1-s2.0-S0304422X13000132-main.pdf

29
Adolescents’ school experience and the importance of having a ‘‘cool’’ mobile phone: Conformity, compensation and resistance? Mariek Vanden Abeele a, * , Keith Roe b a Department Communication and Information Science, University of Tilburg, PO Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands b Leuven School for Mass Communication Research, University of Leuven, Parkstraat 45, Box 3603, B-3000 Leuven, Belgium Available online 8 May 2013 Abstract Based on Bourdieu’s cultural capital theory this study—carried out among 1899 Flemish secondary school pupils—examines (1) whether adolescents’ experiences at school predict their attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object, and (2) whether this attitude, in turn, predicts ownership of a high-status phone. Three concurrent hypotheses were tested. Our ‘‘conformity hypothesis’’ was supported: in line with the social image that the school system bestows on them, adolescents in non-academic tracks attach greater importance to having a ‘‘cool’’ mobile phone, and lagging behind indirectly predicted this attitude via school track. The ‘‘compensation hypothesis’’ was not supported: no direct relationship was found between academic self- concept or lagging behind and adolescents’ attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. The ‘‘resistance hypothesis’’ was supported: adolescents with lower academic self-concept, who have lagged behind and who are in a semi-academic or vocational school track have a more negative attitude towards school, which in turn predicts a more positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. Finally, having a more positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object strongly predicts ownership of a high-status mobile phone. Gender, age and ethnicity were shown to moderate some of the relationships found. # 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Previous studies on adolescent mobile phone use have identified the use of the mobile phone as a status object as a distinctive feature of contemporary ‘‘mobile youth culture’’ (Campbell and www.elsevier.com/locate/poetic Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Poetics 41 (2013) 265–293 * Corresponding author. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M. Vanden Abeele), [email protected] (K. Roe). 0304-422X/$ see front matter # 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001

Transcript of 1-s2.0-S0304422X13000132-main.pdf

  • Adolescents school experience and the importance of

    having a cool mobile phone: Conformity,

    compensation and resistance?

    Mariek Vanden Abeele a,*, Keith Roe b

    a Department Communication and Information Science, University of Tilburg, PO Box 90153,

    5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlandsb Leuven School for Mass Communication Research, University of Leuven, Parkstraat 45,

    Box 3603, B-3000 Leuven, Belgium

    Available online 8 May 2013

    Abstract

    Based on Bourdieus cultural capital theory this studycarried out among 1899 Flemish secondary school

    pupilsexamines (1) whether adolescents experiences at school predict their attitude towards the mobile

    phone as a status object, and (2) whether this attitude, in turn, predicts ownership of a high-status phone. Three

    concurrent hypotheses were tested. Our conformity hypothesis was supported: in line with the social image

    that the school system bestows on them, adolescents in non-academic tracks attach greater importance to

    having a cool mobile phone, and lagging behind indirectly predicted this attitude via school track. The

    compensation hypothesis was not supported: no direct relationship was found between academic self-

    concept or lagging behind and adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. The

    resistance hypothesis was supported: adolescents with lower academic self-concept, who have lagged

    behind and who are in a semi-academic or vocational school track have a more negative attitude towards

    school, which in turn predicts a more positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. Finally,

    having a more positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object strongly predicts ownership of a

    high-status mobile phone. Gender, age and ethnicity were shown to moderate some of the relationships found.

    # 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    Previous studies on adolescent mobile phone use have identified the use of the mobile phone

    as a status object as a distinctive feature of contemporary mobile youth culture (Campbell and

    www.elsevier.com/locate/poetic

    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

    Poetics 41 (2013) 265293

    * Corresponding author.

    E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M. Vanden Abeele), [email protected] (K. Roe).

    0304-422X/$ see front matter # 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/0304422Xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001
  • Park, 2008; Castells et al., 2007). Adolescents regard characteristics such as the brand, color or

    the technical performance of their phone as a means to express their personal style and group

    membership (Caronia and Caron, 2004; Ling, 2004; Skog, 2002). Particularly during early and

    mid-adolescence, when teens are at the height of identity exploration (Erikson, 1968; Waterman,

    1982, 1990), having a cool mobile phone seems to matter greatly (Green, 2003; Ling and Yttri,

    2002, 2006).

    Over the past decade, a number of mostly qualitative studies have provided us with rich

    insights into the symbolic meaning that adolescents assign to the mobile phone (e.g., Blair and

    Fletcher, 2011; Oksman and Turtiainen, 2004; Walsh et al., 2009). Their strong focus on the

    commonalities among adolescents, however, has led to neglect of potential differences between

    adolescents and their contextualization (Goggin and Crawford, 2011; Haddon, 2007; Haddon and

    Vincent, 2009; Katz and Sugiyama, 2006). In this article, differences in adolescents ownership

    of and attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object are contextualized in terms of their

    experiences in the school system.

    School experience was chosen because previous research has shown that adolescents position

    in the school system and other (oftentimes related) indicators of school success and failuresuch

    as school performance or school commitmentare powerful predictors of media use: pupils who

    are successful in the school system, for example, have a stronger preference for media contents

    that are culturally more legitimate, while unsuccessful pupils report a stronger preference for

    media contents generally considered to be culturally less legitimate (Roe, 1992, 1995).

    Bourdieus (1979 [1984]) theory of cultural capital explains these differences in the cultural

    preferences of pupils by referring to a hidden mechanism of the school system, namely

    socialization into the social image (and, thereby, the cultural preferences) that belongs to an

    academic status position. According to this perspective, cultural preferences shared by pupils in

    similar academic positions are evidence of their conforming behavior. Besides the effect that

    ones position in the school system may have, adolescents individual reactions to their position

    and their personal performance may also affect cultural preferences; in particular, youths who

    experience failure in school may seek out media practices and contents as a means to compensate

    for and show resistance to the reward system of the school.

    The purpose of this study is to investigate whether these school experiences relate to attitudes

    towards and ownership of the mobile phone as a status object. We test three hypotheses about the

    mechanisms underlying this relationship among Flemish high school students: conformity,

    compensation and resistance. To begin, however, we shall situate our study in cultural

    capital theory.

    2. Cultural capital and mobile phones as desired goods in youth consumer culture

    2.1. Cultural capital theory and consumer culture

    Skog (2002) found that adolescents from lower class backgrounds and those with low school

    performance were significantly more likely to own a mobile phone than adolescents from higher

    class backgrounds and with high school performance. At first sight, given that higher class youths

    are more likely to have the financial resources to buy an up-scale mobile phone and that the elite

    is believed to distance itself from the lower classes by means of conspicuous consumption

    (Veblen, 1899 [1979]), these results appear contradictory. The insights of Bourdieus cultural

    capital theory, however, may help us understand why the mobile phone may be the object of a

    reversed logic.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293266

  • According to Bourdieu (1979 [1984]), the upper classes achieve distinction foremost by

    displaying distinctive cultural tastes that do not necessarily involve the most expensive or

    luxurious consumer goods. Higher class people are found to be less favorable towards many

    aspects of popular consumer culture. When they reject popular consumer culture, this can be

    understood as a subtle form of distancing themselves from lower classes. Moreover, with their

    preference for simplicity rather than ornateness, they may implicitly mock the wealth of the

    nouveaux riches (Steiner and Weiss, 1951, p. 263). This tendency towards counter-

    snobbery (Steiner and Weiss, 1951), is most notable in those areas in which intellectualism does

    not play a rolefor example, in the area of ostentatious and conspicuous consumption of

    consumer goods (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984]). While people from lower socio-economic status (SES)

    backgrounds more openly embrace materialism and a desire for luxury and abundance, the elite is

    (in)formally educated to downplay materialism and display more abstract humanist ideals

    (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984]; Holt, 1998). Kohn (1977) attributes this to the fact that the values of

    people from lower SES backgrounds emphasize conformity and external rewards (e.g., money)

    as the basis of ones self-esteem, while those of higher class people emphasize self-direction and

    internal rewards (e.g., integrity). Thus, lower class people more often seek out economic capital

    to gain social status and distinguish themselves. If unaccompanied by a certain degree of cultural

    competence, however, this capital will never deliver the same prestige as cultural capital delivers

    to the elite (Bourdieu, 1986).

    Bourdieus emphasis on the distinction between the highbrow (art) culture of the elite

    versus the lowbrow (popular [consumer]) culture of the masses has been criticized because it

    cannot be identified as strongly in American society (Lamont and Lareau, 1988) and because

    there is evidence that the contemporary elite is much more omnivorous in its cultural tastes,

    embracing both highbrow and popular culture, while lower classes are more univorous in their

    tastes (Peterson, 2005; Peterson and Kern, 1996). Despite these nuances to cultural capital theory,

    however, the basic tenet that the elite embraces and benefits from having distinct and more

    intellectual tastes still seems to stand (e.g., Prieur et al., 2008).

