.

    Bregadze, N., Lavric, A., 2006. ERP differences with vs. withoutconcurrent fMRI. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 54–59.

    Brotini, S., Gigli, G.L., 2004. Epidemiology and clinical features of sleep disorder in extrapyramidal disease. Sleep Med. 5,169–179.

    Burke, W.J., Miller, J.P., Rubin, E.H., Morris, J.C., Coben, L.A.,Duchek, J., Wittels, I.G., Berg, L., 1988. Reliability of theWashington University clinical dementia rating. Arch. Neurol.45, 31–32.

    Bylsma, F.W., Peyser, C.E., Folstein, S.E., Folstein, M.F., Ross, C.,Brandt, J., 1994. EEG power spectra in Huntington's disease:clinical and neuropsychological correlates. Neuropsychologia32, 137–150.

    Bylsma, F.W., Moberg, P.J., Doty, R.L., Brandt, J., 1997. Odor identification in Huntington's disease patients andasymptomatic gene carriers. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci.9, 598–600.

    Campodonico, J.R., Codori, A.M., Brandt, J., 1996.Neuropsychologicalstability over 2 years in asymptomatic carriers of theHuntington's disease mutation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry61, 621–624.

    Campodonico, J.R., Aylward, B., Codori, A., Young, C., Krafft, L.,Magdalinsky, M., Ranen, N., Slavney, R.P., Brandt, J., 1998.When does Huntington's disease begin? J. Int. Neuropsychol.Soc. 4, 467–473.

    Castellanos, F.X., Fine, E.J., Kaysen, D.L., Kozuch, P.L., Hamburger,S.D., Rapoport, J.L., Hallett, M., 1996. Sensorimotor gating inboys with Tourette'ssyndrome and ADHD: preliminary results.Biol. Psychiatry 29, 33–41.

    189B R A I N R E S E A R C H R E V I E W S