    The digital revolution has further challenged cultural capital theory. While the elite was

    generally regarded as more conservative and resistant with regard to electronic media

    technologies associated with popular consumer culture (e.g., television, see Bourdieu, 1979

    [1984]), recent studies show that new digital technologies are favored by, and significantly favor,

    the elite. There is a persistent digital divide, for instance, both in terms of material access and in

    terms of the purposes for which the Internet is used, with the elite adopting a more playful and

    exploratory stance towards the Internet, while lower classes are restricted to an informational

    orientationdescribed by Robinson (2009) as a taste for the necessary. Similarly, when

    looking at the contents people produce on the Internet, Schradie (2011) finds that elite voices are

    dominant in the digital realm. Compared to the Internet, the mobile phone has received little

    scholarly attention as a cultural object. The above research indicates, however, that distinction

    may also be played out through ones ownership and use of this new digital technology.

    2.2. Mobile phones as desired consumer goods

    The differential orientation of the elite versus lower classes to popular consumer culture has

    been noticed among youth. Throughout the 20th century, lower class youths were consistently

    found to involve themselves more in consumption-oriented youth cultures and lifestyles, while

    middle-class youths were more involved in intellectualistic youth cultures and lifestyles (Brake,

    1980; Coleman, 1961; Elkin and Westley, 1955; Hebdige, 1979). Despite the evolution towards

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 267

  • greater cultural omnivorism (Peterson and Kern, 1996) and more fluid, dispersed lifestyles

    (cfr. neo-tribalism; Bennett, 1999; Maffesoli, 1996) around the turn of the millennium, social

    class still consistently structures youths lifestyles. For example, underprivileged youths are

    more attracted to popular consumer-oriented lifestyles (e.g., the bling-bling lifestyle) (e.g.,

    Martin, 2009; McCulloch et al., 2006). For these youths, expensive consumer goods are a means

    to gain and display ones status in the peer group (McCulloch et al., 2006).

    Mobile phones can be identified as popular consumer goods that underprivileged youths may

    acquire in order to gain peer rather than school social status, while higher class youths may resist

    using the mobile phone as a status object as a display of counter-snobbery and ideological

    resistance. For example, Pasquier (2005, in Prieur and Rosenlund, 2008) noted how Parisian

    upper class girls refused to adopt a mobile phone.

    The school system is a crucial structuring factor, determining adolescents (future) position in

    society, while simultaneously reproducing existing social inequalities. As a result of this,

    adolescents with more problematic experiences at school may be more likely to turn to the mobile

    phone as a status object. To date, we know of only one study that (briefly) examined this idea:

    Skog (2002) found thatat a time1 when mobile phone ownership was still comparatively low

    adolescents with lower levels of academic self-esteem and lower educational aspirations were

    significantly more likely to own a mobile phone. According to Skog, this finding suggested that

    adolescents who did poorly in school sought compensation by using their mastery of mobile

    phone technology as an alternative display of status in the peer group. Today, given the

    widespread penetration of mobile phones among adolescents, mobile phone ownership no longer

    carries such a potential for symbolic value.2 However, less successful pupils may still try to

    enhance their image and status within the peer group by using mobile phones as a form of

    conspicuous consumption (Veblen, 1899 [1979])e.g., by choosing a technically advanced

    and/or expensive smart-phone.

    3. The school system and the reproduction of social inequality

    The school system plays an important role in shaping youths cultural practices through the

    reproduction of social inequalityreproducing social inequality in two major ways. First,

    pupils from higher social class backgrounds have been socialized into the cultural, academic and

    social values and norms to which the school adheres and, thus, find it easier to assimilate to the

    school environment (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984], 1986; Bourdieu and Passeron, 1977). Particularly in

    school systems where students are grouped into ability-based tracks, this reproduction effect is

    noticeable by the higher percentage of lower class youths in non-academic tracks (e.g., Oakes,

    1987; Oakes and Guiton, 1995; Wielemans, 1991). In the Flemish school system, this

    classification occurs rather explicitly through the division into three types of schools with a

    corresponding social image in which different school tracks are offered to pupils: the academic

    school tracks (accounting for 40% of pupils) focus on theory and general knowledge, the semi-

    academic school tracks (accounting for 32% of pupils) are oriented towards a mix of theory and

    practical skills, and the non-academic or vocational school tracks (accounting for 26% of pupils)

    are oriented towards learning a particular trade; lower class youths are overrepresented in these

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293268

    1 Skogs (2002) study took place in Norway in 1999.2 This is, at least, not the case for adolescents older than 12 years. In tweenhood and early adolescence (812 years old),

    mobile phone ownership in itself is still regarded a status symbol (Martensen, 2007).

  • latter tracks (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007). The three different types of school tracks largely

    correspond to a specific social standing in society (Wielemans, 1991): academic tracks are

    considered the hardest and most prestigious, while semi-academic and vocational tracks are

    considered easier and of lower social standing.3 Although envisioned as a compromise

    between a comprehensive school that would grant all pupils equal opportunities and an

    individualized school that would recognize differences in interests and capacities, in reality, the

    Flemish school system thus reproduces social inequalities rather than dissolving them. Given that

    the school system as a reproducer of social inequality is central to the study reported in this

    article, our first hypothesis links social class background to Flemish youths grouping into school

    tracks:

    Hypothesis 1. Adolescents from lower class backgrounds are more likely to follow lower

    school tracks than adolescents from higher class backgrounds (the first reproduction of

    inequality hypothesis).

    Closely intertwined with the school track system in Flemish schools is the common practice of

    lagging behind: Flemish pupils are frequently forced to repeat a year when their results are

    deemed insufficient. By the end of secondary education, more than one in three Flemish pupils is

    lagging behind by at least one year (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007). Particularly relevant for the

    current study is that lagging behind frequently occurs in combination with a (forced or voluntary)

    drop to a lower school track: of the pupils obtaining their diploma in an academic track, only

    15% lag behind one or more years, in comparison to 44% of those graduating in a semi-academic

    track, and 58% graduating in a vocational track (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007). Well aware of the

    social standing of different schools, many parents encourage their children to start in a school that

    offers academic tracks, disregarding their interests or skills. When pupils fail, the school advises

    or forces them to drop to an easier level, thereby adding to the lower standing of semi- and

    non-academic tracks. This structure produces a waterfall mechanism of downward social

    mobility within the school system (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007; Wielemans, 1991). The parental

    tendency to enroll children in academic school tracks, in combination with lower class youths

    being less adjusted to the social and cultural values and norms of the school environment (which

    prevail particularly in academic tracks; cfr. Byrne, 1990), may make lower class youths more at

    risk of lagging behind in the school system. Thus:

    Hypothesis 2. Adolescents from lower class backgrounds are more likely to have lagged behind

    in the school system than adolescents from higher class backgrounds (the second reproduction

    of inequality hypothesis).

    4. Conformity, compensation and resistance

    4.1. Conformity

    The school system not only reproduces social inequality through its selection of pupils on

    the basis of their cultural capital, it also further develops cultural tastes among them. According

    to Bourdieu and Passeron (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984]; Bourdieu and Passeron, 1977), cultural taste

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 269

    3 The terms lower and higher reflect societys judgment of the social standing that corresponds to the three types

    of school tracks; a value judgment that we, as authors, do not underwrite.

  • preferences for either highbrow or lowbrow (popular) culture continue to be acquired within

    the school system. The schools allocation of status titles of success and failure hide a

    mechanism of cultural capital distribution: when pupils receive their school certificate, they not

    only receive a formal qualification of their academic competence, they are also implicitly

    socialized into the social image that comes with the status position corresponding to this

    qualification. The educational system thus implicitly teaches cultural practices, and youths

    tastes can be regarded as a means of classifying themselves in the social space. Given that

    higher class youths have an advantage over others when entering the school system, the

    classification that takes place in the educational system amplifies the classification resulting

    from ones upbringing (cultural reproduction) (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984]; 1989; Bourdieu and

    Passeron, 1977; Roe, 1992). Additionally, DiMaggio (1982) found that adolescents cultural

    participation during the high school years positively affects educational success (cultural

    mobility). Later research by Aschaffenburg and Maas (1997) demonstrated that the cultural

    capital provided by youths own cultural participation (particularly outside school) affects

    educational success independently of the cultural capital acquired by ones upbringing. Both

    the mechanisms of cultural reproduction and cultural mobility imply a strong relationship

    between school success and adolescents involvement with more legitimate cultural practices.

    Pupils who are successful in the school system conform to the social image that the school

    promotes, and they internalize the cultural preferences belonging to this image into their self-

    image (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984]).

    While schools are no longer ignorant of the added value that popular culture can bring to the

    classroom environment (e.g., Verboord and van Rees (2009) study on the use of popular versus

    canonical literature in school textbooks), the school system, and academic school tracks in

    particular, still celebrate high culture (e.g., Duncan-Andrade, 2004; Giroux and McLaren, 1989;

    Giroux and Simon, 1988). In the Flemish schooling system, this translates into a greater affinity

    with highbrow cultural preferences among pupils in academic tracks. The highbrow preferences

    of the pupils in academic school tracks, then, reflect their cultural competence to recognize, read

    and value certain cultural signals and to use these signals for social and cultural exclusion (Roe,

    1992, 1995).

    With respect to mobile phones, in contemporary society, they are regarded as forms of popular

    rather than high culture (Gordon, 2002). Although initially designed and marketed as cutting

    edge technologies for up-scale professionals (Katz and Sugiyama, 2005; Leung and Wei, 1999),

    today they are seen as mass marketed consumer objects rather than aesthetic, exclusive artifacts

    (Djelic and Ainamo, 2005). In the school context, teachers have been found to deem mobile

    phones an unnecessary technology for young adolescents that causes social conflicts and

    disturbances in the classroom (Mezei et al., 2007). Several schools have banned mobile phones

    from the classroom or even the school premises (Ito, 2005; Lenhart et al., 2010). Given this, it is

    likely that schools in the Flemish system will embed resistance to the mobile phone as a fashion

    or status object into the social image that they pass on to their pupils. Pupils in academic tracks

    who have more negative attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status symbol would thus be

    conforming to the social image that the school transmits to them. As members of the cultural

    elite, teachers in lower tracks will likely also reject mobile phone use, although they will

    probably not reject it as strongly as teachers in higher tracks. Consequently, pupils in semi-

    academic and vocational tracks conform to the social image that pertains to their school type

    more strongly if they accept and value the mobile phone as a status object. These acts of

    conformity can then be regarded as examples of adolescents anticipatory socialization

    (Merton, 1957; see also Bourdieu, 1979 [1984] or Roe, 1992). Thus:

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293270

  • Hypothesis 3. Adolescents in lower school tracks have a more positive attitude towards the

    mobile phone as a status object than adolescents in higher school tracks (the first conformi-

    ty hypothesis).

    Given the observation that lagging behind oftentimes occurs in combination with a drop to a

    lower school track, we also expect to find an indirect relationship between lagging behind and

    adolescents attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object that is mediated by school

    track.

    Hypothesis 4. School track mediates the relationship between lagging behind and the impor-

    tance attached to the mobile phone as a status object (the second conformity hypothesis).

    4.2. Compensation

    The conformity hypotheses examine whether there is evidence for an effect of adolescents

    academic position on their attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. We may also look

    at adolescents individual reactions to their position and performance in the school system to

    explain their attitudes towards the mobile phone. As argued above, pupils who are successful in

    the schooling system are likely to display the (legitimate) cultural taste preferences that are

    valued by the school (Bourdieu, 1979 [1984], 1986; Roe, 1995). When pupils perform poorly in

    school, however, they are ascribed a negative identity of failure by the educational system

    (Stinchcombe, 1964). Adolescents who are ascribed such a negative identity are often found to

    involve themselves in deviant (sub-)cultural and sometimes even delinquent practices (Davies,

    1999). Two (concurrent) hypotheses that are often put forward to explain these kinds of

    involvement are, first, that adolescents may turn to these practices in an attempt to repair or

    compensate for their damaged self-esteem, and, second, that they do so to express their resistance

    against the school system and their sense of futility in it (Van Houtte and Stevens, 2008).

    To cope with the threat to their self-esteem, unsuccessful pupils have been found to

    compensate by involving themselves in media preferences that provide an alternative route to an

    (oftentimes sub-cultural) identity with which they can achieve status within their peer group,

    such as a preference for heavy metal music (Roe, 1987, 1992), heavy video game playing (Roe

    and Muijs, 1998) or violent/pornographic VCR content (Roe, 1989). The same mechanism has

    been found with respect to luxurious consumer goods. Research shows that people oftentimes

    consume these as a compensatory response to their low self-esteem (e.g., Chang and Arkin, 2002;

    Chaplin and John, 2007).

    We may thus expect that having a cool or expensive mobile phone provides a means for

    adolescents to compensate for the threat against their self-esteem that results from their

    experience of school failure (the compensation hypothesis). We include two indicators of

    school failure: lagging behind and adolescents academic self-concept. With respect to lagging

    behind, previous studies have shown that it significantly damages pupils socio-emotional health

    (e.g., Holmes and Matthews, 1984; Jimerson et al., 1997). Thus:

    Hypothesis 5. Lagging behind predicts a more positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a

    status object (the first compensation hypothesis).

    Our second indicator concerns adolescents subjective experience of school failurei.e., their

    academic self-concept. Academic self-concept is formed through social comparison with pupils

    who are in the same frame of reference (Byrne, 1990), and it can be understood as the

    adolescents (self-)perceived scholastic competence (Ireson and Hallam, 2009). It follows that:

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 271

  • Hypothesis 6. Adolescents with a lower academic self-concept have a more positive attitude

    towards the mobile phone as a status object than adolescents with a higher academic self-concept

    (the second compensation hypothesis).

    Previous studies show that both being in a lower school track (e.g., Byrne, 1990; Ireson

    and Hallam, 2009; Vanfossen et al., 1987) and lagging behind (e.g., Holmes and Matthews,

    1984) negatively affect adolescents academic self-concept, as these students perceive their

    scholastic competence as inferior to that of their (same-aged) peers. We thus expect academic

    self-concept to mediate the relationship between school track and grade retention, on the

    one hand, and adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object, on the

    other:

    Hypotheses 7/8. Adolescents in lower school tracks (H7)/who have lagged behind (H8) will

    have a lower academic self-concept than adolescents in higher school tracks/who have not lagged

    behind.

    4.3. Resistance

    As mentioned above, a concurrent hypothesis to the compensation hypothesis is the

    resistance hypothesis, which posits that academically unsuccessful youths are not (only)

    driven to involve themselves in (delinquent) youth cultures by the damage done to their self-

    esteem, but also (and perhaps foremost) by their resistance to the school system (Van Houtte and

    Stevens, 2008): youths who are in lower school tracks and/or youths who experience school

    failure are likely to see no relationship between the schooling they receive and any positive

    outcomes for them, resulting in a sense of futility (Van Houtte and Stevens, 2008) and a

    general disengagement from the school system that is regarded as irrelevant for ones future

    (Davies, 1994).

    Resistance to the school system can be recognized in anti-school attitudes, which, in turn, find

    expression in involvement with sub-cultures, consumer culture and/or disruptive/deviant

    behaviors. Involvement in consumer/youth cultures and/or (media-)delinquency can then be

    regarded as a shifted orientation from the irrelevant reward system of the school to other

    reward systems (Coleman, 1960; Hagan, 2012; Staff and Kreager, 2008). Goldberg et al. (2003),

    for example, found that young adolescents who disliked school had significantly stronger

    materialistic attitudes. Roe (1992, 1995, 1997) found that adolescents who performed poorly in

    school frequently had more negative attitudes towards school and that these attitudes, in turn,

    predict the use of socially disvalued (or media delinquent) media with which status can be

    obtained within the peer group. Similarly, Jenkins (1995) found that adolescents in lower school

    tracks had a significantly lower school commitment, which in turn predicted misbehavior at

    school.

    These studies point out that anti-school attitudes may play an important role in explaining the

    orientation of failed youths towards reward systems other than that of the school. We expect

    that adolescents attitudes towards the school will mediate the relationship between the indicators

    of school failure and the attitude towards the mobile phone as a fashion object. Thus,

    Hypothesis 9. The attitude towards school will negatively predict adolescents attitude towardsthe mobile phone as a status object (the resistance hypothesis).

    Hypotheses 10/11/12. Adolescents school track (H10)/lagging behind (H11)/and academic

    self-concept (H12) will predict the attitude towards school.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293272

  • 5. Smartphones as high-status goods

    Finally, previous studies point out that most teens consider cool mobile phones to be

    expensive and sophisticated. The type and the price of a mobile phone are considered to be

    important status-signaling features (Fortunati, 2005; Katz and Sugiyama, 2005; Skog, 2002;

    Wilska, 2003). Therefore, we expect that adolescents who believe that having a cool mobile

    phone is important will also be more likely to have an expensive and sophisticated one. This

    implies that:

    Hypothesis 13. Adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object positively

    predicts ownership of a more expensive and sophisticated mobile phone.

    This last hypothesis also implies that we expect attitude towards the mobile phone as a status

    object to mediate the relationship between the school variables in our model and smartphone

    ownership.

    The above hypotheses and research question are graphically depicted in a theoretical model

    (see Fig. 1). As parental SES might be a confounding variable explaining both school experience

    variables, on the one hand, and the attitude towards and ownership of a high status mobile phone,

    on the other, direct paths between parental SES and each of these variables were added to the

    theoretical model. Additionally, we added direct paths from parental SES, school track, lagging

    behind, academic self-concept and attitude towards school to smartphone ownership, so as to

    examine whether, and to what extent, the attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object

    mediates these relationships.

    6. The moderating role of gender, age and ethnicity

    Previous research has shown that gender affects almost all the parameters in our model. For

    example, educational attainment has been found to be higher among girls (Dekkers et al., 2000;

    Baye et al., 2005). Moreover, during adolescence, females have been found to have more positive

    attitudes towards school (Jenkins, 1995) and to attach less importance to having a cool mobile

    phone (Wilska, 2003). Hence, we include gender as a control variable. Gender may also moderate

    the relationships postulated in our model. School failure is considered more stressful for males,

    who are socialized to be performance oriented, than for females, who are socialized more towards

    achievements in the interpersonal domain (Kirk, 2009). Preference for more socially disvalued

    media in response to school failure has therefore been identified as occurring more extensively

    among adolescent males (Roe and Jarlbro, 1998). Moreover, Skog (2002) suggested that the

    relationship between (academic) self-esteem and having a mobile phone found in her study was

    particularly strong for males, because of its potential for emphasizing their mastery of this

    technology. This hypothesis is in line with insights on the gender divide in technology use

    (Broos, 2005; Kirk, 2009). Hence, we may expect that the relationship between adolescents

    experience of school failure and the importance they attach to having a cool mobile phone will be

    stronger for male than for female adolescents.

    Previous research also suggests that the mobile phone as a status object is most important for

    younger adolescents (Ling and Helmersen, 2000; Martensen, 2007). Younger adolescents tend to

    be more concerned about conforming to peer group norms (Berndt, 1979; Sumter et al., 2009)

    and having the right mobile phone is likely to be such a norm (Caronia and Caron, 2004; Ling

    and Yttri, 2006). Age is thus included as a control variable in this study. We also see reason to

    include age as a moderator variable. Lagging behind and/or dropping to a lower school track is

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 273

  • M.

    Va

    nd

    en A

    beele

    an

    d K

    . R

    oe

    / P

    oetics

    41

    (20

    13

    ) 2

    652

    93

    27

    4

    H3

    H4SES

    School track

    Lagging

    behind

    H1

    H2

    H6

    H8

    H5

    H12

    H10

    H11H9

    p13

    Academic

    Self-concept

    Attitu de towards MP as a status ob ject

    High-end

    MP

    Price

    Type

    Positiv e attitude towards school

    H7

    Fig. 1. Theoretical model of the postulated relationships between adolescents experiences of school failure, their attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object, and their

    ownership of a high-status mobile phone.

  • likely to be more detrimental in the lives of younger adolescents, as lagging behind is less

    common in the earlier years of secondary school (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007). Consequently,

    the relationship between the experience of school failure and the importance of having a cool

    mobile phone may be stronger for young adolescents.

    Finally, we also include ethnicity as a control and a moderator variable. To date, little is known

    about the mobile phones place in the lives of ethnic minority youths. Soenen and Baes (2011)

    suggest that minority youths attach great importance to the mobile phone as a fashion article and

    status object. This finding conforms with existing studies on ethnic minorities conspicuous

    consumption patterns (Charles et al., 2009; Lamont and Molnar, 2001), and it indicates that

    ethnicity may be a useful control variable for our study. Ethnicity may also moderate some of the

    relationships in our model; indeed, for ethnic minority youths, the negative label of school

    failure is likely to be more detrimental than for majority youths, as it cumulates with the

    existing label of ethnic minority.

    In order to test the aforementioned hypotheses and the (moderating) role of gender, age and

    ethnicity, we make use of structural equation modeling, a technique making it possible to

    examine multiple relationships simultaneously by comparing how well empirical data fit with

    a pre-postulated theoretical model, while comparing the strength of relationships between groups

    (Arbuckle, 2005).

    7. Method

    7.1. Procedure

    During the autumn of 2010, a large-scale survey study was carried out among 13 high schools in

    the 5 provinces of Flanders, Belgium. Schools were selected by means of a random stratified

    sampling procedure. Of the 24 schools that were contacted, 9 agreed to participate. In four schools,

    questionnaires were administered to all the pupils of the school. In five schools, questionnaires were

    administered to a random set of classes that happened to have individual study hours during the data

    collection period. The (anonymous) questionnaires were administered to the adolescents in their

    classroom in the presence of at least two researchers. The sample composition was closely

    monitored. By the end of the data collection period, two gaps in the sample (academic tracks and

    1st grade pupils were underrepresented) were addressed by means of purposive sampling in four

    additional schools, whereby additional questionnaires were gathered only among classes (with

    study hours) that fit these criteria (see Table A1 in the Appendix). This procedure resulted in a total

    sample of 1952 pupils that came from 13 different schools and 30 different classrooms. Forty-four

    pupils (2%) were recruited from a school of arts. Given the somewhat different orientation and

    social image of this type of schooling, these pupils were excluded from the analyses. Nine

    questionnaires were removed from the sample (e.g., when less than half of the questionnaire was

    completed, when the same response was given on each question). The final sample included in the

    analyses thus consisted of 1899 pupils, a sample that is representative in terms of gender (51.1%

    males), age (M = 15.31 years, SD = 1.87) and school track (see below) for the population of high

    school pupils in Flanders.

    7.2. Measures

    The exogenous variables in our model are gender, age, ethnicity and the socio-economic status

    of the parents, as indicated by their educational and occupational position.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 275

  • Gender was coded as 1 for male, 2 for female respondents. Age was calculated by

    subtracting the date of birth from the date on which each questionnaire was administered.

    Because proficiency in the ethnic language is conducive to ethnic minorities psychological

    adaptation (Vedder, 2005), we chose to ask respondents for the languages spoken at home (Dutch,

    French or Other [If Other, specify]); respondents were asked to check all boxes that applied to

    them. For our analyses, we differentiated between adolescents who indicated speaking Western-

    European language(s) at home (ethnic majority youths) and those who (also or exclusively)

    indicated speaking a non-Western-European language(s) at home (ethnic minority youths). In

    total, 7.6% (N = 145) of our sample indicated speaking a non-Western-European language at

    home.

    Parental educational status was indicated by the highest obtained qualification of the

    respondents father and mother (primary school, secondary school, higher education). For

    occupational level, the respondents were asked in an open-ended question to describe as

    thoroughly as possible the jobs held by their parents. Each occupation was classified as a low,

    middle or high class profession with the aid of the classification system of the International Labor

    Association (ISCO-08). As expected, a close relationship was observed between parental

    educational and occupational status. To avoid problems of multi-collinearity, a compound SES-

    variable was constructed by first standardizing each measure, and then calculating the average of

    these four Z-scores. It should be noted that several respondents had difficulty in reporting the

    educational and/or occupational level of their parents: for only 1397 respondents (74% of the

    total sample) the SES-score was based on the parental and occupational level of both parents. For

    the other respondents, the variable is based on either the parental, either the occupational level of

    one or both parents. For 66 pupils, a missing value for the SES-score was replaced with the series

    mean.4 This procedure enabled us to retain the total sample, and had little impact on the

    relationships in the model.

    The intermediary endogenous variables in our model are:

    Respondents school track, coded 1 for vocational tracks, 2 for semi-academic and 3 foracademic school tracks. Almost half (41.6%) of the respondents were in an academic track,

    34.4% in a semi-academic track, and 24% in a non-academic or vocational track.

    Lagging behind, indicating the number of school years that the adolescent is lagging behindin high school (0 = none, 1 = one year, 2 = two years or more). Sixteen per cent (15.6%)

    reported having lagged behind one year in high school and 2.6% reported two years or more.

    Academic self-concept was measured by three items from the Academic Self-Conceptsubscale of Marshs (1992) Self-Description Questionnaire II: I get bad marks in most school

    subjects; I learn things quickly in most school subjects; and I am good at most school

    subjects. Negatively worded items were reversed (a = .65). Response categories ranged from

    1 (completely disagree) to 6 (completely agree). On average, the respondents had a positive

    perception of their own scholastic competence (M = 4.22, SD = .95).

    Adolescents attitude towards school was indicated by four items from Roe and Muijs(1998) school commitment scale: Most of the times, I am bored at school; I like going to

    school; School is usually boring; and I would rather not go to school. The negatively

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293276

    4 These missing values were not randomly distributed; 9.5% of pupils in vocational tracks failed to provide any

    information in parental education or occupation, compared to only 1.8%, resp. 1.4% of the pupils in semi-academic, resp.

    academic tracks (X2(2) = 63.89, p < .000).

  • worded item of this subscale was reversed so that high scores correspond with a more positive

    attitude towards school (a = .80). Response categories ranged from 1 (completely disagree) to

    4 (completely agree). On average, the respondents reported a moderately negative attitude

    towards school (M = 2.38, SD = .66).

    Attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object was measured using six items derivedfrom Martensen (2007): It is important to have a cool phone; Its important to have a

    mobile phone that has the latest features; My mobile doesnt have to be expensive or cool

    but reliable (reverse scored); I am very interested in mobile phones with the latest features;

    A good mobile phone is always expensive; and A cool or expensive mobile phone is like a

    status symbol to me. The response categories range from 1 (completely disagree) to 4

    (completely agree). The Cronbach alpha for the scale was satisfactory (a = .79).

    Finally, the last endogenous variable in our model, ownership of a high status mobile

    phone, is a latent variable constructed by two manifest variables. The respondents were asked to

    indicate the type of mobile phone they currently useda phone without multimedia features

    (value 1), a phone with basic multimedia features (value 2) or a smartphone (Iphone, Blackberry,

    . . .) (value 3)and to indicate the cost of their current phone by means of an open-endedquestion.

    7.3. Preliminary analyses

    Descriptive analyses were conducted with SPSS Statistics 19.0. Given our multistage

    sampling procedure, our observations may be nested in meaningful social groups at the class and

    school level. This nesting potentially violates the assumption of independence. Not accounting

    for multilevel structures in the dataset may lead to parameter estimates that are over-valued in

    terms of their contribution to the outcome variable (Hox, 2010). Therefore, we first estimated

    general linear mixed models in SPSS Statistics 19.0, so as to examine potential classroom and

    school-level variability in the two main variables of interestnamely, adolescents attitude

    towards and their ownership of a high status mobile phone. With regard to ownership of a high

    status mobile phone, after entering all predictor variables as fixed effects into the equation, no

    significant variability was found at the school level (N = 13, s2between :00, SE = .00, Z = 0.00,pone-sided = 1.00, ICC = .00) or at the classroom level (N = 30, s

    2between :00, SE = .01, Z = 0.57,

    pone-sided = .14, ICC = .01). With regard to adolescents attitude towards having a high status

    mobile phone, no significant variability was found at the school level (N = 13, s2between :01,SE = .01, Z = 0.75, pone-sided = .11, ICC = .02). The classroom level, on the other hand, was found

    to account for 6% of the variability in attitudinal scores (N = 30, s2between :01, SE = .01,Z = 2.33, pone-sided < .01, ICC = .06). Although the latter intraclass correlation is consideredsmall in the literature (Hox, 2010), we wish to draw the readers attention to this clustering at the

    classroom level.

    The models were tested using AMOS 19.0. The evaluation of the fit of a model is based on a

    number of goodness-of-fit indices. The X2-square statistic is the only measure with an

    associated significance test; however, as the X2-statistic is known to inflate when large samples

    are used, Joreskog and Sorbom (1993) advise evaluating the X2/df ratio. For large samples, a

    X2/df ratio smaller than 3.0 is considered a good or acceptable model fit. A second fit index

    commonly used is the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). An RMSEA-value

    below .05 is considered a close fit, a value between .05 and .08 an acceptable fit, a value

    between .08 and .10 a mediocre fit and a value above .10 an unacceptable fit

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 277

  • (Diamantopoulos and Siguaw, 2000; Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). A third fit index that we

    report is the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), with a CFI-value above .95 indicating a good fit and

    a value above .90 an acceptable fit (Kelloway, 1998). Both the CFI and RMSEA are relatively

    unaffected by sample size (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003).

    8. Results

    8.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations

    Table 1 presents the correlations between the variables in our study. The experience of school

    failure variables were all significantly related to one another (r > j.07j, p < .01; see Table 1).Boys were significantly more likely to have already lagged behind than girls. Presumably

    partially as a result of their lagging behind, boys were also more likely to be in a semi-academic

    (40.1% versus 30.3%) or non-academic school track (23.6% versus 21.1%; X2(2) = 29.50,

    p < .000), and they expressed more negative attitudes towards school. Age related to each of thethree variables measuring school success/failure, but it did not relate to teens attitude towards

    school. Over half of ethnic minority respondents (55.3%) were in a vocational track, compared to

    only 20.2% of ethnic majority youths (X2(2) = 75.72, p < .000). No relationship was foundbetween ethnicity and lagging behind, academic self-concept or attitude towards school.

    With respect to teens attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object, on average, the

    respondents reported attaching little importance to having a cool mobile phone (M = 2.15 [scale

    mean = 2.5], SD = 0.59). Seventy-six per cent owned a mobile phone with basic multi-media

    functions, followed by 18.1% who owned a smartphone, and 5.6% whose phone lacked any

    multimedia feature. The mean price of adolescents mobile phone was 139.80 euro

    (SD = 103.88). As expected, the price of a mobile phone and its type were strongly related

    (r = .48, p < .000; see Table 1).A small gender difference was found with respect to the importance attached to having a

    cool mobile phone, with boys attaching greater importance to this than girls (r = .06,p = .015). Boys (M = 147.34, SD = 119.24) reported having more expensive mobile phones than

    girls (M = 132.05, SD = 84.64; t(1632.71) = 3.13, p < .01). With respect to age, youngeradolescents were found to attach greater importance to having a cool mobile phone (r = .23,p < .000), while older teens were found to have significantly more expensive mobile phones(r = .10, p < .000). Age did not affect the type of mobile phone. Finally, ethnic minority youthsshared more positive attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object (r = .12, p < .000),were more likely to have a more sophisticated (r = .11, p < .000) and a more expensive mobilephone (r = .10, p < .000).

    8.2. Structural equation models

    8.2.1. Test of hypothesized relationships

    Before examining the fit of the structural model, the fit of the measurement models for the

    latent variables were examined to assess whether the measured variables represent uni-

    dimensional factor structures. The fit indices and factor loadings of individual items for each

    measurement model are listed in Table A2 of the Appendix. The models showed acceptable

    model fit. Next, we fitted the theoretical model as presented in Fig. 1. The model fit the data

    reasonably well (X2 (150) = 501.72, p = .000, X2/df = 3.35, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .04). Following

    Aish and Joreskog (1990), insignificant paths were removed. This trimmed model provided a

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293278

  • M

    . V

    an

    den

    Ab

    eele a

    nd

    K.

    Ro

    e /

    Po

    etics 4

    1 (2

    01

    3)

    26

    52

    93

    2

    79

    Table 1

    Correlation table.

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    1 Gender (male = 1, female = 2) 1

    2 Age .06** 13 School track (1 = vocational) .09*** .14*** 14 Years lagging behind .19*** .43*** .23*** 15 SES .17*** .03 .34*** .03 16 Ethnicity (0 = majority, 1 = minority) .02 .08*** .18*** .04 .20*** 17 Academic self-concept .03 .05* .10*** .07** .03 .00 18 Positive attitude school .17*** .02 .14*** .10*** .03 .00 .23*** 19 Attitude MP status object .06* .23*** .13*** .07** .10*** .12*** .07** .12*** 1

    10 Type of mobile phone (1 = basic phone) .00 .02 .08** .02 .06** .11*** .01 .04 .30*** 111 Price of mobile phone .07** .10*** .07** .09*** .03 .10*** .02 .06** .33*** .48*** 1

    * p < .05.** p < .01.

    *** p < .001.

  • reasonable fit, but did not fit significantly better than the original model (X2(166) = (520.31,

    p = .000, X2/df = 3.13, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .03).

    Fig. 2 shows the standardized beta weights for the (statistically significant) parameter

    estimates; for reasons of clarity, the significant beta weights and correlation coefficients of the

    relationships between the control variables and the variables were not depicted in this figure but

    can be found in Table 2. Several of our hypotheses were supported. Parental SES was a strong

    predictor of school track (b = .33, p < .000; H1 supported), and it was a weak, yet significantpredictor of lagging behind (b = .06, p < .000; H2 supported). Both conformity hypotheseswere also supported: not only did school track significantly predict adolescents attitude towards

    the mobile phone as a status object (b = .12, p < .000; H3 supported), lagging behind was alsoindirectly related to this attitude by predicting adolescents school track (b = .15, p < .000; H4supported). A mediation test of this indirect relationship revealed it to be significant, albeit weak

    (b = .02, SE = .01, p = .01).

    Our compensation hypotheses were not supported by the model: neither lagging behind nor

    academic self-concept significantly predicted adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a

    status object5 (H5 and H6 not supported). While school track significantly predicted academic self-

    concept (b = .11, p < .000; H7 supported), no relationship was found for lagging behind (H8 notsupported). The resistance hypothesis was supported, however: adolescents with a more negative

    attitude towards school attached significantly greater importance to having a cool mobile phone

    (b = .14, p < .000; H9 supported). We also see that school track (b = .08, p < .05), laggingbehind (b = .06, p < .05) and particularly academic self-concept (b = .27, p < .000) predictadolescents attitude towards school (H10, H11 and H12 supported). The mediating role of

    adolescents attitude towards school was significant both for school track (b = .01, SE = .00,p = .01), lagging behind (b = .01, SE = .01, p = .01) and for academic self-concept (b = .03,SE = .01, p < .00). In total, the school failure indicators in our model together with gender, ageand ethnicity explained 13% of the variance in adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a

    status object. The strongest predictor of smartphone ownership was adolescents attitude towards

    the mobile phone as a status object (b = .53, p < .000; H13 supported), which explained 29% of itsvariance (together with gender, age and ethnicity). None of the other school variables, nor parental

    SES, predicted smartphone ownership. Finally, parental SES directly predicted adolescents

    attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object (b = .06, p < .05).Gender, age and ethnicity were included in the theoretical model as direct predictors of each of

    the endogenous variablesand as correlates of parental SES and of each other. Only parameter

    estimates that revealed significant associations were retained in the final trimmed model. These

    are reported in Table 2. Boys were significantly more likely to have lagged behind, to be in a

    lower school track, and to have a less positive attitude towards school. Boys had more positive

    attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object (b = .07, p < .05). Age strongly predictedlagging behind and also significantly predicted the school track of the adolescent.6 Older

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293280

    5 As the latter lack of direct relationships may be attributable to a full mediation effect of attitude towards school, we re-

    estimated our model, leaving out attitude towards school. The fit of this revised model was good (see Figure A1 in the

    Appendix). When examining the parameters of this revised model, we now found a significant relationship between

    academic self-concept and adolescents attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object (b = .06, p = .05). Laggingbehind was still not a direct predictor, however.

    6 Age thus predicts school track, even when controlling for the relationship between age and lagging behind (and, thus,

    the waterfall effect). This relation may be attributed to the sampling procedure, as more higher grade classes were

    sampled in vocational tracks (r = .05, p < .05).

  • M.

    Va

    nd

    en A

    beele

    an

    d K

    . R

    oe

    / P

    oetics

    41

    (20

    13

    ) 2

    652

    93

    2

    81

    R = .19

    R = .21

    R = .02

    R = .12

    R = .13R = .29

    -.1 2** *

    -.1 5** *SES

    School track

    Lagging

    behind

    .33** *

    -.0 6*

    .11** *

    .27** *

    .08*

    -.0 6*-.1 4** *

    .53** *

    -.0 6*Academic Self-conc ept

    High-end

    MP

    Price

    Type

    -.77** *

    -.62** *Attitu de

    towards MP as

    a status ob ject

    Positiv e attitude towards s chool

    Fig. 2. Structural equation model of the postulated relationships between adolescents experiences of school failure, their attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object, and

    their ownership of a high-status mobile phone (X2(166) = (520.31, p = .000, X2/df = 3.13, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .03). Note: For clarity, relationships with control variables (gender,

    age, ethnicity) are not depicted in this figure, but reported in Table 2.

  • adolescents reported having a lower academic self-concept. Younger adolescents were

    significantly more likely to have a positive attitude towards the mobile phone as a status

    object (b = .27, p < .000), yet were significantly less likely to own a smartphone (b = .23,p < .000). Finally, the results for our third control variable, ethnicity, indicate that ethnicminority youths were significantly more likely to have lagged behind, to follow a lower school

    track, to have more positive attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object (b = .08,

    p < .05) and to be more likely to own a smartphone (b = .09, p < .05).

    8.2.2. The moderating role of gender, age and ethnicity

    In order to explore possible differences in the pattern of relationships according to gender, age

    or ethnicitymultiple-group analyses of our initial model were carried out (see Table 3). For

    each of the moderators, we first estimated the full theoretical model (including the remaining

    control variables) and then trimmed this model by removing insignificant paths. Next, we

    assessed measurement invariance by examining whether constraining the measurement weights

    of the latent variables and the structural weights in the model across groups significantly altered

    the fit of the trimmed models. In the case of gender, measurement invariance was supported.

    Hence, the analysis was performed on the model in which measurement weights were

    constrained to be equal. In the case of age and ethnicity, measurement invariance was not

    supported. Here, multi-group analyses were performed on the trimmed models.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293282

    Table 2

    Regression weights, standard errors and standardized regression weights of gender, age and ethnicity.

    Unstandardized estimate SE Standardized estimate

    Regression weights

    Lagging behind Gender .16 .02 .17***Lagging behind Age .11 .01 .42***Lagging behind Ethnicity .11 .04 .06**School track Gender .19 .03 .12***School track Ethnicity .35 .06 .12***School track Age .04 .01 .09***Academic self-concept Age .03 .01 .06*Attitude towards school Gender .18 .03 .17***Attitude towards school Age .01 .01 .05Attitude MP status object Gender .07 .03 .07*Attitude MP status object Age .07 .01 .27***Attitude MP status object Ethnicity .15 .05 .08**High-status mobile phone Age .09 .01 .23***High-status mobile phone Ethnicity .27 .08 .09***Correlations

    Parental SES $Gender .07 .01 .17***Parental SES $Ethnicity .04 .01 .20***Gender $Age .05 .02 .06**Ethnicity $Age .04 .01 .07**

    Note: The current table reports only significant parameter estimates of control variables in relation to other variables as

    obtained in the final fitted model.* p < .05.

    ** p < .01.*** p < .001. p = .07.

  • M.

    Va

    nd

    en A

    beele

    an

    d K

    . R

    oe

    / P

    oetics

    41

    (20

    13

    ) 2

    652

    93

    2

    83

    Table 3

    Moderator effects for gender, age and ethnicity.

    Gender Age Ethnicity

    bmale N = 970 bfemale N = 929 Z byoung N = 948 bold N = 951 Z bmajor N = 1754 bminor N = 145 Z

    Lagging behind Parental SES .1*** .00 2.9** .06 .09** 1.76 .05* .12 .91School track Lagging behind .18*** .11*** .14 .04 .26*** 2.25* .19*** .13 3.48***School track Parental SES .27*** .38*** 2.25* .38*** .27*** 2.23* .36*** .05 3.86***Academic SC School track .15*** .1** .91 .15*** .07 1.4 .12*** .02 2.15*Academic SC Lagging behind / / / .02 .09* .54 .09** .14 2.59**Attitude school School track .06 .09** .45 .07* .1** .75 .08** .11 .27Attitude school Lagging behind .1* .02 .85 / / / / / /Attitude school Academic SC .27*** .27*** .38 .25*** .29*** 1.15 .28*** .25* 1.11Attitude MP Attitude School .1** .19*** 1.82 .22*** .06 3.08** .15*** .15 2.07*Attitude MP School track .12** .12** .21 .08* .14*** 1.13 .14*** .01 1.06Attitude MP Lagging behind / / / .01 .12** 1.65 / / /Attitude MP Parental SES .02 .08* .98 .1** .01 1.55 / / /Attitude MP Academic SC / / / / / / .02 .39** 2.76**High status MP Attitude MP .56*** .50*** 3.63*** .46*** .56*** 4.67*** .53*** .39*** .77High status MP Lagging behind / / / .10* .06 1.13 / / /High status MP Parental SES / / / / / / .01 .22** 2.74**Lagging behind Ethnicity .05 .11*** .49 .07* .07* 1.44 / / /School track Ethnicity .1** .14*** .95 .08** .15*** 2.25* / / /Attitude MP Ethnicity .07* .09* .13 .06 .12*** 1.76 / / /High status MP Ethnicity .09** .12** .24 .07 .13*** 2.62** / / /Lagging behind Age .50*** .32*** 9.05*** / / / .42*** .45*** .89School track Age .12*** .06* 1.15 / / / .07** .28** 2.32*Attitude School Age .07 .01 1.26 / / / .04 .23* 2.76**Attitude MP Age .29*** .25*** 1.06 / / / .28*** .11 .99High status MP Age .26*** .18*** 2.91** / / / .22*** .32*** 2.06*Lagging behind Gender / / / .10** .26*** 6.04*** .17*** .12 .37School track Gender / / / .11*** .13*** .31 .13*** .01 1.28Attitude school Gender / / / .16*** .19*** .79 .18*** .11 .56Attitude MP Gender / / / .04 .11** 1.12 .06* .08 .41Model fit indices

  • M.

    Va

    nd

    en A

    beele

    an

    d K

    . R

    oe

    / P

    oetics

    41

    (20

    13

    ) 2

    652

    93

    28

    4Table 3 (Continued )

    Gender Age Ethnicity

    bmale N = 970 bfemale N = 929 Z byoung N = 948 bold N = 951 Z bmajor N = 1754 bminor N = 145 Z

    Theoretical model X2(279) = 624.90, p < .000 CFI = .96;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(279) = 698.41, p < .000 CFI = .95;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(279) = 633.78, p < .000 CFI = .96;

    RMSEA = .03

    Trimmed model (insignificant paths

    removed)

    X2(308) = 648.70, p < .000 CFI = .96;

    RMSEA = .02

    X2(304) = 720.60, p < .000 CFI = .95;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(304) = 655.64, p < .000 CFI = .96;

    RMSEA = .03

    Nested model: measurement weights

    constrained

    X2(319) = 663.74, p < .000 CFI = .96;

    RMSEA = .02

    X2(315) = 741.80, p < .000 CFI = .95;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(315) = 691.56, p < .000 CFI = .96,

    RMSEA = .03

    Nested model: regression weights

    constrained

    X2(339) = 786.77, p < .000 CFI = .95;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(335) = 848.07, p < .000 CFI = .94;

    RMSEA = .03

    X2(336) = 755.86, p < .000 CFI = .95,

    RMSEA = .03

    * p < .05.** p < .01.

    *** p < .001.

  • The multiple-group analysis for gender resulted in an acceptable fit (model with constrained

    measurement weights: X2(319) = 663.74, p < .000; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .02). To assess whetherparameter estimates for boys might be significantly different from those for girls, the critical

    ratios for differences between the two models parameters were examined. This analysis revealed

    interesting differences (see Table 3). Two concern the reproduction of social inequality: while

    boys from lower socio-economic backgrounds were significantly more likely to have lagged

    behind than girls, girls from lower socio-economic backgrounds were significantly more likely to

    follow a semi-academic or vocational school track. The relationship between attitude towards the

    mobile phone as a status object and ownership of a high status mobile phone was significantly

    stronger for boys (b = .56) than for girls (b = .50).

    The multiple group analysis comparing younger (15.31 years) and older adolescentsresulted in a good fit (X2(304) = 720.60, p < .000, CFI = .95, RMSEA = .03). The relationshipbetween attitude towards school and attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object was

    significantly stronger among younger adolescents. Older adolescents with a more positive

    attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object, on the other hand, were significantly more

    likely to own a smartphone.

    Finally, with regard to ethnicity, the trimmed model achieved a good fit (X2(304) = 655.64,

    p < .000; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .03). Given the small sample size of the ethnic minority youthgroup, few of the parameters in the minority model reached significance. Nonetheless, interesting

    differences can be found between majority and minority youths. Among ethnic minority youths,

    adolescents with positive attitudes towards the school system also had more positive attitudes

    towards the mobile phone as a status object. The compensation hypothesis was partially

    supported among minority youths: academic self-concept (b = .39, p < .05) significantlypredicted these youths attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object. While parental SES

    did not predict ownership of a high status mobile phone among ethnic majority youths, a

    significant positive relationship was found in the group of ethnic minority youths (b = .22,

    p < .000).

    9. Discussion

    The purpose of this article was to examine the relationship between adolescents position in

    the school system and their attitude towards the mobile phone as a status objectwith particular

    focus on the mediating role of academic self-concept and school attitude, and on the moderating

    role of gender, age and ethnicity.

    A first set of hypotheses examined, concerned the schools role in the reproduction of social

    inequality. Confirming earlier studies, we found that lower class youths were overrepresented in

    lower school tracks and that they were more likely to have lagged behind during their high school

    years. Interestingly, we found an outspoken gender difference here: while the relationship

    between class background and school track was much stronger for girls, the relationship between

    class background and lagging behind was significantly stronger for boys. A tentative explanation

    for these findings may be that parents and/or educators apply gender-stereotypical norms for boys

    and girls with respect to their performance in the school system: while academic performance is

    downplayed for girls (for lower class girls, in particular), thereby encouraging them to already

    begin their high school career in a lower school track, it is over-emphasized for boys, forcing

    them to begin high school in an academic track whatever their academic interests and/or

    competency. The result of this double standard in the school system is likely to be detrimental

    for both, as girls chances for upward social mobility are minimized, while boys are at a greater

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 285

  • risk for the frustrating experiencing of downward social mobility (and all the undesirable

    consequences associated with it) (Van Houtte, 2005). The school system thus not only reproduces

    inequality in terms of social class background, but also in terms of gender inequality.

    The major focus of this chapter, however, was on the relationship between adolescents

    experiences in the school system and their attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object.

    Three concurrent hypotheses were postulated: conformity, compensation and resistance.

    The conformity hypothesis postulated that, in the Flemish school system, adolescents would

    conform to the social image that the school bestows on themnamely, acceptance of the mobile

    phone in lower levels of schooling (and resistance in higher levels of schooling). The conformity

    hypothesis found support: adolescents in semi-academic and, particularly, vocational tracks

    attached greater importance to having a cool mobile phone than adolescents in academic

    school tracks. Moreover, lagging behind also indirectly predicted these attitudes. This finding

    supports the idea that the track system in Flemish high schools brings forth certain expectations

    with regard to adolescents social image and that visible material objects such as the mobile

    phone are a part of that image. The indirect relationship with lagging behind points towards

    anticipatory socialization of youths who drop to a lower level as a result of their lagging

    behind. To further validate the conformity hypothesis, however, future research may need to

    focus on school policies on and teachers attitudes towards mobile phones. This could inform us

    about whether and how schools implicitly and explicitly teach critical resistance to the mobile

    phone as a consumer good.

    Our second and third set of hypotheses concerned the idea of compensation for the damage

    that school failure brings to self-esteem and the idea of resistance against the irrelevance of the

    school as a reward system. Our results failed to support the compensation hypothesis (neither

    lagging behind nor academic self-concept were directly related to adolescents attitudes towards

    the mobile phone as a status object), while the resistance hypothesis gained strong support. When

    excluding adolescents attitude towards school from our original model, however, we found

    support for the compensation hypothesis, with academic self-concept negatively predicting

    attitudes towards the mobile phone as a status object. This finding suggests that compensation

    may explain some of the relationship, but that school failure serves mostly as an antecedent to

    anti-school attitudeswhich, in turn, predict more positive attitudes towards the mobile phone as

    a status object. This causal chain is in agreement with what Van Houtte and Stevens (2008)

    found in their study on youth delinquency, and it suggests that youths will attempt to remedy the

    damage to their self-esteem by developing resistance against the school and the reward system

    for which it stands. By adopting cultural values and symbols that the school rejects, but that are

    strongly valued in the peer group, these pupils attempt to develop an alternative identity that is

    rewarded by the status mechanism of the peer group, namely popularity.

    With regard to gender, our finding that boys have more positive attitudes towards the mobile

    phone as a status object is congruent with gender stereotypes, in that boys typically have a greater

    desire to display their masculinity through their technological mastery (Kirk, 2009; Skog,

    2002; Wilska, 2003). Overall, however, little difference was found in the impact of boys and

    girls experiences of school failure on their attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object.

    With regard to age, we found not only that younger adolescents attached significantly greater

    importance to the mobile phone as a status object, but also that these attitudes were more strongly

    predicted by their attitude towards school (resistance) and by their academic self-concept

    (compensation). In line with earlier (qualitative) research (e.g., Green, 2003), our findings thus

    suggest that it is particularly during early and mid-adolescence that having a cool mobile phone

    matters as a means to gain status in the peer group. Older adolescents with a positive attitude

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293286

  • towards the mobile phone as a status object, on the other hand, were significantly more likely to

    own a high-status mobile phone, which is likely due to their higher expenditure.

    Finally, with respect to ethnicity, a striking difference between majority and minority youths is

    the direct negative relationship between academic self-concept and ethnic minority youths

    attitude towards the mobile phone as a status object (compensation), while a positive

    relationship was found between attitude towards school and attitude towards the mobile phone as

    a status object (thus, rejecting the resistance hypothesis). Moreover, parental SES was found to

    directly and positively predict ethnic minority youths smartphone ownership. The latter finding

    may indicate that, while high status mobile phones are no longer an adequate object of distinction

    for majority youths from higher parental SES, they may still serve this function within the ethnic

    minority subgroup. Additional research among this particular group that takes into account the

    intersectionality between ethnicity, SES and gender is necessary to further clarify these

    patterns of relationships.

    In conclusion, the results presented in this article can be seen as providing further evidence for

    Bourdieus (1979 [1984]) claim that, independent of adolescents social origin, the educational

    system determines cultural practices by manipulating the pupils self-image through ascribing

    identities of success or failure to them. Although the status signaling features of the mobile phone

    cannot be considered culturally deviant in the same sense as, for example, heavy metal music

    or violent or pornographic media contents, our findings indicate that the mechanisms expressed

    in Bourdieus cultural capital theory are also relevant for popular cultural artifacts, such as the

    mobile phone.

    The current study documents youths attitudes towards and their ownership of a smartphone in

    a time of flux. In 2010, the year in which our data were collected, mobile phones were still

    becoming commonplace; smartphone ownership was out of reach for most youths. Today,

    however, smartphones have become the standard among youths, with 81% of them owning such a

    phone in Flanders according to the most recent figures. Given that the media landscape is

    currently undergoing fundamental changesas we witness the convergence of almost all media

    forms into a multitude of personal mobile devices such as mobile phones, tablets and

    notebooksa pertinent question, then, is whether our results are not already outdated. The

    continuous advances in technology do not make research on youths use of them obsolete,

    however. On the contrary, answering questions with regard to how and why adolescents choose

    and use particular media technologies within their personal identity projects enables us to better

    understand not only how youths deal with their position in society and the developmental

    changes they undergo, but also the features of these technologies and how they enable and

    constrain human action. It is clear from the current study, for instance, that the personalness of

    mobile devices (see also Campbell and Park, 2008) attracts youths to incorporate these objects

    into their social image.

    Our results are relevant to the ongoing debate over the theoretical bases of Bourdieus cultural

    capital theory. Three of the major criticisms of this theory have been (1) that an ongoing process

    of individualization renders class differences irrelevant, (2) that even when lifestyle differences

    are found between social classes, differences in life styles no longer serve as a means for social

    exclusion or symbolic dominance, and (3) that highbrow culture has lost its significance

    (Prieur et al., 2008). With regard to the first criticism, the findings from our study make clear that

    the preference for a fairly functional object such as the mobile phone is not merely an individuals

    choice: adolescents preferences were linked to their performance in school, and thereby to their

    social class background. Social class background and the educational system are thus still

    powerful determinants of peoples cultural practices.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 287

  • The results of this study also strongly argue against the second criticism that any cultural

    differences found do not reflect symbolic dominance. That the elitedespite their financial

    resources, and in line with what the educational system deems appropriate for themprefers a

    more modest mobile phone can convincingly be interpreted as a rejection of an element of

    popular consumer culture. Conversely, adolescents who experience lower social standing will

    seek out popular culture as a means to conform to, compensate for, and sometimes even resist, the

    social position that the school, and society at large, assigns them.

    The third criticismthat cultural capital theory overly emphasizes highbrow culture, while in

    reality todays elite tends to be rather omnivorous in its cultural consumption (Peterson and Kern,

    1996)can also be recognized in our results. The mobile phone is not totally rejected by the

    elite, but ways are sought through which distinction can still be achieved. Prieur et al. (2008, p.

    53) mention in this regard that the character of the most valued cultural forms is changing.

    Among these changes is a decreasing value attributed to classical compared to scientific culture.

    Bourdieu (1979 [1984], p. 6) originally referred to elitist taste as the primacy of forms over

    function. Given that new media technologies are instrumental objects that leave little room for

    intellectual refinement in terms of the materiality of the device, we appear to be witnessing a

    reversal of function over form, whereby the elite differentiates itself by the ways in which they

    are used (e.g., Robinson, 2009); in the case of the mobile phone, then, the elite may proclaim only

    to need the technology to arrange their everyday lives. Indirect support for this idea can be

    found in a number of recent studies that point towards time poverty (e.g., Jackel and

    Wollscheid, 2007) and voraciousness (a high frequency of leisure participation in a range of

    activities; e.g., Sullivan and Katz-Gerro, 2007) as new ways in which the elite differentiates itself

    in contemporary society. The harriedness of everyday life, then, acts as a status symbol of the

    elite, which can be displayed by the frequent use of ones mobile phone to make the necessary

    arrangements to support this life.

    While this study offers valuable insights into how adolescents experiences in the school

    system structure their attitudes and use of the mobile phone as a status object, some limitations

    should be acknowledged. First, we need to point out the temporal and cultural specificity of this

    study. With respect to temporal specificity, smartphone ownership appears to have risen

    dramatically among Flemish teenagers over the past few years, and the dividing line between a

    smartphone and a regular mobile phone with multimedia features is gradually dissolving.

    The question thus arises whether the findings of this study can still be replicated today, and/or

    whether distinction is reflected in other aspects of mobile phone use (e.g., having access to

    mobile internet and/or a tablet PC). Additionally, we also need to take into account the fact that

    the mobile phone is just one feature among a wide range of potential status objects such as

    clothing and music. Future research is needed in order to clarify to what extent particular mobile

    phone preferences and practices occur in combination with other status markers that demarcate

    boundaries between social reference groups among youth. In addition, it remains to be seen

    whether, and to what extent, the status oriented uses of new media technologies such as the

    mobile phone replace older displays of compensation and resistance (such as involvement in

    visible subcultures and preference for socially disvalued music types) or whether these use

    patterns are merely incorporated alongside them.

    Cultural specificity may also be at stake. What is considered the standard of good taste can

    vary from one culture to the other. Given the differential uptake of mobile phone technology

    across the world, it is possible that in other cultures, particularly in the South, mobile phones

    themselves are still considered cutting-edge technologies with which the elite achieves

    distinction, although Peterson (2005) remarks that cultural standards tend to spread across

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293288

  • geographical boundaries before atrophying. Cultural specificity is certainly also at play with

    respect to the Flemish school systemwithin which the waterfall mechanism, for example, is

    deemed typical (Goedeme and Verbist, 2007).

    An important limitation of this study is the selection and operationalization of measures. The

    large number of missing values for parents educational and occupational level is unfortunate,

    particularly given that these missing values were not randomly distributed in our sample.

    Ethnicity is a complex and multi-dimensional concept, hence a measure that consists of multiple

    questions rather than a single-item on the languages spoken at home is advisable (Burton et al.,

    2010). With respect to adolescents attitude towards school, a more specific measure of sense of

    futility could have provided a better insight into resistance motives.

    Finally, we only included parental education level as a measure of adolescents cultural capital

    in our questionnaire, thereby not asking respondents about other important markers of cultural

    capital such as parental cultural participation and/or the cultural participation of youths

    themselves. DiMaggios (1982) study on cultural mobility and subsequent studies (e.g.,

    Aschaffenburg and Maas, 1997) on this matter have shown that adolescents cultural

    participation is a consistent and even better predictor of educational attainment than parental

    cultural participation. The evidence in support of cultural mobility may point towards a historical

    shift, where cultural participation now provides resources for children from less privileged

    backgrounds with which they can gain access to more privileged societal positions. In the light of

    the findings of the current study, it remains to be seen, however, whether certain kinds of

    involvement with new media technologies such as the mobile phone may present new avenues for

    cultural participation, and, as a result of that, for cultural mobility.

    Appendix A. Supplementary material

    Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001.

    References

    Aish, A.M., Joreskog, K.G., 1990. A panel model for political efficacy and responsiveness: an application of LISREL 7

    with weighted least squares. Quality and Quantity 24, 405426.

    Arbuckle, J.L., 2005. Amos 6.0 Users guide. Amos Development Corporation: Chicago.

    Aschaffenburg, K., Maas, I., 1997. Cultural and educational careers: the dynamics of social reproduction. American

    Sociological Review 62 (4) 573587.

    Baye, A., Benadusi, L., Bottani, N., Bove, G., Demeuse, M., Garca de Cortazar, M., Giancola, O., Gorard, S., Hutmacher,

    W., Matoul, A., Meuret, D., Morlais, S., Nicaise, J., Ricotta, G., Smith, E., Straeten, M.-H., Tiana-Ferrer, A.,

    Vandenderghe, V., 2005. Equity in European Education Systems: A Set of Indicators. Department of Theoretical and

    Experimental Education, Liege.

    Berndt, T.J., 1979. Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents. Developmental Psychology 15, 606616.

    Bennett, A., 1999. Subcultures or neo-tribes? Rethinking the relationship between youth, style and musical taste.

    Sociology 33 (3) 599617.

    Blair, B.L., Fletcher, A.C., 2011. The only 13-year-old on planet earth without a cell phone: meanings of cell phones in

    early adolescents everyday lives. Journal of Adolescent Research 26 (2) 155177.

    Bourdieu, P., 1979 (1984). Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste. Harvard University Press, Cambridge,

    MA.

    Bourdieu, P., 1986. The forms of cultural capital. In: Richardson, J.E. (Ed.), Handbook of Theory and Research for the

    Sociology of Education. Greenwood Press, New York, pp. 241258.

    Bourdieu, P., 1989. Social space and symbolic power. Sociological Theory 7, 1425.

    Bourdieu, P., Passeron, J., 1977. Reproduction in Education, Society and Culture. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA.

    M. Vanden Abeele and K. Roe / Poetics 41 (2013) 265293 289

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2013.03.001http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0005http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0005http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0550http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0550http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0010http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0010http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0015http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0030http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0030http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0545http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0545http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0545http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0035http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0304-422X(13)00013-2/sbref0040
  • Brake, M., 1980. The Sociology of Youth Culture and Youth Subcultures: Sex and Drugs and Rocknroll? Routledge and

    Kegan Paul, London.

    Broos, A., 2005. Gender and information and communication technologies (ICT) anxiety: male self-assurance and female

    hesitation. Cyberpsychology & Behavior 8 (1) 2131.

    Burton, J., Nandi, A., Platt, L., 2010. Measuring ethnicity: challenges and opportunities for survey research. Ethnic and

    Racial Studies 33 (8) 13321349.

    Byrne, B.M., 1990. Self-concept and academic achievement: investigating their importance as discriminators of academic

    track membership in high school. Canadian Journal of Education 15 (2) 173182.

    Campbell, S.W., Park, Y.J., 2008. Social implications of mobile telephony: the rise of personal communication society.

    Sociology Compass 2 (2) 371381.

    Caronia, L., Caron, A.H., 2004. Constructing a specific culture: young peoples use of the mobile phone as a

    social performance. Convergence: The International Journal of Research into New Media Technologies 10,

    2861.

    Castells, M., Fernandez-Ardevol, M., Linchuan Qiu, J., Sey, A., 2007. Mobile Communication and Society: A Global

    Perspective. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Chang, L., Arkin, R.M., 2002. Materialism as an attempt to cope with uncertainty. Psychology &